Family Science Capital Factors Affecting Early Childhood Science Learning: A Systematic Review
Abstract
1. Introduction
- (1)
- What are the descriptive characteristics (e.g., publication year, location, research methods, and research topics) of the included studies?
- (2)
- What are the key elements of family cultural science capital that influence early childhood science learning, and how do they exert this influence?
- (3)
- What are the key elements of family practice science capital that influence early childhood science learning, and how do they exert this influence?
- (4)
- What are the key elements of family social science capital that influence early childhood science learning, and how do they exert this influence?
2. Methods
2.1. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
2.2. Literature Search
2.3. Screening Strategy
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics
3.2. Family Cultural Capital and Early Childhood Science Learning
3.3. Family Science Practice Capital and Early Childhood Science Learning
3.4. Family Science Social Capital and Early Childhood Science Learning
4. Discussion
5. Future Directions and Limitations
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Characteristics of Included Studies
Included Study | Year | Country | Type of Study | Methodology | Sample | Family Science Capital Dimension |
(Andrews & Wang, 2017) | 2017 | the United States | Empirical | Qualitative research | A 7-year-old girl and her mother | Participation in out-of-school science activities |
(Miller-Goldwater et al., 2023a) | 2023 | the United States | Empirical | Mixed research | 38 children and their caregiver | Science talk with children |
(Leech, 2024) | 2024 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | A total of 70 parents with a 4- or 5-year-old child | Science talk with children |
(Young et al., 2024) | 2024 | the United States | Empirical | Mixed research | 29 total educators and 24 Focus Families | Science-linked social networks |
(Suortti et al., 2023) | 2023 | Finland | Empirical | Quantitative research | 740 parents | Parental education level |
(Mantzicopoulos et al., 2013) | 2013 | the United States | Empirical | Mixed research | 243 kindergarten children | Science-linked social networks |
(Bae et al., 2023) | 2023 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 153 children, age range from 3 to 8 years old | Participation in out-of-school science activities |
(Paños & Ruiz-Gallardo, 2020) | 2020 | Spain | Empirical | Quantitative research | 62 children, aged from 5 to 8 years old and their parents | Participation in out-of-school science activities |
(Junge et al., 2021) | 2021 | Germany | Empirical | Quantitative research | 257 5-year-old children and their parents | Participation in out-of-school science activities |
(Shirefley & Leaper, 2022) | 2022 | the United States | Empirical | Mixed research | 50 families and their children aged between 4 and 7 years old | Science talk with children |
(Lloyd et al., 2020) | 2020 | the United Kingdom | Empirical | Qualitative research | 26 children aged under 5 and 19 parents | Science-linked social networks |
(Mills et al., 2021) | 2021 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 148 parent–child | Science talk with children |
(Pagano et al., 2020) | 2020 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 61 6- to 8-year-old children and their families | Participation in out-of-school science activities |
(Haden, 2010) | 2010 | Review | ||||
(Strickler-Eppard et al., 2019) | 2019 | the United States | Empirical | Qualitative research | Five households, each consisting of at least one adult and 2–3 children, one of whom is the target age (4–8 years). | Science-linked social networks |
(Legare et al., 2017) | 2017 | Review | ||||
(Polinsky et al., 2023) | 2023 | the United States | Empirical | Mixed research | 243 families with children aged 6–11 years | Science talk with children |
(Haden et al., 2023) | 2023 | Review | Participation in out-of-school science activities | |||
(Leech et al., 2023) | 2023 | the United States | Empirical | Qualitative research | 49 children aged 4–6 years and their parents | Science talk with children |
(Castañeda et al., 2022) | 2022 | the United States | Empirical | Qualitative research | 243 families with children aged 3–5 years | Science talk with children |
(Ocular et al., 2022) | 2022 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 38 children and their parents | Science talk with children |
(Acosta et al., 2021) | 2021 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 111 families with children aged 5–10 years | Science talk with children |
(Chandler-Campbell et al., 2020) | 2020 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 47 parent–child and the children aged 4–6 years | Science talk with children |
(Chandler-Campbell et al., 2020) | 2020 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 60 parent–child and the children aged 4–5 years | Science talk with children |
(Callanan et al., 2020) | 2020 | the United States | Empirical | Qualitative research | 111 families with children aged 3–4 years | Science talk with children |
(Pattison & Dierking, 2018) | 2018 | the United States | Empirical | Qualitative research | 7 mothers with 4-year-old daughters | Participation in out-of-school science activities |
(Marcus et al., 2018) | 2018 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 64 4- to 8-year-old children and their parents | Science-linked social networks |
(Eberbach & Crowley, 2017) | 2017 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 79 families with children aged 6–10 years | Scientific literacy |
(Jant et al., 2014) | 2014 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 78 families with children aged 4–6 years | Science talk with children |
(Benjamin et al., 2010) | 2010 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 121 4- to 8-year-old children and their caregiver | Science-linked social networks |
(Szechter & Carey, 2009) | 2009 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 60 parent–child and the children aged 5.6–14.9 years | Parental education level |
(Dou et al., 2019) | 2019 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 15,847 students at 27 colleges and universities | Participation in out-of-school science activities |
(Callanan et al., 2017) | 2017 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 83 parent–child and the children aged 3–11 years | Science talk with children |
(Alexander et al., 2012) | 2012 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 192 4- to 7-year-old children and their parents | Participation in out-of-school science activities |
(Fender & Crowley, 2007) | 2007 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | children aged 3–8 years | Science talk with children |
(Dabney et al., 2015) | 2015 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 4285 chemistry and physics doctoral students, scientists, and individuals with other doctoral degrees in physical science. | Parents’ occupational background |
(Dabney et al., 2013) | 2013 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 4285 chemistry and physics doctoral students, scientists, and individuals with other doctoral degrees in physical science. | Parents’ occupational background |
(Westerberg et al., 2022) | 2022 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 125 families with children aged 3 to 5 years | Participation in out-of-school science activities |
(Booth et al., 2020) | 2020 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 153 3-year-old children and their parents | Science talk with children |
(Kähler et al., 2020) | 2020 | Spain | Empirical | Quantitative research | 2937 children in kindergarten | Parental education level |
(Willard et al., 2019) | 2019 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 65 parent–child and the children aged 4–6 years | Science talk with children |
(Vandermaas-Peeler et al., 2017) | 2017 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 32 families with children aged 4 to 5 years | Science-linked social networks |
(Vandermaas-Peeler et al., 2015) | 2015 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 25 families with children with a mean age of 67 months | Science-linked social networks |
(Van Schijndel & Raijmakers, 2016) | 2015 | Netherlands | Empirical | Quantitative research | 89 parent–child and the children aged 4–6 years | Science talk with children |
(Pattison et al., 2020) | 2020 | the United States | Empirical | Qualitative research | Kindergarten children with their parents | Scientific belief |
(Kelly et al., 2020) | 2020 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 22 children aged 3–8 years and their parents | Science talk with children |
(Alexandre et al., 2022) | 2022 | Review | ||||
(Dominke et al., 2025) | 2025 | Germany | Empirical | Quantitative research | 61 parent–child and the children aged 4–7 years | Participation in out-of-school science activities |
(Miller-Goldwater et al., 2023b) | 2023 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 38 children and their caregiver | Science talk with children |
(Salvatierra & Cabello, 2022) | 2022 | Review | ||||
(Kontkanen et al., 2024) | 2024 | Review | ||||
(Bambha et al., 2024) | 2024 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 173 parents and their children | Science talk with children |
(Sheehan et al., 2018) | 2018 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 96 parents of preschool-aged children | The use of scientific media |
(Gin et al., 2025) | 2025 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 68 children aged 4–7 years | Science talk with children |
(Ata-Aktürk & Demircan, 2020) | 2020 | Turkey | Empirical | Qualitative research | Two ECE teachers, five children, and five parents | Science-linked networks |
(Mannweiler et al., 2025) | 2025 | the United States | Empirical | Quantitative research | 75 kindergarten children with their parents | Science talk with children |
Appendix B. Keywords
Keywords | Synonyms |
---|---|
Set A: science capital | scientific capital |
family science capital | |
parental science capital | |
household science capital | |
science-related cultural capital | |
scientific literacy | |
science beliefs | |
science career value | |
science media consumption | |
science television | |
science books | |
popular science articles | |
informal science learning | |
science social capital | |
science-related social ties | |
science network | |
science discussion frequency | |
science reference group | |
Set B: early science learning | early science learning |
early childhood science | |
preschool science | |
young children science | |
science interest | |
science knowledge | |
scientific reasoning | |
science reflection | |
science identity | |
science engagement | |
science practice | |
STEM identity | |
scientific thinking | |
science attitude | |
Set C: home environment | family |
parent | |
home environment | |
caregiver | |
household | |
maternal | |
paternal | |
Set D: Age group | early childhood |
preschool | |
Kindergarten | |
young child | |
nursery school | |
0–8 years | |
pre-primary |
Appendix C. Thematic Framework for Family Science Capital
Final Theme | Subtheme | Representative Initial Codes | Definition/Description | Logic for Code Generation | Basis for Subtheme Clustering |
Family Cultural Science Capital | Scientific Literacy | Mastery of Scientific Knowledge, Use of Disciplinary Terminology | The extent to which family members possess scientific knowledge, methods, and principles. | Directly extracted from raw texts mentioning ‘scientific literacy,’ ‘parents’ mastery of scientific knowledge,’ and ‘parents’ use of scientific knowledge. | Merged all knowledge-related codes (e.g., ‘use of terminology,’ ‘conceptual associations’), focusing on cognitive foundations. |
Science Dispositions and Preferences | Recognition of the Value of Science, Emotional Orientation toward Science, Importance Placed on Science | Family members’ recognition of the social value of science, emotional attitudes, and the level of importance attached to science. | Captured from raw texts reflecting parental attitudes (e.g., ‘science is important’). | Combined scientific value judgments and emotional expressions (interests and preferences) to reflect the attitudinal dimension. | |
Science Labor Market Conversion | Recognition of the Value of Science Careers, Transmission of Career Knowledge | The extent to which family members understand the types, content, and social value of science-related (STEM) professions. | Extracted from raw texts indicating parents’ attitudes toward science careers (e.g., ‘understanding of types and content of science careers,’ ‘awareness of the labor market value of science careers’). | Combined recognition of the social value of science careers with descriptions of their types, content, and value. | |
Family Practice Science Capital | Science Media Consumption | Frequency of Watching Science Programs, Digital Media Interaction Frequency | The frequency and depth with which family members engage with science content via media such as television and the internet. | Directly extracted from raw texts referring to ‘science media, resources, digital tools,’ and ‘usage frequency of digital resources.’ | Combined codes on using science media, resources, and digital tools. |
Participation in Out-of-School Science Activities | Visits to Science Venues, Family Experiments and Exploration, Reading Science Books | Participation in science exploration activities in informal settings (e.g., museums, family-based activities). | Analyzed and directly extracted from texts mentioning ‘family science activities,’ ‘visits to science venues,’ and ‘reading science books.’ | Integrated content focusing on participation in out-of-school activities, especially those emphasizing contextual experiences. | |
Science Dialogue | Questioning, Scientific Explanations, Causal Discussions | Dialogue patterns in which family members engage in questioning, explanations, and causal reasoning around scientific phenomena. | Classified based on question type (open-ended questions with interrogatives vs. closed questions with yes/no judgments). Identified parent explanations and causal reasoning using words like ‘because/mechanism.’ | Unified dialogue strategies (questioning, explaining, causal discussions) as dialogic behaviors, distinguishing them from media activities or venue-based practices. | |
Family Social Science Capital | Science Credentials | Parental Educational Background, Parental Science-Related Occupations | The educational level of family members and their professional backgrounds in science-related fields. | Identified from raw texts referring to ‘parental education level’ and ‘science-related occupations.’ | Combined education level and science-related career background as the credentials dimension. |
Science Network Support | Collaboration with Professional Institutions, Home–School Science Collaboration, Use of Toolkits | Access to scientific resources and support through schools, communities, experts, and other external networks. | Captured key collaborative contexts (e.g., ‘teacher guidance for parents,’ ‘family participation in school science days,’ ‘use of family toolkits,’ ‘parent training’). | Merged external resource engagement behaviors (institutional collaboration, expert guidance, toolkit use), distinguishing them from internally generated family capital. |
Appendix D. Examples of Thematic Coding Process
Raw Data Excerpt (Anonymized) | Initial Codes | Conflict Handling | Subtheme | Theme | Code Generation Rationale |
“Parents with high scientific literacy used terms like ‘stamen’ and ‘pollen transfer’ more frequently while observing flowers in a botanical garden and guided children to understand the adhesion mechanism of pollen to bees’ leg hair.” | Mastery of Scientific Knowledge; Use of Disciplinary Terminology | — | Scientific Literacy | Family Cultural Science Capital | The original codes (“use of terminology”) reflect understanding of scientific principles, representing cognitive foundations. |
“Parents’ use of closed-ended questions (e.g., ‘Is this a fossil?’) in a museum was not significantly associated with children’s science-related talk.” | Closed-Ended Questioning | — | Science Dialogue | Family Practice Science Capital | Generated directly based on sentence structure (yes/no or true/false judgment). |
“Parents with doctoral degrees were better able to help their children establish a complete cognitive framework for circuit principles.” | 1. Parental Educational Background; 2. Scientific Literacy | Marked as a conflicting case and, after analysis, categorized under “Science Credentials.” | Science Credentials | Family Social Science Capital | Conflicting statements were flagged and discussed. Since the focus was on parental education level, it was categorized under science credentials. |
References
- Acosta, D. I., Polinsky, N. J., Haden, C. A., & Uttal, D. H. (2021). Whether and how knowledge moderates linkages between parent–child conversations and children’s reflections about tinkering in a children’s museum. Journal of Cognition and Development, 22(2), 226–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alexander, J. M., Johnson, K. E., & Kelley, K. (2012). Longitudinal analysis of the relations between opportunities to learn about science and the development of interests related to science. Science Education, 96(5), 763–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alexandre, S., Xu, Y., Washington-Nortey, M., & Chen, C. (2022). Informal STEM learning for young children: A systematic literature review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(14), 8299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Andrews, K. J., & Wang, X. C. (2017). Young children’s emergent science competencies in everyday family contexts: A case study. Early Child Development and Care, 189(8), 1351–1368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Archer, L., Dawson, E., DeWitt, J., Seakins, A., & Wong, B. (2015). “Science capital”: A conceptual, methodological, and empirical argument for extending bourdieusian notions of capital beyond the arts. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 52(7), 922–948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ata-Aktürk, A., & Demircan, H. Ö. (2020). Supporting preschool children’s STEM learning with parent-involved early engineering education. Early Childhood Education Journal, 49(4), 607–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bae, J., Shavlik, M., Shatrowsky, C. E., Haden, C. A., & Booth, A. E. (2023). Predicting grade school scientific literacy from aspects of the early home science environment. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1113196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bambha, V. P., Surrain, S., Zucker, T. A., Ahmed, Y., & Leyva, D. (2024). The intersection of parent questions, child skills, and activity context in informal science, technology, engineering, and math learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 246, 106000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benjamin, N., Haden, C. A., & Wilkerson, E. (2010). Enhancing building, conversation, and learning through caregiver–child interactions in a children’s museum. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 502–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Booth, A. E., Shavlik, M., & Haden, C. A. (2020). Parents’ causal talk: Links to children’s causal stance and emerging scientific literacy. Developmental Psychology, 56(11), 2055–2064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Callanan, M. A., Castañeda, C. L., Luce, M. R., & Martin, J. L. (2017). Family science talk in museums: Predicting children’s engagement from variations in talk and activity. Child Development, 88(5), 1492–1504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Callanan, M. A., Legare, C. H., Sobel, D. M., Jaeger, G. J., Letourneau, S., McHugh, S. R., Willard, A., Brinkman, A., Finiasz, Z., Rubio, E., Barnett, A., Gose, R., Martin, J. L., Meisner, R., & Watson, J. (2020). Exploration, explanation, and parent–child interaction in museums. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 85(1), 7–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Castañeda, C. L., Callanan, M. A., Shirefley, T. A., & Jipson, J. L. (2022). Early strengths in science: Young children’s conversations about nature in Latine families. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 83, 101453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chandler-Campbell, I. L., Leech, K. A., & Corriveau, K. H. (2020). Investigating science together: Inquiry-based training promotes scientific conversations in parent-child interactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dabney, K. P., Chakraverty, D., & Tai, R. H. (2013). The association of family influence and initial interest in science. Science Education, 97(3), 395–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dabney, K. P., Tai, R. H., & Scott, M. R. (2015). Informal science: Family education, experiences, and initial interest in science. International Journal of Science Education, Part B, 6(3), 263–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dominke, H., Barenthien, J., Oppermann, E., Burghardt, L., & Steffensky, M. (2025). The quality of interactions in the home science environment and associations with children’s science learning. Science Education. early view. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dou, R., Hazari, Z., Dabney, K., Sonnert, G., & Sadler, P. (2019). Early informal STEM experiences and STEM identity: The importance of talking science. Science Education, 103(3), 623–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eberbach, C., & Crowley, K. (2017). From seeing to observing: How parents and children learn to see science in a botanical garden. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 26(4), 608–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fender, J. G., & Crowley, K. (2007). How parent explanation changes what children learn from everyday scientific thinking. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 189–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gin, S., Yin, H., Boykin, C. M., & Sobel, D. M. (2025). Examining baseline relations between parent–child interactions and STEM engagement and learning. Developmental Science, 28(2), e13611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haden, C. A. (2010). Talking about science in museums. Child development perspectives, 4(1), 62–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haden, C. A., Melzi, G., & Callanan, M. A. (2023). Science in stories: Implications for Latine children’s science learning through home-based language practices. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1096833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holbrook, J., & Rannikmae, M. (2007). The nature of science education for enhancing scientific literacy. International Journal of Science Education, 29(11), 1347–1362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jant, E. A., Haden, C. A., Uttal, D. H., & Babcock, E. (2014). Conversation and object manipulation influence children’s learning in a museum. Child Development, 85(5), 2029–2045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Junge, K., Schmerse, D., Lankes, E.-M., Carstensen, C. H., & Steffensky, M. (2021). How the home learning environment contributes to children’s early science knowledge—Associations with parental characteristics and science-related activities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 56, 294–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kähler, J., Hahn, I., & Köller, O. (2020). The development of early scientific literacy gaps in kindergarten children. International Journal of Science Education, 42(12), 1988–2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelly, K. R., Ocular, G., & Austin, A. (2020). Adult-child science language during informal science learning at an aquarium. The Social Science Journal, 59(4), 532–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kontkanen, S., Koskela, T., Kanerva, O., Kärkkäinen, S., Waltzer, K., Mikkilä-Erdmann, M., & Havu-Nuutinen, S. (2024). Science capital as a lens for studying science aspirations—A systematic review. Studies in Science Education, 61(1), 89–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leech, K. A. (2024). Family science capital moderates gender differences in parent–child scientific conversation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 247, 106020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leech, K. A., Chandler-Campbell, I. L., Alton, J., & Corriveau, K. H. (2023). What would happen if?: A comparison of fathers’ and mothers’ questions to children during a science activity. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1078994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Legare, C. H., Sobel, D. M., & Callanan, M. (2017). Causal learning is collaborative: Examining explanation and exploration in social contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(5), 1548–1554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lloyd, E., Edmonds, C., Downs, C., Crutchley, R., & Paffard, F. (2020). Talking everyday science to very young children: A study involving parents and practitioners within an early childhood centre. In Early years science education (pp. 66–82). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Mannweiler, M. D., Bierman, K. L., & Liben, L. S. (2025). Linking parents’ play strategies with their preschoolers’ STEM skills: The mediating roles of child STEM talk and self-regulated learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 249, 106095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mantzicopoulos, P., Patrick, H., & Samarapungavan, A. (2013). Science literacy in school and home contexts: Kindergarteners’ science achievement and motivation. Cognition and Instruction, 31(1), 62–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marcus, M., Haden, C. A., & Uttal, D. H. (2018). Promoting children’s learning and transfer across informal science, technology, engineering, and mathematics learning experiences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 175, 80–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miller-Goldwater, H. E., Cronin-Golomb, L. M., Hanft, M. H., & Bauer, P. J. (2023a). The influence of books’ textual features and caregivers’ extratextual talk on children’s science learning in the context of shared book reading. Developmental Psychology, 59(2), 390–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller-Goldwater, H. E., Hanft, M. H., Miller, A. G., & Bauer, P. J. (2023b). Young children’s science learning from narrative books: The role of text cohesion and caregivers’ extratextual talk. Journal of Cognition and Development, 25(3), 323–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mills, C. M., Danovitch, J. H., Mugambi, V. N., Sands, K. R., & Pattisapu Fox, C. (2021). “Why do dogs pant?”: Characteristics of parental explanations about science predict children’s knowledge. Child Development, 93(2), 326–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ocular, G., Kelly, K. R., Millan, L., Neves, S., Avila, K., Hsieh, B., & Maloles, C. (2022). Contributions of naturalistic parent-child conversations to children’s science learning during informal learning at an aquarium and at home. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 943648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pagano, L. C., Haden, C. A., & Uttal, D. H. (2020). Museum program design supports parent–child engineering talk during tinkering and reminiscing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 200, 104944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paños, E., & Ruiz-Gallardo, J. R. (2020). Attitude toward informal science in the early years and development of Leisure Time in Science (LeTiS), a pictographic scale. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 58(5), 689–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pattison, S. A., & Dierking, L. D. (2018). Early childhood science interest development: Variation in interest patterns and parent–child interactions among low-income families. Science Education, 103(2), 362–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pattison, S. A., Svarovsky, G., Ramos-Montañez, S., Gontan, I., Weiss, S., Núñez, V. n., Corrie, P., Smith, C., & Benne, M. (2020). Understanding early childhood engineering interest development as a family-level systems phenomenon: Findings from the head start on engineering project. Journal of Pre-College Engineering Education Research (J-PEER), 10(1), 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Philip Bell, B. L., Shouse, A. W., & Feder, M. A. (Eds.). (2009). Learning science in informal environments: People, places, and pursuits. The National Academies Press. [Google Scholar]
- Polinsky, N., Pagano, L. C., Acosta, D. I., Haden, C. A., & Uttal, D. H. (2023). Spatial language in families’ conversational reflections about museum experiences. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 86, 101539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raynal, A., Lavigne, H., Goldstein, M., & Gutierrez, J. (2021). Starting with parents: Investigating a multi-generational, media-enhanced approach to support informal science learning for young children. Early Childhood Education Journal, 50(5), 879–889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salvatierra, L., & Cabello, V. (2022). Starting at home: What does the literature indicate about parental involvement in early childhood STEM education? Education Sciences, 12(3), 218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sheehan, K. J., Hightower, B., Lauricella, A. R., & Wartella, E. (2018). STEM media in the family context: The effect of STEM career and media use on preschoolers’ science and math skills. European Journal of STEM Education, 3(3), 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shirefley, T. A., & Leaper, C. (2022). Mothers’ and fathers’ science-related talk with daughters and sons while reading life and physical science books. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 813572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shouse, A. W., Schweingruber, H. A., & Duschl, R. A. (2007). Taking science to school: Learning and teaching science in grades K-8. National Academies Press. [Google Scholar]
- Strickler-Eppard, L., Czerniak, C. M., & Kaderavek, J. (2019). Families’ capacity to engage in science inquiry at home through structured activities. Early Childhood Education Journal, 47(6), 653–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suortti, E., Havu-Nuutinen, S., & Kärkkäinen, S. (2023). Finnish parents’ science capital and its association with sociodemographic issues. International Journal of Science Education, Part B, 14(3), 257–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szechter, L. E., & Carey, E. J. (2009). Gravitating toward science: Parent–child interactions at a gravitational-wave observatory. Science Education, 93(5), 846–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Terry, G., Hayfield, N., Clarke, V., & Braun, V. (2017). Thematic analysis. In The SAGE handbook of qualitative research in psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 17–37). SAGE Publications Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- Vandermaas-Peeler, M., Massey, K., & Kendall, A. (2015). Parent guidance of young children’s scientific and mathematical reasoning in a science museum. Early Childhood Education Journal, 44(3), 217–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandermaas-Peeler, M., Mischka, M., & Sands, K. (2017). ‘What do you notice?’ Parent guidance of preschoolers’ inquiry in activities at home. Early Child Development and Care, 189(2), 220–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Schijndel, T. J. P., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2016). Parent explanation and preschoolers’ exploratory behavior and learning in a shadow exhibition. Science Education, 100(1), 153–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Westerberg, L., Schmitt, S. A., Eason, S. H., & Purpura, D. J. (2022). Home science interactions and their relation to children’s science core knowledge in preschool. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 222, 105473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willard, A. K., Busch, J. T. A., Cullum, K. A., Letourneau, S. M., Sobel, D. M., Callanan, M., & Legare, C. H. (2019). Explain this, explore that: A study of parent–child interaction in a children’s museum. Child Development, 90(5), e598–e617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, J. M., Hoisington, C., Kook, J. F., & Ramer, M. (2024). Powering up preschool science: A home–school–community partnership to support science learning with a focus on emergent multilingual learners. Education Sciences, 14(7), 785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Gu, B.; Yu, D.; Liu, M. Family Science Capital Factors Affecting Early Childhood Science Learning: A Systematic Review. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 1152. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091152
Gu B, Yu D, Liu M. Family Science Capital Factors Affecting Early Childhood Science Learning: A Systematic Review. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(9):1152. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091152
Chicago/Turabian StyleGu, Bingying, Dongqing Yu, and Meimei Liu. 2025. "Family Science Capital Factors Affecting Early Childhood Science Learning: A Systematic Review" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 9: 1152. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091152
APA StyleGu, B., Yu, D., & Liu, M. (2025). Family Science Capital Factors Affecting Early Childhood Science Learning: A Systematic Review. Behavioral Sciences, 15(9), 1152. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091152