Learning to Read in an Intermediate Depth Orthography: The Longitudinal Role of Grapheme Sounding on Different Types of Reading Fluency
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Verbal and Nonverbal Abilities
2.2.2. Oral Sentence Immediate Recall
2.2.3. Phonemic Awareness (PA)
2.2.4. Grapheme Sounding (GS)
2.2.5. Reading Fluency
2.3. General Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics and Correlational Analyses
3.2. Hierarchical Regression Analyses (HRA)
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Wagner, R.K.; Torgesen, J.K. The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychol. Bull. 1987, 101, 192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Jong, P.F.; Van der Leij, A. Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. J. Educ. Psychol. 1999, 91, 450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Georgiou, G.K.; Parrila, R.; Papadopoulos, T.C. Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. J. Educ. Psychol. 2008, 100, 566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muter, V.; Hulme, C.; Snowling, M.J.; Stevenson, J. Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 2004, 40, 665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parrila, R.; Kirby, J.R.; McQuarrie, L. Articulation rate, naming speed, verbal short-term memory, and phonological awareness: Longitudinal predictors of early reading development? Sci. Stud. Read. 2004, 8, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landerl, K.; Freudenthaler, H.H.; Heene, M.; De Jong, P.F.; Desrochers, A.; Manolitsis, G.; Parrila, R.; Georgiou, G.K. Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming as longitudinal predictors of reading in five alphabetic orthographies with varying degrees of consistency. Sci. Stud. Read. 2019, 23, 220–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDougall, S.; Hulme, C.; Ellis, A.; Monk, A. Learning to read: The role of short-term memory and phonological skills. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1994, 58, 112–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Araújo, S.; Reis, A.; Petersson, K.M.; Faísca, L. Rapid automatized naming and reading performance: A meta-analysis. J. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 107, 868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caravolas, M.; Lervåg, A.; Defior, S.; Seidlová Málková, G.; Hulme, C. Different patterns, but equivalent predictors, of growth in reading in consistent and inconsistent orthographies. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 1398–1407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seymour, P.H.; Aro, M.; Erskine, J.M. Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. Br. J. Psychol. 2003, 94, 143–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reis, A.; Faísca, L.; Castro, S.L.; Petersson, K.M. Preditores da leitura ao longo da escolaridade: Alterações dinâmicas no papel da consciência fonológica e da nomeação rápida. In Proceedings of the Actas do VII Simpósio Nacional de Investigação em Psicologia, Braga, Portugal, 4–6 February 2010; Nogueira, C., Silva, I., Lima, L., Almeida, A.T., Cabecinhas, R., Gomes, R., Machado, C., Maia, A., Sampaio, A., Taveira, M.C., Eds.; Universidade do Minho: Braga, Portugal, 2010; pp. 3117–3132. [Google Scholar]
- Vaessen, A.; Bertrand, D.; Tóth, D.; Csépe, V.; Faísca, L.; Reis, A.; Blomert, L. Cognitive development of fluent word reading does not qualitatively differ between transparent and opaque orthographies. J. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 102, 827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torgesen, J.K.; Wagner, R.K.; Rashotte, C.A.; Burgess, S.; Hecht, S. Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second-to fifth-grade children. Sci. Stud. Read. 1997, 1, 161–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vaessen, A.; Blomert, L. Long-term cognitive dynamics of fluent reading development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2010, 105, 213–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Georgiou, G.K.; Inoue, T.; Papadopoulos, T.C.; Parrila, R. Examining the growth trajectories and cognitive predictors of reading in a consistent orthography: Evidence from a 10-year longitudinal study. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2021, 42, 1287–1311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirby, J.R.; Georgiou, G.K.; Martinussen, R.; Parrila, R. Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Read. Res. Q. 2010, 45, 341–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manolitsis, G.; Georgiou, G.K.; Parrila, R. Revisiting the home literacy model of reading development in an orthographically consistent language. Learn. Instr. 2011, 21, 496–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hulme, C.; Bowyer-Crane, C.; Carroll, J.M.; Duff, F.J.; Snowling, M.J. The causal role of phoneme awareness and letter-sound knowledge in learning to read: Combining intervention studies with mediation analyses. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 572–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervåg, M.; Lyster, S.A.H.; Hulme, C. Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mailing, A.S.B.; Juul, H.; Gejl, A.K.; Damsgaard, L.; Wienecke, J.; Nielsen, A.M.V. Word reading, letter knowledge, and memory skills in Danish children (6-year-olds). Scand. J. Educ. Res. 2022, 66, 1237–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, F.L.; Tortorelli, L.S.; Invernizzi, M.A. An investigation of factors associated with letter-sound knowledge at kindergarten entry. Early Child. Res. Q. 2014, 29, 182–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clayton, F.J.; West, G.; Sears, C.; Hulme, C.; Lervåg, A. A longitudinal study of early reading development: Letter-sound knowledge, phoneme awareness and RAN, but not letter-sound integration, predict variations in reading development. Sci. Stud. Read. 2020, 24, 91–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landerl, K.; Castles, A.; Parrila, R. Cognitive precursors of reading: A cross-linguistic perspective. Sci. Stud. Read. 2022, 26, 111–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Georgiou, G.K.; Torppa, M.; Manolitsis, G.; Lyytinen, H.; Parrila, R. Longitudinal predictors of reading and spelling across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Read. Writ. 2012, 25, 321–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adams, M.J. Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Vadasy, P.F.; Sanders, E.A. Introducing grapheme-phoneme correspondences (GPCs): Exploring rate and complexity in phonics instruction for kindergarteners with limited literacy skills. Read. Writ. 2021, 34, 109–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mateus, M.; Brito, A.; Duarte, I.; Hub Faria, I. Gramática da Língua Portuguesa: Elementos para a Descrição da Estrutura, Funcionamento e Uso do Português Actual (Portuguese Language Grammar: Elements for a Description of the Strucuture, Functioning and Use of Current Portuguese); Livraria Almedina: Coimbra, Portugal, 1983. (In Portuguese) [Google Scholar]
- Castro, I.; Duarte, I. Crítica do Acordo de 1986 (Critique of the 1986 agreement). In A Demanda da Ortografia Portuguesa: Comentário do Acordo Ortográfico de 1986 e Subsídios para a Compreensão da Questão que se lhe Seguiu (In Search for the Portuguese Orthography: Commentary on the 1986 Orthographic Agreement and Contributions for Understanding the Questions that Followed It); Castro, I., Duarte, I., Leiria, I., Eds.; João Sá da Costa: Lisboa, Portugal, 1987; pp. 1–100. (In Portuguese) [Google Scholar]
- Mateus, M.; Andrade, E. The Phonology of Portuguese; University Press: Oxford, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Sprenger-Charolles, L.; Siegel, L.S.; Béchennec, D.; Serniclaes, W. Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading, and in spelling: A four-year longitudinal study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2003, 84, 194–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fernandes, S.; Ventura, P.; Querido, L.; Morais, J. Reading and spelling acquisition in European Portuguese: A preliminary study. Read. Writ. 2008, 21, 805–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ehri, L.C. What teachers need to know and do to teach letter–sounds, phonemic awareness, word reading, and phonics. Read. Teach. 2022, 76, 53–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernandes, S.; Querido, L.; Verhaeghe, A.; Marques, C.; Araújo, L. Reading development in European Portuguese: Relationships between oral reading fluency, vocabulary and reading comprehension. Read. Writ. 2017, 30, 1987–2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ehri, L.C. Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Sci. Stud. Read. 2005, 9, 167–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Larsen, L.; Schauber, S.K.; Kohnen, S.; Nickels, L.; McArthur, G. Children’s knowledge of single-and multiple-letter grapheme-phoneme correspondences: An exploratory study. Early Child. Res. Q. 2020, 51, 379–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blomert, L. The neural signature of orthographic–phonological binding in successful and failing reading development. Neuroimage 2011, 57, 695–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Atteveldt, N.; Ansari, D. How symbols transform brain function: A review in memory of Leo Blomert. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2014, 3, 44–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuchs, L.S.; Fuchs, D.; Hosp, M.D.; Jenkins, J. Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Sci. Stud. Read. 2001, 5, 239–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torgesen, J.K.; Rashotte, C.A.; Alexander, A. Principles of fluency instruction in reading: Relationships with established empirical outcomes. In Dyslexia, Fluency, and the Brain; Wolf, M., Ed.; York Press: Parkton, MD, USA, 2001; pp. 333–356. [Google Scholar]
- Samuels, S.J.; Farstrup, A.E. What Research Has to Say about Fluency Instruction; International Reading Association: Newark, DE, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Daane, M.C.; Campbell, J.R.; Grigg, W.S.; Goodman, M.J.; Oranje, A. Fourth-Grade Students Reading Aloud: NAEP 2002 Special Study of Oral Reading; US Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics, Government Printing Office: Washington, DC, USA, 2005.
- Jenkins, J.R.; Fuchs, L.S.; van den Broek, P.; Espin, C.; Deno, S.L. Sources of individual differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. J. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 95, 719–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernandes, S.; Querido, L.; Verhaeghe, A.; Araújo, L. What is the relationship between reading prosody and reading comprehension in European Portuguese? Evidence from grades 2 to 5. J. Res. Read. 2018, 41, S102–S129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuchs, L.S.; Fuchs, D.; Maxwell, L. The validity of informal measures of reading comprehension. Remedial Spec. Educ. 1988, 9, 20–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hudson, R.F.; Pullen, P.C.; Lane, H.B.; Torgesen, J.K. The complex nature of reading fluency: A multidimensional view. Read. Writ. Q. 2009, 25, 4–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hudson, R.F.; Torgesen, J.K.; Lane, H.B.; Turner, S.J. Relations among reading skills and sub-skills and text-level reading proficiency in developing readers. Read. Writ. 2012, 25, 483–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ouellette, G.; Beers, A. A not-so-simple view of reading: How oral vocabulary and visual-word recognition complicate the story. Read. Writ. 2010, 23, 189–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stahl, S. Vocabulary Development (From Reading Research to Practice, V.2); Brookline Books: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, K.E.; Cunningham, A.E. Where does knowledge come from? Specific associations between print exposure and information acquisition. J. Educ. Psychol. 1993, 85, 211–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alloway, T.P.; Gathercole, S.E. Working memory and short-term sentence recall in young children. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2005, 17, 207–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinez-Perez, T.; Majerus, S.; Poncelet, M. The contribution of short-term memory for serial order to early reading acquisition: Evidence from a longitudinal study. J. Exp. Child Psychol 2012, 111, 708–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ellis, R. Instructed Second Language Acquisition; Basil Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Kosmidis, M.H.; Zafiri, M.; Politimou, N. Literacy versus formal schooling: Influence on working memory. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2011, 26, 575–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Silva, C.; Faísca, L.; Ingvar, M.; Petersson, K.M.; Reis, A. Literacy: Exploring working memory systems. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2012, 34, 369–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simões, M. Investigações no Âmbito da Aferição Nacional do Teste das Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven (M.P.C.R.). Ph.D. Thesis, Universidade de Coimbra, Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ciências da Educação, Coimbra, Portugal, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Dunn, L.W.; Padilla, E.; Lugo, D.; Dunn, L.M. Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody; American Guidance Service: Circle Pines, MN, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Hanten, G.; Martin, R.C. Contributions of phonological and semantic short-term memory to sentence processing: Evidence from two cases of closed head injury in children. J. Mem. Lang. 2000, 43, 335–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, R.C.; Shelton, J.R.; Yaffee, L.S. Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. J. Mem. Lang. 1994, 33, 83–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engelkamp, J.; Rummer, R. Syntaktische Struktur und Wortlänge im Satzrecall. Z. Exp. Psychol. 1999, 46, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willis, C.S.; Gathercole, S.E. Phonological short-term memory contributions to sentence processing in young children. Memory 2001, 9, 349–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schneider, W.; Eschman, A.; Zuccolotto, A. E-Prime User’s Guide: Version 1.0; Psychology Software Tools Inc.: Pittsburgh, PA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, W.; Eschman, A.; Zuccolotto, A. E-Prime Reference Guide: Version 1.0; Psychology Software Tools Inc.: Pittsburgh, PA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Querido, L.; Fernandes, S.; Verhaeghe, A. Orthographic knowledge, and reading and spelling: A longitudinal study in an Intermediate Depth Orthography. Span. J. Psychol. 2021, 24, e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernández-Huerta, J. Medidas sencillas de lecturabilidad (simple readability measures). Consigna 1959, 214, 29–32. [Google Scholar]
- Lyytinen, H.; Erskine, J.; Hämäläinen, J.; Torppa, M.; Ronimus, M. Dyslexia—Early identification and prevention: Highlights from the Jyväskylä longitudinal study of dyslexia. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2015, 2, 330–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Castells, N.; Solé, I. Early literacy development in Catalan: Characteristics and instructional influence. Investig. Práticas Estud. Nat. Educ. 2013, 3, 3–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aravena, S.; Tijms, J.; Snellings, P.; van der Molen, M.W. Predicting individual differences in reading and spelling skill with artificial script–based letter–speech sound training. J. Learn. Disabil. 2018, 51, 552–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baddeley, A. The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grade 2 | Grade 3 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beginning (November–January) | End (April–June) | End (April–June) | |||||||
Mean | SD | Range | Mean | SD | Range | Mean | SD | Range | |
RCPM | 28.90 | 4.08 | 20–36 | ||||||
Peabody Vocabulary | 124.66 | 11.04 | 90–145 | ||||||
Sentence Recall | 0.72 | 0.11 | 0.31–0.92 | ||||||
Grapheme Sounding (accuracy) | 0.90 | 0.08 | 0.65–1 | ||||||
Time per correct item a | 1.31 | 0.39 | 0.68–2.85 | ||||||
Phoneme Deletion (accuracy) | 0.85 | 0.19 | 0.20–1 | ||||||
Time per correct item b | 3227.55 | 1317.82 | 1273–7068 | ||||||
Text Reading Fluency (rate) | 104.42 | 27.02 | 34.85–182.01 | ||||||
Word Reading Fluency (rate) | 76.89 | 18.22 | 34.18–124.06 | ||||||
Pseudoword Reading Fluency (rate) | 45.29 | 12.38 | 20.30–72.74 |
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beginning of Grade 2 | |||||||||||
1 | RCPM | 1 | |||||||||
2 | Vocabulary | 0.199 | 1 | ||||||||
End of Grade 2 | |||||||||||
3 | Sentence Recall | 0.223 | 0.424 ** | 1 | |||||||
4 | Phonemic Awareness accuracy | 0.131 | 0.187 | 0.387 ** | 1 | ||||||
5 | Grapheme Sounding accuracy | 0.179 | −0.243 | 0.156 | 0.020 | 1 | |||||
6 | Phonemic Awareness time per correct item | −0.098 | −0.079 | −0.001 | −0.210 | −0.204 | 1 | ||||
7 | Grapheme Sounding time per correct item | −0.031 | 0.001 | −0.033 | 0.036 | −0.650 ** | 0.174 | 1 | |||
End of Grade 3 | |||||||||||
8 | Pseudoword Reading Fluency | 0.048 | 0.172 | 0.205 | 0.285 * | 0.244 | −0.237 | −0.441 ** | 1 | ||
9 | Word Reading Fluency | 0.070 | 0.309 * | 0.226 | 0.237 | 0.210 | −0.280 * | −0.531 ** | 0.835 ** | 1 | |
10 | Text Reading Fluency | 0.204 | 0.288 * | 0.382 ** | 0.342 ** | 0.250 | −0.290 * | −0.432 ** | 0.776 ** | 0.768 ** | 1 |
Decoding Fluency | Word Fluency | Text Fluency | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step | β | ΔR2 | β | ΔR2 | β | ΔR2 | |
1 | RCPM Vocabulary | −0.082 | 0.03 | −0.080 | 0.10 a | 0.045 | 0.11 * |
0.225 | 0.385 ** | 0.246 | |||||
2 | Sentence recall | −0.020 | 0.02 | −0.041 | 0.01 | 0.135 | 0.07 * |
3 | Phonemic Awareness | 0.255 | 0.05 | 0.185 | 0.02 | 0.233 | 0.04 |
4 | Grapheme Sounding | 0.312 * | 0.08 * | 0.321 * | 0.08 * | 0.276 * | 0.06 * |
Total R2 | 0.18 * | 0.22 * | 0.28 ** |
Decoding Fluency | Word Fluency | Text Fluency | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step | β | ΔR2 | β | ΔR2 | β | ΔR2 | |
1 | RCPM Vocabulary | −0.035 | 0.03 | −0.039 | 0.10 a | 0.080 | 0.11 * |
0.101 | 0.256 * | 0.131 | |||||
2 | Sentence recall | 0.156 | 0.02 | 0.110 | 0.01 | 0.296 * | 0.07 * |
3 | Phonemic Awareness | −0.161 | 0.05 | −0.177 | 0.07 * | −0.205 | 0.07 * |
4 | Grapheme Sounding | −0.409 ** | 0.16 ** | −0.498 *** | 0.24 *** | −0.385 ** | 0.14 ** |
Total R2 | 0.27 ** | 0.42 *** | 0.39 *** |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Fernandes, S.; Querido, L.; Verhaeghe, A. Learning to Read in an Intermediate Depth Orthography: The Longitudinal Role of Grapheme Sounding on Different Types of Reading Fluency. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 396. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050396
Fernandes S, Querido L, Verhaeghe A. Learning to Read in an Intermediate Depth Orthography: The Longitudinal Role of Grapheme Sounding on Different Types of Reading Fluency. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(5):396. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050396
Chicago/Turabian StyleFernandes, Sandra, Luís Querido, and Arlette Verhaeghe. 2024. "Learning to Read in an Intermediate Depth Orthography: The Longitudinal Role of Grapheme Sounding on Different Types of Reading Fluency" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 5: 396. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050396
APA StyleFernandes, S., Querido, L., & Verhaeghe, A. (2024). Learning to Read in an Intermediate Depth Orthography: The Longitudinal Role of Grapheme Sounding on Different Types of Reading Fluency. Behavioral Sciences, 14(5), 396. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050396