Next Article in Journal
Artificial Intelligence Perceptions and Technostress in Staff Radiologists: The Mediating Role of Artificial Intelligence Acceptance and the Moderating Role of Self-Efficacy
Previous Article in Journal
Differential Modulation of Attention by Aversive Associative and Statistical Learning in Distinct Visual Search Modes
Previous Article in Special Issue
Authoritative Filial Piety Rather than Reciprocal Filial Piety Mediated the Relationship Between Parental Support, Career Decision Self-Efficacy, and Discrepancies Between Individual-Set and Parent-Set Career Goals
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

‘Deconstructing Stereotypes to Build Consent’: Evaluation of a Project on Social and Sexual Relationships in Adolescence

1
Department of Psychology, University of Turin, 10124 Turin, Italy
2
AMAE, 10121 Turin, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2025, 15(9), 1275; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091275
Submission received: 7 August 2025 / Revised: 11 September 2025 / Accepted: 15 September 2025 / Published: 18 September 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Psychological Research on Sexual and Social Relationships)

Abstract

Evidence from the international literature indicates alarming prevalence rates associated with various forms of intimate partner violence since adolescence. To prevent gender-based violence and increase psychological well-being in intimate relationships, both the scientific literature and policy makers agree on the importance of implementing specific prevention and education programs targeting adolescents. The purpose of this longitudinal study was to evaluate the impact of an educational intervention to promote awareness of one’s relationship with one’s own body, stereotypes related to gender and sexuality, and the issue of sexual consent. Participants were adolescents aged 15–16 years who filled a questionnaire prior to the start of the intervention (Time 0, N = 192, 55.7% male) and two weeks following its conclusion (Time 1, N = 178, 53.9% male). Results indicate that compared to Time 0, after participation, body surveillance, benevolent sexism, and endorsement of the sexual double standard decreased, while no significant effect emerged in relation to the issue of sexual consent. Implications for research and intervention are discussed, with the goal of providing useful guidance for those implementing interventions for young people to address intimate partner violence and promote relationship well-being.

1. Introduction

The onset of individuals’ dating relationships commonly takes place during adolescence, a developmental stage that plays a crucial role in shaping sexual attitudes, identity, and psychological adjustment (Collins, 2003; Javidi et al., 2020). However, adolescence is also recognized as a period when the first instances of intimate partner violence (IPV) may arise (Lozano-Martínez et al., 2022; Vagi et al., 2015), potentially resulting in serious short- and long-term harm to victims’ well-being. In this framework, the concept of teen dating violence (TDV) refers to psychological, physical, and sexual abuse occurring within romantic relationships among adolescents aged 13 to 19 (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020). Such abusive behaviors can occur in person as well as through digital means, including persistent messaging or the non-consensual distribution of sexually explicit content (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020; Cutter-Wilson & Richmond, 2011).
International studies investigating TDV report concerning prevalence rates. In Spain, nearly 30% of students admitted to perpetrating at least one form of dating abuse, and 35% reported being victims (Lozano-Martínez et al., 2022). Similarly, over a third of Canadian teens experienced psychological TDV in a year (Hébert et al., 2017), while 20% of U.S. adolescents faced electronic dating abuse (Hinduja & Patchin, 2021). In addition, around 20% of girls aged 16–17 who have been in a relationship reported having experienced psychological abuse, such as insults, blaming, and intimidation (Díaz-Aguado Jalón, 2022). Consistent with international data on gender-based violence (European Commission, 2022), girls are disproportionately affected by physical and sexual victimization, while boys more frequently engage in controlling and coercive behaviors (Clayton et al., 2023; Malhi et al., 2020; Wincentak et al., 2017).
Scholars have consistently documented a significant relationship between IPV and long-term physical and mental health issues, including anxiety, depression (Johnson et al., 2014), and eating disorders (Ackard et al., 2007). Girl victims are also more likely to engage in risky behaviors like unprotected sex (Teitelman et al., 2008), binge drinking (Foshee et al., 2013), and substance abuse (Ackard et al., 2007; Parker & Bradshaw, 2015). TDV has also been associated with self-harm and suicidal tendencies (Holmes & Sher, 2013; Quarshie, 2021). In school settings, female victims often report low self-esteem, concentration difficulties, and social withdrawal (Díaz-Aguado Jalón et al., 2014). Lastly, TDV increases the risk of experiencing abusive relationships later in life (Exner-Cortens et al., 2017; Teitelman et al., 2008).

1.1. Psychosocial Factors Associated with TDV

Several psychosocial factors have been identified as influential in shaping the perception of gender-based abuse, including self-objectification processes, sexism, and sexual double standards (SDSs). Self-objectification refers to a form of self-perception in which individuals prioritize their body’s appearance over its function, thereby conceptualizing themselves as a collection of body parts rather than as a whole (Daniels et al., 2020; Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). This phenomenon frequently manifests as persistent body surveillance, defined as the continual monitoring of how closely one’s appearance conforms to culturally valued ideals (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Rollero, 2022). In recent decades, the rising prevalence of social media use has been implicated in the increase in an objectifying self-perspective among both women and men (Ozimek et al., 2023). However, body surveillance is particularly widespread among young women and adolescent girls, with significant repercussions for their physical, psychological, and social well-being (Skowronski et al., 2021; Tiggemann & Slater, 2015). In particular, body surveillance appears to be positively correlated with heightened appearance anxiety, and a higher risk of developing eating disorders, depression, and sexual dysfunction (Moradi & Huang, 2008; Saunders & Eaton, 2018; Wollast et al., 2019). Furthermore, research has identified a direct link between IPV victimization and body surveillance. In this regard, drawing on the self-objectification framework, Davidson and Gervais (2015) examined the relations between IPV and body image-related variables, highlighting that having experienced IPV directly predicted individual levels of body surveillance. In addition, self-objectification significantly mediated the relationship between IPV and body surveillance, while body surveillance significantly mediated the relationship between IPV and body shame (i.e., a form of dissatisfaction toward one’s own body). Overall, women with a history of IPV appear to be more likely to experience body surveillance and body shame compared to those without such experiences (Davidson & Gervais, 2015).
Moreover, the relationship between sexism and individuals’ attitudes toward IPV has been widely investigated drawing on ambivalent sexism theory (Glick & Fiske, 1996). According to this framework, on the one hand, the display of overtly negative attitudes toward women (i.e., hostile sexism) can lead to minimizing the severity of gender-based abuse, including the damaging consequences suffered by victims; on the other hand, viewing women as pure and in need of protection (i.e., benevolent sexism) may foster victim-blaming attitudes when traditional gender norms are perceived as violated (Angelone et al., 2021; Chapleau et al., 2007; Rollero & Tartaglia, 2019). A direct association has been found between the aforementioned dimensions and adolescents’ perception of TDV. Specifically, hostile sexism is linked to a greater likelihood to perpetrate TDV (Fernández-Fuertes et al., 2018; Madrona-Bonastre et al., 2023), while benevolent sexism supports the justification of such violence (Fasanelli et al., 2020).
Furthermore, SDS refers to the tendency to evaluate identical sexual behaviors differently, depending on whether they are exhibited by males or females (M. D. C. Gómez Berrocal et al., 2019; C. Gómez Berrocal et al., 2022). Among adolescents, the belief that girls and boys should adhere to distinct sexual norms—emphasizing modesty for girls and sexual assertiveness for boys—has notable implications for individuals’ sexual behavior and psychosocial well-being. For example, early adolescent girls who reported having engaged in sexual intercourse experienced a significant decline in peer acceptance over time, whereas their male counterparts saw a significant increase (Kreager et al., 2016). Consistently, having a higher number of sexual partners is positively associated with peer acceptance among boys, but negatively associated with peer acceptance among girls (Kreager & Staff, 2009). The display of SDS positively correlates with the endorsement of ambivalent sexist beliefs (Zaikman & Marks, 2014). Moreover, it significantly influences both the perception and the propensity to perpetrate gender-based violence, particularly in the context of adolescent dating relationships (Guerra-Marmolejo et al., 2021; Martínez-Gómez et al., 2021).
In addition, adolescents’ attitudes toward TDV are closely intertwined with their understanding of sexual consent, defined as “one’s freely given verbal or nonverbal communication of their sober and conscious feelings of willingness to engage in a particular sexual behavior, with a particular person, within a particular context” (Willis et al., 2019, p. 227). Indeed, sexual consent represents the determining factor between “just” sexual intercourse and sexual abuse, thereby playing a crucial role in the development of safe and respectful sexual relationships (Fenner, 2017). The ability to communicate internal consent (i.e., the subjective experience of willingness to engage in sexual activity; Jozkowski et al., 2014) has also been linked with enhanced sexual satisfaction and enjoyment, especially among young women (Marcantonio et al., 2020). However, although adolescents—particularly girls—appear to endorse supportive attitudes toward affirmative consent (Javidi et al., 2020), they often exhibit limited sexual negotiation skills, which remain heavily influenced by traditional gender norms (Mfeka-Nkabinde et al., 2024). In particular, notable gender discordance persists in how sexual consent and refusal are both communicated and interpreted, particularly concerning the reliance on verbal vs. non-verbal cues (Richards et al., 2022; Righi et al., 2021).
Holding positive attitudes toward mutual sexual consent has been associated with increased sexual well-being and overall psychological adjustment (Espinosa-Hernández et al., 2024). In addition, school-based interventions that address sexual communication and challenge gender stereotypes have been identified as protective factors, reducing the risk of both perpetration and victimization in various forms of gender-based violence (DeGue et al., 2014) and increasing adolescents’ willingness to intervene in situations involving sexual aggression (Shekar et al., 2020). In this regard, comprehensive sexuality education programs that incorporate discussions on consent, respect, and emotional literacy may be crucial in fostering the development of safe and respectful relationships among adolescents (e.g., Foshee et al., 2005). However, the implementation of such programs is rarely accompanied by scientific evaluation, thereby limiting the ability to demonstrate their effectiveness and hindering their integration into established public health strategies.

1.2. The Use of Psychodrama in Educational Settings

Classical psychodrama (action of the soul, from psyche, and drama) is an experiential, action-based approach conceptualized by psychiatrist Jacob Levi Moreno (Lim et al., 2021). Psychodrama promotes symmetrical participation and dialogic interaction within the group, aligning with socio-constructivist principles of learning (Watson, 2001). Within this framework, the psychodramatist assumes the role of facilitator, fostering a learning environment oriented toward spontaneity, mutual recognition, and co-construction of meaning. Participants are thus encouraged to articulate their “naïve theories,” personal beliefs, and emotional responses in a protected, non-judgmental space that supports both expression and active listening. Through guided reflection and reformulation by the facilitator, individual contributions are integrated into a shared body of knowledge, constructed collaboratively within the group.
Psychodrama and/or other experiential methods have been increasingly applied in school-based interventions aimed at promoting socio-emotional competencies and preventing aggressive behaviors among students. Previous studies have shown that psychodrama can reduce school-related anxiety and disruptive behaviors among adolescents, while fostering empathy, self-awareness, and mental resilience (Giannakopoulos et al., 2012; Ulusoy et al., 2023). Moreover, school-based experiential programs using active, participatory approaches appear to be particularly effective in improving social and emotional skills, while reducing aggressive and/or violent behaviors (Durlak et al., 2011; Healy et al., 2020). Within the field of TDV prevention, multi-level interventions that engage schools, families, and communities have been identified as the most promising strategies to promote change (De Koker et al., 2014). Building on this evidence, our intervention integrates psychodrama-based techniques to stimulate critical reflection on TDV-related attitudes and myths, thereby fostering awareness, encouraging constructive dialogue among peers, and promoting the internalization of healthier beliefs and behaviors.

1.3. The Project

The project “Growing Up as Equals: Deconstructing Stereotypes to Build a New Consent” builds on a previous initiative titled “Getting Excited, Falling in Love, Experiencing Pleasure: A Journey Towards Awareness.” Both projects were designed for third-year upper secondary school students according to the Italian education system. “Growing Up as Equals” was developed and implemented by the association AMAE APS in collaboration with various professionals, including a midwife sexual health consultant. “Amae” is a Japanese term that refers to the emotional state of comfort and well-being experienced when someone acknowledges their need for support and entrusts themselves to the care of another. The project aims to promote awareness and prevent gender-based violence, with a particular focus on romantic relationships during adolescence.
The Psychology Department of the University of Turin contributed to the evaluation of the project by designing a specific questionnaire and analyzing the data. The questionnaire was administered prior to the start of the sessions (T0) and two weeks following their conclusion (T1).
Between October 2024 and March 2025, a team of psychologists and trainers from AMAE conducted the intervention across eleven third-year classes: eight from Scientific High Schools (both Traditional and Applied Sciences tracks) and three from Technical Institutes. The intervention comprised four sessions of two hours each per class. Three sessions were facilitated by professionals with expertise in psychology, psychotherapy, and/or psychodrama, while one session (the third one) was led by a midwife with additional qualifications as a sexual consultant.
Overall, the purpose of the “Growing as Equals” project was to create a relational space for dialogue and exchange, where each person could feel listened to and acknowledged, experiencing a sense of active participation in the process. Active and participatory methods were employed (e.g., interactive discussions, stories and narratives to identify with, role-playing and role training, photographic storytelling techniques, body awareness). These methods were intended to encourage the emergence of questions and doubts, while also facilitating a temporary state of cognitive disorientation and the subsequent development of new insights and more functional ways of relating to oneself and others (Menzies et al., 2022).
In the first session, for example, four cardboards were placed in the corners of the classroom. Each one displayed a type of violence: physical, psychological, economic, and sexual. In the center of the room, an additional cardboard was placed, indicating witnessed (or secondary) violence. The facilitator read various statements aloud to the class, each describing a situation about violence, and asked the participants to physically position themselves near the cardboard that corresponds to the type of violence portrayed. [Students frequently recognized that multiple forms of violence, such as physical and psychological, can coexist in the same situation. The concept of witnessed violence, though less familiar, was often reported as being particularly impactful and emotionally significant].
In the second session, the group engaged in an in-depth exploration of the sexual objectification of bodies. Through the analysis of various advertisements, participants were encouraged to identify those that depict the body, particularly its features and parts, in a reductive and objectifying manner, primarily for commercial purposes. This exercise led to a critical reconstruction of dominant representations of male and female bodies, as well as a reflection on the impact of objectification on self-image and on the internalization of unrealistic body ideals, often associated with anxiety and frustration.
The third session, led by a midwife and sexual health consultant, had the goal of sharing knowledge about the anatomy and physiology of the genital organs, along with a sexuality map, to broaden perspectives on the sexual experience. The discussion also addresses topics such as the negotiation of choices like contraception and the concept of consent in sexual activity.
In the fourth and final session, which focused on the topic of consent, short stories were used as a starting point for discussion. Working in small groups, students read these stories and were invited to identify the characters’ physical sensations, emotions, and thoughts, as well as to reflect on the question “In this story, was consent respected?”. The stories, drawn from real-life situations experienced by adolescents, addressed scenarios linked to the so-called “rape pyramid,” including street harassment and sexually inappropriate remarks.
Beyond the specific objectives of each session, all activities were designed to contribute to the broader aim of deconstructing gender stereotypes (Phillips & De Roos, 2025; Santoniccolo et al., 2023) and promoting open, flexible models of identity and relationships. These models support the expression of each individual’s uniqueness and encourage forms of interaction grounded in mutuality and respect. This process is essential for fostering healthy and mutually respectful interpersonal and sexual relationships.

1.4. The Current Study: Objectives and Hypotheses

To assess the impact of the educational intervention described above, a longitudinal study was conducted. Drawing on the previously outlined literature, the study aimed to examine four primary dimensions.
First, we sought to investigate whether engagement in the intervention influenced individual levels of body surveillance. Therefore, we compared participants’ body surveillance before and after the program. We hypothesized the following:
H1. 
Participation in the project would reduce individual levels of body surveillance.
Our second goal was to evaluate the effect of participation in the intervention on participants’ sexist attitudes. Specifically, we assessed individual levels of both hostile and benevolent sexism before and after engagement with the program. We hypothesized the following:
H2. 
Participation in the project would lead to a reduction in both hostile (H2a) and benevolent (H2b) sexism.
The third aim was to examine the intervention’s effect on individual levels of SDS. To this end, participants’ SDS scores were measured prior to and following their involvement in the project. We expected the following:
H3. 
SDS scores would decrease after participation in the intervention.
Finally, the fourth objective was to explore the intervention’s effect on attitudes toward sexual consent. To address this aim, we assessed participants’ attitudes toward establishing sexual consent before and after the intervention. We hypothesized the following:
H4. 
Participation in the project would enhance positive attitudes toward establishing sexual consent.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedure

A formal agreement with the high school involved had been signed. Prior to participating in the project (T0) and two weeks after the final activity (T1), all students from the classes involved in the educational initiative were invited to complete an online survey. The provided link directed them to a secure and anonymous questionnaire platform. Before starting the survey, both students and their parents reviewed an informed consent form and signed a privacy agreement. Questionnaires at T0 and T1 were administered in class during regular school hours. In accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, participants were informed that their involvement was voluntary and that they could withdraw from the study at any point. No incentives or payments were offered for participation. The Ethics Committee of the University of Turin, Italy approved the study protocol (protocol number 0454951—25 July 2024).
Participants were 192 students (55.7% male, 42.2% female, and 2.1% non-binary/prefer not to say). They all attended the third year of a high school in Turin, Italy. At T1, 178 students responded (53.9% male, 39.9% female, and 6.2% non-binary/prefer not to say). The 14 students who did not take part in the T1 survey were absent from school on that specific day. However, their absence was due to incidental reasons and was not related to an explicit withdrawal or refusal to participate in the study.

2.2. Measures

Data were collected by means of a self-report questionnaire, which took approximately 20 min to complete.
In both waves, the questionnaire included the following measures:
  • The Body Surveillance subscale of the Objectified Body Consciousness Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996). It measures the extent to which individuals habitually think about how their body looks to others, rather than how it feels or functions. This subscale consists of 8 items (e.g., “During the day, I think about how I look many times”, α at T0 = 0.82, 95% CI [0.78, 0.86], α at T1 = 0.84, 95% CI [0.80, 0.88]) rated on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicated that participants were more likely to engage in body surveillance.
  • The short version of the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (Glick & Fiske, 1996; Rollero et al., 2014) including 12 items measuring benevolent sexism (6 items, e.g., “Women should be cherished and protected by men”, α at T0 = 0.69, 95% CI [0.62, 0.76], α at T1 = 0.75, 95% CI [0.69, 0.81]) and hostile sexism (6 items, e.g., “Once a woman gets a man to commit to her, she usually tries to put him on a tight leash”, α at T0 = 0.85, 95% CI [0.81, 0.89], α at T1 = 0.88, 95% CI [0.84, 0.92]). The items were rated on a 6-point point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Higher scores denoted higher levels of sexism.
  • The Acceptance of Sexual Double Standard subscale of the Sexual Double Standard Scale (M. D. C. Gómez Berrocal et al., 2019). It includes 9 items assessing the endorsement of the sexual double standard (e.g., “It’s worse for a woman to sleep around than it is for a man”, α at T0 = 0.75, 95% CI [0.69, 0.81], α at T1 = 0.81, 95% CI [0.76, 0.86]). The items were rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 3 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicated that participants held higher levels of sexual double standards.
  • The Lack of Perceived Behavioral Control and the Positive Attitudes toward Establishing Consent subscales of the Sexual Consent Scale-Revised (Humphreys & Brousseau, 2010; Rollero et al., 2023). Each subscale contains 11 items (Lack of Perceived Behavioral Control, e.g., “I am worried that my partner might think I’m weird or strange if I asked for sexual consent before starting any sexual activity”, α at T0 = 0.89, 95% CI [0.86, 0.92], 95% CI [0.86, 0.92], α at T1 = 0.88, 95% CI [0.85, 0.91]; Positive Attitudes toward Establishing Consent, e.g., “I feel that sexual consent should always be obtained before the start of any sexual activity”, α at T0 = 0.89, 95% CI [0.86, 0.92], α at T1 = 0.91, 95% CI [0.88, 0.94]). The items were rated on a 7-point point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Higher scores in the Lack of Perceived Behavioral Control subscale reflected decreased perceived control during sexual activity, while higher scores in the Positive Attitudes toward Establishing Consent subscale indicated major propensity to establish explicit sexual consent.

2.3. Data Analyses

Prior to conducting analyses, we assessed the normality of the data using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Results indicated that all variables met the assumption of normality (p > 0.05), supporting the use of parametric tests. Then, we carried out zero-order correlations between scales and independent sample t-tests to assess gender differences on the study variables at T0. The same analyses were replicated on scores reported at T1. Finally, paired sample t-tests were performed to assess whether participants’ scores on the investigated variables changed between T0 and T1. Listwise deletion was applied for analyses where missing data occurred. In addition, non-binary students were excluded from the gender-based analyses due to insufficient sample size for meaningful statistical comparison. However, they were included in all other analyses that did not involve gender as a variable. All statistical analyses were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics version 28.0 software.

3. Results

As shown in Table 1, at Time 0, body surveillance was positively related to favorable attitudes toward consent and negatively related to hostile sexism. In turn, hostile sexism was positively linked to benevolent sexism, sexual double standard, and lack of perceived behavioral control, and negatively linked to positive attitudes toward consent. Lack of perceived behavioral control was associated with sexual double standard, whereas positive attitudes toward establishing consent were negatively related to sexual double standard and lack of perceived control.
Concerning gender differences on the study variables at Time 0, as shown in Table 2, women outscored men on body surveillance and positive attitudes toward establishing consent. On the contrary, men reported higher levels of hostile sexism, sexual double standard, and lack of perceived behavioral control. No significant gender differences emerged in relation to benevolent sexism.
Zero-order correlations between scales were then conducted on scores at Time 1. As shown in Table 3, the correlations between the variables were essentially analogous to those observed at T0. The only discrepancy observed was that at T1; the correlation between body surveillance and positive attitudes toward establishing consent was no longer significant.
As seen in Table 4, gender differences at Time 1 were analogous to those reported at Time 0.
Finally, paired sample t-tests were conducted to assess whether participants’ scores on the investigated variables changed between T0 and T1. As can be seen in Table 5, the results showed that body surveillance (H1), benevolent sexism (H2b), and acceptance of sexual double standard (H3) decreased. In contrast, hostile sexism (H2a), lack of perceived behavioral control, and positive attitudes toward establishing consent (H4) did not change substantially.

4. Discussion

This study aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of an intervention that focused on the importance of consent in intimate relationships with adolescents aged 15–16 years, attending the third year of upper secondary school. The study analyzed the relationship between multiple dimensions related to TDV and compared participants scores before and after the intervention. Most of our initial hypotheses were confirmed, suggesting the effectiveness of the school-based intervention in reducing dimensions that scholars consider influential in shaping the perception of gender-based violence (e.g., Davidson & Gervais, 2015; Fernández-Fuertes et al., 2018; Fasanelli et al., 2020; Guerra-Marmolejo et al., 2021).
Concerning the correlational findings, our results generally aligned with the previous psychosocial literature (e.g., Jozkowski et al., 2017; Zaikman & Marks, 2014). However, with specific regard to body surveillance, findings at both Time 0 and Time 1 revealed a positive correlation with the display of positive attitudes toward sexual consent, and a negative correlation with hostile sexism. Concerning the first result, both body surveillance and the propensity to establish explicit sexual consent may be rooted in a heightened sensitivity to the issue of control, referring both to individual bodily appearance and to the preservation of agency during sexual activity. Concerning the second finding, the negative association between body surveillance and hostile sexism may reflect individuals’ perceived necessity to conform to societal standards, in order to gain social approval (Salomon & Brown, 2019). Indeed, while benevolent sexism still appears to be widely accepted (Connelly & Heesacker, 2012), in recent years hostile sexism has been increasingly stigmatized. However, the correlational nature of these results does not allow for any causal inference and thus requires to be further investigated.
In accordance with our hypotheses, data showed a significant decrease in body surveillance, benevolent sexism, and sexual double standard between T0 and T1 after participating in the intervention at school, thereby confirming hypotheses H1, H2b, and H3 for both adolescent girls and boys, respectively. In particular, the changes observed in body surveillance highlight the importance of fostering a collective reflection on the socially constructed nature of bodily perception, physical sensations, and, above all, the gendered norms perpetuated by media discourses regarding how human bodies should appear (Harper & Tiggemann, 2008; Rollero, 2015; Skowronski et al., 2021). This process may facilitate, in turn, the development of a more authentic connection with one’s bodily experiences, particularly in contexts where the body plays a central role, such as intimate and sexual encounters. In addition, it is noticeable that these variables continue to differ between boys and girls despite their decline, with the latter showing lower values at both T0 and T1. Even though the values for male students remain higher, a change can be associated with participation in the project for both genders.
In contrast, participants’ levels of hostile sexism (H2a) and positive attitudes toward giving consent (H4) did not change significantly following the conclusion of the intervention. Concerning H2, this result may be partly attributed to adolescents’ varying levels of pre-existing awareness regarding the two forms of sexism addressed in the intervention. Indeed, hostile sexism is generally expressed through overtly antagonistic attitudes toward women and, in recent decades, has seen a marked decline in social acceptance, particularly among younger generations (Gomes et al., 2022). Benevolent sexism, by contrast, typically manifests in more subtle forms and is still perceived as more socially legitimate, making it more difficult to recognize and critically evaluate (Dai, 2024). Therefore, it is plausible that the intervention offered participants an opportunity to critically reflect, perhaps for the first time, on some of their implicit benevolent sexist beliefs, thereby fostering a process of deconstruction.
Finally, with regard to H4, it should be noted that at T1, unlike at T0, the relationships between positive attitudes towards consent and the other dimensions analyzed are weaker. This suggests that the intervention may have facilitated a critical reflection that undermines the link between representations of gender relations and the legitimacy of TDV.

Limitations

This study has some limitations. First, the single-group, test–retest design inherently prevents drawing strong causal inferences. Indeed, without a control or comparison group, the observed changes cannot be certainly attributed to the intervention rather than to other confounding factors. Future studies may employ randomized controlled designs to more rigorously assess causal relationships and isolate the specific effects of the intervention. Additionally, the small sample size limits the statistical power of the comparative analysis between T0 and T1 and restricts the generalizability of the findings. Moreover, although the present study employed primarily univariate analyses to assess pre–post changes in the target variables, we acknowledge the potential value of incorporating multivariate approaches—particularly those that explore mediating and moderating mechanisms—in future research. However, given the relatively limited sample size in our study, we opted not to conduct more complex statistical procedures, as small samples can lead to biased estimates and unstable standard errors in multivariate models (Schönbrodt & Perugini, 2013). We encourage future evaluations of similar interventions, especially those with larger and more diverse samples, to adopt multivariate frameworks that are theoretically grounded and statistically robust.
Regarding the structure of the questionnaire, in order to minimize participant fatigue, we did not include specific measures to assess individual social desirability bias. Future studies should consider incorporating validated measures of social desirability in order to enhance the validity of self-reported data. Moreover, the questionnaire did not include any specific quality control indicators (e.g., attention check items); hence, although data were collected in a supervised classroom setting, we were not able to ensure that participants did not engage in mindless responding. Furthermore, the instruments we used to measure the dimensions involved, which were developed and validated in previous studies, were not specifically designed for this intervention. As a result, some terms and concepts may not fully align with the verbal expressions employed by the team of professionals who led the school intervention. For example, while the notion of consent was addressed broadly throughout the intervention, the questionnaire explored the more narrowly defined concept of sexual consent (Willis et al., 2019). Finally, some of the dimensions addressed by the school intervention may require more time to produce measurable results. Indeed, while short interventions can raise awareness and promote initial reflection on the topics involved, the attitudes and myths surrounding TDV are strongly rooted in individuals’ core beliefs and may not fully shift through only a few sessions. The literature on brief school-based interventions indicates that, although early changes in attitudes can be promising, more intimate or deeply held beliefs typically require longer engagements to change substantially (e.g., Giannakopoulos et al., 2012). Nonetheless, brief programs can initiate cognitive and emotional processes that may continue to unfold and strengthen over time. A follow-up study conducted several months post-intervention could reveal slower and more profound changes in participants’ attitudes and perception of TDV, thereby providing a more comprehensive understanding of the intervention’s sustained impact.

5. Conclusions

Despite these limitations, this study offers a valuable contribution by providing a scientific evaluation of the intervention. By addressing the underlying social, emotional, and cognitive factors that contribute to harmful gender norms and relationship dynamics, such interventions can play a pivotal role in promoting safe and respectful adolescent dating relationships. In this regard, although several scholars (e.g., DeGue et al., 2014) have emphasized the importance of implementing primary prevention programs in schools to reduce adolescents’ risk of gender-based violence, rigorous evaluations of such interventions remain limited. Therefore, systematic assessment is essential not only to refine and enhance program effectiveness, but also to guide the strategic allocation of resources, including public and private funding.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.B., A.F., M.M., and C.R.; methodology, E.B., A.F., E.C., C.D.C., I.L., M.M., and C.R.; formal analysis, C.R.; investigation, E.B., A.F., E.C., C.D.C., I.L., M.M., and C.R.; data curation, E.B. and C.R.; writing—original draft preparation, E.B., A.F., E.C., C.D.C., I.L., M.M., and C.R.; writing—review and editing, E.B. and M.H.; visualization, E.B. and M.H.; supervision, C.R., A.F., and M.M.; project administration, C.R., A.F., and M.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Turin—Italy (protocol code 0454951—date of approval 25 July 2024).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study and from their parents.

Data Availability Statement

Research data are available upon request to the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ackard, D. M., Eisenberg, M. E., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2007). Long-term impact of adolescent dating violence on the behavioral and psychological health of male and female youth. The Journal of Pediatrics, 151(5), 476–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Angelone, D. J., Cantor, N., Marcantonio, T., & Joppa, M. (2021). Does sexism mediate the gender and rape myth acceptance relationship? Violence Against Women, 27(6–7), 748–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2020). Preventing teen dating violence. National Center for Injury Prevention and Control (U.S.). Division of Violence Prevention. Available online: https://stacks.cdc.gov/view/cdc/140182 (accessed on 15 June 2025).
  4. Chapleau, K. M., Oswald, D. L., & Russell, B. L. (2007). How ambivalent sexism toward women and men support rape myth acceptance. Sex Roles, 57(1–2), 131–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Clayton, H. B., Kilmer, G., DeGue, S., Estefan, L. F., Le, V. D., Suarez, N. A., Lyons, B. H., & Thornton, J. E. (2023). Dating violence, sexual violence, and bullying victimization among high school students—Youth risk behavior survey, United States, 2021. MMWR Supplements, 72(1), 66–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Collins, W. A. (2003). More than Myth: The developmental significance of romantic relationships during adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13(1), 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Connelly, K., & Heesacker, M. (2012). Why is benevolent sexism appealing?: Associations with system justification and life satisfaction. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 36(4), 432–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Cutter-Wilson, E., & Richmond, T. (2011). Understanding teen dating violence: Practical screening and intervention strategies for pediatric and adolescent healthcare providers. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 23(4), 379–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Dai, R. (2024). The causes, manifestations, and effects of benevolent sexism and how it contributes to the perpetuation of gender discrimination. Communications in Humanities Research, 26(1), 80–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Daniels, E. A., Zurbriggen, E. L., & Monique Ward, L. (2020). Becoming an object: A review of self-objectification in girls. Body Image, 33, 278–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Davidson, M. M., & Gervais, S. J. (2015). Violence against women through the lens of objectification theory. Violence Against Women, 21(3), 330–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. DeGue, S., Valle, L. A., Holt, M. K., Massetti, G. M., Matjasko, J. L., & Tharp, A. T. (2014). A systematic review of primary prevention strategies for sexual violence perpetration. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 19(4), 346–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. De Koker, P., Mathews, C., Zuch, M., Bastien, S., & Mason-Jones, A. J. (2014). A systematic review of interventions for preventing adolescent intimate partner violence. The Journal of Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 54(1), 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Díaz-Aguado Jalón, M. J. (2022). Adolescencia, sexismo y violencia contra las mujeres en España: Situación actual y evolución durante una década. Revista de Estudios de Juventud, 125, 11–32. [Google Scholar]
  15. Díaz-Aguado Jalón, M. J., Martínez Arias, R., & Babarro, J. M. (2014). La evolución de la adolescencia española sobre la igualdad y la prevención de la violencia de género. Colección Contra La Violencia de Género. Documentos. Available online: https://violenciagenero.igualdad.gob.es/violenciaencifras/estudios/colecciones/evolucion2014/ (accessed on 17 June 2025).
  16. Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Development, 82(1), 405–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Espinosa-Hernández, G., Vasilenko, S. A., Lombardi, K., McCrimmon, J., & McPherson, J. L. (2024). Patterns of sexual well-being in Mexican adolescents and associations with psychological adjustment: A latent profile analysis approach. Journal of Sex Research, 61(1), 92–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. European Commission. (2022). Visuals with statistics on violence against women. Available online: https://commission.europa.eu/strategy-and-policy/policies/justice-and-fundamental-rights/gender-equality/gender-based-violence/ending-gender-based-violence (accessed on 10 June 2025).
  19. Exner-Cortens, D., Eckenrode, J., Bunge, J., & Rothman, E. (2017). Revictimization after adolescent dating violence in a matched, national sample of youth. Journal of Adolescent Health, 60(2), 176–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Fasanelli, R., Galli, I., Grassia, M. G., Marino, M., Cataldo, R., Lauro, C. N., Castiello, C., Grassia, F., Arcidiacono, C., & Procentese, F. (2020). The use of partial least squares–Path modelling to understand the impact of ambivalent sexism on violence-justification among. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(14), 4991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fenner, L. (2017). Sexual consent as a scientific subject: A literature review. American Journal of Sexuality Education, 12(4), 451–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Fernández-Fuertes, A. A., Carcedo, R. J., Orgaz, B., & Fuertes, A. (2018). Sexual coercion perpetration and victimization: Gender similarities and differences in adolescence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 33(16), 2467–2485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Foshee, V. A., Bauman, K. E., Ennett, S. T., Suchindran, C., Benefield, T., & Linder, G. F. (2005). Assessing the effects of the dating violence prevention program “safe dates” using random coefficient regression modeling. Prevention Science: The Official Journal of the Society for Prevention Research, 6(3), 245–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Foshee, V. A., Reyes, H. L. M. N., Gottfredson, N. C., Chang, L. Y., & Ennett, S. T. (2013). A longitudinal examination of psychological, behavioral, academic, and relationship consequences of dating abuse victimization among a primarily rural sample of adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53(6), 723–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21(2), 173–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Giannakopoulos, G., Assimopoulos, H., Petanidou, D., Tzavara, C., Kolaitis, G., & Tsiantis, J. (2012). Effectiveness of a school-based intervention for enhancing adolescents’ positive attitudes towards people with mental illness. Mental illness, 4(2), e16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 491–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Gomes, A., Gonçalves, G., Sousa, C., Santos, J., & Giger, J.-C. (2022). Are we getting less sexist? A ten-year gap comparison analysis of sexism in a Portuguese sample. Psychological Reports, 125(4), 2160–2177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Gómez Berrocal, C., Moyano, N., Álvarez-Muelas, A., & Sierra, J. C. (2022). Sexual double standard: A gender-based prejudice referring to sexual freedom and sexual shyness. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1006675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gómez Berrocal, M. D. C., Vallejo-Medina, P., Moyano, N., & Sierra, J. C. (2019). Sexual double standard: A psychometric study from a macropsychological perspective among the Spanish heterosexual population. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Guerra-Marmolejo, C., Fernández-Fernández, E., González-Cano-Caballero, M., García-Gámez, M., Del Río, F. J., & Fernández-Ordóñez, E. (2021). Factors related to gender violence and sex education in adolescents: A cross-sectional study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(11), 5836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Harper, B., & Tiggemann, M. (2008). The effect of thin ideal media images on women’s self-objectification, mood, and body image. Sex Roles, 58(9–10), 649–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Healy, S. R., Valente, J. Y., Caetano, S. C., Martins, S. S., & Sanchez, Z. M. (2020). Worldwide school-based psychosocial interventions and their effect on aggression among elementary school children: A systematic review 2010–2019. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 55, 101486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hébert, M., Moreau, C., Blais, M., Lavoie, F., & Guerrier, M. (2017). Child sexual abuse as a risk factor for teen dating violence: Findings from a representative sample of Quebec youth. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 10(1), 51–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hinduja, S., & Patchin, J. W. (2021). Digital dating abuse among a national sample of U.S. youth. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(23–24), 11088–11108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Holmes, K., & Sher, L. (2013). Dating violence and suicidal behavior in adolescents. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 25(3), 257–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Humphreys, T. P., & Brousseau, M. M. (2010). The sexual consent scale–revised: Development, reliability, and preliminary validity. Journal of Sex Research, 47(5), 420–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Javidi, H., Maheux, A. J., Widman, L., Kamke, K., Choukas-Bradley, S., & Peterson, Z. D. (2020). Understanding adolescents’ attitudes toward affirmative consent. Journal of Sex Research, 57(9), 1100–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Johnson, W. L., Giordano, P. C., Longmore, M. A., & Manning, W. D. (2014). Intimate partner violence and depressive symptoms during adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 55(1), 39–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Jozkowski, K. N., Marcantonio, T. L., & Hunt, M. E. (2017). college students’ sexual consent communication and perceptions of sexual double standards: A qualitative investigation. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 49(4), 237–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Jozkowski, K. N., Sanders, S., Peterson, Z. D., Dennis, B., & Reece, M. (2014). Consenting to sexual activity: The development and psychometric assessment of dual measures of consent. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43(3), 437–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Kreager, D. A., & Staff, J. (2009). The sexual double standard and adolescent peer acceptance. Social Psychology Quarterly, 72(2), 143–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Kreager, D. A., Staff, J., Gauthier, R., Lefkowitz, E. S., & Feinberg, M. E. (2016). The double standard at sexual debut: Gender, sexual behavior and adolescent peer acceptance. Sex Roles, 75(7–8), 377–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Lim, M., Carollo, A., Chen, S. H. A., & Esposito, G. (2021). Surveying 80 years of psychodrama research: A scientometric review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 780542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lozano-Martínez, J., Castillo-Reche, I. S., Morales-Yago, F. J., & Ibáñez-López, F. J. (2022). control violence begins in adolescent dating: A research from students’ perception. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(15), 8974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Madrona-Bonastre, R., Sanz-Barbero, B., Pérez-Martínez, V., Abiétar, D. G., Sánchez-Martínez, F., Forcadell-Díez, L., Pérez, G., & Vives-Cases, C. (2023). Sexismo y violencia de pareja en adolescentes [Sexism and intimate partner violence in adolescents]. Gaceta Sanitaria, 37, 102221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Malhi, N., Oliffe, J. L., Bungay, V., & Kelly, M. T. (2020). Male perpetration of adolescent dating violence: A scoping review. American Journal of Men’s Health, 14(5), 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Marcantonio, T. L., Willis, M., & Jozkowski, K. N. (2020). Women’s sexual consent: Potential implications for sexual satisfaction. Current Sexual Health Reports, 12(3), 120–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Martínez-Gómez, J. A., Bolívar-Suárez, Y., Rey-Anacona, C. A., Ramírez-Ortiz, L. C., Lizarazo-Ojeda, M., & Yanez-Peñúñuri, L. Y. (2021). Traditional schemes of sexual gender roles, power in relationships and dating violence. Revista Iberoamericana de Psicologia y Salud, 12(1), 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The objectified body consciousness scale: Development and validation. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20(2), 181–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Menzies, R. G., Menzies, R. E., & Dingle, G. A. (2022). Existential Concerns and Cognitive-Behavioral Procedures: An integrative approach to mental health (pp. 1–310). Springer International Publishing. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Mfeka-Nkabinde, N. G., Moletsane, R., & Voce, A. (2024). ‘There is a No that means Yes!’—Coercive and aggressive sexual behaviours in adolescents’ heterosexual relationships in rural KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 29(1), 2297572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Moradi, B., & Huang, Y.-P. (2008). Objectification theory and psychology of women: A decade of advances and future directions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32(4), 377–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Ozimek, P., Lainas, S., Bierhoff, H. W., & Rohmann, E. (2023). How photo editing in social media shapes self-perceived attractiveness and self-esteem via self-objectification and physical appearance comparisons. BMC Psychology, 11(1), 99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Parker, E. M., & Bradshaw, C. P. (2015). Teen dating violence victimization and patterns of substance use among high school students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 57(4), 441–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Phillips, A., & De Roos, M. S. (2025). Gender stereotypes and perceptions of stranger violence: Attributions of blame and motivation. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 69(1), 43–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Quarshie, E. N. B. (2021). Self-harm among school-going adolescent survivors of sexual violence victimisation: A cross-sectional study. Frontiers in Sociology, 6, 605865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Richards, M. J., Bogart, A., & Sheeder, J. (2022). Communication and interpretation of sexual consent and refusal in adolescents and young adults. The Journal of Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 70(6), 915–921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Righi, M. K., Bogen, K. W., Kuo, C., & Orchowski, L. M. (2021). A qualitative analysis of beliefs about sexual consent among high school students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(15–16), NP8290–NP8316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Rollero, C. (2015). “I know you are not real”: Salience of photo retouching reduces the negative effects of media exposure via internalization. Studia Psychologica, 57, 195–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Rollero, C. (2022). Mass media beauty standards, body surveillance, and relationship satisfaction within romantic couples. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(7), 3833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Rollero, C., Glick, P., & Tartaglia, S. (2014). Psychometric properties of short versions of the ambivalent sexism inventory and ambivalence toward men inventory. TPM—Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 21(2), 149–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Rollero, C., Moyano, N., & Roccato, M. (2023). The Role of sexual consent and past non-consensual sexual experiences on rape supportive attitudes in a heterosexual community sample. Sexuality and Culture, 27(4), 1352–1368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Rollero, C., & Tartaglia, S. (2019). The effect of sexism and rape myths on victim blame. Sexuality and Culture, 23(1), 209–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Salomon, I., & Brown, C. S. (2019). The selfie generation: Examining the relationship between social media use and early adolescent body image. Journal of Early Adolescence, 39(4), 539–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Santoniccolo, F., Trombetta, T., Paradiso, M. N., & Rollè, L. (2023). Gender and media representations: A review of the literature on gender stereotypes, objectification and sexualization. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(10), 5770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Saunders, J. F., & Eaton, A. A. (2018). Snaps, selfies, and shares: How three popular social media platforms contribute to the sociocultural model of disordered eating among young women. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 21(6), 343–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47(5), 609–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Shekar, A., Gross, A., Luebbers, E., & Honsky, J. (2020). Effects of an interprofessional student-led sexual education program on self-efficacy and attitudes about sexual violence in youths in juvenile detention. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 33(3), 302–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Skowronski, M., Busching, R., & Krahé, B. (2021). The effects of sexualized video game characters and character personalization on women’s self-objectification and body satisfaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 92, 104051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Teitelman, A. M., Ratcliffe, S. J., Dichter, M. E., & Sullivan, C. M. (2008). Recent and past intimate partner abuse and HIV risk among young women. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing, 37(2), 219–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Tiggemann, M., & Slater, A. (2015). The role of self-objectification in the mental health of early adolescent girls: Predictors and consequences. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 40(7), 704–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Ulusoy, Y., Sumbas, E., & Sertkaya, B. (2023). Psychodrama as an intervention management instrument for internal/external adolescent problems: A systematic literature review. Arts in Psychotherapy, 83, 102000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Vagi, K. J., O’Malley Olsen, E., Basile, K. C., & Vivolo-Kantor, A. M. (2015). Teen dating violence (physical and sexual) among US high school students: Findings from the 2013 national youth risk behavior survey. JAMA Pediatrics, 169(5), 474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Watson, J. (2001). Social constructivism in the classroom. Support for Learning, 16(3), 140–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Willis, M., Jozkowski, K. N., & Read, J. (2019). Sexual consent in K–12 sex education: An analysis of current health education standards in the United States. Sex Education, 19(2), 226–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Wincentak, K., Connolly, J., & Card, N. (2017). Teen dating violence: A meta-analytic review of prevalence rates. Psychology of Violence, 7(2), 224–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Wollast, R., Riemer, A. R., Bernard, P., Leys, C., Kotsou, I., & Klein, O. (2019). How self-compassion moderates the effect of body surveillance on subjective happiness and depression among women. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 60(5), 464–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Zaikman, Y., & Marks, M. J. (2014). Ambivalent sexism and the sexual double standard. Sex Role, 71(9–10), 333–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Pearson’s correlations between study variables at Time 0.
Table 1. Pearson’s correlations between study variables at Time 0.
1.2.3.4.5.
1. Body Surveillance
2. Benevolent Sexism−0.00
3. Hostile Sexism−0.24 **0.30 **
4. Sexual Double Standard−0.070.26 **0.63 **
5. Lack of Perceived Ctrl−0.140.36 **0.37 **0.30 **
6. Positive Attit. to Consent0.20 *−0.28 **−0.45 **−0.39 **−0.50 **
** p < 0.001; * p < 0.01.
Table 2. Gender differences on the study variables at Time 0: means, standard deviations (SDs), and t-test scores.
Table 2. Gender differences on the study variables at Time 0: means, standard deviations (SDs), and t-test scores.
MeanSDtCohen’s d
Body SurveillanceWomen5.410.936.61 **1.01
Men4.431.06
Benevolent SexismWomen2.231.060.420.95
Men2.170.86
Hostile SexismWomen1.380.91−8.22 **1.01
Men2.611.08
Sexual Double StandardWomen0.810.51−6.73 **0.50
Men1.300.49
Lack of Perceived CtrlWomen2.731.27−2.89 *1.21
Men3.251.16
Positive Attit. to ConsentWomen5.321.224.57 **1.13
Men4.561.06
** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05.
Table 3. Pearson’s correlations between study variables at Time 1.
Table 3. Pearson’s correlations between study variables at Time 1.
1.2.3.4.5.
1. Body Surveillance
2. Benevolent Sexism0.01
3. Hostile Sexism−0.21 *0.40 **
4. SDS−0.100.45 **0.61 **
5. Lack of Perceived Ctrl−0.110.38 **0.41 **0.43 **
6. Positive Attit. to Consent0.14−0.18 *−0.47 **−0.36 **−0.48 **
** p < 0.001; * p < 0.01.
Table 4. Gender differences on the study variables at Time 1: means, standard deviations (SDs), and t-test scores.
Table 4. Gender differences on the study variables at Time 1: means, standard deviations (SDs), and t-test scores.
MeanSDtCohen’s d
Body SurveillanceWomen5.160.955.20 **0.96
Men4.361.00
Benevolent SexismWomen1.831.00−0.690.96
Men1.940.93
Hostile SexismWomen1.190.96−8.14 **1.01
Men2.471.04
Sexual Double StandardWomen0.690.56−5.57 **0.54
Men1.160.52
Lack of Perceived CtrlWomen2.631.18−3.36 **1.09
Men3.201.01
Positive Attit. to ConsentWomen5.291.323.72 **1.12
Men4.630.64
** p < 0.01.
Table 5. Differences between Time 0 and Time 1 on the study variables: means, standard deviations (SDs), and t-test scores.
Table 5. Differences between Time 0 and Time 1 on the study variables: means, standard deviations (SDs), and t-test scores.
MeanSDtCohen’s d
Body SurveillanceTime 04.861.112.03 *1.11
Time 14.621.18
Benevolent SexismTime 02.180.952.76 **0.96
Time 11.900.96
Hostile SexismTime 02.091.201.251.20
Time 11.931.19
Sexual Double StandardTime 01.090.571.99 *0.58
Time 10.980.59
Lack of Perceived CtrlTime 02.991.180.301.19
Time 12.951.01
Positive Attit. to ConsentTime 04.921.210.091.19
Time 14.911.17
** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Berlin, E.; Fedi, A.; Ciampi, E.; Di Chio, C.; Husquin, M.; Luppino, I.; Martini, M.; Rollero, C. ‘Deconstructing Stereotypes to Build Consent’: Evaluation of a Project on Social and Sexual Relationships in Adolescence. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 1275. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091275

AMA Style

Berlin E, Fedi A, Ciampi E, Di Chio C, Husquin M, Luppino I, Martini M, Rollero C. ‘Deconstructing Stereotypes to Build Consent’: Evaluation of a Project on Social and Sexual Relationships in Adolescence. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(9):1275. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091275

Chicago/Turabian Style

Berlin, Elisa, Angela Fedi, Elena Ciampi, Caterina Di Chio, Mélodie Husquin, Ivan Luppino, Mara Martini, and Chiara Rollero. 2025. "‘Deconstructing Stereotypes to Build Consent’: Evaluation of a Project on Social and Sexual Relationships in Adolescence" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 9: 1275. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091275

APA Style

Berlin, E., Fedi, A., Ciampi, E., Di Chio, C., Husquin, M., Luppino, I., Martini, M., & Rollero, C. (2025). ‘Deconstructing Stereotypes to Build Consent’: Evaluation of a Project on Social and Sexual Relationships in Adolescence. Behavioral Sciences, 15(9), 1275. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091275

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop