Next Article in Journal
Coaching Bilingual Speech-Language Student Clinicians and Spanish-Speaking Caregivers to Use Culturally Adapted NDBI Techniques with Autistic Preschoolers
Previous Article in Journal
Relationship Between Sleep Duration and Psychosocial Well-Being in Healthcare Personnel: Identification of Predictors and Vulnerability Patterns
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Systematic Review

Self-Controlled Feedback and Behavioral Outcomes in Motor Skill Learning: A Meta-Analysis

College of Physical Education, Yangzhou University, Yangzhou 225000, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2025, 15(9), 1291; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091291
Submission received: 15 August 2025 / Revised: 18 September 2025 / Accepted: 19 September 2025 / Published: 22 September 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Cognition)

Abstract

Providing feedback is a key instructional strategy in motor learning. Recently, interest has grown in self-controlled (SC) feedback, which allows learners to choose when to receive feedback. However, evidence on its effectiveness remains mixed, and its impact across different learning phases and populations is unclear. This meta-analysis assessed the effect of SC feedback on motor skill learning during acquisition, retention, and transfer phases, compared to passively received (PR) feedback and Yoked (YK) feedback groups. A comprehensive search of three datasets identified 29 studies comprising 1147 participants. Although SC feedback did not bring a significant advantage in the acquisition phase, it facilitated motor skill learning in both the retention and transfer phases. Moderator analyses revealed that cognitive status influenced SC feedback efficacy during the acquisition phase, with significant effects observed only in cognitively impaired individuals. Additionally, skill type moderated outcomes in the transfer phase, with series skills showing greater benefits than discrete skills, and this effect was significant only when compared to the YK group. In summary, SC feedback-enhanced motor skill learning in the retention and transfer phases, supporting its role in the consolidation and generalization of learning. Additionally, cognitively impaired individuals showed greater benefits during the acquisition phase, and series skills demonstrated more substantial gains in the transfer phase.

1. Introduction

Augmented feedback, defined as performance-related information from an external source that supplements sensory input (Moinuddin et al., 2021; Newell, 1976; Winstein & Schmidt, 1990), is a critical factor in motor skill learning, in conjunction with the amount of practice (Newell, 1976; Yadi, 2016). In most studies, augmented feedback has been provided as passively received (PR) information about motor skill performance, with the timing and frequency of feedback determined by the experimenter or another external source (Hermassi et al., 2019; Hicheur et al., 2020; Puklavec et al., 2021; Yamamoto et al., 2022). Recent research has indicated that providing learners with control over feedback, known as self-controlled (SC) feedback, can enhance motor skill learning (Carter et al., 2014; Chiviacowsky, 2014; van der Meer et al., 2024).
SC feedback allows learners to control and determine the timing of feedback (Carter et al., 2014; Carter & Patterson, 2012; Chiviacowsky, 2014; Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2002). Typically, studies comparing SC to PR feedback evaluate separate groups learning the same motor skill tasks (Hansen et al., 2011; Hebert & Coker, 2021). In these studies, SC groups control their feedback schedule autonomously, determining when to receive feedback. In contrast, the PR feedback schedule is controlled by researchers, coaches, peers, or others, thereby leaving learners unable to choose and only passively receiving feedback. Yoked (YK) feedback, a specialized PR feedback technique that features strict experimental control, is used as a comparator in numerous studies on SC feedback. As in PR, the timing of feedback in YK is not determined by the learner. However, unlike conventional PR, YK feedback schedules are synchronized with those of SC groups, ensuring identical temporal delivery of feedback. This method ensures that feedback characteristics are identical between the SC and YK groups, allowing the attribution of observed differences to the control of feedback rather than the type of feedback.
Janelle et al. (1995) were the first to test the effect of SC feedback on motor skill learning, and they demonstrated its benefit. SC feedback improves motor learning of several tasks, which include serial (Huet et al., 2009; Patterson & Carter, 2010; Woodard & Fairbrother, 2020) and discrete motor skill learning (Grand et al., 2015; Hebert & Coker, 2021; Souissi et al., 2021), and it enhances motor learning not only in healthy adults (Chiviacowsky, 2014; Couvillion et al., 2019; Hebert & Coker, 2021; Kaefer et al., 2014; Patterson et al., 2019; Post et al., 2016), but also in children (Chiviacowsky et al., 2008; Hemayattalab et al., 2013), elderly persons (Kwon et al., 2014), and patients with Parkinson’s disease (Chiviacowsky et al., 2012b) and cerebral palsy (Hemayattalab & Rostami, 2010).
However, it should be noted that some studies failed to replicate the benefits of SC feedback (Bacelar et al., 2022; McKay & Ste-Marie, 2020; McRae et al., 2015; St Germain et al., 2022). For example, learning the non-dominant arm bean bag toss task was not significantly improved by SC compared to YK feedback (Bacelar et al., 2022). In a serial-timing task, delayed retention test results showed a lower variable error for learners with PR compared to SC feedback (McRae et al., 2015). Therefore, the influence of SC feedback on motor skill learning continues to be debated. Because meta-analysis enables the extraction of data from numerous studies to quantify the effect of SC feedback on motor skill learning and identify sources of heterogeneity through the analysis of moderating variables, it is used to discuss the aggregated results of multiple studies (Page et al., 2021).
Existing meta-analyses on SC feedback have employed varying methods and produced conflicting conclusions, indicating a need for updated evaluation. For instance, Jimenez-Diaz et al. (2020) analyzed 18 articles published up to 2019 and reported significant benefits of SC feedback in acquisition that persisted into retention. However, their analysis focused on within-group performance changes over time, rather than the more common group-by-group comparison, and did not address the transfer phase, a critical indicator of skill generalization. In contrast, McKay et al. (2022, 2023) applied advanced bias-correction methods (e.g., weight-function models, p-curve analysis, robust Bayesian meta-analysis, z-curve), which challenged earlier overly optimistic interpretations and emphasized the need for larger and better-controlled studies. Research underscores the importance of both retention and transfer tests for evaluating the permanence and generalization of learned skills (Kantak & Winstein, 2012). While some studies reported SC feedback advantages in both retention and transfer (Carter et al., 2014; Hansen et al., 2011; van Maarseveen et al., 2018), others provided evidence for benefits only in transfer (Carter et al., 2017; Magill & Hall, 1990). Recent meta-analyses were last updated in 2019, excluding more recent studies (Bacelar et al., 2022; St Germain et al., 2022; van der Meer et al., 2024; Yantha et al., 2021). Moreover, although comparability was ensured by inclusion criteria, the aggregated analyses combined heterogeneous task types and participant populations with limited exploration of condition-specific effects, and some included experiments exhibited methodological inconsistencies such as imperfectly matched controls. These issues, alongside the publication and reporting biases already identified, highlight the need for updated meta-analytic work that incorporates recent studies, applies more nuanced subgroup analyses, and considers a broader range of outcomes.
Furthermore, the aforementioned meta-analysis concentrated on the effectiveness of SC feedback in healthy participants, neglecting factors such as the cognitive states of special populations, which are intrinsically linked to real-life motor skill learning. Individuals with cognitive impairments often exhibit deficits in motor proficiency, including posture, gross motor skills, and fine motor skills (Alesi & Battaglia, 2019; David et al., 2012; Fernandes et al., 2024; McQuillan et al., 2021). Fortunately, motor interventions have been shown to significantly enhance motor proficiency levels in these populations (Anderson et al., 2024; Fernandes et al., 2022), and SC feedback also holds potential to facilitate their motor skill learning. Another meta-analysis evaluated the effect of SC feedback on retention test performance measures (Hemayattalab, 2014; Pashabadi et al., 2021; Zamani et al., 2015). Although positive effects were found in published studies, they were insufficient to demonstrate that SC feedback is more effective than YK practice (McKay et al., 2022).Other moderator variables were selected based on their potential influence on motor skill learning. The environment (indoor vs. outdoor) was considered because outdoor settings provide higher ecological validity, whereas laboratory environments are more controlled and stringent, which may affect performance and learning outcomes. Feedback models likewise require further scrutiny. Verbal and visual feedback are common methods of providing SC feedback. In a double-mini trampoline task in which verbal feedback was used, skill progress in the retention test was significantly greater among recipients of SC compared to YK feedback (Ste-Marie et al., 2015). With visual feedback, young adults receiving SC feedback showed a greater learning advantage in the release slider task (Carter & Patterson, 2012). The role of feedback models on the impact of SC feedback on motor skill learning remains controversial. Mödinger et al. (2021) suggested that video-based visual feedback enhances motor learning more effectively than solely verbal feedback. Jaszczur-Nowicki et al. (2021) found that combining verbal with visual feedback proved more effective than providing either form alone. Therefore, this study also sought to clarify how cognitive states and feedback models modulate the impact of SC feedback on motor skill learning.
Consequently, we aimed to conduct a meta-analysis to assess the effects of SC feedback during the acquisition, retention, and transfer phases, while also exploring relevant moderating factors.

