Next Article in Journal
Through Another’s Eyes: Implicit SNARC-like Attention Bias Reveals Allocentric Mapping of Numerical Magnitude
Previous Article in Journal
Monkey Do, Monkey See? The Effect of Imitation Strategies on Visuospatial Perspective-Taking and Self-Reported Social Cognitive Skills
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Altruism Prioritization Engine: How Empathic Concern Shapes Children’s Inequity Aversion in the Ultimatum Game
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Value-Directed Remembering: A Dual-Process Perspective

1
Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300387, China
2
School of Landscape Architecture and Architecture, Jiangxi Environmental Engineering Vocational College, Ganzhou 341000, China
3
School of Sociology, University of Sanya, Sanya 572022, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2025, 15(8), 1113; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081113
Submission received: 2 July 2025 / Revised: 14 August 2025 / Accepted: 15 August 2025 / Published: 17 August 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Children’s Cognitive Development in Social and Cultural Contexts)

Abstract

Value-directed remembering involves two key mechanisms: automatic processing and strategic processing. Automatic processing relies on the brain’s reward system and is associated with midbrain dopaminergic pathways and medial temporal-lobe activity. Strategic processing, in contrast, involves conscious, effortful encoding strategies and engages semantic-processing regions and executive control systems. This article reviews the developmental trajectory of value-directed remembering from childhood to old age through the lens of a dual-process model. Children and adolescents primarily rely on automatic processing; adults are capable of flexibly switching between the two processes; older adults tend to rely more on strategic processing. These findings reflect the dynamic developmental changes in the brain’s reward and executive-control systems. Future research should further investigate the synergistic interplay between dual-processing mechanisms, the moderating role of cultural contexts, and the efficacy of intervention strategies to deepen our understanding of the developmental trajectory of value-directed memory.

1. Introduction

Human memory is inherently limited in its capacity to encode and retain information. This limitation becomes especially salient in the current era of information overload, where individuals are bombarded with a vast array of content. To achieve efficient memory performance, it is essential to selectively encode and retain information that is most relevant or valuable while ignoring less important content (Castel et al., 2007). This process—prioritizing and deliberately encoding high-value information for future retrieval—is referred to as value-directed remembering (VDR) (Castel et al., 2007; Yan et al., 2013). A growing body of research has demonstrated that individuals can flexibly allocate cognitive resources based on the perceived importance of information, preferentially remembering high-value content (Castel et al., 2007; Siegel et al., 2021; Elliott et al., 2020a, 2020b).
Value modulates memory performance through two distinct mechanisms: strategic and automatic processing. Strategic processing involves consciously selecting and elaborately encoding important information through deep semantic processing (M. S. Cohen et al., 2017). This top-down mechanism critically depends on metacognitive skills—particularly the capacity to monitor, select, and adaptively apply optimal encoding strategies (Murphy et al., 2021). In contrast, automatic processing operates through bottom-up mechanisms, where memory enhancement occurs when information is intrinsically linked to reward salience or prediction violations. Such effects are generally driven by dopaminergic signaling and prediction error mechanisms in the brain (Knowlton & Castel, 2022).
From the perspective of developmental cognitive neuroscience, the interactive mechanisms supporting value-directed remembering demonstrate dynamic reorganization across the lifespan. During childhood and adolescence, memory for valuable information primarily depends on automatic processes—a pattern likely linked to the early development of the brain’s reward system and its preferential modulation of memory encoding. As individuals mature into adulthood, cognitive flexibility increases, allowing for dynamic switching between automatic and strategic modes of processing depending on task demands. In older adulthood, however, strategic processing becomes increasingly dominant, possibly reflecting both a decline in reward-related function and a compensatory reliance on executive control mechanisms (Knowlton & Castel, 2022; Zhong & Jiang, 2024).
From a dual-process perspective, this paper aims to systematically examine the roles of automatic and strategic processing across different age groups and their underlying neural mechanisms, while also attempting to construct an integrative theoretical framework encompassing mechanisms, development, and methodology (see Figure 1). This framework visually depicts the dual-processing pathways in value-directed memory, their dynamic interactions, lifespan developmental trajectories, and the primary experimental paradigms, providing a theoretical reference for understanding the selectivity and adaptive nature of memory.

2. The Dual-Process Mechanism of Value-Directed Remembering

Traditional memory theories have focused on the processes of encoding, storage, and retrieval but have not fully explained why individuals tend to remember some information better than others (Castel et al., 2007). With the advancement of cognitive psychology and neuroscience, researchers have increasingly recognized that memory selectivity is influenced not only by factors such as salience and repetition but also by the subjective value of the information (Knowlton & Castel, 2022). Prior research has revealed that two distinct mechanisms—automatic processing and strategic processing—are responsible for the differential encoding of high-value versus low-value information (Knowlton & Castel, 2022; M. S. Cohen et al., 2017; Bowen et al., 2020). The brain’s reward system primarily drives automatic processing. When information is associated with potential rewards, this system is automatically activated, enhancing the encoding and retrieval of valuable content (Adcock et al., 2006; Cheng et al., 2020).
In contrast, strategic processing involves deliberate, deep semantic encoding strategies and engages regions associated with semantic elaboration and executive control (M. S. Cohen et al., 2014). Based on these findings, researchers have proposed a dual-process theoretical framework to explain both the neural and behavioral aspects of value-directed remembering.

2.1. Automatic Processing

Automatic processing refers to the spontaneous encoding, storage, and retrieval of information with relatively low cognitive-resource consumption. In the context of value-directed memory, this process is primarily regulated by neural mechanisms driven by reward prediction error (RPE) (Knowlton & Castel, 2022). Midbrain dopaminergic neurons dynamically modulate synaptic plasticity by encoding RPE (RPE = actual reward − expected reward): a positive RPE, accompanied by phasic dopamine release, significantly enhances encoding efficiency, whereas a negative RPE suppresses memory processing (Schultz et al., 1997). In human declarative memory studies (S. Wang et al., 2023), positive RPE selectively enhances activity within the ventral tegmental area (VTA)–hippocampal circuit, facilitating long-term consolidation of unexpectedly high-reward items (Gruber et al., 2016). Under this mechanism, high-value information preferentially activates the striatal nucleus accumbens network, which not only encodes reward salience (Knutson et al., 2001) but also participates in value computation based on reinforcement learning principles (Daw et al., 2006), thereby biasing attention and memory toward high-value items. When individuals anticipate high rewards, VTA-hippocampal functional connectivity is enhanced, directly optimizing memory storage (Adcock et al., 2006).
The spontaneous nature of automatic processing is particularly evident in involuntary autobiographical memories (IAMs). According to the direct retrieval theory, IAMs are triggered by bottom-up, automatic associative activation rather than conscious control (Berntsen, 2024). Developmental research indicates that IAMs emerge early in life and maintain a relatively stable frequency across age, contrasting with the gradual increase in voluntary autobiographical memory with age. For instance, young children often spontaneously recall past experiences in response to specific environmental cues, such as particular smells or scenes, demonstrating that automatic memory-retrieval mechanisms are already functional before the maturation of executive control.
Automatic processing in value-directed remembering not only relies on the coordinated activity of the reward system and memory-related brain regions but is also closely linked to motivation. As an intrinsic driver of behavior, motivation determines the allocation of attention, the prioritization of information processing, and memory storage (Murty & Adcock, 2014; Weinstein, 2023). High motivational states can significantly enhance preferential processing of high-value information, even under unconscious conditions. Pessiglione et al. (2007) found that participants’ behavioral responses were amplified for high-value stimuli even when they were not consciously aware of them, indicating that reward effects can occur without conscious awareness. Similarly, Capa et al. (2011) demonstrated that individuals performed better on high-reward tasks than on low-reward tasks under unconscious conditions. These findings provide compelling evidence for the central role of motivation in automatic processing.
Rewards not only enhance memory for directly associated information but can also strengthen the encoding of stimuli occurring before or after the reward via a retroactive effect (Braun et al., 2018). Motivation is generally classified into extrinsic (e.g., monetary or material rewards) and intrinsic (e.g., autonomy, sense of achievement) types (Dickerson & Adcock, 2018). Extrinsic rewards can facilitate memory during both encoding and consolidation phases (Zhang et al., 2023).
During the encoding phase, reward anticipation preferentially triggers automatic processing, primarily dependent on activation of the brain’s reward system (Adcock et al., 2006). Using fMRI combined with computational modeling, M. X. Cohen (2007) revealed how multiple brain regions dynamically participate in value computation and decision guidance. Activity patterns in the caudate, amygdala, and orbitofrontal cortex adjusted dynamically according to reward expectation and reward prediction error (RPE). Furthermore, individual differences in reinforcement learning parameters, such as learning rate and risk preference, significantly enhanced the prediction of both behavior and neural activity. Subsequent studies further elucidated mechanisms of value integration. Gläscher et al. (2009) identified the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) as a key hub in value-based decision-making, responsible for integrating behaviorally derived value signals. A meta-analysis by Garrison et al. (2013) highlighted the striatum as a core region for encoding prediction errors, with distinct neural representations for reward versus punishment: reward prediction errors were concentrated in the striatum, whereas punishment prediction errors involved the insula and pallidum.
During the consolidation phase, reward-induced dopamine signals can directly act on memory-related structures, such as the hippocampus, to enhance the stability of long-term memories (Patil et al., 2017). By contrast, research on the mnemonic effects of intrinsic motivation is comparatively limited. Murty et al. (2019) demonstrated that even in the absence of explicit rewards, active decision-making significantly reduced forgetting rates in both immediate and delayed (24 h) tests, accompanied by increased striatal activation and enhanced post-encoding hippocampus–perirhinal cortex (PRC) functional connectivity. This suggests that autonomous choice can support memory consolidation through the striatum–hippocampus–PRC network, thereby improving long-term memory performance.
However, value-directed remembering does not rely solely on automatic processing. Value evaluation often requires integrating multidimensional information, including emotional and social cues, which necessitates the deep involvement of strategic processing. Moreover, goal-directed behavior depends on strategic processing for monitoring and adjustment, thereby enhancing adaptability and flexibility (Schwartz et al., 2023; Murphy et al., 2022b). Consequently, in value-directed memory, automatic and strategic processes operate in a complementary and synergistic manner.

2.2. Strategic Processing

In value-directed remembering (VDR), strategic processing represents a critical cognitive mechanism by which individuals intentionally employ specific encoding strategies to prioritize and remember high-value information, thereby enhancing both memory efficiency and performance. When presented with high-value items, individuals are more likely to engage in deep semantic encoding, forming meaningful contextual associations that increase memory strength and familiarity while also facilitating semantic understanding. Compared to automatic processing, strategic processing has a more substantial effect on the memory of high-value information (M. S. Cohen et al., 2017). This deliberate engagement of semantic elaboration not only optimizes memory performance but also plays a pivotal role in supporting cognitive decision-making when confronted with complex information.
A growing body of research has demonstrated that individuals actively apply mnemonic strategies to prioritize high-value items, leading to significantly better recall compared to low-value items (Stefanidi et al., 2018; Nguyen et al., 2019, 2020; Jackson, 2021; Villaseñor et al., 2021; Murphy & Castel, 2022a, 2022b; Murphy et al., 2022b; Silaj et al., 2023). This selective encoding advantage is closely linked to metacognitive abilities (Siegel & Castel, 2019; Murphy & Castel, 2020; Murphy et al., 2021, 2022a; Murphy & Knowlton, 2022; Murphy, 2023b; Silaj et al., 2023). Individuals with stronger metacognitive insight are better able to assess their memory capacity and adaptively modify their encoding strategies to optimize performance. As task experience and feedback accumulate, the ability to selectively remember high-value information improves significantly (Anquillare & Selmeczy, 2023; Schwartz et al., 2020, 2023). For instance, Castel et al. (2013) demonstrated that under time-constrained learning conditions, participants successfully prioritized high-value items by strategically allocating greater cognitive resources to their encoding. This selective investment of study time resulted in optimized memory performance for the most valuable information.
Neuroimaging studies have shown that strategic processing in VDR is associated with increased activation in several key brain regions, including the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA), and posterior lateral temporal cortex (M. S. Cohen et al., 2016). These regions are critically involved in deep semantic elaboration. Among them, the VLPFC plays a central role as part of the executive control network. Recent findings further suggest that successful strategic encoding of high-value information depends on the coordinated activation of semantic-processing regions and executive-control systems. Hennessee et al. (2019) demonstrated that enhanced semantic processing significantly improves memory for high-value items. When individuals are constrained to remembering only a subset of items, they tend to selectively allocate attention to the most valuable ones while ignoring less valuable content, thereby maximizing overall memory output. Strategic processing also entails prioritized refreshing and attentional allocation toward high-value information during working-memory maintenance. Recent research has shown that high-value items are more likely to be refreshed in working memory, not by increasing the time spent on each item but by increasing the frequency with which these items are refreshed (H. Li et al., 2023).
While strategic and automatic processing relies on different underlying neural systems, they are both essential to value-guided memory. Strategic encoding of high-value content is associated with increased activation in left-hemispheric regions involved in semantic processing, supporting the role of elaborative encoding in this process (Knowlton & Castel, 2022). In contrast, automatic processing depends more on the activation of the midbrain dopaminergic reward system, which selectively enhances encoding through dopamine-mediated signals (Gruber et al., 2016). However, Knowlton and Castel (2022) emphasized that these mechanisms are not mutually exclusive; rather, encoding in value-directed remembering reflects a dynamic interaction between strategic and automatic processes.

2.3. The Interaction Between Automatic and Strategic Processing

Automatic and strategic processing in value-directed remembering are not independent mechanisms but operate with close interaction. Automatic processing of high-value information may lay the groundwork for subsequent strategic processing, further amplifying memory performance (M. S. Cohen et al., 2014). Specifically, when the value of an item exceeds an individual’s expectations, these high-value items automatically capture attention, thereby prompting the allocation of greater attentional resources and the selection of optimal memory strategies to ensure their effective encoding and retrieval (Knowlton & Castel, 2022).
Research on prospective memory offers a unique perspective for understanding the interactive mechanisms between automatic and strategic processing within the memory system. The classic study by S. C. Somerville et al. (1983) on intentional reminding behavior in young children elucidates the developmental trajectory of this synergistic mechanism. Their findings revealed that children aged 2 to 4 demonstrated a significant advantage in high-value tasks (e.g., “remind to buy candy”), achieving up to 80% accuracy in unprompted recall. First, even before the full maturation of the prefrontal cortex, high-value intentions received prioritized encoding via reward systems such as the striatum, reflecting the foundational role of automatic processing within memory. Second, the study observed that 4-year-olds outperformed 2-year-olds in low-interest tasks, indicating that with the progressive maturation of prefrontal cortex functions, the capacity for intention maintenance was markedly enhanced—an early sign of strategic processing development. The multiprocess theory of prospective memory proposed by McDaniel and Einstein (2000) further supports a dual-processing account: on the one hand, when target cues are salient or highly relevant to the current context, automatic processing dependent on the striatum–hippocampal circuitry enables rapid intention retrieval; on the other hand, for non-salient cues, strategic monitoring mediated by the frontoparietal network requires sustained cognitive resources and impacts concurrent task performance. These findings demonstrate that a single mechanism does not support prospective memory but instead constitutes a dynamic system underpinned by automatic triggering and strategic monitoring.
In the field of value-directed memory, both behavioral and neuroimaging evidence support the notion of dual-process cooperation. M. S. Cohen et al. (2017) manipulated retrieval conditions. They found that experience with free-recall testing during the encoding phase not only enhanced subsequent recognition of high-value information but also facilitated detail recollection dependent on prefrontal control and familiarity-based retrieval reliant on the dopaminergic system. The underlying mechanisms may include activation of the prefrontal–hippocampal circuit during free recall, prompting participants to adopt more refined semantic encoding, while the VTA–striatum–hippocampus dopaminergic pathway strengthens the representational stability of high-value items. Metacognitive monitoring plays a regulatory role in this process: when participants recognize limitations in their memory through test feedback, they proactively adjust attention allocation, prioritize high-value information, and further engage the reward system’s automatic response.
Hennessee et al. (2019) provided additional evidence for dual-process cooperation. Participants studied value-tagged words and subsequently received “remember” or “forget” cues. Results indicated that for high-value items, “remember” cues significantly enhanced memory performance; however, even under “forget” cues, recognition sensitivity for high-value information remained higher than for low-value items, suggesting that high-value cues can trigger automatic processing even in the absence of external reinforcement.
Neuroimaging studies offer direct support for this cooperative mechanism. M. S. Cohen et al. (2014) observed that encoding high-value information was associated with increased activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus and left posterior lateral temporal cortex—regions linked to deep semantic processing—indicating a central role of strategic processing in high-value encoding. Simultaneously, modest activation in the midbrain and ventral striatum correlated with memory performance, demonstrating the contribution of automatic processing within strategic encoding. Rothkirch et al. (2014), using an fMRI monetary incentive delay (MID) task, revealed a hierarchical neural mechanism for reward processing: the subcortical reward regions (ventral striatum and ventral tegmental area) responded to reward cues independently of attentional tasks, displaying typical automatic features. The higher-order visual cortex (fusiform gyrus) integrated input from both subcortical reward pathways and frontoparietal attention networks, reflecting dual-pathway modulation; in contrast, reward processing in the anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortex was attention-dependent, showing enhanced activation only when attention was directed toward reward cues. Dynamic causal modeling indicated that such modulation is implemented via attentional regulation of ventral striatum–anterior insula connectivity. Giuffrida et al. (2023) further demonstrated that reward anticipation significantly enhances prefrontal cortex activity, optimizing core executive functions such as response inhibition and cognitive control, thereby providing critical neural support for the cooperative operation of strategic and automatic processing.
These theoretical reviews, behavioral experiments, and neuroimaging findings illuminate the cooperative mechanisms between automatic and strategic processing in value-directed remembering. They provide robust theoretical and empirical support for understanding their complementary and interactive roles in memory formation.

3. Development of Value-Directed Remembering

The capacities for automatic and strategic processing exhibit marked differences across developmental stages. From childhood to adolescence, automatic processing abilities show substantial enhancement, closely linked to the maturation of the midbrain–dopaminergic system. This developmental change is accompanied by increased sensitivity to reward signals, thereby promoting the prioritized encoding and storage of high-value information (Galván, 2013). Neuroimaging meta-analyses indicate that reward-related regions, such as the ventral striatum, reach peak activation during adolescence (Silverman et al., 2015). Encoding studies in children further demonstrate that reward contexts can enhance memory sensitivity (Ngo et al., 2019) and that reward processing and associative memory capacities undergo dynamic changes throughout development (Meyer & Pattwell, 2020; A. O. Cohen et al., 2022). Behavioral evidence suggests that reward-seeking behaviors follow an inverted U-shaped trajectory during adolescence, with dual peaks observed around ages 12–15 and 17–18 (Steinberg et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2012). Enhanced reward sensitivity during adolescence not only affects cognitive performance but may also relate to changes in depression risk (Rohde et al., 2013). For example, neural sensitivity to eudaimonic rewards may buffer depressive symptoms (Telzer et al., 2014). Moreover, the interactive dynamics among reward-system function, life-stress exposure, and emotional states during adolescence may differ from those in early adulthood (Corral-Frías et al., 2015). Recent evidence indicates that ventral-striatum and amygdala activation during reward anticipation can modulate the association between life-stress and depressive symptoms in adolescents, suggesting that heightened neural-reward sensitivity may buffer the adverse emotional effects of stress (Fassett-Carman et al., 2023).
In contrast, the development of strategic processing is relatively protracted. During childhood and early adolescence, the prefrontal cortex is not fully mature, and higher-order cognitive functions such as inhibitory control and executive regulation are limited, making reward processing more likely to interfere with strategic information processing (Casey, 2015; Padmanabhan et al., 2011). Structural immaturity may also reduce the efficiency of value-evaluation systems (Davidow et al., 2018). In late adolescence, structural and functional maturation of the prefrontal cortex supports significant improvements in executive control, thereby facilitating the development of strategic processing (Ofen et al., 2007). Nevertheless, compared to adults, adolescents still exhibit deficits in the selection and implementation efficiency of memory strategies (L. H. Somerville & Casey, 2011). Cross-sectional studies show that children (5–9 years), adolescents (10–17 years), and young adults (18–23 years) all demonstrate a priority recall effect for high-value information; however, children and adolescents display significantly lower selection accuracy than young adults (Castel et al., 2011). Longitudinal evidence indicates that Selective Learning Efficiency (SLE) steadily increases with age, likely reflecting brain structural maturation (Hanten et al., 2007; Posthuma et al., 2003). From a neural perspective, the functional integration of frontal–basal ganglia circuits is considered a core mechanism underlying the development of inhibitory control, providing a critical neural foundation for the maturation of strategic processing (Kang et al., 2022).
Age-related dynamic studies further elucidate the neural mechanisms underlying memory development in children and adolescents. A. O. Cohen et al. (2022) examined participants aged 8–25 years. They found that enhanced functional connectivity between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and the ventral tegmental area (VTA) during the encoding phase was significantly associated with improvements in high-reward item-specific memory, with this association increasing with age. Changes in connectivity between the anterior hippocampus and VTA were more strongly linked to gains in high-reward gist memory among children and adolescents. Adolescents exhibited superior performance in high-reward general-source memory compared to both children and adults, but their performance under low-reward conditions was comparatively weaker. These findings suggest that heightened reward sensitivity during adolescence may selectively facilitate or interfere with memory performance depending on the context, and they also highlight the potentially greater role of post-encoding interactions within the mesolimbic system in the formation of reward-based memories in children (Kurdziel et al., 2018).
Adulthood marks the peak of both automatic and strategic processing abilities. Functional connectivity between the midbrain dopaminergic system and medial temporal regions becomes more stable, significantly enhancing memory for high-value information. Functional MRI studies indicate that the reward system can rapidly evaluate the value of information through automatic processing mechanisms and modulate memory systems to optimize encoding of high-value items (Adcock et al., 2006). Functional connectivity between the ventral striatum and hippocampus is strengthened during adulthood, supporting prioritized encoding and retrieval of high-value information (Casey, 2015; Davidow et al., 2016). In addition, adults demonstrate high cognitive-control flexibility, allowing them to dynamically adjust memory strategies according to task demands—for example, engaging in deep semantic processing when encoding important information (Hennessee et al., 2019; Knowlton & Castel, 2022). Experimental evidence further shows that when participants are instructed to use a uniform strategy for all items (e.g., mental rehearsal or imagery), memory for low-value items is also enhanced (Hennessee et al., 2019). Thus, the encoding advantage for high-value information arises not only from motivational drives but also from strategic-processing involvement.
In older adulthood, prefrontal cortical volume gradually declines (Raz et al., 2005), accompanied by substantial reductions in memory and cognitive function (Salthouse, 2019; Thomas & Gutchess, 2020). Functional decline in the midbrain dopaminergic system leads to decreased automatic-processing capacity, impairing the enhancement of memory for important information (Samanez-Larkin & Knutson, 2015). Nevertheless, despite deterioration in neural-reward mechanisms and prefrontal function, older adults continue to exhibit selective memory for high-value information. This phenomenon can be explained by the Socioemotional Selectivity Theory (SST) (Reed & Carstensen, 2012), which posits that as perceived future time diminishes, older adults adjust their goal systems to prioritize emotional regulation over knowledge acquisition. Consequently, they tend to preferentially select and maintain positive information, producing the characteristic “positivity effect” (Kennedy et al., 2004). Neurobehavioral evidence indicates that this effect is both delayed and context-dependent, manifesting as sustained attention to positive stimuli and inhibition of negative stimuli, particularly under free-recall conditions (Isaacowitz et al., 2009; Kisley et al., 2007; Kensinger et al., 2002).
At the level of cognitive processing, although automatic processing declines, older adults display adaptive strategic processing and metacognitive regulation (Hargis et al., 2019; Murphy & Castel, 2021). They leverage accumulated semantic knowledge and life experience, employing selective attention and flexible memory strategies to prioritize encoding of high-value information relevant to current goals. This optimization of cognitive resources, closely linked with metacognitive awareness, enables older adults to manage cognitive load and maintain task-performance effectively (Siegel & Castel, 2019).

4. Research Methods in Value-Directed Memory

In the study of value-directed remembering, free recall and recognition tests represent two core-memory measurement paradigms, revealing significant mechanistic differences in how the memory system processes value-related information. Much empirical research shows that free recall primarily relies on controlled, strategic processing, enabling individuals to prioritize the encoding and retrieval of high-value information selectively. In contrast, recognition tasks depend more heavily on automatic processing, with neural mechanisms predominantly involving dopaminergic reward-system modulation (M. S. Cohen et al., 2017). This mechanistic distinction results in different manifestations of value effects across paradigms: the facilitative impact of value on memory is pronounced in free recall, whereas it tends to be comparatively weaker in recognition tasks (Murphy, 2023a).

4.1. Free-Recall Tests

The most commonly employed paradigm within free-recall research is Value-Directed Remembering (VDR). A typical VDR task consists of three stages. First is the learning phase, during which participants are presented with study materials paired with point values (e.g., “television—8”) and are informed that they will earn corresponding points for each correctly recalled item, to maximize their total score. Second, the interference phase introduces a filler task before recall to eliminate short-term memory effects. Third, the recall phase requires participants to retrieve as many studied items as possible, with total scores computed accordingly. Measurement typically involves the number of items recalled and the Selectivity Index (SI), which quantifies an individual’s sensitivity to high-versus low-value information. The Selectivity Index (SI) is mathematically expressed as: SI = (Actual Score − Chance Score)/(Ideal Score − Chance Score).
For example, if the study list contains 12 words with values ranging from 1 to 12 points, and a participant recalls six words valued at 12, 10, 9, 7, 6, and 4 points, respectively, the actual score is 48 (12 + 10 + 9 + 7 + 6 + 4). The ideal score is the sum of the highest six values, 57 (12 + 11 + 10 + 9 + 8 + 7). The chance score equals the average value (6.5) multiplied by the number of recalled items (6), resulting in 39 (6.5 × 6). Substituting these into the formula yields an SI of 0.5. The SI ranges from −1 to 1, where values closer to 1 indicate stronger selectivity for high-value items, values near 0 indicate no value-directed memory, and values approaching −1 indicate a bias toward recalling low-value items.
Researchers frequently manipulate the value distribution of study materials to examine the effect of value gradients on memory. These manipulations include binary high–low structures (e.g., 1 vs. 10 points) (Murphy et al., 2022a; Park et al., 2022), ternary low–medium–high structures (e.g., 1, 5, 10 points) (McDonough et al., 2015), repeated 1–10 point structures (each value presented twice) (Middlebrooks et al., 2017; Schwartz et al., 2023), fully continuous 1–20 point structures (each value paired with a unique item) (Murphy et al., 2024; Murphy & Knowlton, 2022), and other value arrangements.
For data analysis, analysis of variance (ANOVA) is commonly employed to compare overall selectivity. In contrast, multilevel modeling (MLM), treating value as a continuous variable at the trial level, captures individual differences more effectively and allows precise assessment of value effects in complex data structures (Murphy, 2023a).

4.2. Recognition Tasks

Recognition tests in value-directed memory research commonly employ the Monetary Incentive Encoding (MIE) paradigm or recognition tasks adapted from the Value-Directed Remembering (VDR) framework. These experiments typically include three phases: reward-cued encoding, an interference task, and a recognition test, during which participants make “old/new” judgments. Recognition tasks are often combined with Remember/Know (R/K) judgments to dissociate recollection-based from familiarity-based processing (Tulving, 1985). Evidence indicates that high-value information primarily enhances recollection, with relatively limited effects on familiarity (Elliott & Brewer, 2019; Hennessee et al., 2017).
To improve measurement precision, the Recognition Without Cued Recall (RWCR) paradigm, combined with signal detection theory, allows for more accurate quantification of recollection and familiarity components while reducing error accumulation (Cleary, 2004). Additionally, Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) curve analysis is frequently used to examine the dual-process characteristics of memory; asymmetries in the ROC curve reflect the joint contributions of recollection and familiarity.
Metrics for recollection (R) and familiarity (F) are typically derived from the independent process model proposed by Yonelinas and Jacoby (1995), with corrections for guessing by subtracting false alarm rates. Two approaches are commonly applied for handling false alarms. The first is the overall false alarm method, which assumes that false alarm rates for new high- and low-value items are equal; this approach is simple but may underestimate false alarms for high-value items. The second is the value-specific false alarm method, which infers false-alarm rates for each category based on the distribution of hits. This method aligns better with the assumption that high-value items are encoded more thoroughly, but it requires that the likelihood of misidentifying new versus old items is symmetric across value categories; otherwise, recollection and familiarity estimates may be biased. Researchers often conduct sensitivity analyses to assess the robustness of results across different false-alarm correction methods, thereby ensuring the reliability of memory-parameter estimates.
In summary, free recall and recognition tasks each offer distinct measurement advantages. By integrating multilevel analyses with methodological refinements, these paradigms can systematically elucidate the contributions of strategic and automatic processing in value-directed memory.

5. General Discussion and Future Directions

This review examined the developmental trajectory of value-directed remembering (VDR) through the lens of dual-process mechanisms, highlighting age-related differences in the roles of automatic and strategic processing. Prior research has consistently shown that both processing modes contribute to enhanced memory for high-value information (Murphy & Castel, 2022b, 2022c). However, due to differences in the maturation timelines of relevant brain regions, children and adolescents tend to be less effective at strategically encoding high-value items compared to adults. Nonetheless, their ability to selectively remember valuable information improves steadily throughout adolescence, reaching full maturity in adulthood (Castel et al., 2011; A. O. Cohen et al., 2022). As individuals age further, memory encoding and retrieval capacities decline. Yet, older adults maintain the ability to selectively remember important information by relying on preserved cognitive control, metacognitive skills, and life experience (Castel et al., 2011; Siegel & Castel, 2019). While substantial progress has been made, several critical questions remain. Future research can address these gaps through the following directions:

5.1. The Interaction of Dual-Process Mechanisms in Adolescents and Adults

Although previous studies have often examined automatic and strategic processes in isolation, recent empirical findings suggest that the memory advantage for high-value items results from their dynamic interaction (Knowlton & Castel, 2022). For example, in adults, strategic processing may exert a stronger influence on memory than automatic mechanisms (M. S. Cohen et al., 2017). By contrast, adolescents often outperform adults on reinforcement learning tasks, possibly due to heightened connectivity between reward evaluation and memory systems during puberty (Davidow et al., 2016). Future research should examine how these mechanisms interact across age groups to influence value-based memory selectivity and neural-activation patterns. Multimodal neuroimaging techniques such as fMRI and EEG could elucidate the underlying neural dynamics at different developmental stages.

5.2. Neural and Cognitive Mechanisms of VDR in Older Adults

Selective encoding of valuable information is closely linked to metacognitive functioning. When presented with items of varying value, individuals tend to allocate more cognitive resources to higher-value content (Castel et al., 2011). Despite age-related declines in general memory capacity, healthy older adults can use metacognitive strategies to maintain selective memory performance (Siegel & Castel, 2019). However, patients with neurodegenerative conditions, such as behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD), often exhibit impairments in value-based encoding. Wong et al. (2019) suggested that reduced motivational responses in bvFTD may be partially due to reliance on point-based reward systems. Future research should explore whether more tangible incentives (e.g., monetary rewards) and high-resolution neuroimaging can reveal how different reward types influence VDR in populations with cognitive decline.

5.3. The Shaping Role of Cultural and Contextual Factors on Dual-Processing Mechanisms

Cultural values shape memory-processing patterns from early developmental stages (Q. Wang & Ross, 2007). Western individualistic cultures reinforce an independent self-construal, promoting the development of goal-directed, detail-oriented strategic memory processing, which at the neural level manifests as enhanced control in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Hedden et al., 2008). In contrast, East Asian collectivistic cultures emphasize the automatic processing of social context and background information, with memory content often reconstructed semantically and relationally; this is closely associated with increased activation of the default mode network (Gutchess et al., 2006). Such differences may stem from distinct early perceptual processing strategies (Davenport & Potter, 2004; Miyamoto et al., 2006) and exhibit a degree of plasticity in response to changes in task context (Gutchess et al., 2006). Future research should further explore the interaction between cultural background and developmental stages to elucidate the adaptation and transformation of value-directed memory across cross-cultural contexts.

5.4. Developmentally Informed Intervention Strategies for Value-Directed Memory

Age-related differences are evident in memory performance and influence intervention efficacy. Children and adolescents, due to incomplete prefrontal-cortex development and limited metacognitive capacities, show markedly lower selective memory than adults (Castel et al., 2011). Recent findings indicate that test-feedback experience can significantly enhance memory selectivity in value contexts (Q. Li et al., 2025). Although adults and older adults possess stronger strategic processing abilities, their attentional resource allocation is susceptible to disruption by emotional states and stress (Knowlton & Castel, 2022). Therefore, integrative interventions combining value cues with stress management may be more effective. Large-scale projects such as the ACTIVE study (Willis et al., 2006) and meta-analyses (Hudes et al., 2019; Gross et al., 2012; Verhaeghen et al., 1992) have confirmed that systematic strategy training substantially improves memory performance in older adults. Integrating these training approaches within the Value-Directed Remembering (VDR) framework holds promise for optimizing memory selectivity across different age groups. Nevertheless, the field lacks long-term follow-up, transfer-effect validation, and fine-grained neural mechanism modeling.

5.5. Directions for Methodological and Experimental Design Optimization

Future VDR research requires further methodological refinement to enhance ecological and internal validity. For example, assigning value labels to old and new items in recognition paradigms could reduce systematic biases in false-alarm-rate estimation. Analytically, incorporating multilevel linear modeling (MLM) allows simultaneous handling of item-level and individual-level variables, while Bayesian methods provide robust parameter estimation and credible intervals. Combining signal detection theory (SDT) with reinforcement learning models can elucidate the dynamic influence of value on memory encoding and retrieval processes. Furthermore, integrating neurophysiological data such as eye-tracking, fMRI, or EEG can facilitate the construction of computational maps of value-modulated memory pathways, clarifying the neural implementation of reward and motivation across different processing stages and providing empirical support for refining the VDR theoretical framework.

Author Contributions

Q.L.: conceptualization, systematic literature search and screening, core thesis development, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing. W.T.: study framework design, references verification, writing—review and editing. X.L.: study framework design, quality control, writing—review and editing, submission coordination All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created or analyzed in this study.

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our gratitude to all participants who took part in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest, including any financial or personal relationships that could influence the work reported in this paper.

References

  1. Adcock, R. A., Thangavel, A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Knutson, B., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2006). Reward-motivated learning: Mesolimbic activation precedes memory formation. Neuron, 50(3), 507–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Anquillare, E., & Selmeczy, D. (2023). Developmental differences in value-based remembering: The role of feedback and metacognition. Developmental Psychology, 59(7), 1181–1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Berntsen, D. (2024). Direct retrieval as a theory of involuntary autobiographical memories: Evaluation and future directions. Memory, 32(6), 709–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bowen, H. J., Gallant, S. N., & Moon, D. H. (2020). Influence of reward motivation on directed forgetting in younger and older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Braun, E. K., Wimmer, G. E., & Shohamy, D. (2018). Retroactive and graded prioritization of memory by reward. Nature Communications, 9(1), 4886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Capa, R. L., Bustin, G. M., Cleeremans, A., & Hansenne, M. (2011). Conscious and unconscious reward cues can affect a critical component of executive control: (Un)conscious updating? Experimental Psychology, 58(5), 370–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Casey, B. J. (2015). Beyond simple models of self-control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 295–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Castel, A. D., Farb, N. A. S., & Craik, F. I. M. (2007). Memory for general and specific value information in younger and older adults: Measuring the limits of strategic control. Memory & Cognition, 35(4), 689–700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Castel, A. D., Humphreys, K. L., Lee, S. S., Galván, A., Balota, D. A., & McCabe, D. P. (2011). The development of memory efficiency and valuedirected remembering across the life span: A cross-sectional study of memory and selectivity. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1553–1564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Castel, A. D., Murayama, K., Friedman, M. C., McGillivray, S., & Link, I. (2013). Selecting valuable information to remember: Age-related differences and similarities in self-regulated learning. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 232–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Cheng, S., Jiang, T., Xue, J., Wang, S., Chen, C., & Zhang, M. (2020). The influence of rewards on incidental memory: More does not mean better. Learning & Memory, 27(11), 462–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cleary, A. M. (2004). Orthography, phonology, and meaning: Word features that give rise to feelings of familiarity in recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(3), 446–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Cohen, A. O., Glover, M. M., Shen, X., Phaneuf, C. V., Avallone, K. N., Davachi, L., & Hartley, C. A. (2022). Reward enhances memory via age-varying online and offline neural mechanisms across development. The Journal of Neuroscience, 42(33), 6424–6434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cohen, M. S., Rissman, J., Hovhannisyan, M., Castel, A. D., & Knowlton, B. J. (2017). Free recall test experience potentiates strategy-driven effects of value on memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(10), 1581–1601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Cohen, M. S., Rissman, J., Suthana, N. A., Castel, A. D., & Knowlton, B. J. (2014). Value-based modulation of memory encoding involves strategic engagement of fronto-temporal semantic processing regions. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(2), 578–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Cohen, M. S., Rissman, J., Suthana, N. A., Castel, A. D., & Knowlton, B. J. (2016). Effects of aging on value-directed modulation of semantic network activity during verbal learning. NeuroImage, 125, 1046–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Cohen, M. X. (2007). Individual differences and the neural representations of reward expectation and reward prediction error. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(1), 20–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Corral-Frías, N. S., Nikolova, Y. S., Michalski, L. J., Baranger, D. A. A., Hariri, A. R., & Bogdan, R. (2015). Stress-related anhedonia is associated with ventral striatum reactivity to reward and transdiagnostic psychiatric symptomatology. Psychological Medicine, 45(12), 2605–2617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Davenport, J. L., & Potter, M. C. (2004). Scene consistency in object and background perception. Psychological Science, 15(8), 559–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Davidow, J. Y., Foerde, K., Galván, A., & Shohamy, D. (2016). An upside to reward sensitivity: The hippocampus supports enhanced reinforcement learning in adolescence. Neuron, 92(1), 93–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Davidow, J. Y., Insel, C., & Somerville, L. H. (2018). Adolescent development of value-guided goal pursuit. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(8), 725–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Daw, N. D., O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature, 441(7095), 876–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Dickerson, K. C., & Adcock, R. A. (2018). Motivation and memory. In J. T. Wixted, E. A. Phelps, & L. Davachi (Eds.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience: Learning and memory (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 215–150). John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  24. Elliott, B. L., Blais, C., McClure, S. M., & Brewer, G. A. (2020a). Neural correlates underlying the effect of reward value on recognition memory. NeuroImage, 206, 116296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Elliott, B. L., & Brewer, G. A. (2019). Divided attention selectively impairs value-directed encoding. Collabra: Psychology, 5(1), 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Elliott, B. L., McClure, S. M., & Brewer, G. A. (2020b). Individual differences in value-directed remembering. Cognition, 201, 104275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Fassett-Carman, A. N., Moser, A. D., Ruzic, L., Neilson, C., Jones, J., Barnes-Horowitz, S., Schneck, C. D., & Kaiser, R. H. (2023). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens activation during reward anticipation moderates the association between life stressor frequency and depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 330, 309–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Galván, A. (2013). The teenage brain: Sensitivity to rewards. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(2), 88–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Garrison, J., Erdeniz, B., & Done, J. (2013). Prediction error in reinforcement learning: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(7), 1297–1310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Giuffrida, V., Marc, I. B., Ramawat, S., Fontana, R., Fiori, L., Bardella, G., Fagioli, S., Ferraina, S., Brunamonti, E., & Pani, P. (2023). Reward prospect affects strategic adjustments in stop signal task. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1125066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Gläscher, J., Hampton, A. N., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2009). Determining a role for ventromedial prefrontal cortex in encoding action-based value signals during reward-related decision making. Cerebral Cortex, 19(2), 483–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Gross, A. L., Parisi, J. M., Spira, A. P., Kueider, A. M., Ko, J. Y., Saczynski, J. S., Samus, Q. M., & Rebok, G. W. (2012). Memory training interventions for older adults: A meta-analysis. Aging & Mental Health, 16(6), 722–734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Gruber, M. J., Ritchey, M., Wang, S.-F., Doss, M. K., & Ranganath, C. (2016). Post-learning hippocampal dynamics promote preferential retention of rewarding events. Neuron, 89(5), 1110–1120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Gutchess, A. H., Welsh, R. C., Boduroglu, A., & Park, D. C. (2006). Cultural differences in neural function associated with object processing. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(2), 102–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hanten, G., Li, X., Chapman, S. B., Swank, P., Gamino, J., Roberson, G., & Levin, H. S. (2007). Development of Verbal Selective Learning. Developmental Neuropsychology, 32(1), 585–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hargis, M. B., Siegel, A. L. M., & Castel, A. D. (2019). Motivated memory, learning, and decision-making in older age: Shifts in priorities and goals. In G. Samanez-Larkin (Ed.), The aging brain: Functional adaptation across adulthood (pp. 135–164). American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19(1), 12–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hennessee, J. P., Castel, A. D., & Knowlton, B. J. (2017). Recognizing what matters: Value improves recognition by selectively enhancing recollection. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 195–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Hennessee, J. P., Patterson, T. K., Castel, A. D., & Knowlton, B. J. (2019). Forget me not: Encoding processes in value-directed remembering. Journal of Memory and Language, 106, 29–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Hudes, R., Rich, J. B., Troyer, A. K., Yusupov, I., & Vandermorris, S. (2019). The impact of memory-strategy training interventions on participant-reported outcomes in healthy older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 34(4), 587–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Isaacowitz, D. M., Allard, E. S., Murphy, N. A., & Schlangel, M. (2009). The time course of age-related preferences toward positive and negative stimuli. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 64B(2), 188–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Jackson, R. L. (2021). The neural correlates of semantic control revisited. NeuroImage, 224, 117444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Kang, W., Hernández, S. P., Rahman, M. S., Voigt, K., & Malvaso, A. (2022). Inhibitory control development: A network neuroscience perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 651547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Kennedy, Q., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). The role of motivation in the age-related positivity effect in autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15(3), 208–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Kensinger, E. A., Brierley, B., Medford, N., Growdon, J. H., & Corkin, S. (2002). Effects of normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease on emotional memory. Emotion, 2(2), 118–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Kisley, M. A., Wood, S., & Burrows, C. L. (2007). Looking at the sunny side of life: Age-related change in an event-related potential measure of the negativity bias. Psychological Science, 18, 838–843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Knowlton, B. J., & Castel, A. D. (2022). Memory and reward-based learning: A value-directed remembering perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 73(1), 25–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., & Hommer, D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(16), RC159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Kurdziel, L. B. F., Kent, J., & Spencer, R. M. C. (2018). Sleep-dependent enhancement of emotional memory in early childhood. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 12609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Li, H., Lin, S., & Wan, B. (2023). Value-directed attentional refreshing and its mechanism. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(8), 1234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Li, Q., Tang, W., & Liu, X. (2025). The modulatory mechanisms of free-recall testing experience on value-directed memory [Manuscript in preparation]. Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University. [Google Scholar]
  52. McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(7), S127–S144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. McDonough, I. M., Bui, D. C., Friedman, M. F., & Castel, A. D. (2015). Retrieval monitoring is influenced by information value: The interplay between importance and confidence on false memory. Acta Psychologica, 161, 7–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Meyer, H. C., & Pattwell, S. S. (2020). Memory across development, with insights from emotional learning: A nonlinear process. In D. Poeppel, G. R. Mangun, & M. S. Gazzaniga (Eds.), The cognitive neurosciences (6th ed., pp. 245–255). MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  55. Middlebrooks, C. D., Kerr, T. K., & Castel, A. D. (2017). Selectively distracted: Divided attention and memory for important information. Psychological Science, 28(8), 1103–1115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Miyamoto, Y., Nisbett, R. E., & Masuda, T. (2006). Culture and the physical environment: Holistic versus analytic perceptual affordances. Psychological Science, 17(2), 113–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Murphy, D. H. (2023a). Does point value structure influence measures of memory selectivity? Memory, 31(8), 1074–1088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Murphy, D. H. (2023b). Strategic offloading: How the value of to-be-remembered information influences offloading decision-making. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 37(4), 749–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Murphy, D. H., Agadzhanyan, K., Whatley, M. C., & Castel, A. D. (2021). Metacognition and fluid intelligence in value-directed remembering. Metacognition and Learning, 16(3), 685–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Murphy, D. H., & Castel, A. D. (2020). Responsible remembering: How metacognition impacts adaptive selective memory. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 228(4), 301–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Murphy, D. H., & Castel, A. D. (2021). Responsible remembering and forgetting as contributors to memory for important information. Memory & Cognition, 49(5), 895–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Murphy, D. H., & Castel, A. D. (2022a). Differential effects of proactive and retroactive interference in value-directed remembering for younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 37(7), 787–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Murphy, D. H., & Castel, A. D. (2022b). Responsible remembering and forgetting in younger and older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 48(5), 455–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Murphy, D. H., & Castel, A. D. (2022c). The role of attention and ageing in the retrieval dynamics of value-directed remembering. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(5), 954–968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Murphy, D. H., Huckins, S. C., Rhodes, M. G., & Castel, A. D. (2022a). The effect of perceptual processing fluency and value on metacognition and remembering. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29(3), 910–921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Murphy, D. H., & Knowlton, B. J. (2022). Framing effects in value-directed remembering. Memory & Cognition, 50(6), 1350–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Murphy, D. H., Schwartz, S. T., & Castel, A. D. (2022b). Serial and strategic memory processes in goal-directed selective remembering. Cognition, 225, 105178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Murphy, D. H., Schwartz, S. T., & Castel, A. D. (2024). Value-directed retrieval: The effects of divided attention at encoding and retrieval on memory selectivity and retrieval dynamics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 50(1), 17–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Murty, V. P., & Adcock, R. A. (2014). Enriched encoding: Reward motivation organizes cortical networks for hippocampal detection of unexpected events. Cerebral Cortex, 24(8), 2162–2178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Murty, V. P., DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2019). Decision-making increases episodic memory via postencoding consolidation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(9), 1308–1317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Ngo, C. T., Newcombe, N. S., & Olson, I. R. (2019). Gain-loss framing enhances mnemonic discrimination in preschoolers. Child Development, 90(5), 1569–1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Nguyen, L. T., Marini, F., Shende, S. A., Llano, D. A., & Mudar, R. A. (2020). Investigating EEG theta and alpha oscillations as measures of value-directed strategic processing in cognitively normal younger and older adults. Behavioural Brain Research, 391, 112702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Nguyen, L. T., Marini, F., Zacharczuk, L., Llano, D. A., & Mudar, R. A. (2019). Theta and alpha band oscillations during value-directed strategic processing. Behavioural Brain Research, 367, 210–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Ofen, N., Kao, Y.-C., Sokol-Hessner, P., Kim, H., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2007). Development of the declarative memory system in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 10(9), 1198–1205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Padmanabhan, A., Geier, C. F., Ordaz, S. J., Teslovich, T., & Luna, B. (2011). Developmental changes in brain function underlying the influence of reward processing on inhibitory control. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(4), 517–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Park, J. S., Kelly, M. O., Hargis, M. B., & Risko, E. F. (2022). The effect of external store reliance on actual and predicted value-directed remembering. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29(4), 1367–1376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Patil, A., Murty, V. P., Dunsmoor, J. E., Phelps, E. A., & Davachi, L. (2017). Reward retroactively enhances memory consolidation for related items. Learning & Memory, 24(1), 65–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Pessiglione, M., Schmidt, L., Draganski, B., Kalisch, R., Lau, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007). How the brain translates money into force: A neuroimaging study of subliminal motivation. Science, 316, 904–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Posthuma, D., Baaré, W. F. C., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Kahn, R. S., Boomsma, D. I., & De Geus, E. J. C. (2003). Genetic correlations between brain volumes and the WAIS-III dimensions of verbal comprehension, working memory, perceptual organization, and processing speed. Twin Research, 6(2), 131–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., Dahle, C., Gerstorf, D., & Acker, J. D. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1676–1689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Reed, A. E., & Carstensen, L. L. (2012). The theory behind the age-related positivity effect. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. M., Klein, D. N., Seeley, J. R., & Gau, J. M. (2013). Key characteristics of major depressive disorder occurring in childhood, adolescence, emerging adulthood, and adulthood. Clinical Psychological Science, 1(1), 41–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Rothkirch, M., Schmack, K., Deserno, L., Darmohray, D., & Sterzer, P. (2014). Attentional modulation of reward processing in the human brain: Attentional Modulation of Reward Processing. Human Brain Mapping, 35(7), 3036–3051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Salthouse, T. A. (2019). Trajectories of normal cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging, 34(1), 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Samanez-Larkin, G. R., & Knutson, B. (2015). Decision making in the ageing brain: Changes in affective and motivational circuits. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(5), 278–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275(5306), 1593–1599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Schwartz, S. T., Siegel, A. L. M., & Castel, A. D. (2020). Strategic encoding and enhanced memory for positive value-location associations. Memory & Cognition, 48(6), 1015–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Schwartz, S. T., Siegel, A. L. M., Eich, T. S., & Castel, A. D. (2023). Value-directed memory selectivity relies on goal-directed knowledge of value structure prior to encoding in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 38(1), 30–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Siegel, A. L. M., & Castel, A. D. (2019). Age-related differences in metacognition for memory capacity and selectivity. Memory, 27(9), 1236–1249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Siegel, A. L. M., Schwartz, S. T., & Castel, A. D. (2021). Selective memory disrupted in intra-modal dual-task encoding conditions. Memory & Cognition, 49(7), 1453–1472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Silaj, K. M., Agadzhanyan, K., & Castel, A. D. (2023). Value-directed learning: Schematic reward structure facilitates learning. Memory & Cognition, 51(7), 1527–1546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Silverman, M. H., Jedd, K., & Luciana, M. (2015). Neural networks involved in adolescent reward processing: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage, 122, 427–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Smith, D., Xiao, L., & Bechara, A. (2012). Decision making in children and adolescents: Impaired Iowa Gambling Task Performance in early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1180–1187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Somerville, L. H., & Casey, B. (2011). Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 236–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Somerville, S. C., Wellman, H. M., & Cultice, J. C. (1983). Young children’s deliberate reminding. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 143(1), 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Stefanidi, A., Ellis, D. M., & Brewer, G. A. (2018). Free recall dynamics in value-directed remembering. Journal of Memory and Language, 100, 1831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80(1), 28–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Telzer, E. H., Fuligni, A. J., Lieberman, M. D., & Galván, A. (2014). Neural sensitivity to eudaimonic and hedonic rewards differentially predict adolescent depressive symptoms over time. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(18), 6600–6605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Thomas, A. K., & Gutchess, A. (Eds.). (2020). The Cambridge handbook of cognitive aging: A life course perspective. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  100. Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 26(1), 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Verhaeghen, P., Marcoen, A., & Goossens, L. (1992). Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: A meta-analytic study. Psychology and Aging, 7(2), 242–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Villaseñor, J. J., Sklenar, A. M., Frankenstein, A. N., Levy, P. U., McCurdy, M. P., & Leshikar, E. D. (2021). Value-directed memory effects on item and context memory. Memory & Cognition, 49(6), 1082–1100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Wang, Q., & Ross, M. (2007). Culture and memory. In S. Kitayama, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 645–667). Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  104. Wang, S., Cheng, S., Jiang, T., Liu, X., & Zhang, M. (2023). The effect of external rewards on declarative memory. Advances in Psychological Science, 31(1), 78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Weinstein, A. M. (2023). Reward, motivation and brain imaging in human healthy participants—A narrative review. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 17, 1123733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Willis, S. L., Tennstedt, S. L., Marsiske, M., Ball, K., Elias, J., Koepke, K. M., Morris, J. N., Rebok, G. W., Unverzagt, F. W., Stoddard, A. M., Wright, E., & ACTIVE Study Group. (2006). Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA, 296(23), 2805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Wong, S., Irish, M., Savage, G., Hodges, J. R., Piguet, O., & Hornberger, M. (2019). Strategic value-directed learning and memory in Alzheimer’s disease and behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia. Journal of Neuropsychology, 13(2), 328–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Yan, Y., Jiang, Y., & Yang, L. (2013). The effect of value sequence on value-directed metamemory: The effect of value sequence on value-directed metamemory. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45(10), 1094–1103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Yonelinas, A. P., & Jacoby, L. L. (1995). The relation between remembering and knowing as bases for recognition: Effects of size congruency. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 622–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Zhang, M., Li, Y., Li, J., & Liu, X. (2023). The influence of extrinsic and intrinsic motivation on memory in adolescents and the underlying neural mechanisms. Advances in Psychological Science, 31(1), 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Zhong, Y., & Jiang, Y. (2024). How does value influences memory: A perspective from specificity. Advances in Psychological Science, 32(1), 75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. A dual-process framework of value-directed memory. The upper part of the figure illustrates the two core memory-processing pathways: automatic processing, which relies on the midbrain–limbic reward system, and strategic processing, which depends on the executive-control network. These two mechanisms exhibit dynamic interactions across the lifespan: during childhood and adolescence, reward-driven automatic processing predominates due to limited cognitive control; in adulthood, strategic processing shows peak flexibility and optimal integration of the dual processes; in older age, automatic processing declines, leading to greater reliance on frontal executive networks for compensation, accompanied by the positivity effect. The lower part of the figure summarizes the two main experimental paradigms: free recall tasks (value-directed remembering, VDR) primarily assess strategic processing, whereas recognition tasks (e.g., Remember/Know paradigm and Rewarded Word-Classification-Recognition, RWCR paradigm) allow the dissociation of recollection and familiarity, providing experimental tools for investigating the dual-process mechanisms.
Figure 1. A dual-process framework of value-directed memory. The upper part of the figure illustrates the two core memory-processing pathways: automatic processing, which relies on the midbrain–limbic reward system, and strategic processing, which depends on the executive-control network. These two mechanisms exhibit dynamic interactions across the lifespan: during childhood and adolescence, reward-driven automatic processing predominates due to limited cognitive control; in adulthood, strategic processing shows peak flexibility and optimal integration of the dual processes; in older age, automatic processing declines, leading to greater reliance on frontal executive networks for compensation, accompanied by the positivity effect. The lower part of the figure summarizes the two main experimental paradigms: free recall tasks (value-directed remembering, VDR) primarily assess strategic processing, whereas recognition tasks (e.g., Remember/Know paradigm and Rewarded Word-Classification-Recognition, RWCR paradigm) allow the dissociation of recollection and familiarity, providing experimental tools for investigating the dual-process mechanisms.
Behavsci 15 01113 g001
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Li, Q.; Tang, W.; Liu, X. Value-Directed Remembering: A Dual-Process Perspective. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081113

AMA Style

Li Q, Tang W, Liu X. Value-Directed Remembering: A Dual-Process Perspective. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(8):1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081113

Chicago/Turabian Style

Li, Qiong, Weihai Tang, and Xiping Liu. 2025. "Value-Directed Remembering: A Dual-Process Perspective" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 8: 1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081113

APA Style

Li, Q., Tang, W., & Liu, X. (2025). Value-Directed Remembering: A Dual-Process Perspective. Behavioral Sciences, 15(8), 1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081113

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop