Next Article in Journal
The Correlation between Motor Skill Proficiency and Academic Performance in High School Students
Previous Article in Journal
Associations between Unsociability and Peer Problems in Chinese Children and Adolescents: A Meta-Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Subjective Socioeconomic Status Influences Pro-Environmental Behavior: The Mediating Role of Sense of Control and Life History Strategy

1
CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
2
Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
3
Department of Psychology, Hunan Normal University, Changsha 410081, China
4
Cognition and Human Behavior Key Laboratory of Hunan Province, Changsha 410081, China
5
Institute of Interdisciplinary Studies, Hunan Normal University, Changsha 410081, China
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14(7), 591; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070591
Submission received: 1 June 2024 / Revised: 9 July 2024 / Accepted: 10 July 2024 / Published: 11 July 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Social Psychology)

Abstract

:
Understanding the psychological drivers of pro-environmental behavior across different socioeconomic statuses (SESs) is crucial for effectively addressing environmental challenges. To assist businesses and management departments in adequately identifying the psychological characteristics of target consumer groups from different SES backgrounds, our research manipulated subjective SES through three experimental studies to investigate the influence of subjective SES on pro-environmental behavior. Studies 1 and 2 adopted online experiments to examine the influence of subjective SES on pro-environmental behavior within the private sphere and the public sphere. Subsequently, Study 3 further investigated the psychological mechanisms through which subjective SES influences pro-environmental behavior. These results indicated that individuals from high SES backgrounds exhibit a greater propensity for green consumption behavior and contribute more financially to environmental organizations than those from low SES backgrounds. In addition, these studies further elucidated that the sense of control and life history strategy sequentially mediate the relationship. These findings provide empirical evidence for understanding whether and how subjective SES influences pro-environmental behavior, and enriching the theoretical framework of the relationship between subjective SES on pro-environmental behavior.

1. Introduction

Pro-environmental behavior refers to the actions taken by individuals that benefit the environment, involving a trade-off between short-term self-interests and long-term environmental benefits [1,2,3,4,5,6], at the expense of self-interests [7,8]. In modern society, rapid economic development often causes human activities that are detrimental to the environment [1], and these have become global threats in the 21st century. Given the adverse effects of self-interested behaviors on the environment, the necessity for environmental protection has escalated in the field of ecological psychology [9].
SES is a complex structure that encompasses both objective material resources, as well as subjective perceptions [10]. Object SES refers to control over tangible or symbolic social resources and is typically measured using indicators, such as wealth, education level, and occupational prestige. Subjective SES is a self-perceived measure of one’s position in the social hierarchy [11,12]. Previous literature has found that the brief priming of subjective SES can elicit behavioral patterns similar to those exhibited by individuals with long-term exposure to a specific SES [13], making it feasible to manipulate subjective SES and study its causal relationship with pro-environmental behavior. Notably, the National Bureau of Statistics released a Gini coefficient of 0.47 for China’s per capita disposable income in 2023, indicating a distinct difference in wealth distribution. Throughout history, society has advocated a cultural value that expects those with abundant resources (i.e., high SES individuals) to contribute more to the overall welfare of society [14]. However, whether individuals of high SES engage more in pro-environmental behavior, as society anticipates, remains inconclusive.
The existing literature provides two research orientations regarding the effect of SES on pro-environmental behavior. On one hand, individuals from high SES backgrounds are more likely to engage in pro-environmental behavior [15]. For example, engaging in green consumption signifies a high social status, with individuals from high SES backgrounds exhibiting higher pro-environmental intentions [16,17]. On the other hand, research suggests that high SES individuals do not necessarily demonstrate greater motivation to support and engage in pro-environmental behavior. For instance, income exhibits a negative correlation with the inclination to participate in pro-environmental behavior, such as increasing the frequency of using public transportation [18]. Additionally, studies have found a weak correlation between pro-environmental behavior and income [19,20,21]. Considering that previous studies have examined either subjective [15,22] or objective SES [20,23] to explore the effect of SES on pro-environmental behavior, the inconsistent results associated with this relationship may originate from different measurement methods. Therefore, the current study employed a comparative approach to prime participants’ subjective SES and explored the causal relationship between subjective SES and pro-environmental behavior.
Sense of control refers to individuals’ perception of their ability to control events and the degree to which they feel constrained by external factors [24,25], which is the personality variable and is also influenced by the social environment [25,26]. Notably, individuals from low SES backgrounds exposed to resource scarcity experience a diminished sense of control [27,28] and tend to give higher priority to immediate rewards [16,29]. In contrast, individuals from high SES backgrounds experience an elevated sense of control and psychological entitlement [30], which may prioritize long-term benefits. Based on the above findings, considering that pro-environmental behavior involves a trade-off between short-term self-interests and long-term environmental benefits, we discussed the psychological mechanism to examine whether the sense of control might act as a mediator in the relationship between subjective SES and pro-environmental behavior.
Importantly, existing studies have shown that environmental factors, including financial scarcity (e.g., lacking money), can influence individuals’ life history strategy [31,32]; there are two fundamental life strategies that fall along a continuum ranging from a slower strategy marked by a delayed pace of reproduction and emphasis on offspring quality to a faster strategy characterized by a quicker pace of reproduction and focus on the quantity of offspring [32]. When external cues are predictable and controllable, slower strategies involving delayed reproduction and future investments become adaptive [33]. Conversely, instability, uncertainty, and the severity of the external environment shape individuals’ life history strategies [34], prompting them to follow the fast life history strategy [32], which prioritizes immediate reproduction. Furthermore, the sense of control related to SES may also affect the life history strategy. For example, recent studies have suggested that individuals’ sense of control serves as the psychological motivator linked to the behavioral tendencies associated with both fast and slow life history strategies [33,35]. Based on the above, we posited that individuals from different subjective SESs exhibit distinct senses of control, adopting corresponding behavioral strategies, which may ultimately result in differences in pro-environmental behavior.
Taken together, we conducted three studies using diverse methods to examine whether and why subjective SES influences pro-environmental behavior. Study 1 investigated the causal relationship between subjective SES and the preference for green consumption. Study 2 investigated whether subjective SES influences environmental donation behavior. Study 3 further examined the psychological mechanisms by which subjective SES influences pro-environmental behavior, specifically by investigating the sequential mediation involving the sense of control and life history strategy (See Figure 1).

2. Study 1

Given the prevalence of green consumer behavior in individuals’ daily lives as a form of private-sphere pro-environmental behavior, Study 1 aimed to examine the influence of subjective SES on green consumer behavior.

2.1. Method

2.1.1. Participants

According to G*Power 3.1 [36] with a medium effect size (effect size d = 0.5, alpha = 0.05), a sample size of 102 is required to ensure 80% power. To increase the reliability of the results concerning the inconsistent relationship between SES and pro-environmental behavior, we recruited 270 participants from a non-student population using the online platform Credamo [37], a widely used experimental platform that facilitates participant recruitment and data collection. In total, 11 participants who did not follow the instructions (e.g., not following prime imagination of SES) were excluded from the analysis, and 2 exceeded three standard deviations from the mean, thus leaving data from 257 participants for the final analysis. A total of 101 men and 156 women were included (Mage = 29.01 years, SD = 8.03). The high SES condition comprised 132 participants, and the low SES condition included 125 participants. Finally, participants were compensated appropriately for their participation.

2.1.2. Procedure and Measurement

Subjective SES manipulation. Participants were shown an image of a 10-rung ladder as a representation of different SESs in China. They were told that the bottom rung represented the lowest SES, with individuals at this level having the fewest resources (lowest income, lowest education level, and least prestigious occupation). Conversely, the top rung represented the highest SES with the most resources (highest income, highest education level, and most prestigious occupation). Importantly, in the high subjective SES priming condition, participants were instructed to compare themselves with individuals of the lowest hierarchy. Conversely, in the low subjective SES priming condition, participants were instructed to compare themselves with individuals of the highest hierarchy [27,38]. In addition, participants were asked to write their feelings when imagining how they would interact with individuals from either the highest or lowest SES, enhancing the priming effects of rank-related states [27,38]. To test the effectiveness of subjective SES, we used the MacArthur Scale [39], in which participants were asked to subjectively choose the position on the ladder that best represented their SES. A higher score indicated a higher subjective SES.
Green consumption preference. The green consumption behavior scale used in this study was based on research by Al Mamun et al. (2018), comprising seven items (e.g., I use energy-efficient appliances) (See Supplementary Materials). Participants were required to rate the extent to which they intended to engage in the aforementioned green consumption behaviors over the upcoming three months using a 7-point Likert scale [40]. A composite measure of green consumption preference was established by averaging the scores for the seven items (α = 0.74), with a higher score indicating heightened levels of green consumption preference.
Objective SES measurement. The objective SES was measured by the family’s annual income, educational level, and occupation (See Supplementary Materials), which was calculated according to previous research [14,41,42]. This involved standardizing and averaging the three indicators of family income, highest educational level, and occupational prestige.
In conclusion, participants completed the subjective SES priming and corresponding manipulation check. They were then assessed for their green consumption preference. Finally, demographic information, including the objective SES, was collected (See Figure 2).

2.2. Results

2.2.1. Manipulation Check

An independent samples t-test was conducted to examine the manipulation check for subjective SES, which revealed a significant difference between the high (M = 5.56, SD = 1.06) and low SES priming conditions (M = 4.51, SD = 1.22, t (255) = 7.39, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.92), indicating the successful manipulation of the subjective SES.

2.2.2. Green Consumption Behavior

An independent sample t-test was conducted to examine the influence of SES on individuals’ preference for green consumption. The results revealed a significant difference between the high (M = 5.86, SD = 0.60) and low SES priming conditions (M = 5.66, SD = 0.66) regarding the participants’ green consumption preference (t (255) = 2.54, p = 0.012, Cohen’s d = 0.32), which experimentally supports that individuals with a higher subjective SES have a greater preference for green consumption (See Figure 3). Additionally, when the data within three standard deviations were not excluded, the results still revealed a significant difference between the high (M = 5.86, SD = 0.60) and low SES priming conditions (M = 5.60, SD = 0.82), t (257) = −2.94, p < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.36.
Furthermore, a regression analysis was conducted with green consumption preference as the dependent variable, subjective SES as the independent variable, and gender, age, and objective SES as the control variables. The results indicated that even after controlling for sex, age, and objective SES, the subjective SES still significantly predicted the participants’ preference for green consumption (b = 0.19, SE = 0.08, p = 0.02) (See Supplementary Materials).

2.3. Discussion

Study 1 used experimental manipulation to investigate the impact of subjective SES on the participants’ preference for green consumption, indicating that high subjective SES individuals exhibit a greater preference for green consumption. However, whether these results can be consistently applied to pro-environmental behavior in the public sphere remains unclear.

3. Study 2

Study 2 adopted a real environmental donation paradigm, manipulating participants’ subjective SES and further examining the SES differences in public-sphere pro-environmental behavior (i.e., environmental donation behavior).

3.1. Method

3.1.1. Participants

We recruited 150 participants through the Credamo online platform. Participants who did not comply with the instructions were excluded, leaving 146 participants for final analysis: 61 men and 85 women (Mage = 28.60 years, SD = 7.29). Among these, 73 participants were from the high subjective SES, and the other 73 participants were from the low subjective SES.

3.1.2. Procedure and Measurement

Before the beginning of the experiment, participants were introduced to the World Wildlife Fund (WWF), an organization dedicated to fostering a future in which humans coexist harmoniously with nature. In addition, participants were provided with a CNY 20 participation fee. Subsequently, participants were informed that they had the freedom to donate any amount between CNY 0 and CNY 20 to the WWF. The final participant fee equals the initial participant fee of CNY 20 minus the donated amount [43,44].
After the experiment began, participants first completed the subjective SES priming and manipulation check. Subsequently, they were tasked with deciding what portion of their participation fee they would donate to the WWF. Finally, the objective SES and other demographic information were collected.

3.2. Results

3.2.1. Manipulation Check

An independent sample t-test was conducted, which revealed a significant difference between the high and low subjective SES conditions (t (144) = 5.14, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.85). Participants in the high subjective SES (M = 5.53, SD = 1.07) reported significantly higher ratings than those in the low subjective SES (M = 4.59, SD = 1.15), suggesting the successful manipulation of the subjective SES.

3.2.2. Amount of Environmental Donation

We conducted an independent sample t-test to analyze data on donation amounts. The results indicated a significant difference between participants in the high and low SES conditions (t (144) = 4.41, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.73). Participants in the high subjective SES condition (M = 12.78, SD = 5.06) donated significantly more than those in the low subjective SES condition (M = 9.17, SD = 4.83), further demonstrating consistent SES differences (See Figure 4).
Moreover, a regression analysis was further conducted, with environmental donation behavior as the dependent variable, subjective SES as the independent variable, and objective SES, sex, and age as control variables. The result showed that even when accounting for sex, age, and objective SES, subjective SES continued to be a significant predictor of environmental donation behavior (b = 3.36, SE = 0.84, p < 0.001) (See Supplementary Materials).

3.3. Discussion

Study 2 found that high subjective SES individuals donated more to environmental organizations compared to low subjective SES individuals. This study expands on previous findings. Pro-environmental behavior with financial costs typically leads to long-term rewards, which necessitate resistance to immediate gratification [29]. Due to the lack of resources, low SES individuals tend to prefer instant gratification and are less likely to engage in environmentally friendly behavior compared to high SES individuals [16]. Taken together, through these behavioral experiments, we have found the consistent influence of subjective SES on pro-environmental behavior in both the public and private spheres within the cultural context of China, prompting further consideration of the corresponding psychological mechanisms underlying these disparities.

4. Study 3

Study 3 adopted the environmental donation task, aiming to further examine whether individuals’ sense of control and life history strategy may be potential psychological antecedents through which subjective SES influences pro-environmental behavior.

4.1. Method

4.1.1. Participants

To investigate the mediating effect, 370 participants were recruited via the online platform Credamo. After excluding 14 participants who did not comply with the instructions and an additional 5 participants with data exceeding three standard deviations from the mean, a total of 351 participants remained. Among these, 128 were men and 223 were women (Mage = 29.08 years, SD = 7.53). The low SES prime condition comprised 172 participants, while the high SES prime condition consisted of 179 participants.

4.1.2. Procedure and Measurement

The manipulation of the subjective SES and the subsequent manipulation check followed the procedures used in Study 1. Likewise, the measurement of the objective SES used the same methods as those used in Study 1. Additionally, the procedure used to assess environmental donation behavior was consistent with that of Study 2.
Sense of control scale. Participants filled out the sense of control scale developed by Lachman and Weaver (1998), as revised by Li (2014) [24,33]. The scale comprises two dimensions, namely Personal Mastery and Perceived Constraints (with eight reverse scores), totaling 12 items (e.g., I can achieve almost anything I set my mind to) (See Supplementary Materials). Participants rated each item on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = completely disagree, 7 = completely agree). A higher score indicated a greater sense of control (Cronbach’s α = 0.95).
Life history strategy scale. The life history strategy scale was evaluated using the Chinese version of the Mini-K scale, which has been translated and revised [45], comprising 20 items (e.g., I often make plans in advance). Participants rated each item on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = completely disagree, 7 = completely agree). A higher score indicated a stronger inclination toward the slow life history strategy, whereas a lower score indicated a stronger inclination toward the fast life history strategy (Cronbach’s α = 0.80).
In conclusion, participants first completed the subjective SES priming and manipulation check. Subsequently, they filled out the sense of control and life history strategy scales (See Supplementary Materials) and then completed the environmental donation task. Finally, demographic information, including the objective SES, was collected.

4.2. Results

4.2.1. Common Method Biases

Harman’s single-factor test was employed to identify common method bias [46], which involved the variables of subjective SES, objective SES, sense of control, and life history strategy. The results revealed seven factors with eigenvalues >1 before rotation. Additionally, the first factor explained 30.31% of the variance before rotation; below 40% indicated no significant common method bias.

4.2.2. Manipulation Check

An independent sample t-test was conducted, which showed a significant difference between the high (M = 5.73, SD = 1.15) and low SES conditions (M = 4.78, SD = 1.17, t (349) = 7.64, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.82), suggesting the effective manipulation of the subjective SES.

4.2.3. Amount of Environmental Donation

A t-test was performed on independent samples to confirm the disparities in environmental donation behavior across different SESs. The results revealed that participants in the high subjective SES condition (M = 12.22, SD = 5.21) donated significantly more to environmental organizations than those in the low subjective SES condition (M = 9.66, SD = 5.15, t (349) = 4.63, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.49), further confirming the consistent SES differences in environmental donation behavior.
Additionally, to control the potential influences of objective SES, sex, and age, a regression analysis was conducted, using environmental donation behavior as the dependent variable, subjective SES as the independent variable, and objective SES, gender, and age as control variables. The results indicated that even with controlling for sex, age, and objective SES, subjective SES remained a significant predictor of environmental donation behavior (b = 2.11, SE = 0.55, p < 0.001) (See Supplementary Materials).

4.2.4. Amount of Sense of Control and Life History Strategy

An independent samples t-test was performed to examine the disparities in the respondents’ sense of control and life history strategy. The results revealed that participants in the high subjective SES condition (M = 5.13, SD = 0.93) had higher sense of control scores compared to those in the low subjective SES condition (M = 4.18, SD = 1.29), indicating a stronger sense of control (t (349) = 7.96, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.84). Additionally, participants in the high subjective SES condition (M = 5.68, SD = 0.46) had higher life history strategy scores compared to those in the low subjective SES condition (M = 5.44, SD = 0.50), indicating a tendency to adopt slower life history strategies (t (349) = 4.71, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.50).

4.2.5. Effect of Mediation

A regression analysis was conducted using the PROCESS macro Model 6 [47], with subjective SES as the independent variable; sex, age, and objective SES as control variables; sense of control and life history strategy as mediating variables; and environmental donation behavior as the dependent variable. Table 1 presents the regression analysis results. The mediation analysis utilized bootstrap resampling (5000 iterations), for which the results are presented in Table 2. These findings indicated that the confidence intervals for the total indirect effect, indirect effect 1, and indirect effect 2 did not include zero, indicating significant mediating roles in the relationship between subjective SES and environmental donation behavior. Specifically, the results suggested that individuals’ sense of control and life history strategy serve as sequential mediators in this relationship. Additionally, the results confirmed that sense of control is an independent mediator of the impact of SES on pro-environmental behavior, but failed to prove that individuals’ life history strategy is an independent mediator in this context.

4.3. Discussion

Study 3 further supported the hypothesis that individuals’ sense of control and life history strategy act as mediators in explaining the relationship between subjective SES and pro-environmental behavior. According to social cognition theory, owing to limited access to resources, low SES individuals are inclined toward contextual cognition, experience a lower sense of control over the environment and are more sensitive to external threats [24,27,28]. If individuals perceive themselves as unable to control future outcomes, they will tend to choose immediate rewards and engage in short-term behavior for immediate gains, reflecting the tendency of a fast life history strategy. Conversely, individuals with a stronger sense of control are more likely to prefer long-term benefits, reflecting their preference for a slow life history strategy [33]. These results indicate that individuals’ subjective SES is associated with their sense of control, which in turn predicts their tendency to adopt distinct life history strategies, ultimately influencing the SES differences in pro-environmental behavior.

5. General Discussion

Throughout three separate series of studies conducted at various time points, our studies found that compared to low subjective SES individuals, those with high subjective SES backgrounds show a greater preference for green consumerism and donate more money to environmental organization. In addition, these studies further elucidated that the sense of control and life history strategy sequentially mediate the relationship between subjective SES and pro-environmental behavior, revealing the psychological antecedents of this relationship. These findings provide empirical evidence for understanding whether and how subjective SES influences pro-environmental behavior, and providing a potential theoretical basis for promoting pro-environmental behavior among different SESs.
Importantly, consistent findings emerged regarding the influence of SES on pro-environmental behavior in both the private and public domains. These results are supported by previous research. For example, studies have shown a positive correlation between subjective SES and pro-environmental intentions, indicating that individuals from high SES backgrounds have higher pro-environmental intentions [16]. Similarly, pro-environmental behavior is more prevalent among high SES individuals [48,49]. As discussed above, our results indicate distinct pro-environmental behavior patterns among various subjective SESs in the Chinese cultural context.
What is more, our study found that inducing SES affects individuals’ sense of control and life history strategy. These findings are supported by previous research and theories. According to the social cognition theory, individuals from low SES backgrounds typically adopt a contextual cognitive orientation due to limited access to resources and a lower sense of control over the environment, which manifests as feelings of helplessness and a belief that events are uncontrollable [28]. In contrast, high SES individuals are more likely to form self-concepts and social cognitive tendencies based on individual attributes [50], developing a solipsistic cognitive orientation owing to their greater access to resources and higher sense of control over the environment [51]. In addition, life history theory posits that resources are limited, and that all organisms encounter crucial trade-offs in allocating and utilizing their resource “budget” between short-term interests and long-term benefits [31]. According to the life history theory, individuals develop and adjust their strategies for allocating resources to more effectively adapt to the environment [45]. For example, researchers have observed that individuals nearing payday tend to prioritize immediate benefits in intertemporal monetary decisions, indicating higher levels of time discounting [52]. Experience with poverty leads individuals to favor immediate small incomes over potential higher future earnings, resulting in reduced investment in long-term gains [53]. Additionally, experiments manipulating negative income shocks, common among those with a low SES, have shown that such shocks increase delay discounting rates, while positive income changes decrease them [54]. These findings indicate that even a short-term prime of SES can significantly put individuals into a state that is similar to being in that hierarchy for a long time, affecting their sense of control and life history strategies.
Additionally, our study investigated the mediating effects of individuals’ sense of control and life history strategy in the relationship between subjective SES and pro-environmental behavior. The Psychological Shift Model from the perspective of threat highlights three primary ways in which they may influence how individuals behave, namely resource scarcity, environmental instability and unpredictability, and adverse childhood experiences [55,56]. Firstly, individuals control over their life outcomes diminishes, shifting their focus from the future to the present, from distant to local concerns, from distant social relationships to close ones, and from hypothetical to concrete thinking. Secondly, their perceived control weakens, prompting individuals to shift from pursuing long-term goals to short-term ones, from seeking rewards to avoiding threats, and from employing slow strategies to fast ones. The third aspect involves the concentration of cognitive resources on tasks and stimuli to meet immediate urgent needs, leading to adaptive decision-making [56,57]. Based on above, the feeling of scarcity triggered by comparisons with those in the highest hierarchy may reduce individuals’ sense of control and induce psychological shifts in individuals to cope with threats, which may foster a temporary preference for short-term behaviors that provide benefits immediately. Therefore, low subjective SES individuals with a weaker sense of control tend to prioritize immediate rewards, resulting in a reduced consideration of the potential long-term benefits associated with pro-environmental behavior [16], reflecting a fast life history strategy, which prioritizes immediate benefits over engaging in pro-environmental behavior. Conversely, high subjective SES individuals with a strong sense of control lean toward long-term benefits, reflecting a slow life history strategy [33], which tends to engage in pro-environmental behavior that is beneficial for long-term development. Based on these theories and empirical research, these findings indicate that individuals’ sense of control and life history strategy sequentially mediate the influence of subjective SES on pro-environmental behavior, enriching the theoretical framework associated with the relationship between subjective SES and pro-environmental behavior.
The practical implications of this study are twofold. First, it enhances our understanding of how subjective SES influences pro-environmental behavior, providing valuable insights for promoting such behavior across different SESs, which can help businesses and management departments adequately identify the psychological needs of target consumer groups throughout the entire life cycle of green products. Second, considering the distinct psychological and behavioral patterns observed across SESs, efforts to promote pro-environmental behavior should emphasize the long-term benefits of the natural environment and enhance individuals’ sense of control over external events, particularly among low SES individuals.
Our study also has some limitations that warrant further investigation. First, while our study examined the causal connection between subjective SES and pro-environmental behavior and elucidated the corresponding psychological mechanism, a crucial direction for future experimental research involves exploring effective measures to promote pro-environmental behavior across different SESs [9]. Second, our study built upon prior research and categorized SES into high and low hierarchies [50,58,59], unveiling SES disparities within the context of Chinese culture. However, it is noteworthy that SES can also be further stratified into high, middle, and low hierarchies. Future research is warranted to investigate the variations in pro-environmental behavior among individuals from high, middle, and low SES. Third, our study did not find that individuals’ life history strategy mediates the relationship between subjective SES and pro-environmental behavior. Previous studies found that factors such as environmental instability are more likely to influence individuals who grew up poorer, leading them to adopt more short-term behaviors that are consistent with a faster life-history strategy [60]. It is essential to recognize that one’s subjective SES changes over one’s lifetime, resulting in potential differences between one’s childhood and current subjective SES [61], which may be why individuals’ life history strategy failed to mediate the impact of SES on pro-environmental behavior. Therefore, further exploration is needed to investigate the effects of current subjective SES, childhood subjective SES, and individuals’ life history strategy on pro-environmental behavior. Finally, there is a possibility that the smaller donation amounts from individuals with low subjective SES backgrounds may mean that the remaining funds are allocated to other types of pro-environmental behavior post-experiment. Future research could design dedicated studies to more comprehensively rule out this possibility.

6. Conclusions

Our study manipulated SES to explore the causal relationship between subjective SES and pro-environmental behavior, highlighting the mediating influence of sense of control and life history strategy. Furthermore, this research lays the foundation for future investigations aimed at devising targeted strategies to enhance pro-environmental behavior among individuals from different socioeconomic backgrounds.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/bs14070591/s1, Table S1. Regression analysis of subjective SES on green consumption behavior (Study 1: N = 257); Table S2. Regression analysis of subjective SES on environmental donation behavior (Study 2: N = 146); Table S3. Regression analysis of subjective SES on environmental donation behavior (Study 3: N = 351).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.Z.; methodology, B.Z. and N.N.; software, J.L.; formal analysis, B.Z. and N.N.; investigation, B.Z., N.N. and Y.W.; resources, J.L. and W.F.; data curation, B.Z., N.N. and Y.W.; writing—original draft preparation, B.Z.; writing—review and editing, B.Z. and N.N.; visualization, B.Z. and N.N.; supervision, W.F. and J.L.; project administration, W.F. and J.L.; funding acquisition, W.F. and J.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number: 32000769, 32371126) and the Hunan Provincial Natural Science Foundation (grant number: 2023JJ30400, 2022JJ30378).

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted by the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Hunan Normal University (Ethics approval number: 2023330, 9 June 2023).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data supporting the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Niu, N.; Fan, W.; Ren, M.; Li, M.; Zhong, Y. The Role of Social Norms and Personal Costs on Pro-Environmental Behavior: The Mediating Role of Personal Norms. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2023, 16, 2059–2069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Yi-Beng, Z.; Mei, L.; Jin, L.; Min, T.; Wei, F.; Hui, E.L. “Pursue Reputation for Profit”: The Influence of social observation and social distance on the pro-environmental behavior. J. Psychol. Sci. 2022, 45, 1398. [Google Scholar]
  3. Li, M.; Tan, M.; Wang, S.; Li, J.; Zhang, G.; Zhong, Y. The effect of preceding self-control on green consumption behavior: The moderating role of moral elevation. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2021, 14, 2169–2180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Barclay, P.; Barker, J.L. Greener than thou: People who protect the environment are more cooperative, compete to be environmental, and benefit from reputation. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 72, 101441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Lange, F.; Steinke, A.; Dewitte, S. The Pro-Environmental Behavior Task: A laboratory measure of actual pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 56, 46–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Li, M.; Li, J.; Zhang, G.F.; Fan, W.; Li, H.; Zhong, Y.P. Social distance modulates the influence of social observation on pro-environmental behavior: An event-related potential (ERP) study. Biol. Psychol. 2023, 178, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Kollock, P. Social dilemmas: The anatomy of cooperation. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 1998, 24, 183–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Steg, L.; Vlek, C. Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: An integrative review and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 309–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Klein, S.A.; Horsten, L.K.; Hilbig, B.E. The effect of environmental versus social framing on pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2022, 84, 101897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hu, X.-Y.; Jing, L.; Lu, H.-Z.; Guo, Y.-Y. The psychological study of social class: Social cognitive perspective. J. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 37, 1509. [Google Scholar]
  11. Kraus, M.W.; Piff, P.K.; Keltner, D. Social class as culture: The convergence of resources and rank in the social realm. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 20, 246–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kraus, M.W.; Tan, J.J.; Tannenbaum, M.B. The social ladder: A rank-based perspective on social class. Psychol. Inq. 2013, 24, 81–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Guo, Y.; Yang, S.; Li, J.; Hu, X. Social Fairness Researches in Perspectives of Social Class Psychology. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 23, 1299–1311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bai, J.Y.S.; Xu, B.; Guo, Y. How can successful people share their goodness with the world: The psychological mechanism underlying the upper social classes’ redistributive preferences and the role of humility. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2021, 53, 1161–1172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Sun, J.; Ma, B.; Wei, S. Same gratitude, different pro-environmental behaviors? Effect of the dual-path influence mechanism of gratitude on pro-environmental behavior. J. Clean. Prod. 2023, 415, 137779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Grandin, A.; Guillou, L.; Sater, R.A.; Foucault, M.; Chevallier, C. Socioeconomic status, time preferences and pro-environmentalism. J. Environ. Psychol. 2022, 79, 101720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Niu, L.; Lu, C.; Fan, L. Social Class and Private-Sphere Green Behavior in China: The Mediating Effects of Perceived Status and Environmental Concern. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2023, 20, 4329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. O’Connor, R.E.; Bord, R.J.; Yarnal, B.; Wiefek, N. Who wants to reduce greenhouse gas emissions? Soc. Sci. Q. 2002, 83, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Lo, A.Y. National income and environmental concern: Observations from 35 countries. Public Underst. Sci. 2016, 25, 873–890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Blankenberg, A.-K.; Alhusen, H. On the Determinants of Pro-Environmental Behavior: A Literature Review and Guide for the Empirical Economist; Center for European, Governance, and Economic Development Research (CEGE): Göttingen, Germany, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  21. Marquart-Pyatt, S.T. Contextual influences on environmental concerns cross-nationally: A multilevel investigation. Soc. Sci. Res. 2012, 41, 1085–1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Yan, L.; Keh, H.T.; Chen, J.; Campbell, M.C.; Reczek, R.W. Assimilating and Differentiating: The Curvilinear Effect of Social Class on Green Consumption. J. Consum. Res. 2021, 47, 914–936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Laidley, T.M. The Influence of Social Class and Cultural Variables on Environmental Behaviors. Environ. Behav. 2011, 45, 170–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Lachman, M.E.; Weaver, S.L. The sense of control as a moderator of social class differences in health and well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 74, 763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Yang, S.; Guo, Y.; Hu, X.; Shu, S.; Jing, L.I. Do lower class individuals possess higher levels of system justification? An examination from the social cognitive perspectives. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2016, 48, 1467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Turiano, N.A.; Chapman, B.P.; Agrigoroaei, S.; Infurna, F.J.; Lachman, M. Perceived control reduces mortality risk at low, not high, education levels. Health Psychol. J. Div. Health Psychol. Am. Psychol. Assoc. 2014, 33, 883–890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Piff, P.K.; Kraus, M.W.; Cote, S.; Cheng, B.H.; Keltner, D. Having less, giving more: The influence of social class on prosocial behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 99, 771–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Kraus, M.W.; Piff, P.K.; Mendoza-Denton, R.; Rheinschmidt, M.L.; Keltner, D. Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: How the rich are different from the poor. Psychol. Rev. 2012, 119, 546–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Bruderer Enzler, H.; Diekmann, A.; Meyer, R. Subjective discount rates in the general population and their predictive power for energy saving behavior. Energy Policy 2014, 65, 524–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Guan, J. Socio-psychological and behavioral tendency of lower-class: A review from Positive and negative perspectives. J. Nanjing Normal Univ. 2016, 6, 136–144. [Google Scholar]
  31. Griskevicius, V.; Delton, A.W.; Robertson, T.E.; Tybur, J.M. Environmental contingency in life history strategies: The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on reproductive timing. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 241–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Griskevicius, V.; Tybur, J.M.; Delton, A.W.; Robertson, T.E. The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: A life history theory approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 1015–1026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Zhang, K.; Geng, L. The effect of childhood socioeconomic status on people’s life history strategy: The role of foresight for the future and sense of control. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 2021, 4, 838–841. [Google Scholar]
  34. Ellis, B.J.; Figueredo, A.J.; Brumbach, B.H.; Schlomer, G.L. Fundamental Dimensions of Environmental Risk: The Impact of Harsh versus Unpredictable Environments on the Evolution and Development of Life History Strategies. Hum. Nat. 2009, 20, 204–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Mittal, C.; Griskevicius, V. Sense of control under uncertainty depends on people’s childhood environment: A life history theory approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 107, 621–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.-G.; Buchner, A. G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Zhong, N.; Peng, K.; Yu, F.; Wei, X. Psychological richness increases behavioral intention to protect the environment. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2023, 55, 1330–1343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Xiaoyong, H.U.; Guo, Y.; Jing, L.I.; Yang, S. Perceived societal fairness and goal attainment: The differnet effects of social class and their mechanism. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2016, 48, 271. [Google Scholar]
  39. Adler, N.E.; Epel, E.S.; Castellazzo, G.; Ickovics, J.R. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychol. Off. J. Div. Health Psychol. Am. Psychol. Assoc. 2000, 19, 586–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Al Mamun, A.; Mohamad, M.R.; Bin Yaacob, M.R.; Mohiuddin, M. Intention and behavior towards green consumption among low-income households. J. Environ. Manag. 2018, 227, 73–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Chen, S.; Yang, S.; Wang, H.; Wan, F. Subjective social class positively predicts altruistic punishment. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2022, 54, 1548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Tan, J.J.; Kraus, M.W. Lay theories about social class buffer lower-class individuals against poor self-rated health and negative affect. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 41, 446–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Cakanlar, A.; Nikolova, H.; Nenkov, G.Y. I Will Be Green for Us: When Consumers Compensate for Their Partners’ Unsustainable Behavior. J. Mark. Res. 2022, 60, 110–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Tam, K.-P. Gratitude to nature: Presenting a theory of its conceptualization, measurement, and effects on pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2022, 79, 101754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Wang, Y.; Lin, Z.; Hou, B.; Sun, S. The intrinsic mechanism of life history trade-offs: The mediating role of control striving. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2017, 49, 783–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Podsakoff, P.M.; Mackenzie, S.B.; Lee, J.Y.; Podsakoff, N.P. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Am. Psychol. Assoc. 2003, 88, 879–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  48. Huddart Kennedy, E.; Baumann, S.; Johnston, J. Eating for Taste and Eating for Change: Ethical Consumption as a High-Status Practice. Soc. Forces 2019, 98, 381–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Kennedy, E.H.; Givens, J.E. Eco-habitus or Eco-powerlessness? Examining Environmental Concern across Social Class. Sociol. Perspect. 2019, 62, 646–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Kraus, M.W.; Piff, P.K.; Keltner, D. Social class, sense of control, and social explanation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 97, 992–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Kraus, M.W.; Horberg, E.J.; Goetz, J.L.; Keltner, D. Social class rank, threat vigilance, and hostile reactivity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2011, 37, 1376–1388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Carvalho, L.S.; Meier, S.; Wang, S.W. Poverty and Economic Decision-Making: Evidence from Changes in Financial Resources at Payday. Am. Econ. Rev. 2016, 106, 260–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Haushofer, J.; Fehr, E. On the psychology of poverty. Science 2014, 344, 862–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Haushofer, J.; Schunk, D.; Fehr, E. Negative Income Shocks Increase Discount Rates; University of Zurich: Zürich, Switzerland, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  55. Sheehy-Skeffington, J.; Haushofer, J. The behavioural economics of poverty. Barriers Oppor. Poverty Reduct. 2014, 10, 96–112. [Google Scholar]
  56. Wang, T.; Li, L.; Yang, J.; Hu, X.; Du, T. Low Socioeconomic Status and intertemporal choice: The mechanism of “psychological-shift” from the perspective of threat. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2022, 30, 1894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Sheehy-Skeffington, J. The effects of low socioeconomic status on decision-making processes. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2020, 33, 183–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Eom, K.; Kim, H.S.; Sherman, D.K. Social class, control, and action: Socioeconomic status differences in antecedents of support for pro-environmental action. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 77, 60–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sherman, D.K.; Updegraff, J.A.; Handy, M.S.; Eom, K.; Kim, H.S. Beliefs and social norms as precursors of environmental support: The joint influence of collectivism and socioeconomic status. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2022, 48, 463–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Griskevicius, V.; Ackerman, J.M.; Cantu, S.M.; Delton, A.W.; Robertson, T.E.; Simpson, J.A.; Thompson, M.E.; Tybur, J.M. When the economy falters, do people spend or save? Responses to resource scarcity depend on childhood environments. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Côté, S. A Multidimensional Framework for Examining the Effects of Social Class on Organizational Behavior. J. Manag. 2024, 50, 928–948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Hypothetical model.
Figure 1. Hypothetical model.
Behavsci 14 00591 g001
Figure 2. Experimental flowchart.
Figure 2. Experimental flowchart.
Behavsci 14 00591 g002
Figure 3. The green consumption preference across different SESs, * p < 0.05.
Figure 3. The green consumption preference across different SESs, * p < 0.05.
Behavsci 14 00591 g003
Figure 4. The amount of environmental donation across different SESs. *** p < 0.001. Error bars indicate the standard error of the means (SE).
Figure 4. The amount of environmental donation across different SESs. *** p < 0.001. Error bars indicate the standard error of the means (SE).
Behavsci 14 00591 g004
Table 1. Hierarchical multiple linear regression predicting impact of subjective SES, sense of control, and life history strategy on environmental donation behavior.
Table 1. Hierarchical multiple linear regression predicting impact of subjective SES, sense of control, and life history strategy on environmental donation behavior.
VariableSense of ControlLife History StrategyEnvironmental Donation Behavior
bSEtCIbSEtCIbSEtCI
Intercept3.800.2614.68 ***[3.29; 4.31]4.540.1237.12 ***[4.30; 4.78]−5.143.25−1.58[−11.54; 1.26]
Subjective SES0.860.127.28 ***[0.63; 1.10]0.020.050.52[−0.07; 0.12]0.920.561.63[−0.19; 2.03]
Sense of control 0.230.0211.32 ***[0.19; 0.27]1.050.283.79 ***[0.51; 1.60]
Life history strategy 1.270.641.98 *[0.01; 2.53]
Objective SES0.330.093.82 ***[0.16; 0.50]0.030.030.80[−0.04; 0.09]0.260.390.66[−0.51; 1.03]
Sex0.060.120.47[−0.18; 0.30]−0.050.05−1.01[−0.14; 0.04]1.080.542.00 *[0.02; 2.14]
Age0.010.011.62[−0.00; 0.03]0.000.00−0.15[−0.01; 0.01]0.100.042.85 **[0.03; 0.17]
R20.200.330.19
F22.06 ***34.45 ***13.77 ***
Notes. b = unstandardized coefficients. CI = percentile bootstrapped confidence intervals. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Mediating effects of sense of control and life history strategy between subjective SES and environmental donation behavior.
Table 2. Mediating effects of sense of control and life history strategy between subjective SES and environmental donation behavior.
PathwayEffectSE95% CI
LLUL
Total indirect effect1.1890.2690.7031.746
Indirect effect 10.2480.1200.0180.492
Indirect effect 20.9100.2720.4211.500
Indirect effect 30.0310.065–0.0890.177
Notes. Indirect effect 1 = subjective SES → sense of control → life history strategy → environmental donation behavior. Indirect effect 2 = subjective SES → sense of control → environmental donation behavior. Indirect effect 3 = subjective SES → life history strategy → environmental donation behavior. CI = percentile bootstrapped confidence intervals, LL = lower limit, UL = upper limit.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zhong, B.; Niu, N.; Li, J.; Wu, Y.; Fan, W. How Subjective Socioeconomic Status Influences Pro-Environmental Behavior: The Mediating Role of Sense of Control and Life History Strategy. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 591. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070591

AMA Style

Zhong B, Niu N, Li J, Wu Y, Fan W. How Subjective Socioeconomic Status Influences Pro-Environmental Behavior: The Mediating Role of Sense of Control and Life History Strategy. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(7):591. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070591

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zhong, Bowei, Nana Niu, Jin Li, Yun Wu, and Wei Fan. 2024. "How Subjective Socioeconomic Status Influences Pro-Environmental Behavior: The Mediating Role of Sense of Control and Life History Strategy" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 7: 591. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070591

APA Style

Zhong, B., Niu, N., Li, J., Wu, Y., & Fan, W. (2024). How Subjective Socioeconomic Status Influences Pro-Environmental Behavior: The Mediating Role of Sense of Control and Life History Strategy. Behavioral Sciences, 14(7), 591. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070591

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop