Not All Levels of Social Re-Inclusion Allow for Recovery from Negative Outcomes of Social Exclusion: The Moderating Role of Self-Esteem
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Social Re-Inclusion after Exclusion
1.2. Self-Esteem
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Overview
- Pre-tests: Prior to the start of the Cyberball game, all participants completed the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) and the Self-Esteem Scale.
- Stage 1: Participants played the Cyberball game under either the exclusion or inclusion condition.
- Mid-tests: At the end of Stage 1, all participants repeated the PANAS and completed a Person Perception Questionnaire. Afterwards, participants under the inclusion condition were debriefed and dismissed, and the excluded participants continued to Stage 2.
- Stage 2: Formerly excluded participants played the Cyberball game under either the token, replica, MOR, or HOR condition.
- Post-tests: At the end of the second game, participants completed the PANAS for a third time and the Person Perception Questionnaire for a second time.
2.2. Participants
2.3. Measures
2.4. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Stage 1: Being Excluded
3.2. Stage 2: Being Re-Included
4. Discussion
4.1. The Impact of Social Exclusion
4.2. Recovery Effect of Social Re-Inclusion
4.3. The Moderating Role of Self-Esteem
4.4. Limitations
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Appendix A.1. PANAS
- interested
- distressed
- excited
- upset
- strong
- guilty
- scared
- hostile
- enthusiastic
- proud
- irritable
- alert
- ashamed
- inspired
- nervous
- determined
- attentive
- jittery
- active
- afraid
Appendix A.2. Person Perception
- How friendly do you feel toward other individuals involved in the game?
- How warm do you feel toward other individuals in the game?
- How hostile do you feel toward other individuals in the game?
- How angry do you feel toward other individuals who are playing the game?
Appendix A.3. Self-Esteem Scale
- On the whole, I am satisfied with myself.
- At times I think I am no good at all.
- I feel that I have a number of good qualities.
- I am able to do things as well as most other people.
- I feel I do not have much to be proud of.
- I certainly feel useless at times.
- I feel that I am a person of worth.
- I wish I could have more respect for myself.
- All in all, I am inclined to think that I am a failure.
- I take a positive attitude toward myself.
References
- Wesselmann, E.D.; Grzybowski, M.R.; Steakley-Freeman, D.M.; DeSouza, E.R.; Nezlek, J.B.; Williams, K.D. Social exclusion in everyday life. In Social Exclusion: Psychological Approaches to Understanding and Reducing Its Impact; Riva, P., Eck, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2016; pp. 3–23. [Google Scholar]
- Lara, E.; Caballero, F.F.; Rico-Uribe, L.A.; Olaya, B.; Haro, J.M.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L.; Miret, M. Are loneliness and social isolation associated with cognitive decline? Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2019, 34, 1613–1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thomas, V.L.; Saenger, C. Feeling excluded? Join the crowd: How social exclusion affects approach behavior toward consumer-dense retail environments. J. Bus. Res. 2020, 120, 520–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nuñez, T.R.; Radtke, T.; Eimler, S.C. A third-person perspective on phubbing: Observing smartphone-induced social exclusion generates negative affect, stress, and derogatory attitudes. Cyberpsychology 2020, 14, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cielo, F.; Ulberg, R.; Di Giacomo, D. Psychological impact of the COVID-19 outbreak on mental health outcomes among youth: A rapid narrative review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 6067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Clair, R.; Gordon, M.; Kroon, M.; Reilly, C. The effects of social isolation on well-being and life satisfaction during pandemic. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2021, 28, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ng, S.H.; Cheung, C.K. COVID-19 pandemic and positive ageing. Psychol. Dev. Soc. 2023, 35, 251–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, K.D.; Jarvis, B. Cyberball: A program for use in research on interpersonal ostracism and acceptance. Behav. Res. Methods 2006, 38, 174–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartgerink, C.H.; Van Beest, I.; Wicherts, J.M.; Williams, K.D. The ordinal effects of ostracism: A meta-analysis of 120 Cyberball studies. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0127002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heeren, A.; Dricot, L.; Billieux, J.; Philippot, P.; Grynberg, D.; De Timary, P.; Maurage, P. Correlates of Social Exclusion in Social Anxiety Disorder: An fMRI study. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Warburton, J.; Ng, S.H.; Shardlow, S.M. Social inclusion in an ageing world: Introduction to the special issue. Ageing Soc. 2013, 33, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fakoya, O.A.; McCorry, N.K.; Donnelly, M. Loneliness and social isolation interventions for older adults: A scoping review of reviews. BMC Public Health 2020, 20, 129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gidron, N.; Hall, P.A. Populism as a problem of social integration. Comp. Polit. Stud. 2020, 53, 1027–1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelly, S. Gender equality and social inclusion (GESI) in village development. Leg. Brief 2021, 10, 245–252. [Google Scholar]
- Lambert, S.R. Do MOOCs contribute to student equity and social inclusion? A systematic review 2014–2018. Comput. Educ. 2020, 145, 103693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mennini, F.S.; Trabucco Aurilio, M.; Gazzillo, S.; Nardone, C.; Sciattella, P.; Marcellusi, A.; Migliorini, R.; Sciannamea, V.; Piccioni, A.; Bolcato, M.; et al. An analysis of the social and economic costs of breast cancer in Italy. Int. J. Environ. 2021, 18, 9005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Desmet, P.T.M.; De Cremer, D.; van Dijk, E. In money we trust? The use of financial compensations to repair trust in the aftermath of distributive harm. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process 2011, 114, 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haesevoets, T.; Folmer, C.R.; De Cremer, D.; Van Hiel, A. Money isn’t all that matters: The use of financial compensation and apologies to preserve relationships in the aftermath of distributive harm. J. Econ. Psychol. 2013, 35, 95–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haesevoets, T.; Van Hiel, A.; Folmer, C.R.; De Cremer, D. What money can’t buy: The psychology of financial overcompensation. J. Econ. Psychol. 2014, 42, 83–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haesevoets, T.; Van Hiel, A.; De Cremer, D.; Delplanque, J.; De Coninck, S.; Van Overwalle, F. The myth of the extra mile: Psychological processes and neural mechanisms underlying overcompensation effects. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2022, 100, 104282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Körner, R.; Schütz, A. Power, self-esteem, and body image. Soc. Psychol. 2023, 54, 136–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reitz, A.K. Self-esteem development and life events: A review and integrative process framework. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2022, 16, e12709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- James, W. The Principles of Psychology; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Leary, M.R. The need to belong, the sociometer, and the pursuit of relational value: Unfinished business. Self Identity 2021, 20, 126–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greenwald, A.G.; Lai, C.K. Implicit social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2020, 71, 419–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Orth, U.; Robins, R.W. Is high self-esteem beneficial? Revisiting a classic question. Am. Psychol. 2022, 77, 5–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moradi, M.; Mozaffari, H.; Askari, M.; Azadbakht, L. Association between overweight/obesity with depression, anxiety, low self-esteem, and body dissatisfaction in children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 2021, 62, 555–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lee, D.S.; Way, B.M. Perceived social support and chronic inflammation: The moderating role of self-esteem. Health Psychol. 2019, 38, 563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sadat, A.A.; Shameem, R.G.; Masoumeh, A.; Mansour, A. Relationship between body mass index, body image, and fear of negative evaluation: Moderating role of self-esteem. Health Psychol. Open 2018, 5, 2055102918774251. [Google Scholar]
- Arslan, G. Mediating role of the self-esteem and resilience in the association between social exclusion and life satisfaction among adolescents. Pers. Individ. 2019, 151, 109514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keiichi, O.; Yasumasa, O.; Ken’Ichiro, N.; Hiroshi, N.; Shinpei, Y.; Sigeto, Y.; Shuhei, Y.; Mitsuhiro, U. Does low self-esteem enhance social pain? the relationship between trait self-esteem and anterior cingulate cortex activation induced by ostracism. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neur. 2010, 5, 385–391. [Google Scholar]
- Kong, F.; Zhao, J.; You, X. Self-esteem as mediator and moderator of the relationship between social support and subjective well-Being among Chinese University Students. Soc. Indic. Res. 2013, 112, 151–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, (Rev. ed.); Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.G.; Buchner, A.G. G* power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, M.; Cai, B.; Wu, Y.; Dai, X. The factor structure of Chinese Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale affected by item statement method. Psychol. Explor. 2010, 30, 63–68. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg, M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, W.D.; Diao, J.; Schnick, C.J. The cross-cultural measurement of positive and negative affect: Examining the dimensionality of PANAS. Psychol. Sci. 2004, 27, 77–79. [Google Scholar]
- Watson, D.; Clark, L.A.; Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 1063–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hayes, A.F. PROCESS: A Versatile Computational Tool for Observed Variable Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling [White Paper]. 2012. Available online: https://is.muni.cz/el/1423/podzim2014/PSY704/50497615/hayes_2012_navod_process.pdf (accessed on 19 January 2024).
- Hayes, A.F. Model Templates for PROCESS for SPSS and SAS [White Paper]. 2013. Available online: http://www.afhayes.com/public/templates.pdf (accessed on 7 March 2014).
- De Gennaro, M.; Krumhuber, E.G.; Lucas, G. Effectiveness of an empathic chatbot in combating adverse effects of social exclusion on mood. Front. Psychol. 2020, 10, 3061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jobst, A.; Sabass, L.; Palagyi, A.; Bauriedl-Schmidt, C.; Mauer, M.C.; Sarubin, N.; Buchheim, A.; Renneberg, B.; Falkai, P.; Zill, P.; et al. Effects of social exclusion on emotions and oxytocin and cortisol levels in patients with chronic depression. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2015, 60, 170–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fredickson, B.L. The value of positive emotions. Am. Sci. 2003, 91, 330–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Branch, K. Guide to Social Impact Assessment: A Framework for Assessing Social Change; Routledge: London, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Abrams, D.; Hogg, M.A.; Marques, J.M. (Eds.) The Social Psychology of Inclusion and Exclusion; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Randel, A.E. Inclusion in the Workplace: A Review and Research Agenda. Group Organ. Manag. 2023, 10596011231175578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wesselmann, E.D.; Williams, K.D. Social life and social death: Inclusion, ostracism, and rejection in groups. Group Process Intergroup Relat. 2017, 20, 693–706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aguinis, H.; Beaty, J.C.; Boik, R.J.; Pierce, C.A. Effect Size and Power in Assessing Moderating Effects of Categorical Variables Using Multiple Regression: A 30-Year Review. J. Appl. Psychol. 2005, 90, 94–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfundmair, M.; Graupmann, V.; Du, H.; Frey, D.; Aydin, N. Suddenly included: Cultural differences in experiencing re-inclusion. Int. J. Psychol. 2015, 50, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- He, Z.; Muhlert, N.; Elliott, R. Emotion regulation of social exclusion: A cross-cultural study. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2021, 8, 173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Latina, D.; Goreis, A.; Sajko, P.; Kothgassner, O.D. Does being ignored on WhatsApp hurt? A pilot study on the effect of a newly developed ostracism task for adolescents. J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12, 2056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zamperini, A.; Menegatto, M.; Mostacchi, M.; Barbagallo, S.; Testoni, I. Loss of close relationships and loss of religious belonging as cumulative ostracism: From social death to social resurrection. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Re-Inclusion | Number of Supporters Out of Three | Probability of Receiving Balls from Each Supporting Player | Re-Inclusion Levels (Supporters × Probability) |
---|---|---|---|
Under (token) re-inclusion | 1 | 2/3 | 2/3 |
Replica re-inclusion | 3 | 1/3 | 3/3 |
Moderate over- re-inclusion (MOR) | 2 | 2/3 | 4/3 |
High over- re-inclusion (HOR) | 3 | 2/3 | 6/3 |
Experimental Conditions | Positive Affect | Negative Affect | Person Perception | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pre-Test | Mid-Test | Pre-Test | Mid-Test | F | H | ||
Inclusion n = 30 | M | 30.69 | 27.27 | 18.47 | 15.63 | 7.23 | 3.43 |
SD | 5.95 | 7.25 | 6.25 | 5.16 | 2.13 | 1.78 | |
Exclusion n = 124 | M | 30.23 | 27.18 | 19 | 17.65 | 5.81 | 4.77 |
SD | 6.33 | 7.18 | 6.56 | 6.14 | 2.17 | 2.27 |
Re-Inclusion | Friendliness | Hostility | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mid-Test | Post-Test | Mid-Test | Post-Test | ||
Token (n = 33) | M | 5.35 | 6.88 | 5.15 | 3.67 |
SD | 2.3 | 1.67 | 2.56 | 1.73 | |
Replica (n = 31) | M | 6.13 | 8.58 | 4.81 | 2.71 |
SD | 2.01 | 1.52 | 2.21 | 1.3 | |
Moderately over (MOR) (n = 28) | M | 6.52 | 8.46 | 3.86 | 2.54 |
SD | 2.2 | 1.14 | 1.89 | 0.96 | |
Highly over (HOR) (n = 29) | M | 5.33 | 8.28 | 5.17 | 2.59 |
SD | 2.01 | 1.69 | 2.17 | 0.98 |
Re-Inclusion | Positive Affect | Negative Affect | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mid-Test | Post-Test | Mid-Test | Post-Test | ||
Token (n = 34) | M | 26.44 | 26.5 | 17.06 | 16.91 |
SD | 6.2 | 7.46 | 5.35 | 4.83 | |
Replica (n = 31) | M | 27.65 | 29.26 | 18.87 | 17.68 |
SD | 6.74 | 6.93 | 6.72 | 6.1 | |
MOR (n = 29) | M | 29.41 | 30.93 | 16.86 | 15.66 |
SD | 6.93 | 6.42 | 4.98 | 5.6 | |
HOR (n = 30) | M | 25.37 | 28.23 | 17.87 | 14.87 |
SD | 8.52 | 9.12 | 7.34 | 5.88 |
Predictors | b | t | |
Constant | 0.33 | 2.86 ** | |
Mid-test negative affect | 0.79 | 15.38 *** | |
Social re-inclusion level | −0.14 | −3.19 ** | |
Self-esteem | 0.12 | 1.1 | |
Social re-inclusion level × Self-esteem | −0.10 | −2.21 * | |
R2 | 0.727 | ||
F | 77.91 *** | ||
Conditional effects | effect | t | |
High self-esteem | −0.24 | −4.02 *** | |
Medium self-esteem | −0.14 | −3.21 ** | |
Low self-esteem | −0.03 | −0.51 |
Includers Distribute Their Ball Tosses | |||
---|---|---|---|
Number of includers | Disproportionately more to participant (High impact) | Equally (Medium impact) | Disproportionately fewer to participant (Low impact) |
Entire group (High impact) | Consensual enthusiastic re-inclusion (High over-re-inclusion) | Consensual equal re-inclusion (Replica re-inclusion) | Consensual casual re-inclusion |
Majority (Medium impact) | Majority enthusiastic re-inclusion (Moderate over-re-inclusion) | Majority equal re-inclusion | Majority casual re-inclusion |
Minority (Low impact) | Minority enthusiastic re-inclusion (Token re-inclusion) | Minority equal re-inclusion | Minority casual re-inclusion |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kuang, B.; Ng, S.H.; Peng, S.; Hu, P.; Wei, Y. Not All Levels of Social Re-Inclusion Allow for Recovery from Negative Outcomes of Social Exclusion: The Moderating Role of Self-Esteem. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020088
Kuang B, Ng SH, Peng S, Hu P, Wei Y. Not All Levels of Social Re-Inclusion Allow for Recovery from Negative Outcomes of Social Exclusion: The Moderating Role of Self-Esteem. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(2):88. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020088
Chicago/Turabian StyleKuang, Beibei, Sik Hung Ng, Shenli Peng, Ping Hu, and Yanqiu Wei. 2024. "Not All Levels of Social Re-Inclusion Allow for Recovery from Negative Outcomes of Social Exclusion: The Moderating Role of Self-Esteem" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 2: 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020088
APA StyleKuang, B., Ng, S. H., Peng, S., Hu, P., & Wei, Y. (2024). Not All Levels of Social Re-Inclusion Allow for Recovery from Negative Outcomes of Social Exclusion: The Moderating Role of Self-Esteem. Behavioral Sciences, 14(2), 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020088