Examining the Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Decision-Making: A Scoping Review
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Methods
2.1. Protocol and Registration
2.2. Eligibility Criteria
2.3. Information Sources and Search
2.4. Selection of Sources of Evidence
2.5. Data Items and Charting Process
2.6. Summary Measures and Synthesis of Results
3. Results
3.1. Summary of Included Studies
3.2. Types of Tasks Used to Assess Decision-Making Ability
3.3. The Empirical Quantification of Effective Fatigue Induction via Sleep Deprivation
3.4. The Effect of the Duration of Sleep Deprivation
3.5. Types of Decision-Making Explored
4. Discussion
4.1. Implications for Policy, Planning and Future Research Directions
4.2. Limitations
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
References
- Anderson, C., & Platten, C. R. (2011). Sleep deprivation lowers inhibition and enhances impulsivity to negative stimuli. Behavioural Brain Research, 217(2), 463–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aran, A., Wasserteil, N., Gross, I., Mendlovic, J., & Pollak, Y. (2017). Medical decisions of pediatric residents turn riskier after a 24-hour call with no sleep. Medical Decision Making, 37(1), 127–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barnes, C. M., & Watson, N. F. (2019). Why healthy sleep is good for business. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 47, 112–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bendrick, G. A., Beckett, S. A., & Klerman, E. B. (2016). Human fatigue and the crash of the airship. Italia Polar Research, 35, 27105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Besedovsky, L., Lange, T., & Born, J. (2012). Sleep and immune function. Pflügers Archiv-European Journal of Physiology, 463(1), 121–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brunet, J.-F., McNeil, J., Doucet, E., & Forest, G. (2020). The association between REM sleep and decision-making: Supporting evidences. Physiology & Behavior, 225, 113109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chee, M. W. L., & Chuah, L. Y. M. (2008). Functional neuroimaging insights into how sleep and sleep deprivation affect memory and cognition. Current Opinion in Neurology, 21(4), 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Demos, K. E., Hart, C. N., Sweet, L. H., Mailloux, K. A., Trautvetter, J., Williams, S. E., Wing, R., & McCaffery, J. (2016). Partial sleep deprivation impacts impulsive action but not impulsive decision-making. Physiology & Behavior, 164((Pt A)), 214–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dickinson, D. L., & Masclet, D. (2023). Unethical decision making and sleep restriction: Experimental evidence. Games and Economic Behavior, 141, 484–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferrara, M., Bottasso, A., Tempesta, D., Carrieri, M., De Gennaro, L., & Ponti, G. (2015). Gender differences in sleep deprivation effects on risk and inequality aversion: Evidence from an economic experiment. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0120029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ford, E. S., Cunningham, T. J., & Croft, J. B. (2015). Trends in self-reported sleep duration among US adults from 1985 to 2012. Sleep, 38(5), 829–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gold, J. I., & Shadlen, M. N. (2007). The neural basis of decision making. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 535–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hafner, M., Stepanek, M., Taylor, J., Troxel, W. M., & van Stolk, C. (2017). Why sleep matters-the economic costs of insufficient sleep: A cross-country comparative analysis. Rand Health Q, 6(4), 11. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Hamilton, N. A., Nelson, C. A., Stevens, N., & Kitzman, H. (2007). Sleep and psychological well-being. Social Indicators Research, 82(1), 147–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harrison, Y., & Horne, J. A. (2000). The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: A review. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6(3), 236–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hirshkowitz, M., Whiton, K., Albert, S. M., Alessi, C., Bruni, O., DonCarlos, L., Hazen, N., Herman, J., Katz, E. S., Kheirandish-Gozal, L., Neubauer, D. N., O’dOnnell, A. E., Ohayon, M., Peever, J., Rawding, R., Sachdeva, R. C., Setters, B., Vitiello, M. V., Ware, J. C., … Hillard, P. J. A. (2015). National sleep foundation’s sleep time duration recommendations: Methodology and results summary. Sleep Health, 1(1), 40–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hobson, J. A., & Pace-Schott, E. F. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: Neuronal systems, consciousness and learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(9), 679–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jackson, M. L., Gunzelmann, G., Whitney, P., Hinson, J. M., Belenky, G., Rabat, A., & Van Dongen, H. P. (2013). Deconstructing and reconstructing cognitive performance in sleep deprivation. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 17(3), 215–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Killgore, W. D. (2010). Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Progress in Brain Research, 185, 105–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krause, A. J., Simon, E. B., Mander, B. A., Greer, S. M., Saletin, J. M., Goldstein-Piekarski, A. N., & Walker, M. P. (2017). The sleep-deprived human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(7), 404–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lau, E. Y. Y., Wong, M. L., Rusak, B., Lam, Y. C., Wing, Y. K., Tseng, C. H., & Lee, T. M. C. (2019). The coupling of short sleep duration and high sleep need predicts riskier decision making. Psychology and Health, 34(10), 1196–1213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lei, Y., Wang, L., Chen, P., Li, Y., Han, W., Ge, M., Yang, L., Chen, S., Hu, W., Wu, X., & Yang, Z. (2017). Neural correlates of increased risk-taking propensity in sleep-deprived people along with a changing risk level. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 11(6), 1910–1921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lim, J., & Dinges, D. F. (2010). A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 375–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lim, J. Y. L., Boardman, J., Dyche, J., Anderson, C., Dickinson, D. L., & Drummond, S. P. A. (2022). Sex moderates the effects of total sleep deprivation and sleep restriction on risk preference. Sleep, 45(9), zsac120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lim, J. Y. L., Boardman, J. M., Anderson, C., Dickinson, D. L., Bennett, D., & Drummond, S. P. A. (2024). Sleep restriction alters the integration of multiple information sources in probabilistic decision-making. Journal of Sleep Research, 33(5), e14161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mao, T., Fang, Z., Chai, Y., Deng, Y., Rao, J., Quan, P., Goel, N., Basner, M., Guo, B., Dinges, D. F., Liu, J., Detre, J. A., & Rao, H. (2024). Sleep deprivation attenuates neural responses to outcomes from risky decision-making. Psychophysiology, 61(4), e14465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maquet, P. (2001). The Role of Sleep in Learning and Memory. Science, 294(5544), 1048–1052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Massar, S. A. A., & Chee, M. W. L. (2015). Preserved calibration of persistence based on delay-timing distribution during sleep deprivation. Journal of Sleep Research, 24(6), 673–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McElroy, T., & Dickinson, D. L. (2019). Thinking about complex decisions: How sleep and time-of-day influence complex choices. Consciousness and Cognition, 76, 102824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Minkel, J. D., Banks, S., Htaik, O., Moreta, M. C., Jones, C. W., McGlinchey, E. L., Simpson, N. S., & Dinges, D. F. (2012). Sleep deprivation and stressors: Evidence for elevated negative affect in response to mild stressors when sleep deprived. Emotion, 12(5), 1015–1020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mullette-Gillman, O. D. A., Kurnianingsih, Y. A., & Liu, J. C. J. (2015). Sleep deprivation alters choice strategy without altering uncertainty or loss aversion preferences. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palmer, C. A., & Alfano, C. A. (2017). Sleep and emotion regulation: An organizing, integrative review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 31, 6–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peng, J., Lu, H., Zhang, J., Shao, Y., Wang, L., & Lv, J. (2022). Need for cognition moderates the impairment of decision making caused by nightshift work in nurses. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quan, S. F., Landrigan, C. P., Barger, L. K., Buie, J. D., Dominguez, C., Iyer, J. M., Majekodunmi, A., Papautsky, E. L., Robbins, R., Shen, B. H., Stephens, J. T., Weaver, M. D., & Czeisler, C. A. (2023). Impact of sleep deficiency on surgical performance: A prospective assessment. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 19(4), 673–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosekind, M. R. (2005). Underestimating the societal costs of impaired alertness: Safety, health and productivity risks. Sleep Medicine, 6(Suppl. S1), S21–S25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ruiz-Herrera, N., Friedman, M., St Hilaire, M. A., Arrona-Palacios, A., Czeisler, C. A., & Duffy, J. F. (2024). Time of day and sleep deprivation effects on risky decision making. Clocks Sleep, 6(2), 281–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rusnac, N., Spitzenstetter, F., & Tassi, P. (2019). Chronic sleep loss and risk-taking behavior: Does the origin of sleep loss matter? Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 17(6), 729–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saberi Moghadam, S., Samsami Khodadad, F., & Khazaeinezhad, V. (2019). An algorithmic model of decision making in the human brain. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience Journal, 10(5), 443–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saddoud, A., Rekik, G., Belkhir, Y., Kammoun, N., Hidouri, S., Chlif, M., & Jarraya, M. (2023). One night of total sleep deprivation impairs decision-making skills, anaerobic performances, mood states, and perceptual responses in elite Kung-Fu athletes. Biological Rhythm Research, 54(1), 102–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salfi, F., Lauriola, M., Tempesta, D., Calanna, P., Socci, V., De Gennaro, L., & Ferrara, M. (2020). Effects of total and partial sleep deprivation on reflection impulsivity and risk-taking in deliberative decision-making. Nature and Science of Sleep, 12, 309–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samkoff, J. S., & Jacques, C. H. (1991). A review of studies concerning effects of sleep deprivation and fatigue on residents’ performance. Academic Medicine, 66(11), 687–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Santisteban, J. A., Brown, T. G., Ouimet, M. C., & Gruber, R. (2019). Cumulative mild partial sleep deprivation negatively impacts working memory capacity but not sustained attention, response inhibition, or decision making: A randomized controlled trial. Sleep Health, 5(1), 101–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schaedler, T., Santos, J. S., Vincenzi, R. A., Pereira, S. I. R., & Louzada, F. M. (2018). Executive functioning is preserved in healthy young adults under acute sleep restriction. Sleep Science, 11(3), 152–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tricco, A. C., Lillie, E., Zarin, W., O’BRien, K. K., Colquhoun, H., Levac, D., Moher, D., Peters, M. D., Horsley, T., Weeks, L., Hempel, S., Akl, E. A., Chang, C., McGowan, J., Stewart, L., Hartling, L., Aldcroft, A., Wilson, M. G., Garritty, C., … Straus, S. E. (2018). PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (Prisma-Scr): Checklist and explanation. Annals of Internal Medicine, 169(7), 467–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Cauter, E., & Plat, L. (1996). Physiology of growth hormone secretion during sleep. The Journal of Pediatrics, 128((5 Pt 2)), S32–S37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vincent, G. E., Onay, Z., Scanlan, A. T., Elsworthy, N., Pitchford, N. W., & Lastella, M. (2021). The impact of self-reported sleep quantity on perceived decision-making in sports officials during a competitive season. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 92(1), 156–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walker, M. P., & Stickgold, R. (2004). Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron, 44(1), 121–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Walker, M. P., & van der Helm, E. (2009). Overnight therapy? The role of sleep in emotional brain processing. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 731–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Y., Dai, C., Shao, Y., Wang, C., & Zhou, Q. (2022). Changes in ventromedial prefrontal cortex functional connectivity are correlated with increased risk-taking after total sleep deprivation. Behavioural Brain Research, 418, 113674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, N. F., Badr, M. S., Belenky, G., Bliwise, D. L., Buxton, O. M., Buysse, D., Dinges, D. F., Gangwisch, J., Grandner, M. A., Kushida, C., Malhotra, R. K., Martin, J. L., Patel, S. R., Quan, S., & Tasali, E. (2015). Recommended amount of sleep for a healthy adult: A joint consensus statement of the American academy of sleep medicine and sleep research society. Sleep, 38(6), 843–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wei, R., Duan, X., & Guo, L. (2022). Effects of sleep deprivation on coronary heart disease. Korean Journal of Physiology & Pharmacology, 26(5), 297–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wei, X., Ma, J., Liu, S., Li, S., Shi, S., Guo, X., & Liu, Z. (2024). The effects of sleep deprivation on risky decision making. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 32(1), 80–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitney, P., Hinson, J. M., Jackson, M. L., & Van Dongen, H. P. A. (2015). Feedback Blunting: Total Sleep Deprivation Impairs Decision Making that Requires Updating Based on Feedback. Sleep, 38(5), 745–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Xu, W., Wang, L., Yang, L., Zhu, Y., & Chen, P. (2024). Sleep deprivation alters utilization of negative feedback in risky decision-making. Front Psychiatry, 15, 1307408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, S., Ring, J., Methot, M., & Zevin, B. (2021). Medication errors in surgery: A classification taxonomy and a pilot study in postcall residents. Journal of Surgical Research, 264, 402–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Author/Year | Country | Study Design | Methods | Sample Size | Mean Age of Sample | Impact of Sleep Deprivation on Decision-Making | Decision-Makings Test | Measurement of Sleepiness |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Bendrick et al., 2016) | America | Case study | The conditions surrounding the crash of the airship Italia were investigated with the aim of uncovering the possible fatigue-related causes of the crash. | 1 | N/A | The three erroneous decisions that the captain made that caused the crash were likely a result of his fatigue caused by sleep deprivation. | Ability to Fly the Plane | Excessive Hours of Wakefulness |
(Brunet et al., 2020) | Canada | Within-subjects experimental design | Participants completed both the control, well-rested condition, and the partial sleep deprivation condition (one night of 50% sleep deprivation). After each condition, they completed the IGT. | 18 | 23 | The duration of REM sleep and deprivation of REM sleep were linked to making riskier decisions on the IGT. This revealed difficulty in decision-making under conditions of uncertainty. | Iowa Gambling Task | Polysomnography |
(Demos et al., 2016) | America | Within-subjects experimental design | Participants had four nights of restful sleep (9 h) and four nights of partial SD (6 h). After each sleep condition, participants completed the delayed discounting task, balloon analog task, and go/no-go task. | 34 | 37.0 | Multiple nights of partial sleep deprivation were associated with increased impulsive action but not impulsive decision-making. | Delay Discounting Task Balloon Analogue Risk Task Go/No-Go | None |
(Dickinson & Masclet, 2023) | America | Experiment | Participants were assigned to either one week of sleep restriction (5–6 h/night) of well-rested sleep (8–9 h/night in which they slept at home. The coin flip task was administered after the fifth night while the two other tasks were given after the seventh night. | 237 | Not stated | Participants in the sleep restriction condition cheated significantly more than well rested participants in two honesty tasks (coin flip task and matrix task), indicating a decrease in ethical decision-making | Money Burning Task Coin Flip Task Matrix Task | Karolinska Sleepiness Scale |
(Ferrara et al., 2015) | Italy | Experiment | Participants performed two sessions. In one session, they had a restful night of sleep before performing a decision-making task. In the second session, participants underwent a night of total sleep deprivation before doing the task. | 32 | 24 | Following sleep deprivation, males tended to make riskier decisions than when they were well-rested, whereas females became more cautious. | Random Lottery Pair Linear Dictator Game | Karolinska Sleepiness Scale |
(Lau et al., 2019) | China | Longitudinal design | Participants underwent a 7-day protocol with assessments of sleep and risk-related decision-making at baseline (T1) and again 12 months later (T2). Participants completed a Consensus Diary to measure sleep debt, sleep time, sleep need, and other sleep-related details. | 166 | Not stated | Individuals with high sleep needs but short sleep duration had an increased propensity for riskier decisions. | Risky Gains Task | A Chinese version of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale |
(Lei et al., 2017) | China | Within-subjects, repeat-measure, experimental design | Participants underwent two counterbalanced experimental sessions, rested wakefulness (normal sleep routine) and total sleep deprivation (36–40 h), separated by at least 2 weeks. Decision tasks were conducted during both sessions, along with regular assessments of sleepiness, mood, and alertness. MRI scans were taken after each condition. | 32 | 23.1 | The results showed that sleep deprivation increased activation related to risk modulation in the left inferior frontal gyrus, and this increased activation was positively correlated with higher risk-taking propensity after sleep deprivation. In sleep-deprived participants, activation in the ventral striatum and thalamus increased during cash-out events, while activation in the middle temporal gyrus decreased following explosions (i.e., loss of money), adding evidence for greater risk propensity following SD. | Balloon Analogue Risk Task. | Psychomotor Vigilance Task Likert-type Rating of Sleepiness |
(J. Y. L. Lim et al., 2022) | Australia | Experiment | Participants were either assigned to the well-rested (9 h time in bed for 6 nights) and total sleep deprivation (1 night) protocol or the WR and sleep restricted (4 h time in bed for 4 nights) protocol. After a night of WR, SD, or SR, participants completed the lottery choice task. The risk of choices was analyzed afterwards. | 47 | 24.57 | In the gains-only model, Males did not change risk preference during either TSD or SR, while females became significantly more risk-averse during both. In the LOSSES only model, female participants did not change their risk preference after TSD, while male participants became significantly more risk-seeking after TSD than WR. | Lottery Choice Task | Karolinska Sleepiness Scale |
(J. Y. L. Lim et al., 2024) | Australia | Experiment | Participants all completed a well-rested condition and one of two randomly assigned experimental conditions: SR, which involved four hours’ time in bed for four nights or one night of TSD. After each condition, they completed the Bayes decision task. | 46 | 24.76 | Participants in the first experiment demonstrated a decreased reliance on both information sources when making probabilistic decisions following sleep restriction. Sleep restriction reduced the integration of multiple information sources during decision making. No significant change was observed after TSD | Bayes decisions task | Karolinska Sleepiness Scale |
(Mao et al., 2024) | America | Within-subjects experimental design | Participants completed an fMRI scan after one night of TSD and another scan after RW (8 h’ time in bed) in counterbalanced order. A decision-making task was performed during each scan. | 56 | 32.59 | Sleep deprivation did not alter participants’ risk-taking behavior but significantly reduced brain activity in regions associated with processing wins and losses, including the anterior cingulate cortex, insula, and putamen. Risk induced activation of the insula was negatively correlated with risk propensity during RW but not TSD. | Balloon Analogue Risk Task (modified) | None |
(Massar & Chee, 2015) | Singapore | Experiment | Participants completed a willingness-to-wait task either when they were well-rested or following a night of complete sleep deprivation. | 29 | 22.28 | Sleep deprivation and decreased vigilance did not affect participants’ ability to make decisions in the adjustment of waiting times. | Willingness-to-Wait Task | Psychomotor Vigilance Test |
(McElroy & Dickinson, 2019) | America | Mixed study design | There were three conditions that the participants were assigned to: a control, a 1-week partial sleep deprivation (5–6 h), or a well-rested (9 h) condition. There was also either a circadian match or a mismatch condition. Decision-making tasks were completed afterwards. | 140 | 21.9 | Participants in the SD circadian mismatched condition performed more poorly on only one decision-making task compared to the well-rested condition. | Complex Decision Tasks | None |
(Mullette-Gillman et al., 2015) | Germany | Experiment | All participants completed both a TSD session and a restful wakefulness session. In each session, all participants completed a gains choice task, a losses choice task, and a loss aversion task. Choice strategy, uncertainty preference, and decision-making ability were evaluated. | 29 | 21.66 | One night of SD alters the information the participants rely upon to make their choices. Sleep deprivation alters a person’s decision-making by altering the informational strategies that participants use. After sleep deprivation, participants relied less on complex, maximizing information and more on simpler, satisfying information. Sleep deprivation shifted the decision-making strategy rather than changing the underlying preferences. | Gains Choice Task Losses Choice Task | Psychomotor Vigilance Test and Subjective Sleepiness Assessment |
(Peng et al., 2022) | China | Experiment | Participants performed in both daytime and nighttime shifts conditions. In the first condition, the night shift group completed the fatigue assessment test, A-DMC, and IGT. The second condition was that the participants completed these tests after a daytime shift. | 107 | 25.33 | The IGT scores and decision-making abilities of nurses significantly deteriorated after working a night shift (mentally fatigued), with a clear connection between the decline in decision-making competence and their performance on the IGT. | Iowa Gambling Task Adult Decision-making Competence Scale | Stanford Sleepiness Scale |
(Quan et al., 2023) | America | Repeated measures Observational design | Surgeons were studied prospectively using direct observation and self-reported data during post-call (>2 h of nighttime clinical duties) and non-post-call. | 60 | 47.6 | Non-technical skills for surgeons’ ratings demonstrated poorer performance for decision-making while on post-call. Fewer hours of sleep were also related to lower ratings for decision-making. | Performing Surgical Duties Self-reported Decision-making Ability on a 4-point Scale | None |
(Ruiz-Herrera et al., 2024) | America | Within-subjects, repeated measures experimental design | Participants were allowed 9 h of sleep for the two nights before undergoing 39 h of total sleep deprivation. Vigilance and decision-making tasks were administered every 2 hours on the second day (baseline) for a total of 7 times, and on the third and fourth days (constant routine) for a total of 19 times. | 13 | 26.46 | In the balloon analogue risk task, participants showed a greater risk-taking propensity around midday before sleep deprivation, and a reduced risk-taking tendency after prolonged wakefulness (29.5 h awake). | Balloon Analogue Risk Task | Psychomotor Vigilance Task |
(Rusnac et al., 2019) | France | Quasi-experimental design | Participants in the voluntary sleep loss, normal sleep, and insomnia groups were shown videos of dangerous traffic scenarios from a 1st person POV. The participants assess and react while the researchers measure the risk propensity. | 536 | 22.25 | Participants with voluntary sleep deprivation (around 2 h) made more risky decisions in virtual driving situations compared to normal sleepers and participants with insomnia. | The Vienna Risk-Taking Test–Traffic | Insomnia Severity Index |
(Saddoud et al., 2023) | Tunisia | Experiment | Participants were assigned to either the sleep deprivation condition (One night) or the well-rested condition. They then completed a series of tasks, including a decision-making task. | 24 | 20.2 | The total sleep deprivation (TSD) condition led to reduced decision accuracy and increased decision time compared to the normal sleep (NS) condition. | Video-based Decision-making Task | None |
(Salfi et al., 2020) | Italy | Experiment | Exp 1: Participants were tested for decision-making ability after one night of regular sleep and one night of complete sleep deprivation. Exp 2: Participants were tested after five nights of restful sleep and five nights of partial sleep deprivation. | 32 42 | 22.13 | A single night of complete sleep deprivation did not significantly impact data gathering to make decisions or risky decision making. Multiple nights of partial sleep deprivation increased impulsive and risky decision-making. | Mosaic Task Columbia Card Task | Karolinska Sleepiness Scale |
(Santisteban et al., 2019) | Canada | Randomized controlled between-subjects experimental design | A 6-night baseline sleep assessment was completed at home by each participant using actigraphy. Participants were then randomly assigned to either a sleep restriction group (1 less hour of sleep for 6 nights) or a placebo group exposed to non-therapeutic light, while maintaining normal sleep patterns. Cognitive testing and sleepiness questionnaires were completed throughout the week. | 93 | Not stated | Performance on the decision-making task was not significantly affected by partial sleep deprivation, reflected by similar performances between the two conditions. | Cambridge Gambling Task | Visual Analogue Scale-Sleepiness |
(Schaedler et al., 2018) | Brazil | Experiment | Participants were assigned to the normal sleep condition, morning sleep restriction condition (3 h less sleep), or evening sleep restriction condition (3 h less sleep). Afterward, they completed various tasks to assess their cognitive ability. | 48 | Not stated | Compared to the control group, individuals with either morning or evening sleep restriction did not exhibit any significant impairments in decision-making. | Iowa Gambling Task Go-NoGo Test Stroop Test | Epworth Sleepiness Scale Karolinska Sleepiness Scale |
(Vincent et al., 2021) | Australia | Cross-sectional survey design | Sports officials (n = 371) completed an online questionnaire assessing self-reported sleep quantity and quality on regular nights, before and after competitions, and their perceived decision-making. | 371 | 37.2 | Self-reported reduced sleep had a negative impact on perceived decision-making abilities. | Self-rating on a Likert scale. | Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index |
(Wang et al., 2022) | China | Within-subjects experimental design | Participants completed two fMRI sessions: one following a night of normal sleep and another after 36 h of total sleep deprivation. Sleep and wakefulness were closely monitored. After each scan, participants performed a vigilance test, a risk-taking decision-making task, and an alertness assessment. | 21 | 23.48 | A single night of sleep deprivation led to a reduction in functional connectivity between the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and thalamus bilaterally, alongside an increase in connectivity between the vmPFC, both the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and the parietal lobe. Greater risk-taking was linked to stronger vmPFC–dlPFC connectivity and weaker vmPFC–thalamus connectivity. These findings demonstrate that a lack of sleep alters brain connectivity in ways that predict the increased risk-taking behavior found after SD. | Balloon Analogue Risk Task | Psychomotor Vigilance Task |
(Whitney et al., 2015) | America | Randomized controlled design | Participants were either assigned to the sleep-deprived condition (62-h sleep deprivation) or the control condition (no SD). The participants completed the decision task, and their abilities were assessed. | 26 | Not stated | Sleep deprivation decreased the decision-making ability of the participants during the task. | Reversal Learning Decision Task | Psychomotor Vigilance Test |
(Xu et al., 2024) | China | Repeated measures experimental design | The study involved healthy male college students in Beijing who underwent 36 h of total sleep deprivation, during which they were repeatedly assessed at seven time points. | 37 | 23.18 | Sleep deprivation led to an increased tendency for risky decision-making, with the effects often appearing 15–20 h after SD. Their ability to use negative feedback was impaired after 8 h of SD. | Game of Dice Task | The Stanford Sleepiness Scale Fatigue Questionnaire Psychomotor Vigilance Test |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Agyapong-Opoku, F.; Agyapong-Opoku, N.; Agyapong, B. Examining the Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Decision-Making: A Scoping Review. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 823. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060823
Agyapong-Opoku F, Agyapong-Opoku N, Agyapong B. Examining the Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Decision-Making: A Scoping Review. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(6):823. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060823
Chicago/Turabian StyleAgyapong-Opoku, Felix, Nadine Agyapong-Opoku, and Belinda Agyapong. 2025. "Examining the Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Decision-Making: A Scoping Review" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 6: 823. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060823
APA StyleAgyapong-Opoku, F., Agyapong-Opoku, N., & Agyapong, B. (2025). Examining the Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Decision-Making: A Scoping Review. Behavioral Sciences, 15(6), 823. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060823