Constraints on Acceleration in Bilingual Development: Evidence from Word Segmentation by Spanish Learning Infants
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Previous Proposals to Account for Acceleration
1.1.1. Language Dominance Accounts
1.1.2. Perceptual Salience Accounts
1.1.3. Domain-General Accounts
1.1.4. Frequency Effects
1.2. The Current Study
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Materials and Methods
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Stimuli
2.1.3. Procedure and Design
2.2. Analyses
2.3. Results
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Materials and Methods
Participants
3.2. Stimuli, Procedure and Design, and Analysis
3.3. Results and Discussion
4. General Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Gancho passage Ese gancho le rasgó la camiseta. Se necesita un gancho para pescar. Mi madre colgó el abrigo en el gancho. El ladrón le hirió con el gancho. Hay un gancho detrás de la puerta. El gancho sobresalía de la pared. |
Salsa passage Esa salsa era muy picante. Me encanta la salsa de mi abuela. Se ensució la ropa con una salsa. No es tan difícil escoger una salsa. Busca la salsa para la pasta. La salsa bechamel es mi favorita. |
Gesto passage Un gesto bonito siempre gusta. No me gustó el gesto que me hizo. Le dije que viniera con un gesto. Cuando lo echó le hizo este gesto. Juan le hizo un gesto de aprobación. El gesto de Mona Lisa es un misterio. |
Venda passage La venda le tapaba la rodilla. Quítate ya la venda de los ojos. Se cubrió la herida con esa venda. La enfermera le puso una venda. Llevaba una venda en la mano. La venda le apretaba demasiado. |
References
- Cutler, A.; Carter, D.M. The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary. Comput. Speech Lang. 1987, 2, 133–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cutler, A.; Norris, D. The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1988, 14, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, E.K.; Jusczyk, P.W. Word segmentation by 8-month-olds: When speech cues count more than statistics. J. Mem. Lang. 2001, 44, 548–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jusczyk, P.W.; Houston, D.M.; Newsome, M. The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cogn. Psychol. 1999, 39, 159–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Höhle, B.; Weissenborn, J. German-learning infants’ ability to detect unstressed closed-class elements in continuous speech. Dev. Sci. 2003, 6, 122–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Houston, D.M.; Jusczyk, P.W.; Kuijpers, C.; Coolen, R.; Cutler, A. Cross-language word segmentation by 9-month-olds. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2000, 7, 504–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mateu, V.; Sundara, M. Spanish input accelerates bilingual infants’ segmentation of English words. Cognition 2022, 218, 104936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, J.; Genesee, F. Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 1996, 18, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Argyri, E.; Sorace, A. Crosslinguistic influence and language dominance in older bilingual children. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2007, 10, 79–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernardini, P.; Schlyter, S. Growing syntactic structure and code-mixing in the weaker language: The Ivy Hypothesis. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2004, 7, 49–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bosch, L.; Costa, A.; Sebastián-Gallés, N. First and second language vowel perception in early bilinguals. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 12, 189–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosjean, F. Life with Two Languages: An Introduction to Bilingualism; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Lleó, C.; Cortés, S.; Benet, A. Contact-induced phonological changes in the Catalan spoken in Barcelona. Lang. Contact Contact Lang. 2008, 7, 185–212. [Google Scholar]
- Paradis, J. Do bilingual two-year-olds have separate phonological systems? Int. J. Biling. 2001, 5, 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Dijk, C.; Dijkstra, T.; Unsworth, S. Cross-linguistic influence during online sentence processing in bilingual children. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2022, 25, 691–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yip, V.; Matthews, S. Syntactic transfer in a Cantonese–English bilingual child. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2000, 3, 193–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lleó, C.; Kuchenbrandt, I.; Kehoe, M.; Trujillo, C. Syllable final consonants in Spanish and German monolingual and bilingual acquisition. In Vulnerable Domains in Multilingualism; Muller, N., Ed.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2003; pp. 191–220. [Google Scholar]
- Lleó, C.; Rakow, M. The prosody of early two-word utterances by German and Spanish monolingual and bilingual children. In Interfaces in Multilingualism; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2006; pp. 1–26. [Google Scholar]
- Delattre, P. Comparing the Phonetic Features of English, German, Spanish and French; Julius Groos Verlag: Heidelberg, Germany, 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Stahnke, J. The Acquisition of French Determiners by Bilingual Children: A Prosodic Account. Languages 2022, 7, 200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kupisch, T. Determiners in bilingual German–Italian children: Relation between language influence and language dominance. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2007, 10, 57–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kupisch, T. Dominance, mixing and cross-linguistic influence. In First Language Acquisition of Morphology and Syntax: Perspectives across Languages and Learners; Guijarro-Fuentes, P., Larrañaga, M.P., Clibbens, K., Eds.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2008; pp. 209–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polka, L.; Orena, A.J.; Sundara, M.; Worrall, J. Segmenting words from fluent speech during infancy in a bilingual context. Dev. Sci. 2017, 20, e12419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lleó, C.; Demuth, K. Prosodic constraints on grammatical morphemes: Crosslinguistic evidence from Germanic and Romance languages. In Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA, USA, 6–8 November 1998; Greenhill, A., Littlefield, H., Tano, C., Eds.; Cascadilla: Somerville, MA, USA, 1999; Volume 2, pp. 407–418. [Google Scholar]
- Ortega-Llebaria, M.; Prieto, P. Acoustic correlates of stress in Central Catalan and Castilian Spanish. Lang. Speech 2011, 54, 73–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beckman, M.E.; Edwards, J. Articulatory evidence for differentiating stress categories. In Phonological Structure and Phonetic Form Papers in Laboratory Phonology; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1994; pp. 7–33. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell, N.; Beckman, M.E. Accent, stress, and spectral tilt. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1997, 101, 3195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morrill, T. Acoustic correlates of stress in English adjective–noun compounds. Lang. Speech 2012, 55, 167–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Patel, R.; Niziolek, C.; Reilly, K.; Guenther, F.H. Prosodic adaptations to pitch perturbation in running speech. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2011, 54, 1051–1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Adesope, O.O.; Lavin, T.; Thompson, T.; Ungerleider, C. Systematic review and meta-analysis of cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Rev. Educ. Res. 2010, 80, 207–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barac, R.; Bialystok, E. Cognitive development of bilingual children. Lang. Teach. 2011, 44, 36–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E. Bilingualism in Development: Language, Literacy, and Cognition; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E. Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2009, 12, 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I.; Luk, G. Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. J. Neurolinguist. 2008, 21, 522–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Cat, C.; Gusnanto, A.; Serratrice, L. Identifying a threshold for the executive function advantage in bilingual children. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 2018, 40, 119–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vega, C.; Fernandez, M. Errors on the WCST correlate with language proficiency scores in Spanish-English bilingual children. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2011, 26, 158–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber, R.C.; Johnson, A.; Riccio, C.A.; Liew, J. Balanced bilingualism and executive functioning in children. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2016, 19, 425–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E.; Barac, R.; Blaye, A.; Poulin-Dubois, D. Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. J. Cogn. Dev. 2010, 11, 485–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conboy, B.T.; Sommerville, J.A.; Kuhl, P.K. Cognitive control factors in speech perception at 11 months. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 44, 1505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daily, L.Z.; Lovett, M.C.; Reder, L.M. Modeling individual differences in working memory performance: A source activation account. Cogn. Sci. 2001, 25, 315–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hendriks, P. Cognitive modeling of individual variation in reference production and comprehension. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kovelman, I.; Baker, S.A.; Petitto, L.A. Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2008, 11, 203–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moser, D.C.; Fridriksson, J.; Healy, E.W. Sentence comprehension and general working memory. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2007, 21, 147–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Service, E. Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sec. A 1992, 45, 21–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- St Clair-Thompson, H.L.; Gathercole, S.E. Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2006, 59, 745–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- van Rij, J.; van Rijn, H.; Hendriks, P. How WM load influences linguistic processing in adults: A computational model of pronoun interpretation in discourse. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2013, 5, 564–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Müller, N. Different sources of delay and acceleration in early child bilingualism. Z. Sprachwiss. 2017, 36, 7–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meisel, J.M. Code-switching in young bilingual children: The acquisition of grammatical constraints. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 1994, 16, 413–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meisel, J.M. The weaker language in early child bilingualism: Acquiring a first language as a second language? Appl. Psycholinguist. 2007, 28, 495–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Müller, N.; Patuto, M. Really competence-driven cross-linguistic influence in bilingual first language acquisiton? The role of the language combination. In Melanges Plurilingues Offerts a Suzanne Schlyter A L’occasion de son 65ème Anniversaire; Bernardini, P., Egerland, V., Granfeld, J., Eds.; Språk-och Litteraturcentrum, Lunds Universitet: Lund, Sweden, 2009; pp. 299–319. [Google Scholar]
- Clahsen, H.; Eisenbeiss, S.; Penke, M. Lexical learning in early syntactic development. Lang. Acquis. Lang. Disord. 1996, 14, 129–160. [Google Scholar]
- Kuhl, P.K.; Conboy, B.T.; Coffey-Corina, S.; Padden, D.; Rivera-Gaxiola, M.; Nelson, T. Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: New data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Bio. Sci. 2008, 363, 979–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Petitto, L.A.; Berens, M.S.; Kovelman, I.; Dubins, M.H.; Jasinska, K.; Shalinsky, M. The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain Lang. 2012, 121, 130–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- García-Sierra, A.; Rivera-Gaxiola, M.; Percaccio, C.R.; Conboy, B.T.; Romo, H.; Klarman, L.; Ortiz, S.; Kuhl, P.K. Bilingual language learning: An ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word production. J. Phon. 2011, 39, 546–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferjan Ramírez, N.; Ramírez, R.R.; Clarke, M.; Taulu, S.; Kuhl, P.K. Speech discrimination in 11-month-old bilingual and monolingual infants: A magnetoencephalography study. Dev. Sci. 2017, 20, e12427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rivera-Gaxiola, M.; Silva-Pereyra, J.; Kuhl, P.K. Brain potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and 11-month-old American infants. Dev. Sci. 2005, 8, 162–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Toro, J.M.; Sinnett, S.; Soto-Faraco, S. Speech segmentation by statistical learning depends on attention. Cognition 2005, 97, B25–B34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Weiss, D.J.; Gerfen, C.; Mitchel, A.D. Speech segmentation in a simulated bilingual environment: A challenge for statistical learning? Lang. Learn. Dev. 2009, 5, 30–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antovich, D.M.; Graf Estes, K. Learning across languages: Bilingual experience supports dual language statistical word segmentation. Dev. Sci. 2018, 21, e12548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Bree, E.H.; Verhagen, J.; Kerkhoff, A.O.; Doedens, W.J.; Unsworth, S. Language learning from inconsistent input: Bilingual and monolingual toddlers compared. Infant Child Dev. 2017, 26, e1996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kovács, Á.M.; Mehler, J. Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science 2009, 325, 611–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Singh, L.; Fu, C.S.; Rahman, A.A.; Hameed, W.B.; Sanmugam, S.; Agarwal, P.; Jiang, B.; Chong, Y.S.; Meaney, M.J.; Rifkin-Graboi, A. Back to basics: A bilingual advantage in infant visual habituation. Child Dev. 2015, 86, 294–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lleó, C.; Cortés, S. Modeling the outcome of language contact in the speech of German-Spanish and Catalan-Spanish bilingual children. Int. J. Sociol. Lang. 2013, 2013, 101–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tamburelli, M.; Sanoudaki, E.; Jones, G.; Sowinska, M. Acceleration in the bilingual acquisition of phonological structure: Evidence from Polish–English bilingual children. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2015, 18, 713–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kupisch, T. On the relation between input frequency and acquisition patterns from a cross-linguistic perspective. LOT Occas. Ser. 2004, 3, 281–292. [Google Scholar]
- Hsin, L. Accelerated acquisition in Spanish-English bilinguals: The structural transfer hypothesis. In Proceedings of the 29th West Coast Conference of Formal Linguistics, Tucson, AZ, USA, 22–24 April 2011; Choi, J., Hogue, A., Punske, J., Tat, D., Schertz, J., Trueman, A., Eds.; Cascadilla: Somerville, MA, USA, 2012; pp. 108–116. [Google Scholar]
- Brunetti, L.; Bott, S.; Costa, J.; Vallduví, E. A multilingual annotated corpus for the study of Information Structure 1. In Grammatik und Korpora 2009. Dritte Internationale Konferenz, Mannheim, Germany, 22–24 September 2009; Grammar & Corpora; Narr: Tübingen, Germany, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Gregory, M.L.; Michaelis, L.A. Topicalization and left-dislocation: A functional opposition revisited. J. Pragmat. 2001, 33, 1665–1706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slabakova, R. The effect of construction frequency and native transfer on second language knowledge of the syntax–discourse interface. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2015, 36, 671–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roark, B.; Demuth, K. Prosodic constraints and the learner’s environment: A corpus study. In Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA, USA, 1 November 1999; Howell, S.C., Fish, S.A., Keith-Lucas, T., Eds.; Cascadilla: Somerville, MA, USA, 2000; Volume 2, pp. 597–608. [Google Scholar]
- Bosch, L.; Figueras, M.; Teixidó, M.; Ramon-Casas, M. Rapid gains in segmenting fluent speech when words match the rhythmic unit: Evidence from infants acquiring syllable-timed languages. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pons, F.; Bosch, L. Stress pattern preference in Spanish-learning infants: The role of syllable weight. Infancy 2010, 15, 223–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sundara, M.; Mateu, V.E. Lexical stress constrains English-learning infants’ segmentation in a non-native language. Cognition 2018, 181, 105–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bergmann, C.; Tsuji, S.; Piccinini, P.E.; Lewis, M.L.; Braginsky, M.; Frank, M.C.; Cristia, A. Promoting replicability in developmental research through meta-analyses: Insights from language acquisition research. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 1996–2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sundara, M.; Scutellaro, A. Rhythmic distance between languages affects the development of speech perception in bilingual infants. J. Phon. 2011, 39, 505–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, v. 4.2.3; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2021; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 31 October 2023).
- Bürkner, P. Bayesian Item Response Modeling in R with brms and Stan. J. Stat. Softw. 2021, 100, 1–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lenth, R. Emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, Aka Least-Squares Means. R Package Version 1.8.7. 2023. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans (accessed on 31 October 2023).
- Marcus, G.F.; Pinker, S.; Ullman, M.; Hollander, M.; Rosen, T.J.; Xu, F.; Clahsen, H. Overregularization in language acquisition. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1992, 57, 1–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Clahsen, H.; Aveledo, F.; Roca, I. The development of regular and irregular verb inflection in Spanish child language. J. Child Lang. 2002, 29, 591–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Demuth, K. The role of frequency in language acquisition. In Frequency Effects in Language Acquisition: Defining the Limits of Frequency as an Explanatory Concept; Gagarina, N., Gülzow, I., Eds.; De Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2007; pp. 383–388. [Google Scholar]
- Perkins, L.; Feldman, N.H.; Lidz, J. The power of ignoring: Filtering input for argument structure acquisition. Cogn. Sci. 2022, 46, e13080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Culbertson, J.; Smolensky, P.; Wilson, C. Cognitive biases, linguistic universals, and constraint-based grammar learning. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2013, 5, 392–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perfors, A. When do memory limitations lead to regularization? An experimental and computational investigation. J. Mem. Lang. 2012, 67, 486–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saldana, C.; Smith, K.; Kirby, S.; Culbertson, J. Is regularization uniform across linguistic levels? Comparing learning and production of unconditioned probabilistic variation in morphology and word order. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2021, 17, 158–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hudson Kam, C.L.; Newport, E.L. Regularizing unpredictable variation: The roles of adult and child learners in language formation and change. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2005, 1, 151–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hudson Kam, C.L.; Newport, E.L. Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages. Cogn. Psychol. 2009, 59, 30–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, C. On productivity. Linguist. Var. Yearb. 2005, 5, 265–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, C. The Price of Linguistic Productivity: How Children Learn to Break the Rules of Language; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Perkins, L.; Hunter, T. Noise-tolerant learning as selection among deterministic grammatical hypotheses. Proc. Soc. Comput. Linguist. 2023, 6, 186–198. [Google Scholar]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Mateu, V.; Sundara, M. Constraints on Acceleration in Bilingual Development: Evidence from Word Segmentation by Spanish Learning Infants. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 108. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020108
Mateu V, Sundara M. Constraints on Acceleration in Bilingual Development: Evidence from Word Segmentation by Spanish Learning Infants. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(2):108. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020108
Chicago/Turabian StyleMateu, Victoria, and Megha Sundara. 2024. "Constraints on Acceleration in Bilingual Development: Evidence from Word Segmentation by Spanish Learning Infants" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 2: 108. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020108
APA StyleMateu, V., & Sundara, M. (2024). Constraints on Acceleration in Bilingual Development: Evidence from Word Segmentation by Spanish Learning Infants. Behavioral Sciences, 14(2), 108. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14020108