Intergenerational Synchrony and Its Effect on Bonding and Group Closeness among Young and Older Adults
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Sample
2.2. Procedure
2.3. Instruments
2.3.1. Social Bonding
2.3.2. Ingroup–Outgroup Overlap
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. World Population Prospects 2022: Summary of Results; UN DESA/POP/2022/TR/NO. 3; United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division: New York, NY, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. Global Report on Ageism: Executive Summary. 2021. Available online: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240020504 (accessed on 2 February 2023).
- Myers-JDC-Brookdale-Institute. The 65+ Population in Israel: Statistical Abstract 2023; Shnoor, Y., Cohen, Y., Eds.; 2023; Available online: https://brookdale.jdc.org.il/publication/statistical-abstract-elderly-israel-2023-2/ (accessed on 1 June 2024). (In Hebrew)
- Ayalon, L.; Tesch-Römer, C. Contemporary Perspectives on Ageism; Springer Nature: Cham, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hagestad, G.O.; Uhlenberg, P. The social separation of old and young: A root of ageism. J. Soc. Issues 2005, 61, 343–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Apriceno, M.; Levy, S.R. Systematic review and meta-analyses of effective programs for reducing ageism toward older adults. J. Appl. Gerontol. 2023, 42, 1356–1375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Burnes, D.; Sheppard, C.; Henderson, C.R.; Wassel, M.; Cope, R.; Barber, C.; Pillemer, K. Interventions to reduce ageism against older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Am. J. Public Health 2019, 109, e1–e9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Christian, J.; Turner, R.; Holt, N.; Larkin, M.; Cotler, J.H. Does intergenerational contact reduce ageism: When and how contact interventions actually work? J. Arts Humanit. 2014, 3, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levy, S.R. Toward reducing ageism: PEACE (positive education about aging and contact experiences) model. Gerontologist 2018, 58, 226–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tarr, B.; Launay, J.; Dunbar, R.I. Silent disco: Dancing in synchrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2016, 37, 343–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Phillips-Silver, J.; Aktipis, C.A.; Bryant, G.A. The ecology of entrainment: Foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music Percept. 2010, 28, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Woolhouse, M.H.; Tidhar, D.; Cross, I. Effects on inter-personal memory of dancing in time with others. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McGarry, L.M.; Russo, F.A. Mirroring in dance/movement therapy: Potential mechanisms behind empathy enhancement. Arts Psychother. 2011, 38, 178–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirschner, S.; Tomasello, M. Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2010, 31, 354–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keller, P.E.; Novembre, G.; Hove, M.J. Rhythm in joint action: Psychological and neurophysiological mechanisms for real-time interpersonal coordination. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 2014, 369, 20130394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hurley, S. The shared circuits model (SCM): How control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading. Behav. Brain Sci. 2008, 31, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Galinsky, A.D.; Ku, G.; Wang, C.S. Perspective-taking and self-other overlap: Fostering social bonds and facilitating social coordination. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 2005, 8, 109–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rennung, M.; Göritz, A.S. Prosocial consequences of interpersonal synchrony: A meta-analysis. Z. Psychol. 2016, 224, 168–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chartrand, T.L.; Bargh, J.A. The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 76, 893–910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chartrand, T.L.; Lakin, J.L. The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 285–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yabar, Y.; Johnston, L.; Miles, L.; Peace, V. Implicit behavioral mimicry: Investigating the impact of group membership. J. Nonverbal Behav. 2006, 30, 97–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Repp, B.H.; Su, Y. Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2013, 20, 403–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNeill, W.H. Keeping Together in Time: Dance and Drill in Human History; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995; ISBN 978-1597406741. [Google Scholar]
- Macrae, C.N.; Duffy, O.K.; Miles, L.K.; Lawrence, J. A case of hand waving: Action synchrony and person perception. Cognition 2008, 109, 152–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rabinowitch, T.C. Musical engagement as a duet of tight synchrony and Loose interpretability. Phys. Life Rev. 2023, 44, 122–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bamford, J.S.; Burger, B.; Toiviainen, P. Turning Heads on the Dance Floor: Synchrony and Social Interaction Using a Silent Disco Paradigm. Music Sci. 2023, 6, 20592043231155416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wiltermuth, S.S.; Heath, C. Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 20, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hove, M.J.; Risen, J.L. It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cogn. 2009, 27, 949–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valdesolo, P.; DeSteno, D. Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion 2011, 11, 262–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koehne, S.; Behrends, A.; Fairhurst, M.T.; Dziobek, I. Fostering social cognition through an imitation-and synchronization-based dance/movement intervention in adults with autism spectrum disorder: A controlled proof-of-concept study. Psychother. Psychosom. 2016, 85, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cross, L.; Turgeon, M.; Atherton, G. Moving with the in-crowd: Cooperation and interpersonal entrainment in in- vs. out- groups. Curr. Psychol. 2019, 40, 3393–3400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Good, A.; Choma, B.; Russo, F.A. Movement synchrony influences intergroup relations in a minimal groups paradigm. Basic Appl. Soc. Psych. 2017, 39, 231–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miles, L.K.; Lumsden, J.; Richardson, M.J.; Neil Macrae, C. Do birds of a feather move together? Group membership and behavioral synchrony. Exp. Brain Res. 2011, 211, 495–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Atherton, G.; Sebanz, N.; Cross, L. Imagine all the synchrony: The effects of actual and imagined synchronous walking on attitudes towards marginalised groups. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0216585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shuper Engelhard, E. Free-Form Dance as an Alternative Interaction for Adult Grandchildren and Their Grandparents. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cross, L.; Turgeon, M.; Atherton, G. How moving together binds us together: The social consequences of interpersonal entrainment and group processes. Open Psychol. 2019, 1, 273–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mogan, R.; Fischer, R.; Bulbulia, J.A. To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2017, 72, 13–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCrae, R.R.; Costa, P.T., Jr. A Five-Factor theory of personality. In Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research; Pervin, L.A., John, O.P., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 139–153. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, B.W.; Walton, K.E.; Viechtbauer, W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol. Bull. 2006, 132, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Specht, J.; Egloff, B.; Schmukle, S.C. Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 101, 862–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wortman, J.; Lucas, R.E.; Donnellan, M.B. Stability and change in the Big Five personality domains: Evidence from a longitudinal study of Australians. Psychol. Aging 2012, 27, 867–874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haslam, C.; Haslam, S.A.; Jetten, J. Social determinants of cognitive change: Identity processes as the source of both enhancement and decline. In Psychology of Change: Life Contexts, Experiences, and Identities; Reynolds, K.J., Branscombe, N.R., Eds.; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 133–150. [Google Scholar]
- Bhattacharya, K.; Ghosh, A.; Monsivais, D.; Dunbar, R.I.; Kaski, K. Sex differences in social focus across the life cycle in humans. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2016, 3, 160097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sutcliffe, A.; Dunbar, R.; Binder, J.; Arrow, H. Relationships and the social brain: Integrating psychological and evolutionary perspectives. Br. J. Psychol. 2012, 103, 149–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carstensen, L.L. The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science 2006, 312, 1913–1915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Charles, S.T.; Carstensen, L.L. Social and emotional aging. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2010, 61, 383–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- English, T.; Carstensen, L.L. Selective narrowing of social networks across adulthood is associated with improved emotional experience in daily life. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2014, 38, 195–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robinson, O.C. Values and adult age: Findings from two cohorts of the European social survey. Eur. J. Ageing 2012, 10, 11–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lachman, M.E.; Lewkowicz, C.; Marcus, A.; Peng, Y. Images of midlife development among young, middle-aged, and older adults. J. Adult Dev. 1994, 1, 201–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dave, P. Indigenous Conceptions of Aging. In International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences; Smelser, N.J., Baltes, P.B., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2001; Volume 11, pp. 7287–7289. [Google Scholar]
- Janata, P.; Tomic, S.T.; Haberman, J.M. Sensorimotor coupling in music and the psychology of the groove. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2012, 141, 54–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Etani, T.; Miura, A.; Kawase, S.; Fujii, S.; Keller, P.E.; Vuust, P.; Kudo, K. A review of psychological and Neuroscientific research on musical groove (2006−2022). Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2023, 158, 105522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Etani, T.; Marui, A.; Kawase, S.; Keller, P.E. Optimal tempo for groove: Its relation to directions of body movement and Japanese nori. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zelechowska, A.; Gonzalez Sanchez, V.E.; Laeng, B.; Vuoskoski, J.K.; Jensenius, A.R. Who Moves to Music? Empathic Concern Predicts Spontaneous Movement Responses to Rhythm and Music. Music Sci. 2020, 3, 205920432097421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schubert, T.W.; Otten, S. Overlap of self, ingroup, and outgroup: Pictorial measures of self-categorization. Self Identity 2002, 1, 353–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aron, A.; Aron, E.N.; Smollan, D. Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 63, 596–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Helgeson, V.S.; Van Vleet, M. Short report: Inclusion of other in the self scale: An adaptation and exploration in a diverse community sample. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 2019, 36, 4048–4056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Z.; Zhang, X. We were all once young: Reducing hostile ageism from younger adults’ perspective. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 793373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hardin, J.W.; Hilbe, J.M. Generalized Estimating Equations, 2nd ed.; Chapman and Hall/CRC: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Cirelli, L.K.; Einarson, K.M.; Trainor, L.J. Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev. Sci. 2014, 17, 1003–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tunçgenç, B.; Cohen, E. Movement synchrony forges social bonds across group divides. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tarr, B.; Launay, J.; Cohen, E.; Dunbar, R. Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biol. Lett. 2015, 11, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tarr, B.; Slater, M.; Cohen, E. Synchrony and social connection in immersive Virtual Reality. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Keisari, S.; Feniger-Schaal, R.; Palgi, Y.; Golland, Y.; Gesser-Edelsburg, A.; Ben-David, B. Synchrony in old age: Playing the mirror game improves cognitive performance. Clin. Gerontol. 2022, 45, 312–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luong, G.; Raeurs, A.; Fingerman, K.L. The multifaceted nature of late-life socialization: Older adults as agents and targets of socialization. In Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research, 2nd ed.; Grusec, J., Hasting, P.H., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 109–134. ISBN 978-1462525829. [Google Scholar]
- Ryder, N.B. The Cohort as a Concept in the Study of Social Change. In Cohort Analysis in Social Research; Mason, W.M., Fienberg, S.E., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Francioli, S.P.; Shakeri, A.; North, M.S. Americans harbor much less favorable explicit sentiments toward young adults than toward older adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2024, 121, e2311009121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tajfel, H. Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1982, 33, 1–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaspar, R.; Gabrian, M.; Brothers, A.; Wahl, H.; Diehl, M. Measuring awareness of age-related change: Development of a 10-Item short form for use in large-scale surveys. Gerontologist 2018, 59, e130–e140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diehl, M.; Wahl, H.; Barrett, A.E.; Brothers, A.F.; Miche, M.; Montepare, J.M.; Westerhof, G.J.; Wurm, S. Awareness of aging: Theoretical considerations on an emerging concept. Dev. Rev. 2014, 34, 93–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tropp, L.R.; Wright, S.C. Ingroup identification as the inclusion of ingroup in the self. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 27, 585–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cross, L.; Michael, J.; Wilsdon, L.; Henson, A.; Atherton, G. Still want to help? Interpersonal coordination’s effects on helping behaviour after a 24 hour delay. Acta Psychol. 2020, 206, 103062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gordon, I.; Gilboa, A.; Cohen, S.; Milstein, N.; Haimovich, N.; Pinhasi, S.; Siegman, S. Physiological and behavioral synchrony predict group cohesion and performance. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 8484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barrett, A.E. Socioeconomic status and age identity: The role of dimensions of health in the subjective construction of age. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2003, 58, S101–S109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Håman, L.; Källstrand, J.; Carlsson, I.; Ivarsson, A.; Kristén, L.; Lindgren, E. An empowerment-based physical activity intervention for older people with advanced age-related macular degeneration: An exploratory qualitative case study design. J. Clin. Med. 2024, 13, 3918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
B | SE(b) | 95%CI | Wald’s Chi-Square | Sig. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Synchrony (sync vs. async) | 0.51 | 0.21 | [0.10, 0.91] | 6.09 | 0.014 |
Congruency (in-age-group vs. out-age-group) | 0.09 | 0.21 | [−0.33, 0.50] | 0.17 | 0.683 |
Age-group (20–45 vs. 65–90) | −0.35 | 0.23 | [−0.80, 0.09] | 2.45 | 0.117 |
Health condition | 0.04 | 0.14 | [−0.25, 0.32] | 0.06 | 0.805 |
Economic situation | −0.02 | 0.13 | [−0.28, 0.23] | 0.03 | 0.867 |
Religiosity | 0.15 | 0.09 | [−0.03, 0.33] | 2.63 | 0.105 |
QICC (Quasi-likelihood under independence model criterion) | 257.33 |
B | SE (b) | 95%CI | Wald’s Chi-Square | Sig. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Synchrony (sync vs. async) | −0.06 | 0.19 | [−0.42, 0.31] | 0.10 | 0.754 |
Congruency (in-age-group vs. out-age-group) | 0.10 | 0.19 | [−0.27, 0.48] | 0.30 | 0.586 |
Age-group (20–45 vs. 65–90) | 0.69 | 0.21 | [0.27, 1.11] | 10.39 | 0.001 |
Health condition | −0.17 | 0.16 | [−0.49, 0.15] | 1.08 | 0.299 |
Economic situation | 0.14 | 0.12 | [−0.09, 0.37] | 1.42 | 0.234 |
Religiosity | 0.14 | 0.10 | [−0.05, 0.34] | 2.10 | 0.148 |
Perceived closeness—pre | −0.56 | 0.07 | [−0.70, −0.42] | 61.22 | <0.001 |
QICC | 275.10 |
Wald’s Chi-Square | Sig. | |
---|---|---|
Two-way interactions | ||
Synchrony × Congruency | 5.40 | 0.020 |
Synchrony × Age-group | 0.42 | 0.517 |
Congruency × Age-group | 0.01 | 0.914 |
Three-way interaction | ||
Synchrony × Congruency × Age-group | 0.01 | 0.913 |
QICC two-way interaction model | 272.73 | |
QICC three-way interaction model | 278.88 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Suberry, A.; Bodner, E. Intergenerational Synchrony and Its Effect on Bonding and Group Closeness among Young and Older Adults. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 607. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070607
Suberry A, Bodner E. Intergenerational Synchrony and Its Effect on Bonding and Group Closeness among Young and Older Adults. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(7):607. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070607
Chicago/Turabian StyleSuberry, Assaf, and Ehud Bodner. 2024. "Intergenerational Synchrony and Its Effect on Bonding and Group Closeness among Young and Older Adults" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 7: 607. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070607
APA StyleSuberry, A., & Bodner, E. (2024). Intergenerational Synchrony and Its Effect on Bonding and Group Closeness among Young and Older Adults. Behavioral Sciences, 14(7), 607. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070607