2. Materials and Methods

This study was reported in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines and was registered with the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO, CRD42023401718).

2.1. Search Strategy

Systematic literature searches were conducted in the PubMed, Web of Science and Cochrane library through December 2024. The following keywords were used: motor terms (“motor learning” OR “motor skill” OR “skill acquisition” OR “motor performance”), AND feedback terms (“augmented feedback” OR “self-controlled feedback” OR “self-regulated” OR “learner-controlled” OR “self-control”). To find additional pertinent papers, the reference lists of the retrieved articles were examined manually. Two impartial reviewers (B.W, T.T) independently conducted the initial screening using titles and abstracts. Full-text evaluation was used to further screen the remaining articles. Consensus and discussion with another two authors (Y.Y, W.G) facilitated the resolution of any disputes between the two reviewers.

2.2. Eligibility Criteria

Studies were included for this meta-analysis if the following criteria were met: (1) randomized controlled trial design; (2) intervention method: self-controlled feedback was used in the intervention group but not in the control group; (3) measurements of motor performance included at least one outcome that could be used to calculate an effect size; and (4) written in the English language. The following were excluded: (1) non-interventional studies; (2) editorials or conference abstracts; (3) studies that cannot extract relevant data or datasets that could not be used in the meta-analysis due to inconsistencies or extreme values; (4) review or theoretical articles; and (5) reports not retrieved.

2.3. Data Extraction and Statistical Analysis

The main outcome of this study was the standardized mean difference (SMD) of motor performance of motor skills calculated from the mean and standard deviation of the acquisition, retention, and transfer phases reported in the original study. The SMD between the experimental and control groups served as a measure of the intervention effect (Maher et al., 2003). Positive SMDs indicated intervention effectiveness. The pooled SMD was computed by averaging the effect sizes of each study. Statistics were quantified and effect sizes were calculated using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software (version 3.3). The homogeneity of interstudy effect size changes was tested based on the Q-statistic.
To evaluate the heterogeneity of the included studies, the I2-statistic was utilized. A fixed-effects model would be employed for analysis if the heterogeneity test resulted in p ≥ 0.05 and I2 < 50%, indicating no statistical heterogeneity between studies. A random effects model would be utilized if p < 0.05 and I2 ≥ 50%, indicating statistical heterogeneity between studies. Egger’s regression tests and funnel plots were employed to evaluate potential small study effects (publication bias, etc.).
In each phase, motor skill performance was determined based on the provided data. Acquisition phase performance, if not explicitly specified, was derived from the last block of acquisition data. For the retention and transfer phases, performance was calculated as the mean value of the corresponding blocks. In several studies, PR conditions included multiple groups with different feedback frequencies, and unclear selection of comparison groups could potentially bias the results. To ensure consistency and transparency, we selected the PR group with a feedback frequency most closely matching that of the SC group. When multiple eligible PR groups were present, either merging procedures or statistical adjustments were applied to avoid duplicate counting. When multiple measures were reported, we prioritized the primary outcome specified by the original authors (e.g., performance score). If no primary outcome was indicated, we selected the measure most relevant to the learning task (e.g., absolute constant errors in a key-pressing task).

2.4. Risk of Bias Assessment

Two reviewers assessed the included articles’ methodological quality independently. Quality was evaluated using the PEDro scale from the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (Maher et al., 2003). Before arriving at a conclusion, discordant ratings were resolved by consensus or discussion with author D. Potential quality scores for each item ranged from 0 to 10, with higher scores reflecting research of a better caliber.

3. Results

3.1. Study Characteristics

The meta-analysis included 29 eligible studies comprising 1147 participants in combined intervention groups, 555 in self-controlled (SC) groups, and 592 in passively received (PR) groups, including 470 in yoked (YK) groups. An overview of the selection process is provided in Figure 1. A detailed list of the studies in the “reports excluded” section and the corresponding rationale can be found in Supplementary File S1.
The mean participant age ranged from 7 to 70 years old; however, it is noteworthy that the majority of the research participants were college students. Sample sizes ranged from 12 to 152 (median sample size = 28). The main characteristics of the 29 articles are summarized in Table 1.

3.2. SC Versus PR Feedback in Acquisition Phase

The acquisition phase featured significant heterogeneity (Q (20) = 39.353, I2 = 49.178, p < 0.01), prompting the use of the random effects model. To confirm the results of the sensitivity analysis, a meta-analysis was conducted by sequential removal of single studies. After removal of single studies, I2 values ranged from 45.293 to 51.717, with little difference from the overall I2 = 49.178, with the exception of two studies (Kim et al., 2019; Lim et al., 2015), which shifted I2 to 38.065 and 28.856, respectively. Overall I2 values after removal of single studies are shown in Supplementary File S2. The funnel plot did not reveal significant asymmetry (Figure 2A), and the Egger’s test did not reveal publication bias (t = 1.550; df = 19; p = 0.137). Consequently, the meta-analysis of the acquisition phase included 21 studies and did not exclude any. Motor learning performance did not differ between SC and PR groups (SMD = 0.199, 95% confidence interval (CI) [−0.012, 0.411], p = 0.064; Figure 3).

3.3. SC Versus PR Feedback in Retention and Transfer Phases

The retention phase exhibited significant heterogeneity (Q(25) = 94.149, I2 = 73.446, p < 0.001), so the random effects model was used. In terms of sensitivity analysis, I2 values ranged from 70.623 to 74.506 after removal of single studies, with little difference from the overall I2 = 73.446, suggesting that all studies contributed to the stability of the overall results. The funnel plot revealed slight asymmetry (Figure 2B). The Egger’s test indicated a significant publication bias (t = 3.167; df = 24; p < 0.01). Sensitivity analysis revealed that the SMD fluctuated between 0.576 and 0.679 after excluding any single study, a range that encompassed the overall SMD, indicating a relatively stable effect. Consequently, the meta-analysis of retention included 26 studies.
The transfer phase also exhibited significant heterogeneity (Q(14) = 46.626, I2 = 69.974, p < 0.001); consequently, the random effects model was used. In terms of sensitivity analysis, I2 values ranged from 62.822 to 72.118 following the removal of single studies, with little difference from overall I2 = 58.292. The funnel plot revealed significant asymmetry. (Figure 2C). The Egger’s test indicated a significant publication bias (t = 4.272; df = 13; p < 0.001). Sensitivity analysis showed that the SMD ranged from 0.585 to 0.755 after excluding any single study, encompassing the overall SMD and indicating a relatively stable effect. Consequently, the meta-analysis of transfer included 15 studies.
The SC groups demonstrated superior motor learning performance compared to the PR groups in both the retention (SMD = 0.630, 95%CI [0.364, 0.897], p < 0.001) and transfer phases (SMD = 0.684, 95%CI [0.356, 1.012], p < 0.001; Figure 3).

3.4. Moderator Analysis of SC Compared to PR Feedback

The results of the moderator analysis, including SMD values, 95% CIs, and homogeneity test statistical values, are summarized in Table 2. The following variables were analyzed: cognitive status (normal or impaired); feedback model (verbal or visual); environment (outdoor or indoor); skill type (series or discrete); and mean participant age (adult or adolescent). Skill level was not analyzed due to the small sample size (n < 3), and for the same reason, cognitive and environment status were not analyzed in the transfer phase.
Moderating variables had specific effects on motor skill learning outcomes during different learning phases. In the acquisition phase, significant heterogeneity was observed between the two cognitive status subgroups (Q(1) = 4.895, p < 0.05). Specifically, SC feedback improved motor performance significantly among cognitively impaired participants (SMD = 0.789, 95% CI [0.246, 1.333], p < 0.01) but not among cognitively normal participants (SMD = 0.130, 95% CI [−0.082, 0.343], p = 0.229). Regarding the feedback model, neither verbal (SMD = 0.118, 95% CI [−0.150, 0.387], p = 0.388) nor visual (SMD = 0.270, 95% CI [−0.034, 0.575], p = 0.082) feedback exerted significant effects. Neither indoor (SMD = 0.205, 95% CI [−0.076, 0.486], p = 0.152), nor outdoor (SMD = 0.241, 95% CI [−0.098, 0.580], p = 0.163) environments brought significant effects. No significant intervention effects were observed for discrete (SMD = 0.087, 95% CI [−0.020, 0.287], p = 0.087) or series (SMD = 0.446, 95% CI [−0.705, 1.598], p = 0.448) skill types or for adolescents (SMD = 0.332, 95% CI [−0.421, 1.084], p = 0.388) or adults (SMD = 0.176, 95% CI [−0.030, 0.381], p = 0.093).
In the retention phase, significant intervention effects were observed for both normal (SMD = 0.535, 95% CI [0.275, 0.797], p < 0.001) and impaired (SMD = 1.768, 95% CI [0.479, 3.056], p < 0.01) cognitive status; verbal (SMD = 0.666, 95% CI [0.306, 1.025], p < 0.001) and visual (SMD = 0.598, 95% CI [0.206, 0.990], p < 0.01) feedback models; for both outdoor (SMD = 0.550, 95% CI [0.098, 1.002], p < 0.05) and indoor (SMD = 0.656, 95% CI [0.321, 0.990], p < 0.001) environments; for both series (SMD = 0.628, 95% CI [0.085, 1.170], p < 0.05) and discrete (SMD = 0.630, 95% CI [0.291, 0.968], p < 0.001) skill types; and for both adolescents (SMD = 1.072, 95% CI [0.430, 1.715], p < 0.01) and adults (SMD = 0.533, 95% CI [0.246, 0.821], p < 0.001).
In the transfer phase, significant intervention effects were observed for both visual (SMD = 0.941, 95% CI [0.445, 1.437], p < 0.001) and verbal (SMD = 0.372, 95% CI [0.009, 0.735], p < 0.05) feedback; for both series (SMD = 1.040, 95% CI [0.697, 1.383], p < 0.001) and discrete (SMD = 0.553, 95% CI [0.157, 0.948], p < 0.01) skill types; and for both adolescents (SMD = 0.704, 95% CI [0.269, 1.140], p < 0.01) and adults (SMD = 0.719, 95% CI [0.289, 1.140], p < 0.01).

3.5. SC Versus YK Feedback in Three Phases

Considering that the YK group had better control over experimental conditions since it matched the feedback schedule (number of times, order, etc.) to that of the SC group, and noting that most studies used the YK group as the control for SC, this meta-analysis included only studies comparing the SC and YK groups. The analysis focused on these groups under strict control conditions to assess differences. Consequently, two effect sizes from one study (Janelle et al., 1995) and all data from five studies (Ahmadi et al., 2011; Hebert & Coker, 2021; Hemayattalab, 2014; Patterson et al., 2019; Zamani et al., 2015) were excluded.
The three phases of acquisition (Q(15) = 35.328, I2 = 57.541, p < 0.01), retention (Q(20) = 58.795, I2 = 65.984, p < 0.001), and transfer (Q(13) = 43.932, I2 = 70.409, p < 0.001) indicated significant heterogeneity; consequently, the random effects model was used. Sensitivity analyses indicated that most studies made stable contributions to the overall results (Supplementary File S2), with the exceptions of two studies in the acquisition phase (Kim et al., 2019; Lim et al., 2015) and one effect size from one study in the transfer phase (Carter et al., 2014) that caused a large change in I2 values after removal. The funnel plot exhibited symmetry similar to that observed with PR (see Figure 2D–F). Egger’s test for the acquisition phase (t = 0.929, df = 14, p = 0.369) indicated no significant publication bias, while significant publication bias was found in the retention (t = 2.863, df = 19, p < 0.01) and transfer (t = 3.896, df = 12, p < 0.01) phases. Sensitivity analysis revealed that, overall, SMDs remained within the fluctuation ranges of the retention (from 0.462 to 0.745) and transfer (from 0.545 to 0.722) phases after the removal of individual studies, indicating good stability. Consequently, 16 studies were included in the acquisition phase, 21 in the retention phase, and 14 in the transfer phase for the final meta-analysis.
Figure 4 depicts a summary of the random effects meta-analysis and the corresponding forest plot showing SMDs and 95% CIs. The findings were consistent with those of the meta-analysis of SC vs. PR. In the acquisition phase, motor learning performance was similar for the SC and YK groups (SMD = 0.175, 95%CI [−0.087, 0.437], p = 0.191). However, the SC groups demonstrated superior motor learning performance compared to the YK groups in both the retention (SMD = 0.695, 95%CI [0.434, 0.955], p < 0.001) and transfer (SMD = 0.648, 95%CI [0.315, 0.981], p < 0.001) phases.

3.6. Moderator Analysis of SC Compared to YK Feedback

The results of the moderator analysis including SMD values, 95% CIs, and homogeneity test statistical values are summarized in Table 3. The following variables were analyzed: feedback model (verbal or visual); environment (outdoor or indoor); skill type (series or discrete), and mean participant age (adult or adolescent). Cognitive status and skill level were not analyzed due to the small sample size (n < 3), and for the same reason, environment and mean age were not analyzed in the transfer phase. Moderating variables have different effects on motor skill learning outcomes in different learning phases. The results showed an overall pattern similar to that of the moderator analysis of SC vs. PR.
In the acquisition phase, neither verbal (SMD = 0.092, 95% CI [−0.326, 0.509], p = 0.667) nor visual (SMD = 0.234, 95% CI [−0.118, 0.586], p = 0.192) feedback; indoor (SMD = 0.155, 95% CI [−0.177, 0.487], p = 0.361) nor outdoor (SMD = 0.309, 95% CI [−0.221, 0.839], p = 0.254) environments; discrete (SMD = 0.098, 95% CI [−0.077, 0.273], p = 0.272) nor series (SMD = 0.630, 95% CI [−1.042, 2.301], p = 0.460) skill types; and adolescent (SMD = 0.079, 95% CI [−0.872, 1.029], p = 0.871) nor adult (SMD = 0.199, 95% CI [−0.070, 0.467], p = 0.147) age groups exerted significant effects.
In the retention phase, significant intervention effects were observed for both verbal (SMD = 0.646, 95% CI [0.401, 0.891], p < 0.001) and visual (SMD = 0.763, 95% CI [0.342, 1.184], p < 0.001) feedback; for both indoor (SMD = 0.694, 95% CI [0.370, 1.018], p < 0.001) and outdoor (SMD = 0.722, 95% CI [0.346, 1.098], p < 0.001) environments; series (SMD = 0.793, 95% CI [0.277, 1.310], p < 0.01) and discrete (SMD = 0.649, 95% CI [0.311, 0.986], p < 0.001) skill types; and for both adolescents (SMD = 0.748, 95% CI [0.329, 1.167], p < 0.001) and adults (SMD = 0.690, 95% CI [0.387, 0.993], p < 0.001).
In the transfer phase, significant heterogeneity was observed between the two skill type subgroups (Q(1) = 4.435, p < 0.05). Specifically, significant intervention effects were observed for both series (SMD = 1.040, 95% CI [0.697, 1.383], p < 0.001) and discrete (SMD = 0.478, 95% CI [0.083, 0.873], p < 0.05) skill types and for both verbal (SMD = 0.372, 95% CI [0.009, 0.735], p < 0.05) and visual (SMD = 0.900, 95% CI [0.381, 1.418], p < 0.01) feedback.

4. Discussion

We conducted a meta-analysis of 29 quantitative studies to examine the effect of SC feedback in enhancing performance in motor skill learning. Across retention and transfer phases, SC feedback outperformed both PR and YK feedback. However, it did not provide a significant advantage during the acquisition phase, except among cognitively impaired participants. These findings complement earlier work but also highlight the need for careful methodological comparison when evaluating SC feedback’s influence on skill acquisition.
Our results diverge somewhat from Jimenez-Diaz et al. (2020), who reported gains in acquisition that persisted into retention. In contrast, we found SC feedback benefits were more evident in retention and transfer. Key differences include the number of studies (18 vs. 29 through 2024) and the analytic approach: they assessed “remaining” learning from acquisition to retention, whereas we compared performance under different feedback conditions within each phase. Additionally, prior work did not analyze transfer performance, a critical indicator of skill generalization, whereas our meta-analysis found significant advantages for SC feedback. Despite some publication bias, effect sizes remained robust. Our focus was on addressing specific research questions rather than introducing new statistical methods (McKay et al., 2023).
The benefits of SC feedback can be understood within the OPTIMAL (Optimizing Performance through Intrinsic Motivation and Attention for Learning) theory of motor learning (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016). According to this framework, motivational and attentional factors enhance motor performance and learning by strengthening the coupling of goals to actions. SC feedback promotes learner autonomy, which enhances performance expectancies and provides a sense of control and competence, increasing interest in the task and self-efficacy (Chiviacowsky et al., 2012a; Ryan & Deci, 2000). In addition, receiving feedback at self-selected times may produce a rewarding effect, triggering dopamine release, which facilitates memory consolidation and skill learning. SC feedback also encourages active engagement with the task, supporting deeper cognitive processing and more effective retention and transfer of skills (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016). Together, these mechanisms provide a coherent, theory-based explanation for the observed advantages of SC feedback across retention and transfer phases.
Our meta-analysis corroborates findings that SC feedback’s immediate effects in acquisition can vary depending on feedback frequency. In a few studies, participants receiving SC feedback at a high rate (60–70%) exhibited worse acquisition performance than those in YK conditions (Kim et al., 2019; Tsai & Jwo, 2015). Overreliance on feedback can promote trial-and-error without substantive performance gains, possibly reflecting an arbitrary approach to learning. Although frequent feedback may increase motivation, it can also trigger maladaptive short-term corrections (Schmidt, 1991). Future empirical work is needed to clarify how feedback frequency shapes SC feedback’s influence on skill learning (Hebert & Coker, 2021).
We found that skill type significantly moderated SC feedback’s transfer-phase benefits, with serial tasks showing greater improvement than discrete tasks, particularly when compared to YK. Serial tasks often demand continuous coordination and integration of multiple sub-skills, which may encourage learners to engage more deeply with feedback and apply it more strategically (Patterson & Carter, 2010; Patterson et al., 2011). Engaging with SC feedback under these cognitively demanding conditions may facilitate deeper processing and more flexible learning strategies, thereby supporting stronger transfer performance (Hansen et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2019). Notably, when comparing SC with PR in serial tasks, we observed an effect size in favor of SC feedback in the transfer phase. However, this effect did not reach statistical significance, and thus should be interpreted with caution.
SC feedback’s benefits were especially pronounced among cognitively impaired learners in the acquisition phase. Compared with typically developing peers, these individuals may value feedback opportunities more and engage with them more attentively, thereby extracting greater learning benefits (Zamani et al., 2015). For instance, children with Down syndrome spontaneously adopted a fading feedback schedule to avoid overreliance (Chiviacowsky et al., 2012c). Similarly, SC feedback has been shown to benefit children with ADHD (Pashabadi et al., 2021) and cerebral palsy (Hemayattalab, 2014; Hemayattalab et al., 2013). This tendency to use feedback more deliberately may explain why cognitively impaired learners demonstrated greater gains than healthy participants during the acquisition phase. However, data limitations restricted our moderator analyses largely to the acquisition phase when comparing SC and PR. Further research is needed to solidify our understanding of cognitive status as a moderator.
This study has several limitations. Firstly, many of the included primary studies had relatively small sample sizes, which reduces statistical power, may increase susceptibility to sampling bias, and limits the reliability of the results. Secondly, the majority of participants were college students, restricting the generalizability of the findings to broader populations and age groups. Future research should therefore aim to recruit larger and more diverse samples, ideally through multi-site designs, to enhance statistical power and external validity. Thirdly, although sensitivity analyses suggested the stability of the results, Egger’s tests indicated the presence of publication bias in both the retention and transfer phases, which might have inflated the effect size estimates. Future studies in this field would benefit from strategies to mitigate publication bias, such as preregistration of study protocols, open sharing of data and materials, and greater efforts to publish null findings. Addressing these limitations will not only improve the robustness of the evidence base but will also provide more solid guidance for the practical application of self-controlled feedback in motor learning.

5. Conclusions

This meta-analysis highlights the positive impact of self-controlled (SC) feedback on motor skill learning, particularly in terms of retention and transfer. By examining moderating factors such as cognitive state, skill type, and feedback frequency, the findings contribute to a deeper understanding of how SC feedback can be strategically implemented to optimize learning outcomes.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/bs15091291/s1, Supplementary File S1: Excluded studies with exclusion reason; Supplementary File S2: Overall I values after removal of single studies.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.W. and W.G.; methodology, B.W.; software, T.T. and Y.Y.; validation, B.W. and W.G.; formal analysis, T.T.; writing—original draft preparation, B.W., T.T. and Y.Y.; writing—review and editing, W.G.; visualization, T.T.; supervision, W.G.; funding acquisition, B.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Social Science Fund of China (21CTY013).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

Sincere thanks are extended to the authors of the primary studies included in this systematic review and meta-analysis, whose work provided the essential foundation for this research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
SCSelf-Controlled
PRPassively Received
YKYoked
PRISMAPreferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis
SMDStandardized Mean Difference

References

  1. Ahmadi, P., Sabzi, H. A., Heirani, A., & Hasanvand, B. (2011). The effect of feedback after good, poor, good poor trials, and self-control conditions in an acquisition and learning of force production task. Facta Universitatis. Series Physical Education and Sport, 9, 35–43. [Google Scholar]
  2. Alesi, M., & Battaglia, G. (2019). Motor development and Down syndrome. In International review of research in developmental disabilities: State of the art of research on down syndrome (pp. 169–211). Elsevier Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson, J. T., Toolan, C., Coker, E., Singer, H., Pham, D., Jackson, N., Lord, C., & Wilson, R. B. (2024). A novel dance intervention program for children and adolescents with developmental disabilities: A pilot randomized control trial. BMC Sports Science Medicine and Rehabilitation, 16(1), 109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bacelar, M. F. B., Parma, J. O., Cabral, D., Daou, M., Lohse, K. R., & Miller, M. W. (2022). Dissociating the contributions of motivational and information processing factors to the self-controlled feedback learning benefit. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 59, 102119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Batista, M. T. S., Figueiredo, L. S., Martins, C. A., Nogueira, N., Ferreira, B. D., & Benda, R. N. (2022). Expectation of teaching and self-controlled KR in motor skills acquisition: Are there additive effects? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 94(2), 391–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Carter, M. J., Carlsen, A. N., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2014). Self-controlled feedback is effective if it is based on the learner’s performance: A replication and extension of Chiviacowsky and Wulf (2005). Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Carter, M. J., & Patterson, J. T. (2012). Self-controlled knowledge of results: Age-related differences in motor learning, strategies, and error detection. Human Movement Science, 31(6), 1459–1472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Carter, M. J., Smith, V., Carlsen, A. N., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2017). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the primary motor cortex does not enhance the learning benefits of self-controlled feedback schedules. Psychological Research, 82(3), 496–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Chiviacowsky, S. (2014). Self-controlled practice: Autonomy protects perceptions of competence and enhances motor learning. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15(5), 505–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2002). Self-controlled feedback: Does it enhance learning because performers get feedback when they need it? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 73(4), 408–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., de Medeiros, F. L., Kaefer, A., & Tani, G. (2008). Learning benefits of self-controlled knowledge of results in 10-Year-Old children. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79(3), 405–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2012a). Self-controlled learning: The importance of protecting perceptions of competence. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., Lewthwaite, R., & Campos, T. (2012b). Motor learning benefits of self-controlled practice in persons with Parkinson’s disease. Gait & Posture, 35(4), 601–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., Machado, C., & Rydberg, N. (2012c). Self-controlled feedback enhances learning in adults with Down syndrome. Brazilian Journal of Physical Therapy, 16(3), 191–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Couvillion, K. F., Bass, A. D., & Fairbrother, J. T. (2019). Increased cognitive load during acquisition of a continuous task eliminates the learning effects of self-controlled knowledge of results. Journal of Sports Sciences, 38(1), 94–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. David, F. J., Baranek, G. T., Wiesen, C., Miao, A. F., & Thorpe, D. E. (2012). Coordination of precision grip in 2–6 years-old children with autism spectrum disorders compared to children developing typically and children with developmental disabilities. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Fernandes, J. M., de Milander, M., & van der Merwe, E. (2022). The effect of a motor intervention programme for learners identified with moderate to severe intellectual disabilities. Heliyon, 8(10), e11165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Fernandes, J. M., de Milander, M., & van der Merwe, E. (2024). Motor proficiency of learners with moderate to severe intellectual disabilities. African Journal of Disability, 13, 1262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gil, M. J., & Kim, J. G. (2023). Effects of self-controlled feedback on the performance and learning of easy versus difficult golf putting in college students. Asia-Pacific Journal of Convergent Research Interchange (APJCRI), 9(9), 745–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Grand, K. F., Bruzi, A. T., Dyke, F. B., Godwin, M. M., Leiker, A. M., Thompson, A. G., Buchanan, T. L., & Miller, M. W. (2015). Why self-controlled feedback enhances motor learning: Answers from electroencephalography and indices of motivation. Human Movement Science, 43, 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Hansen, S., Pfeiffer, J., & Patterson, J. T. (2011). Self-control of feedback during motor learning: Accounting for the absolute amount of feedback using a yoked group with self-control over feedback. Journal of Motor Behavior, 43(2), 113–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Hebert, E. P., & Coker, C. (2021). Optimizing feedback frequency in motor learning: Self-controlled and moderate frequency KR enhance skill acquisition. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 128(5), 2381–2397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Hemayattalab, R. (2014). Effects of self-control and instructor-control feedback on motor learning in individuals with cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(11), 2766–2772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Hemayattalab, R., Arabameri, E., Pourazar, M., Ardakani, M. D., & Kashefi, M. (2013). Effects of self-controlled feedback on learning of a throwing task in children with spastic hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(9), 2884–2889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hemayattalab, R., & Rostami, L. R. (2010). Effects of frequency of feedback on the learning of motor skill in individuals with cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(1), 212–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hermassi, S., Sellami, M., Bouhafs, E. G., Schwesig, R., & De Giorgio, A. (2019). Effect of verbal instruction on motor learning ability of anaerobic and explosive exercises in physical education university students. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Hicheur, H., Chauvin, A., Cavin, V., Fuchslocher, J., Tschopp, M., & Taube, W. (2020). Augmented-feedback training improves cognitive motor performance of soccer players. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 52(1), 141–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Huet, M., Camachon, C., Fernandez, L., Jacobs, D. M., & Montagne, G. (2009). Self-controlled concurrent feedback and the education of attention towards perceptual invariants. Human Movement Science, 28(4), 450–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Janelle, C. M., Kim, J., & Singer, R. N. (1995). Subject-controlled performance feedback and learning of a closed motor skill. Percept Mot Skills, 81(2), 627–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Jaszczur-Nowicki, J., Romero-Ramos, O., Rydzik, L., Ambrozy, T., Biegajlo, M., Nogal, M., Wiśniowski, W., Kruczkowski, D., Łuszczewska-Sierakowska, I., & Niznikowski, T. (2021). Motor learning of complex tasks with augmented feedback: Modality-dependent effectiveness. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(23), 12495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed Central]
  31. Jimenez-Diaz, J., Chaves-Castro, K., & Morera-Castro, M. (2020). Effect of self-controlled and regulated feedback on motor skill performance and learning: A Meta-analytic study. Journal of Motor Behavior, 53(3), 385–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Kaefer, A., Chiviacowsky, S., Meira, C. d. M., & Tani, G. (2014). Self-controlled practice enhances motor learning in introverts and extroverts. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 85(2), 226–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kantak, S. S., & Winstein, C. J. (2012). Learning–performance distinction and memory processes for motor skills: A focused review and perspective. Behavioural Brain Research, 228(1), 219–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kim, Y., Kim, J., Kim, H., Kwon, M., Lee, M., & Park, S. (2019). Neural mechanism underlying self-controlled feedback on motor skill learning. Human Movement Science, 66, 198–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kwon, S.-M., Lee, S.-M., & Lee, H.-W. (2014). The effect of transformed high frequency self-controlled feedback on learning of older adult’s gate ball stroke task. Korean Journal of Sports Science, 23(4), 485–495. [Google Scholar]
  36. Lim, S., Ali, A., Kim, W., Kim, J., Choi, S., & Radlo, S. J. (2015). Influence of self-controlled feedback on learning a serial motor skill. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 120(2), 462–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Magill, R. A., & Hall, K. G. (1990). A review of the contextual interference effect in motor skill acquisition. Human Movement Science, 9(3), 241–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Maher, C. G., Sherrington, C., Herbert, R. D., Moseley, A. M., & Elkins, M. (2003). Reliability of the PEDro scale for rating quality of randomized controlled trials. Physical Therapy, 83(8), 713–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. McKay, B., Bacelar, M. F. B., Parma, J. O., Miller, M. W., & Carter, M. J. (2023). The combination of reporting bias and underpowered study designs has substantially exaggerated the motor learning benefits of self-controlled practice and enhanced expectancies: A meta-analysis. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18(1), 242–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. McKay, B., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2020). Autonomy support and reduced feedback frequency have trivial effects on learning and performance of a golf putting task. Human Movement Science, 71, 102612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. McKay, B., Yantha, Z., Hussien, J., Carter, M., & Ste-Marie, D. (2022). Meta-analytic findings in the self-controlled motor learning literature: Underpowered, biased, and lacking evidential value. Meta-Psychology, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. McQuillan, A. V., Swanwick, A. R., Chambers, E. M., Schlüter, K. D., & Sugden, A. D. (2021). A comparison of characteristics, developmental disorders and motor progression between children with and without developmental coordination disorder. Human Movement Science, 78, 102823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. McRae, M., Patterson, J. T., & Hansen, S. (2015). Examining the preferred self-controlled KR schedules of learners and peers during motor skill learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 47(6), 527–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Moinuddin, A., Goel, A., & Sethi, Y. (2021). The role of augmented feedback on motor learning: A systematic review. Cureus, 13(11), e19695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Mödinger, M., Woll, A., & Wagner, I. (2021). Video-based visual feedback to enhance motor learning in physical education—A systematic review. German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research, 52(3), 447–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Newell, K. M. (1976). Motor learning without knowledge of results through the development of a response recognition mechanism. Journal of Motor Behavior, 8(3), 209–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Nijmeijer, E. M., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., McCrory, S., Cortes, N., & Benjaminse, A. (2023). How to improve movement execution in sidestep cutting? Involve me and I will learn. Human Movement Science, 90, 103115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., Shamseer, L., Tetzlaff, J. M., Akl, E. A., Brennan, S. E., & Chou, R. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Bmj-British Medical Journal, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Pashabadi, A., Salehi, Y., & Pakzamir, F. (2021). Effects of self-controlled feedback on learning a sports skill in children with ADHD: Performance under pressure. Life Span and Disability, 24(2), 221–235. [Google Scholar]
  50. Patterson, J. T., & Carter, M. (2010). Learner regulated knowledge of results during the acquisition of multiple timing goals. Human Movement Science, 29(2), 214–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Patterson, J. T., Carter, M., & Hansen, S. (2013). Self-controlled KR schedules: Does repetition order matter? Human Movement Science, 32(4), 567–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Patterson, J. T., Carter, M., & Sanli, E. (2011). Decreasing the proportion of self-control trials during the acquisition period does not compromise the learning advantages in a self-controlled context. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82(4), 624–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Patterson, J. T., McRae, M., & Hansen, S. (2019). On whether task experience of the peer differentially impacts feedback scheduling and skill acquisition of a learner. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Post, P. G., Aiken, C. A., Laughlin, D. D., & Fairbrother, J. T. (2016). Self-control over combined video feedback and modeling facilitates motor learning. Human Movement Science, 47, 49–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Puklavec, A., Antekolović, L., & Mikulić, P. (2021). Acquisition of the long jump skill using varying feedback. Croatian Journal of Education, 23(1). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Schmidt, R. A. (1991). Frequent augmented feedback can degrade learning: Evidence and interpretations. In Tutorials in motor neuroscience (pp. 59–75). Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  58. Souissi, M. A., Souissi, H., Elghoul, Y., Masmoudi, L., Trabelsi, O., Ammar, A., Chtourou, H., & Souissi, N. (2021). Information processing and technical knowledge contribute to self-controlled video feedback for children learning the snatch movement in weightlifting. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 128(4), 1785–1805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Ste-Marie, D. M., Carter, M. J., Law, B., Vertes, K., & Smith, V. (2015). Self-controlled learning benefits: Exploring contributions of self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation via path analysis. Journal of Sports Sciences, 34(17), 1650–1656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. St Germain, L., Williams, A., Balbaa, N., Poskus, A., Leshchyshen, O., Lohse, K. R., & Carter, M. J. (2022). Increased perceptions of autonomy through choice fail to enhance motor skill retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 48(4), 370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Tsai, M.-J., & Jwo, H. (2015). Controlling absolute frequency of feedback in a self-controlled situation enhances motor learning. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 121(3), 746–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. van der Meer, B. R., van den Hoven, M. A. C., van der Kamp, J., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2024). Self-controlled video feedback facilitates the learning of tactical skills in tennis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 95(2), 537–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. van Maarseveen, M. J. J., Oudejans, R. R. D., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2018). Self-controlled video feedback on tactical skills for soccer teams results in more active involvement of players. Human Movement Science, 57, 194–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Winstein, C., & Schmidt, R. (1990). Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 677–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Woodard, K. F., & Fairbrother, J. T. (2020). Cognitive loading during and after continuous task execution alters the effects of self-controlled knowledge of results. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2016). Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(5), 1382–1414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Yadi, S. (2016, August 28). The role of augmented feedback on motor skill learning. 6th International Conference on Educational, Management, Administration and Leadership, Bandung, Indonesia. [Google Scholar]
  68. Yamamoto, R., Akizuki, K., Yamaguchi, K., Yabuki, J., & Kaneno, T. (2022). A study on how concurrent visual feedback affects motor learning of adjustability of grasping force in younger and older adults. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 10755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Yantha, Z., McKay, B., & Ste-Marie, D. (2021). The recommendation for learners to be provided with control over their feedback schedule is questioned in a self-controlled learning paradigm. Journal of Sports Sciences, 40, 769–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Zamani, M. H., Fatemi, R., & Soroushmoghadam, K. (2015). Comparing the effects of self-controlled and examiner-controlled feedback on learning in children with developmental coordination disorder. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, 9(4), e2422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Selection process for the meta-analysis.
Figure 1. Selection process for the meta-analysis.
Behavsci 15 01291 g001
Figure 2. Funnel plots for the three phases. (AC) compare SC and PR feedback, while (DF) compare SC and YK feedback.
Figure 2. Funnel plots for the three phases. (AC) compare SC and PR feedback, while (DF) compare SC and YK feedback.
Behavsci 15 01291 g002
Figure 3. Forest plot of the self-controlled (SC) group compared to the passively received (PR) group. Black squares represent effect sizes from individual studies; red diamonds indicate the pooled effects for the acquisition (a), retention (r), and transfer (t) phases; purple diamonds indicate the overall effect across all phases.
Figure 3. Forest plot of the self-controlled (SC) group compared to the passively received (PR) group. Black squares represent effect sizes from individual studies; red diamonds indicate the pooled effects for the acquisition (a), retention (r), and transfer (t) phases; purple diamonds indicate the overall effect across all phases.
Behavsci 15 01291 g003
Figure 4. Forest plot of the self-controlled (SC) group compared to the yoked (YK) group. Black squares represent effect sizes from individual studies; red diamonds indicate the pooled effects for the acquisition (a), retention (r), and transfer (t) phases; purple diamonds indicate the overall effect across all phases.
Figure 4. Forest plot of the self-controlled (SC) group compared to the yoked (YK) group. Black squares represent effect sizes from individual studies; red diamonds indicate the pooled effects for the acquisition (a), retention (r), and transfer (t) phases; purple diamonds indicate the overall effect across all phases.
Behavsci 15 01291 g004
Table 1. Main characteristics of included studies.
Table 1. Main characteristics of included studies.
StudyFeedback TypePhaseSample SizeSkill LevelCognitive
Status
Mean
Age
EnvironmentLearning TaskFeedback
Model
Number of Trials
During Acquisition
Janelle et al. (1995)YKA R36UN19.6OThrowing taskVe40
Patterson and Carter (2010)YKR T24UN21.4IKey-pressingVi90
Ahmadi et al. (2011)PRA R48NN22.3IForce production taskVi72
Hansen et al. (2011)YKA R T16UN21.8IKey-pressingVi80
Patterson et al. (2011)YKR T20NN22.3IKey-pressingVi90
Carter and Patterson (2012)YKR40UN45.95ILow-friction slide taskVi60
Patterson et al. (2013)YKR T48UN21.3IKey-pressingVi90
Carter et al. (2014)YKA R T16UN21.35ILow-friction slider taskVi60
Chiviacowsky (2014)YKA R28NN22.5ICoincident-anticipation timingVe30
Hemayattalab (2014)PRA R T15NI12.26IThrowing taskVi100
Grand et al. (2015)YKA R T36NN23.1IThrowing taskVe60
Lim et al. (2015)YKA R24NN27.2ITaekwondo PoomsaeVi72
Ste-Marie et al. (2015)YKR92NN11.1IDouble-mini trampolineVe60
Tsai and Jwo (2015)YKA R T24UN25.1IHand gripVe60
Zamani et al. (2015)PRA R12NI10IThrowing taskVe240
Post et al. (2016)YKA44NN21.8OGolf puttingVi60
Kim et al. (2019)YKA R T42NN16.91IKey-pressingVi200
Patterson et al. (2019)PRA R24UN21.9IKey-pressingVi80
Woodard and Fairbrother (2020)YKT24NN20.38IContinuous tracing taskVe32
Hebert and Coker (2021)PRA R95NN21.08OThrowing taskVe50
Pashabadi et al. (2021)YKA R T30NI9OFootball chip pass skillVe60
Souissi et al. (2021)YKA24EN10.84IWeightlifting snatchVi144
Yantha et al. (2021)YKR T40NN21.77OGolf puttingVe50
Batista et al. (2022)YKR T20NN24.86IThrowing taskVe45
Bacelar et al. (2022)YKA R T100NN20.65IThrowing taskVi100
St Germain et al. (2022)YKA R T152NN20.64IRapid aiming taskVi72
Gil and Kim (2023)YKA R28NN22.3IGolf puttingVe20
Nijmeijer et al. (2023)YKA R22NN22.9USidestep cutting taskVi20
van der Meer et al. (2024)YKA R23EN43.3OTennisVi20
Note: Feedback type: PR—passively received; YK—yoked, YK represents inclusion in both the PR and YK groups, while PR represents inclusion only in the PR group. Phase: A—acquisition; R—retention; T—transfer. Skill level: U—unspecified; N—novice; E—experts. Cognitive status: N—normal; I—impaired. Environment: U—unspecified; I—indoor; O—outdoor. Feedback model: Ve—verbal; Vi—visual.
Table 2. Moderator analysis for the SC vs. PR feedback in three phases.
Table 2. Moderator analysis for the SC vs. PR feedback in three phases.
PhaseModeratorLevelNo. of StudiesSMD95%CIHomogeneity Test
Qdfp
AcquisitionCognitive status *Normal180.130[−0.082, 0.343]4.89510.027
Impaired30.789 **[0.246, 1.333]
Feedback modelVerbal80.118[−0.150, 0.387]0.53810.463
Visual130.270[−0.034, 0.575]
EnvironmentIndoor150.205[−0.076, 0.486]0.02610.872
Outdoor50.241[−0.098, 0.580]
Skill typeSeries40.446[−0.705, 1.598]0.27810.598
Discrete150.134[−0.020, 0.287]
Mean ageAdult160.176[−0.030, 0.381]0.15410.695
Adolescent50.332[−0.421, 1.084]
RetentionCognitive statusNormal230.535 ***[0.275, 0.797]3.37310.066
Impaired31.768 **[0.479, 3.056]
Feedback modelVerbal110.666 ***[0.306, 1.025]0.06210.803
Visual150.598 **[0.206, 0.990]
EnvironmentIndoor200.656 ***[0.321, 0.990]0.13610.713
Outdoor50.550 *[0.098, 1.002]
Skill typeSeries80.628 *[0.085, 1.170]0.00110.995
Discrete160.630 ***[0.291, 0.968]
Mean ageAdult210.533 ***[0.246, 0.821]2.25210.133
Adolescent51.072 **[0.430, 1.715]
TransferFeedback modelVerbal60.372 *[0.009, 0.735]3.28810.070
Visual90.941 ***[0.445, 1.437]
Skill typeSeries51.040 ***[0.697, 1.383]3.33410.068
Discrete90.553 **[0.157, 0.948]
Mean ageAdult120.719 **[0.289, 1.066]0.00810.929
Adolescent30.704 **[0.269, 1.140]
Note: SMD: standardized mean difference; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Moderator analysis for the SC vs. YK feedback in three phases.
Table 3. Moderator analysis for the SC vs. YK feedback in three phases.
PhaseModeratorLevelNo. of StudiesSMD95%CIHomogeneity Test
Qdfp
AcquisitionFeedback modelVerbal60.092[−0.326, 0.509]0.26110.609
Visual100.234[−0.118, 0.586]
EnvironmentIndoor110.155[−0.177, 0.487]0.23310.630
Outdoor40.309[−0.221, 0.839]
Skill typeSeries30.630[−1.042, 2.301]0.38510.535
Discrete110.098[−0.077, 0.273]
Mean ageAdult130.199[−0.070, 0.467]0.05710.812
Adolescent30.079[−0.872, 1.029]
RetentionFeedback modelVerbal90.646 ***[0.401, 0.891]0.22310.637
Visual120.763 ***[0.342, 1.184]
EnvironmentIndoor160.694 ***[0.370, 1.018]0.01210.912
Outdoor40.722 ***[0.346, 1.098]
Skill typeSeries70.793 **[0.277, 1.310]0.21110.646
Discrete120.649 ***[0.311, 0.986]
Mean ageAdult180.690 ***[0.387, 0.993]0.04810.827
Adolescent30.748 ***[0.329, 1.167]
TransferFeedback modelVerbal60.372 *[0.009, 0.735]2.67010.102
Visual80.900 ***[0.381, 1.418]
Skill type *Series51.040 ***[0.697, 1.383]4.43510.035
Discrete80.478 *[0.083, 0.873]
Note: SMD: standardized mean difference; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wang, B.; Tao, T.; Yuan, Y.; Guo, W. Self-Controlled Feedback and Behavioral Outcomes in Motor Skill Learning: A Meta-Analysis. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 1291. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091291

AMA Style

Wang B, Tao T, Yuan Y, Guo W. Self-Controlled Feedback and Behavioral Outcomes in Motor Skill Learning: A Meta-Analysis. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(9):1291. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091291

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wang, Biye, Tao Tao, Yuchen Yuan, and Wei Guo. 2025. "Self-Controlled Feedback and Behavioral Outcomes in Motor Skill Learning: A Meta-Analysis" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 9: 1291. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091291

APA Style

Wang, B., Tao, T., Yuan, Y., & Guo, W. (2025). Self-Controlled Feedback and Behavioral Outcomes in Motor Skill Learning: A Meta-Analysis. Behavioral Sciences, 15(9), 1291. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091291

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop