Next Article in Journal
Social Connectedness in Schizotypy: The Role of Cognitive and Affective Empathy
Next Article in Special Issue
Student Perceptions of the Resilience in a Confinement Due to COVID-19 in University of A Coruña: A Qualitative Research
Previous Article in Journal
Boredom Proneness and Rule-Breaking: A Persistent Relation One Year into the COVID-19 Pandemic
Previous Article in Special Issue
Knowledge, Attitude and Practice during the COVID-19 Pandemic in South-East Gabon
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Institutional Trust as a Protective Factor during the COVID-19 Pandemic in China

1
Faculty of Education, Henan Normal University, Xinxiang 453007, China
2
School of Education, Tianjin University, Tianjin 300072, China
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2022, 12(8), 252; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080252
Submission received: 14 June 2022 / Revised: 21 July 2022 / Accepted: 22 July 2022 / Published: 25 July 2022

Abstract

:
Previous research has demonstrated that institutional trust protects subjective well-being during pandemics. However, the potential mediation mechanisms underlying this linkage remain unclear. This study constructs a mediating model to investigate the effect of institutional trust on subjective well-being and the mediating roles of belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 in the Chinese context. To this end, we survey a sample of 881 participants. The results show that institutional trust, belief in a just world, fear of COVID-19, and subjective well-being (i.e., life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect) are significantly interrelated. The results also indicate a significant impact of institutional trust on life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect. Belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19, independently and in sequence, mediate the relationship between institutional trust and subjective well-being.

1. Introduction

The coronavirus disease that emerged in 2019 (COVID-19) has caused a health pandemic and still is one of the world’s most prominent issues today. The World Health Organization has reported, since the outbreak, over 523 million confirmed cases of COVID-19 and more than 6.2 million deaths [1]. The COVID-19 outbreak has severely threatened people’s physical and mental health [2,3,4,5]. Individuals have faced the worry of being infected and the pressure of interpersonal isolation during the COVID-19 crisis, resulting in increased depression and fear [6], severe sleep disruptions [7], and decreased subjective well-being [8]. Therefore, how individuals cope with the ongoing fear of COVID-19 and sustain their subjective well-being has become a crucial concern [9].
Institutional trust is pivotal in protecting subjective well-being during pandemics and disasters [6,10,11]. However, the mechanisms through which institutional trust relates to or influences subjective well-being in the context of the COVID-19 crisis remain unclear. This study addresses this research gap by focusing on the mediating roles of belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 in the relationship between institutional trust and subjective well-being among the Chinese population.
Subjective well-being, a primary mental health index, is usually described as cognitive and emotional evaluations of different aspects of life and involves life satisfaction and affects [12]. Prior research has demonstrated that institutional trust substantially contributes to subjective well-being [13,14,15]. The logic behind this link relates to theoretical accounts of social capital [16,17]. Institutional trust, a cognitive component of social capital, reflects the security and confidence of the individual in particular institutions such as the government, media, health institutions, and neighborhoods [15]. On the one hand, a high level of institutional trust mirrors sufficient social support and available resources, which prevent individuals from feeling powerless and helpless within a community and foster their subjective well-being [18]. On the other hand, institutional trust may improve quality of life and mental wellness by encouraging social participation and decreasing depression, anxiety, and stress [19]. Empirical research confirms these theoretical assumptions. For example, Ciziceno and Travaglino show that perceived trust in institutions such as the healthcare system and the government is significantly associated with life satisfaction [13]. Lee reveals that older adults’ institutional trust plays an essential role in buffering against the negative well-being outcomes of the COVID-19 [11]. Matsushima et al. have surveyed postpartum women during the COVID-19 pandemic, indicating that higher trust in neighbors is related to lower negative affect, namely, depression and fear [6]. Recent research shows that trust in government is significantly associated with Chinese participants’ fewer depressive symptoms and higher level of life satisfaction [20]. These studies suggest that institutional trust positively impacts individual subjective well-being, affecting life satisfaction and affect during the COVID-19 pandemic.
Institutional trust likely depends on belief in a just world, affecting subjective well-being indirectly. Belief in a just world has been defined as individuals’ views that they live in a just, stable, orderly, and predictable world, in which people receive what they deserve [21]. On the one hand, Zhang and Zhang propose that public institutions’ perceived trustworthiness helps Chinese people believe that the physical and social environments are just [15]. Empirical research has shown that trust in institutions (such as the government, employing units, and communities) is significantly associated with a firm belief in a just world [22,23]. On the other hand, the theoretical literature highlights that the belief in a just world prevents an individual from experiencing negative feelings, such as anxiety and depression, allowing people to feel negative life events less personally [24]. Those with a solid belief in a just world also have a lower perception of risk and interpret events from a positive perspective, maintaining subjective well-being even when they face hardships or stressful events. They tend to believe that they do not deserve negative outcomes [25]. Many empirical studies support a positive relationship between belief in a just world and subjective well-being [26,27,28]. For instance, the research by Li and Li shows that high level of belief in a just world is related to Chinese adolescents’ subjective well-being [29]. Li and colleagues also report that belief in a just world makes significant contributions to Chinese university students’ life satisfaction and positive affect [30]. A recent study has found that belief in a just world may protect healthy Chinese citizens’ positive affect and reduce negative affect during the COVID-19 crisis [31]. Therefore, it seems reasonable to assume that institutional belief may influence individuals’ subjective well-being, most likely via belief in a just world in the Chinese context.
An additional and unique perspective links institutional trust and subjective well-being through the fear of COVID-19, caused by infectious spread, unexpected lockdown, and social distancing [32]. According to Legido-Quigley et al. and Yuan et al., trust in the government and health institutions may weaken the COVID-19 fear by increasing participants’ willingness to comply with the official instructions or commands, such as using face masks and testing for COVID-19 [33,34]. Matsushima et al. show that those trusting that their neighbors follow infection control measures exhibit less fear of COVID-19 [6]. The COVID-19 fear may induce a generalized negative affect [35,36]. For example, Chen and colleagues have collected data from 2445 Chinese students and found that fear of COVID-19 is associated with generalized feelings of strain, depression, and anxiety [37]. This finding is further replicated by research based on a sample of Chinese primary school students, middle school students, and adults [38]. Recent studies demonstrate that the fear of COVID-19 negatively predicts life satisfaction or well-being [3,39]. Based on the above evidence, we infer that the fear of COVID-19 bridges the relationship between institutional trust and subjective well-being. Solid institutional trust may reduce the fear of COVID-19, thus maintaining well-being in the Chinese context.
Furthermore, we contend that belief in a just world and the fear of COVID-19 (in sequence) may mediate the relationship between institutional trust and subjective well-being. Individuals with a firm belief in a just world assume that they may protect against COVID-19 if they adhere to preventive measures [40]. Hence, belief in a just world may help overcome the fear of COVID-19. Furthermore, previous research based on Chinese samples indicates that belief in a just world reduces the risk of fear and anxiety during a disaster such as an earthquake [41,42]. Moreover, an experimental study conducted in China has shown that belief in a just world reduces the fear and anxiety of COVID-19 [31]. Therefore, we contend that institutional trust indirectly affects subjective well-being through the serial mediation of belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19.
Correctly identifying predictors of individual well-being and their underlying mechanisms of action is essential to target intervention against COVID-19. Although the critical role of institutional trust for subjective well-being has already been documented in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, its mechanism of action has not been clearly identified.
The purposes of the current study are twofold: (a) to explore the protective effect of institutional trust on Chinese people’s subjective well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic, and (b) to test whether belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 mediate the relation between institutional trust and subjective well-being among the Chinese population. To this end, we collect data on participants’ institutional trust, belief in a just world, fear of COVID-19, and subjective well-being (i.e., life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect) from China. Then, we construct mediating model to investigate the effects and mediating mechanisms of institutional trust. The study’s findings provide meaningful insights into the indirect effects of institutional trust on subjective well-being and support its role in alleviating the negative consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic. Based on a synthesis of relevant theoretical perspectives and empirical research, we developed the following research hypotheses to guide our data analysis and interpretation of results.
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Institutional trust is significantly associated with subjective well-being.
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
Belief in a just world mediates the association between institutional trust and subjective well-being.
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
Fear of COVID-19 mediates the association between institutional trust and subjective well-being.
Hypothesis 4 (H4).
Belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 play a sequential mediatorship role in the relation between institutional trust and subjective well-being.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Data Collection

The online-based questionnaire survey approach was adopted in this study for data collection. This cross-sectional investigation was conducted in Zhengzhou city from 2–21 August 2021, when the city applied strict social distancing rules. Thus, an online data collection tool is more appropriate during the COVID-19 lockdown [3,9,37]. All the constructs in the current research were measured by structured scales that have been repeatedly used in different Chinese populations with good psychometric properties [15,37,42,43,44]. First, we delivered the study questionnaire electronically via a professional online survey platform. Convenience sampling and snowball sampling techniques were used to collect data in a cost-effective manner [7,11,37]. That is, we asked the initial participants to pass on the questionnaire through their social networks. Second, participants were informed clearly that the survey was anonymous and voluntary and their personal information was kept in confidence. Then, 881 participants (351 men and 530 women; Mage = 35.15, SD = 5.55) completed the online survey. More than half of the participants held academic degrees (n = 537, 60.95%). Electronic informed consent was obtained from the participants before their participation in the formal survey. The Institutional Review Board of the corresponding author’s university approved this research.

2.2. Measurement

We measured trust in public institutions (i.e., central government, local government, social security institutions, health institutions, mass media, employing units, communities, and neighborhoods) using the Chinese version of the Institutional Trust Scale [15]. This questionnaire includes eight items rated on a five-point scale (from one = very distrustful to five = very trustful). Higher scores indicate a higher level of institutional trust. In the current study, the Cronbach’s alpha is 0.86.
We assessed belief in a just world using the Chinese version of the General Belief in a Just World Scale [42]. Participants rated six items on a six-point Likert scale (from one = strongly disagree to six = strongly agree), with higher scores indicating a higher level of belief in a just world. In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha is 0.88.
We used the seven-item Fear of COVID-19 Scale (FCV-19S) to assess the fear of COVID-19 [2]. We recorded responses to these items using a five-point Likert-type scale, ranging from one (strongly disagree) to five (strongly agree). Higher scores indicate a greater fear of COVID-19. Previous research has reported that the Chinese version is acceptable and reliable [37,38]. In the current study, the Cronbach’s alpha of FCV-19S equals 0.88.
We measured subjective well-being using the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) and the Positive Affect and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) [45,46]. The SWLS includes five items rated on a seven-point Likert scale (one = strongly disagree, seven = strongly agree). The PANAS comprises a 10-item positive affect scale and a 10-item negative affect scale, rated on a five-point Likert scale (one = never, five = always). These Chinese-version scales are acceptable and reliable [43,44]. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients of SWLS, positive affect scale, and negative affect scale are equal to 0.88, 0.89, and 0.93, respectively.

2.3. Data Analysis

The research hypotheses were tested in three steps. First, Cronbach’s alpha and confirmatory factor analysis were conducted employing SPSS 24.0 and Mplus 7.0 to examine reliability and validity of all scales for measuring the constructs, respectively. Second, we calculated the descriptive statistics (i.e., mean, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis) and bivariate Pearson correlations utilizing SPSS 24.0. Third, the Mplus 7.0 was used to test the mediating effects of belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 on the relationship between the institutional trust and subjective well-being were examined using 5000 bootstrap samples with confidence intervals of 95%. In the analysis, demographic variables, i.e., gender, age, and education, were included as covariates.

3. Results

3.1. Preliminary Analysis and Descriptive Statistics

We performed confirmatory factor analysis to assess all measures’ construct and discriminant validity. The measurement model comprised the following latent variables: institutional trust, belief in a just world, fear of COVID-19, life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect. We treated the items of each measure as manifest variables. The six factors model guarantees a good degree of fit, and all items have significant loadings on their latent factor: χ2/df = 3.696 < 5, CFI = 0.908 > 0.90, TLI = 0.902 > 0.90, RMSEA = 0.055 < 0.08, and SRMR = 0.053 < 0.08.
Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, skewness, kurtosis, and bivariate correlations for all measures. We find significant correlations between institutional trust and subjective well-being measurements (life satisfaction: r = 0.240, positive affect: r = 0.344, negative affect: r = −0.255), indicating that the higher level of institutional trust is related to higher level of subjective well-being. Belief in a just world is significantly related to institutional trust (r = 0.332) and subject well-being measurements (life satisfaction: r = 0.237, positive affect: r = 0.230, negative affect: r = −0.223), suggesting that higher level of belief in a just world is associated with a higher level of institutional trust and subjective well-being. The results also indicated that the fear of COVID-19 is significantly correlated with institutional trust (r = −0.258), belief in a just world (r = −0.164), and subjective well-being measurements (life satisfaction: r = −0.270, positive affect: r = −0.216, negative affect: r = 0.443). It demonstrates that increased fear of COVID-19 is associated with decreased institutional trust, belief in a just world, and subjective well-being.

3.2. Serial Multiple Mediational Analyses

To examine the sequential mediation of belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 in the relationship between institutional trust and life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect, respectively, we constructed three mediational models (see Figure 1). In addition, we specified gender, age, and education as covariate variables. We applied the bias correction bootstrap technique based on 5000 samples to test the total, direct, and indirect effect. As Hayes indicates, the effect is significant if the 95% confidence intervals do not comprise zero [47].
Institutional trust directly predicts life satisfaction (total effect, β = 0.244, 95% CI = [0.176, 0.309], positive affect (total effect, β = 0.349, 95% CI = [0.277, 0.422]), and negative affect (total effect, β = −0.252, 95% CI = [−0.326, −0.172]) at a moderate level. When we consider the mediation variables, as shown in Figure 1, institutional trust still has significant impact on life satisfaction (direct effect, β = 0.137, 95% CI = [0.055, 0.220]), positive affect (direct effect, β = 0.276, 95% CI = [0.187, 0.365]), and negative affect (direct effect, β = −0.113, 95% CI = [−0.185, −0.048]). It indicates that institutional trust elicits moderate direct effects on subjective well-being measures. In addition, institutional trust is a direct and significant predictor of belief in a just world (β = 0.327, 95% CI = [0.254, 0.398]) and fear of COVID-19 (β = −0.220, 95% CI = [−0.296, −0.134]), suggesting that institutional trust predicts belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 at moderate levels.
Table 2 illustrates the results of the mediation test. First, institutional trust is an indirect predictor of life satisfaction (β = 0.046, 95% CI = [0.022, 0.080]), positive affect (β = 0.037, 95% CI = [0.012, 0.066]), and negative affect (β = −0.039, 95% CI = [−0.065, −0.014]) through belief in a just world. It suggests that institutional trust predicts subjective well-being indirectly through belief in a just world at a small level. Likewise, fear of COVID-19 mediates the effect of institutional trust on life satisfaction (β = 0.051, 95% CI = [0.025, 0.081]), positive affect (β = 0.031, 95% CI = [0.011, 0.057]), and negative affect (β = −0.087, 95% CI = [−0.121, −0.055]). Such results demonstrate that institutional trust predicts subjective well-being indirectly via fear of COVID-19 at a small level. In addition, institutional trust affects each subjective well-being measure via belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 in a sequential manner at a small level (life satisfaction, β = 0.007, 95% CI = [0.001, 0.014]; positive affect, β = 0.005, 95% CI = [0.001, 0.010]; negative affect, β = −0.013, 95% CI = [−0.023, −0.003]).

4. Discussion

The goal of the current study was to investigate the positive effect of institutional trust on subjective well-being during the COVID-19 crisis. Furthermore, we particularly focused on examining whether belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 mediated the relationship between institutional trust and subjective well-being. This study provides novel contribution to the literature as empirical research has not yet explored the mediation mechanism undying the beneficial impact of institutional trust on mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. To this end, we collect new data on self-reported institutional trust, belief in a just world, fear of COVID-19, and subjective well-being from a sample of 881 Chinese citizens. Our primary findings confirm that institutional trust contributes significantly to life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect but they also reveal that belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 play a sequential mediational role in these linkages.
The study’s results show significant correlations between institutional trust and subjective well-being (i.e., life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect), indicating that those who trust in public institutions exhibit higher levels of life satisfaction, positive affect, and lower levels of negative affect. This finding is consistent with theoretical perspectives highlighting the crucial role of institutional trust in subjective well-being [17]. According to the theoretical social capital model, institutional trust is a critical social resource for subjective well-being as it helps individuals maintain stable social relationships and gain support from public institutions [16,17]. Moreover, when an individual faces adverse environmental challenges, such as disasters or pandemics, institutional trust may foster people’s security feelings and help-seeking behaviors as they believe that public institutions are working for the best interests of the population and operating effectively. These beliefs are crucial for individual subjective well-being [11,48]. Our results also align with existing experimental studies suggesting that positive attitudes toward institutional relationships are significantly associated with better life satisfaction, more positive affect, and less negative affect during the COVID-19 crisis [6,11,49].
The mediation effect analysis indicates that, in line with our expectations, high institutional trust increases individual subjective well-being through belief in a just world. This result echoes the assumptions of Zhang and Zhang that the belief in a just world depends on a safe and trusted environment, and mistrust in social institutions contributes to the belief that the world is unreliable, uncontrollable, and unpredictable, decreasing the quality of life and subjective well-being [15]. Our findings are consistent with previous studies indicating a close link between institutional trust and individual belief in a just world. This result is in line with prior research demonstrating that belief in a just world has a protective effect on subjective well-being [26,27,28]. Furthermore, higher institutional trust reflects higher satisfaction with institutions, which further influences individual confidence and attitude toward society [15]. Individuals with a positive attitude and strong confidence in social institutions tend to believe that the world is just, orderly, reliable, organized, and safe [50]. This generalized positive view may increase the experience of positive affect and life satisfaction and reduce the likelihood of negative affect [27,28].
Institutional trust also affects subjective well-being through the fear of COVID-19. Previous research has suggested that solid institutional trust is critical in alleviating the fear of COVID-19 [6]. Individuals with high institutional trust are more likely to believe that public institutions may take adequate measures to curb the spread of highly infectious diseases, such as COVID-19. A recent study based on Chinese sample shows that a higher level of institutional trust led more people follow infection control measures such as proper preventive and help seeking behaviors [51]. Hence, adhering to infection control instructions may decrease their fear of COVID-19 [34]. Individuals with higher fear of COVID-19 may experience more general negative affect and perceive a lower level of life satisfaction or well-being [3,39]. Therefore, a solid institutional trust may protect subjective well-being by decreasing the fear of COVID-19.
In addition, we show that the serial effect of belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19 mediate the relationship between institutional trust and subjective well-being. This result contributes to a deeper understanding of how institutional trust has protected individual mental health during the COVID-19 crisis. Confidence in social institutions may contribute to the general view that the world is fair to everyone and people receive what they deserve (i.e., belief in a just world) [15]. Individuals with a strong belief in a just world are more likely to think those who contract COVID-19 “deserve it” as they do not adhere to epidemic prevention norms such as wearing masks and keeping social distance [40]. Thus, individuals with a firm belief in a just world are more inclined to consider the COVID-19 epidemic controllable and consciously abide by public health instructions to prevent COVID-19 infection and protect their health, reducing anxiety and the fear of COVID-19 and preserving their subjective well-being during the pandemic [31,39].
Overall, the study’s results indicate that institutional trust influences subjective well-being directly and indirectly through belief in a just world and fear of the COVID-19. To the best of our knowledge, this is one of the few studies to examine the relationships between institutional trust, subjective well-being, belief in a just world, and fear of COVID-19. From a theoretical perspective, our study offers a valid framework to explain how institutional trust affects individual subjective well-being in the context of the COVID-19 crisis. Those who trust social institutions strongly believe in a just world, which helps overcome COVID-19 fear and further increases their subjective well-being. These results enrich the current knowledge of the mediating mechanism underlying institutional trust’s buffer and protective effect for those who suffer from disasters. From a practical perspective, our study may help institutions, such as government and health institutions, realize the vital role of institutional trust during crises, such as the COVID-19 pandemic. These measures may successfully alleviate people’s fear and anxiety of COVID-19 and increase their subjective well-being.

5. Conclusions

During the pandemic, individuals in China may experience fear and decreased subject well-being. Our study indicates that institutional trust may assist in decreasing these negative outcomes. This paper further confirms that institutional trust protects Chinese people’s subject well-being indirectly through enhancing belief in a just world and reducing fear of COVID-19.
Along with our finding, this study has several limitations, suggesting future research directions. First, restricted to many factors such as environment, time, and sampling condition, we use convenience sampling and snowball sampling to collect data and investigate adult residents in Zhengzhou city through anonymous online survey. Such procedures do not allow us to consider our sample as a representative one, even though we statistically controlled for the demographic variables (i.e., gender, age, and education) and thus our findings may apply to the general Chinese adults. Future research could further employ other sampling methods such as random sampling and collect more nationally representative data, which would increase the robustness of our study results. Second, we mainly focus on the protective role of institutional trust in the Chinese context. However, there is also the possibility of cultural differences in the measure of institutional trust and its influence on individuals’ response to COVID-19 [11]. That is, cultural factors should be considered in future study. For example, future research could carry out a worldwide survey to explore and compare the mediating mechanisms underlying the relationship between institutional trust and subject well-being across different countries. Third, due to the study’s cross-sectional nature, we do not disentangle the causal relationships between the four variables of interest. Nevertheless, even though we should be cautious about the causal interpretation of our findings, previous literature sustains the mediating effect we investigated. Future longitudinal or experimental studies should explore the influencing factors and mechanisms of action of subjective well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Finally, the present analysis mainly employs self-reported measures, which may produce common-method bias and affect intercorrelations between the measures. The confirmatory factor analysis indicates that all measures present a good discriminant validity, suggesting that common-method bias is not a significant problem in our study [52]. Future research may combine other methods such as observations and interviews to control common-method bias and test the proposed mediating model.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.L.; methodology, J.J. and Y.S.; formal analysis, S.L. and E.W.; investigation, J.J. and Y.S.; writing—original draft preparation, S.L. and J.J.; writing—review and editing, E.W.; supervision, S.L. and E.W.; funding acquisition, S.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by Starting Research Fund from the Henan Normal University (Grant No. 5101109171807).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Henan Normal University (Study # 13, approval date 31 May 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Data are presented in anonymized and aggregated format and participants could not be identified (written informed consent for publication was not applicable).

Data Availability Statement

Data available on request from the authors.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. WHO Coronavirus (COVID-19) Dashboard. Available online: https://covid19.who.int/ (accessed on 8 June 2022).
  2. Ahorsu, D.K.; Lin, C.Y.; Imani, V.; Saffari, M.; Griffiths, M.D.; Pakpour, A.H. The fear of COVID-19 scale: Development and initial validation. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2020, 20, 1537–1545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Duong, C.D. The impact of fear and anxiety of COVID-19 on life satisfaction: Psychological distress and sleep disturbance as mediators. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 178, 110869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Feng, L.; Dong, Z.; Yan, R.; Wu, X.; Zhang, L.; Ma, J.; Zeng, Y. Psychological distress in the shadow of the COVID-19 pandemic: Preliminary development of an assessment scale. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 291, 113202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Lee, S.A.; Jobe, M.C.; Mathis, A.A.; Gibbons, J.A. Incremental validity of coronaphobia: Coronavirus anxiety explains depression, generalized anxiety, and death anxiety. J. Anxiety Disord. 2020, 74, 102268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Matsushima, M.; Tsuno, K.; Okawa, S.; Hori, A.; Tabuchi, T. Trust and well-being of postpartum women during the COVID-19 crisis: Depression and fear of COVID-19. SSM-Popul. Health. 2021, 15, 100903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Salfi, F.; Lauriola, M.; D’Atri, A.; Amicucci, G.; Viselli, L.; Tempesta, D.; Ferrara, M. Demographic, psychological, chronobiological, and work-related predictors of sleep disturbances during the COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 11416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Krautter, K.; Friese, M.; Hart, A.; Reis, D. No party no joy?—Changes in university students’ extraversion, neuroticism, and subjective well-being during two COVID-19 lockdowns. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 2022, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Reizer, A.; Geffen, L.; Koslowsky, M. Life under the COVID-19 lockdown: On the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and psychological distress. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy. 2021, 13, 432–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Gotanda, H.; Miyawaki, A.; Tabuchi, T.; Tsugawa, Y. Association between trust in government and practice of preventive measures during the COVID-19 pandemic in Japan. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2021, 36, 3471–3477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Lee, S. Subjective well-being and mental health during the pandemic outbreak: Exploring the role of institutional trust. Res. Aging 2022, 44, 10–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Diener, E.; Suh, E.M.; Lucas, R.E.; Smith, H.L. Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychol. Bull. 1999, 125, 276–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ciziceno, M.; Travaglino, G.A. Perceived corruption and individuals’ life satisfaction: The mediating role of institutional trust. Soc. Indic. Res. 2019, 141, 685–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Piumatti, G.; Magistro, D.; Zecca, M.; Esliger, D.W. The mediation effect of political interest on the connection between social trust and wellbeing among older adults. Ageing Soc. 2018, 38, 2376–2395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Zhang, Z.; Zhang, J. Belief in a just world mediates the relationship between institutional trust and life satisfaction among the elderly in China. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 83, 164–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Kawachi, I.; Subramanian, S.V.; Kim, D. Social Capital and Health; Kawachi, I., Subramanian, S.V., Kim, D., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 1–26. [Google Scholar]
  17. Kroll, C. Social Capital and the Happiness of Nations: The Importance of Trust and Networks for Life Satisfaction in a Cross-National Perspective, 1st ed.; Peter Lang GmbH, Internationaler Verlag der Wissenschaften: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  18. Lindström, M. Social capital, anticipated ethnic discrimination and self-reported psychological health: A population-based study. Soc. Sci. Med. 2008, 66, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Yamaguchi, M.; Inoue, Y.; Shinozaki, T.; Saito, M.; Takagi, D.; Kondo, K.; Kondo, N. Community social capital and depressive symptoms among older people in Japan: A multilevel longitudinal study. J. Epidemiol. 2019, 29, 363–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Wang, C.; Tang, N.; Zhen, D.; Wang, X.R.; Zhang, J.; Cheong, Y.; Zhu, Q. Need for cognitive closure and trust towards government predicting pandemic behavior and mental health: Comparing United States and China. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Lipkus, I.M.; Dalbert, C.; Siegler, I.C. The importance of distinguishing the belief in a just world for self versus for others: Implications for psychological well-being. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1996, 22, 666–677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Correia, I.; Vala, J. Belief in a Just World, Subjective Well-Being and Trust of Young Adult, 1st ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  23. Otto, K.; Glaser, D.; Dalbert, C. Mental health, occupational trust, and quality of working life: Does belief in a just world matter? J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 39, 1288–1315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Fox, C.L.; Elder, T.; Gater, J.; Johnson, E. The association between adolescents’ beliefs in a just world and their attitudes to victims of bullying. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 80, 183–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Furnham, A. Belief in a just world: Research progress over the past decade. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2003, 34, 795–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Jiang, F.; Yue, X.; Lu, S.; Yu, G.; Zhu, F. How belief in a just world benefits mental health: The effects of optimism and gatitude. Soc. Indic. Res. 2016, 126, 411–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Khera, M.L.K.; Harvey, A.J.; Callan, M.J. Beliefs in a just world, subjective well-being and attitudes towards refugees among refugee workers. Soc. Justice Res. 2014, 27, 432–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Sutton, R.M.; Stoeber, J.; Kamble, S.V. Belief in a just world for oneself versus others, social goals, and subjective well-Being. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017, 113, 115–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Li, X.; Li, S. The effect of belief in a just world on adolescents’ subjective well-being: The chain mediating roles of resilience and self-esteem. Chin. J. Spec. Educ. 2016, 3, 71–76. [Google Scholar]
  30. Li, S.; Zhang, Y.; Li, X. Belief in a just world on psychological well-being among college students: The effect of trait empathy and altruistic behavior. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 2017, 25, 359–362. [Google Scholar]
  31. Wang, J.; Wang, Z.; Liu, X.; Yang, X.; Zheng, M.; Bai, X. The impacts of a COVID-19 epidemic focus and general belief in a just world on individual emotions. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 168, 110349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Odriozola-González, P.; Planchuelo-Gómez, Á.; Irurtia, M.J.; de Luis-García, R. Psychological effects of the COVID-19 outbreak and lockdown among students and workers of a Spanish university. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 290, 113108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Legido-Quigley, H.; Asgari, N.; Teo, Y.Y.; Leung, G.M.; Oshitani, H.; Fukuda, K.; Cook, A.R.; Hsu, L.Y.; Shibuya, K.; Heymann, D. Are high-performing health systems resilient against the COVID-19 epidemic? Lancet 2020, 395, 848–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Yuan, H.; Long, Q.; Huang, G.; Huang, L.; Luo, S. Different roles of interpersonal trust and institutional trust in COVID-19 pandemic control. Soc. Sci. Med. 2022, 293, 114677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Daly, M.; Robinson, E. Psychological distress and adaptation to the COVID-19 crisis in the United States. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2021, 136, 603–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Ye, B.; Wu, D.; Im, H.; Liu, M.; Wang, X.; Yang, Q. Stressors of COVID-19 and stress consequences: The mediating role of rumination and the moderating role of psychological support. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2020, 118, 105466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Chen, W.; Liang, Y.; Yin, X.; Zhou, X.; Gao, R. The factor structure and rasch analysis of the Fear of COVID-19 Scale (FCV-19S) among Chinese students. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 678979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Chi, X.; Chen, S.; Chen, Y.; Chen, D.; Yu, W.; Guo, T.; Cao, Q.; Zheng, S.; Hossain, M.M.; Stubbs, B.; et al. Psychometric evaluation of the Fear of COVID-19 Scale among Chinese population. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2022, 20, 1273–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Deniz, M.E. Self-compassion, intolerance of uncertainty, fear of COVID-19, and well-being: A serial mediation investigation. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 177, 110824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Devereux, P.G.; Miller, M.K.; Kirshenbaum, J.M. Moral disengagement, locus of control, and belief in a just world: Individual differences relate to adherence to COVID-19 guidelines. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 182, 111069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Xie, X.; Liu, H.; Gan, Y. Belief in a just world when encountering the 5/12 Wenchuan earthquake. Environ. Behav. 2011, 43, 566–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Wu, S.T.; Wang, L.; Zhou, M.; Wang, W.; Zhang, J. Belief in a just world and subjective well-being: Comparing disaster sites with normal areas. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 17, 579–587. [Google Scholar]
  43. Lin, L. Seeking pleasure or growth? The mediating role of happiness motives in the longitudinal relationship between social mobility beliefs and well-being in college students. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2022, 184, 111170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Jia, N.; Li, W.; Zhang, L.; Kong, F. Beneficial effects of hedonic and eudaimonic motivations on subjective well-being in adolescents: A two-wave cross-lagged analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 2021, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Diener, E.; Emmons, R.A.; Larsen, R.J.; Griffin, S. The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 1985, 49, 71–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Watson, D.; Clark, L.A.; Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 1063–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  48. Oksanen, A.; Kaakinen, M.; Latikka, R.; Savolainen, I.; Savela, N.; Koivula, A. Regulation and trust: 3-month follow-up study on COVID-19 mortality in 25 European countries. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 2020, 6, e19218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Paolini, D. COVID-19 Lockdown in Italy: The role of social identification and social and political trust on well-being and distress. Curr. Psychol. 2020, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Sutton, R.M.; Douglas, K.M.; Wilkin, K.; Elder, T.J.; Cole, J.M.; Stathi, S. Justice for whom, exactly? beliefs in justice for the self and various others. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 34, 528–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Wong, L.P.; Wu, Q.; Hao, Y.; Chen, X.; Chen, Z.; Alias, H.; Shen, M.; Hu, J.; Duan, S.; Zhang, J.; et al. The role of institutional trust in preventive practices and treatment-seeking intention during the coronavirus disease 2019 outbreak among residents in Hubei, China. Int. Health 2022, 14, 161–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Podsakoff, P.M.; MacKenzie, S.B.; Lee, J.-Y.; Podsakoff, N.P. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 879–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The results of serial multiple mediational models, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001. Values shown are standardized coefficients.
Figure 1. The results of serial multiple mediational models, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001. Values shown are standardized coefficients.
Behavsci 12 00252 g001
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, skewness, kurtosis, and bivariate correlations among measures.
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, skewness, kurtosis, and bivariate correlations among measures.
MeasureMSDSkewness Kurtosis 12345
1. Institutional trust34.965.33−1.382.88
2. Belief in a just world25.485.50−0.300.840.33 ***
3. Fear of COVID-1920.037.300.21−0.62−0.26 ***−0.16 ***
4. Life satisfaction21.786.15−0.020.070.24 ***0.24 ***−0.27 ***
5. Positive affect32.466.58−0.271.370.34 ***0.23 ***−0.22 ***0.47 ***
6. Negative affect27.508.62−0.01−0.23−0.26 ***−0.22 ***0.44 ***−0.09 **0.011
** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Indirect effect of institutional trust on subjective well-being via belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19.
Table 2. Indirect effect of institutional trust on subjective well-being via belief in a just world and fear of COVID-19.
PathStandardized Coefficient95% CI
LLUL
Model 1
Institutional trust → BJW → Life satisfaction0.0490.0220.080
Institutional trust→ Fear of COVID-19 → Life satisfaction0.0510.0250.081
Institutional trust → BJW → Fear of COVID-19 → Life satisfaction0.0070.0010.014
Total effect0.2440.1760.309
Direct effect0.1370.0550.220
Total indirect effect0.1070.0670.150
Model 2
Institutional trust → BJW → Positive affect0.0370.0120.066
Institutional trust → Fear of COVID-19 → Positive affect0.0310.0110.057
Institutional trust → BJW → Fear of COVID-19→ Positive affect0.0050.0010.010
Total effect0.3490.2770.422
Direct effect0.2760.1870.365
Total indirect effect0.0730.0400.111
Model 3
Institutional trust → BJW → Negative affect−0.039−0.065−0.014
Institutional trust → Fear of COVID-19 → Negative affect−0.087−0.121−0.055
Institutional trust → BJW → Fear of COVID-19 → Negative affect−0.013−0.023−0.003
Total effect−0.252−0.326−0.172
Direct effect−0.113−0.185−0.048
Total indirect effect−0.139−0.180−0.098
CI, confidence interval; LL, lower limit; UL, upper limit; BJW, belief in a just world.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Li, S.; Sun, Y.; Jing, J.; Wang, E. Institutional Trust as a Protective Factor during the COVID-19 Pandemic in China. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 252. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080252

AMA Style

Li S, Sun Y, Jing J, Wang E. Institutional Trust as a Protective Factor during the COVID-19 Pandemic in China. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(8):252. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080252

Chicago/Turabian Style

Li, Shuangshuang, Yijia Sun, Jiaqi Jing, and Enna Wang. 2022. "Institutional Trust as a Protective Factor during the COVID-19 Pandemic in China" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 8: 252. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080252

APA Style

Li, S., Sun, Y., Jing, J., & Wang, E. (2022). Institutional Trust as a Protective Factor during the COVID-19 Pandemic in China. Behavioral Sciences, 12(8), 252. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12080252

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop