Next Article in Journal
Psychometric Investigation of the Workplace Social Courage Scale (WSCS): New Evidence for Measurement Invariance and IRT Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Does Insomnia Increase the Risk of Suicide in Hospitalized Patients with Major Depressive Disorder? A Nationwide Inpatient Analysis from 2006 to 2015
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Moral Foundational Framing and Its Impact on Attitudes and Behaviours

1
Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science, The London School of Economics and Political Science, London WC2A 3PH, UK
2
Wolfson College, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6UD, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2022, 12(5), 118; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12050118
Submission received: 9 October 2021 / Revised: 31 March 2022 / Accepted: 4 April 2022 / Published: 20 April 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Cognition)

Abstract

:
This preregistered, randomized controlled experiment attempts to distil the effects of Moral Foundational Framing on attitudes and behaviours towards refugees in the UK. First, moral foundations were found to robustly predict both attitudes and behaviours practised towards refugees. Next, a degree of support was found for the effectiveness of moral foundational framing in adjusting attitudes, but not behaviour, toward refugees in the UK. Individuals who scored highly on certain morals were susceptible to influence by moral foundational framing, but not always in ways that may have been expected. We conclude that the robust relationship found between moral foundations and attitudes towards refugees stresses the importance of actively shaping moral foundations. This notion is strengthened by the comparatively less robust effects of framing. Findings could be used to inform the practice of individuals interested in influencing opinion and behaviour, particularly in support of refugee acceptance.

1. Introduction

The world is facing unprecedented challenges in the form of the plight of refugees. 70.8 million people have been forced to leave their homes and live away from them today, of which 25.9 million are refugees [1]. Of this 70.8 million, it is estimated that more than a third were forced to move due to sudden onset weather events [2]. According to the UNHCR, in 2018, there were a total of 126,720 refugees in the UK, including 18,519 who had been offered asylum that year [1]. The Migration Observatory at the University of Oxford reported that more than 50% of British people believe migration should be reduced. Blinder [3] reported that more than 75% of British citizens want to reduce immigration.
Negative attitudes centre around a triad of attitudinal bunches; economic burden, sectarian/ethnic considerations, and safety of the host population. Such attitudes have been reported widely, including within the context of Turkey [4], Japan [5], and Europe-wide [6,7,8]. Buffers against holding negative attitudes towards refugees seem to come from higher levels of education [5,9] while the informational flow from elites also holds sway [5].
Researchers have employed a range of approaches in their attempts to change attitudes towards refugees, including making salient certain social norms [10], holding individuals accountable for decisions they make about other groups [11], employing an influencing confederate [12], or close friends and acquaintances [13], information dissemination that challenges ill-attitudes with facts [5,7,9], addressing specific concerns such as ethnic composition and value worries [14], and consciousness raising [15].

1.1. Framing in Political Contexts

Framing grew from Prospect Theory; this theory centres on the finding that “decisions taken by individuals can be altered by presenting information in logically equivalent but semantically different ways” ([16], p. 5). Their research found a significant relationship between the way in which information was presented and how it would be weighed by individuals. Framing can be described as a pointed packaging of information that might draw emphasis to one (or certain) aspect(s) of it; crucially, it does not result in any factual changes to the information.
In politicized decision contexts, Druckman and McDermott identify two key deviations by way of emotions and susceptibility to the framing effect [17]; different emotions, both deemed negative, e.g., anger and distress, can lead to differential impacts of framing. The same authors identified emotions as one potential way to moderate the framing effect. Ref. [18] used conflicting elite discourse to see if it could reduce the impact of the framing effect, with mixed success. Researchers at Stanford University [19] deployed framing to tackle a phenomenon researchers call the ‘progressive paradox’, wherein individuals favour a more egalitarian society yet readily vote against candidates who seek to realize that goal at the ballot box. This built on previous research, such as Feygina, Goldsmith and Jost [20] who reported that they could increase support for environmental policies if they were framed in terms of preserving American traditions.

1.2. Moral Foundations Theory

One common approach to framing in these politicized contexts [19,20] is by means of Moral Foundations Theory (MFT) [21]. MFT attempts to formulate a comprehensive theory of human morality, identifying moral values that extend across cultures. By examining a range of cultures, with theoretical foundations in social psychology and anthropology, they extract five (or six [22]) universal moral dimensions: care/harm, fairness/cheating, loyalty/betrayal, authority/subversion and sanctity/degradation, which can be found within all cultures, to varying intensities and differential manifestations (the proposed sixth moral is liberty/oppression). In this study, we look at the effect of four of these frames: care, fairness, loyalty or sanctity. Authority was deselected due to both resource constraints and expectations it would not be influential (though this exclusion proved to be erroneous: statistical analyses on data collected through this study found strong, significant relationships between the moral of authority and some of the outcome measures). Each of the five foundations is rooted in evolutionary benefit, which we outline below.

1.2.1. Care/Harm

This evolutionarily important value is prominent across different mammals, one form of which is embodied by a mother’s care for her offspring; the development of expressions of care is often socially driven by the propagation of normative ideals. While the biopsychological hardware for this foundation might be similar between humans and societies, its manifestation varies greatly. Compare the expression of care/harm in Ancient Spartan society with that of contemporary California.

1.2.2. Fairness/Cheating

Fairness/Cheating is linked to the notion of reciprocal altruism, where one individual (temporarily) sacrifices for another unrelated individual. Fairness is derived from overarching reciprocal benefit, following the initial cost. Sensitivity to fairness is universal [23] and observed early in human development (before the age of five, and possibly before the age of one).

1.2.3. Loyalty/Betrayal

Loyalty is defined as strong feelings of allegiance toward a person or group and is equally foundational. Both chimps and humans have been found to group together and initiate conflicts with other groups for territory, with humans having intensified this ability due to the development of ‘language, weapons, and tribal markers’ ([21] p. 70). These predispositions towards loyalty were famously activated in Sherif’s [24] seminal study that took a group of schoolboys on a camping trip, divided them into groups and had them face off against each other in competitions; they exhibited commitment to in-group members and stark hostility towards ‘the other’. Indeed, in-group loyalty is often associated with hostile attitudes towards out-group members [25].

1.2.4. Authority/Subversion

Dominance hierarchies are seen in many animals, especially larger mammals. Within human beings, social hierarchies vary greatly from culture to culture and might include patriarchy or matriarchy as defining features. Within modern nation states, various cultures might exist (and coexist); liberals might deem obedience to authority as a vice while conservatives champion it as a virtue [26].

1.2.5. Sanctity/Degradation

Sanctity and degradation are related to the immune system, physiological aversion and disgust. The evolutionary advantages of this system are great: parasites, bacteria and viruses pose significant threats to our collective survival. Revulsion keeps these (where possible) out of our food, bodies and societies [27]. This system also impacted our social world; “Disgust and the behavioral immune system have come to undergird a variety of moral reactions, for example, to immigrants and sexual deviants” ([21], p. 71).

1.3. Moral Framing to Shape Pro-Refugee Attitudes and Behaviour

In this randomized controlled experiment, we deployed moral foundational framing to influence attitudes and behaviours in relation to refugees in the UK. Moral foundational framing has successfully been deployed in contexts including attitudes toward the environmental [20] and fiscal policy [19]. In proximity to this study, Nath et al. [28] applied Moral Foundational framing to immigrant contexts, focusing on the differences between individualizing and binding (The individualizing morals are care/harm and fairness/cheating; the binding morals are loyalty/betrayal, authority/subversion and sanctity/degradation) morals and the way in which these moral groupings predict attitudes. Their experiments successfully applied this form of framing to influence stated attitudes regarding immigration to the USA. Kaufman [14] successfully applied moral framing, showing that reframing discussions around migration could lessen it as a concern. However, most research did not consider how these attitudinal shifts respond to behaviour [16,29]. Grigorieff, Roth and Ubfal [9] found that while certain morally aligned frames changed attitudes, critically, they did not observe behaviourally-inclined changes, such as in decisions made about policy or resource distribution. In the current experiment, we therefore supplement the examination of attitudes by measures approximating pro-refugee behaviours in the real world by asking participants to sign a petition either in favour or against increasing the number of refugees in the UK and inviting them to donate 20% of their experimental earnings to a refugee charity.
Another missing element from the body of literature is the notion of personalization. Studies tend to focus on identifying which one frame may function more effectively than another, or which group responds most to a certain frame. However, increasingly, this one-size-fits-all perspective is being replaced by a more tailored approach [30]. Attempting to understand how moral foundational underpinning might influence individual-level responses is key to the impact of any informational campaign.
In sum, we expected that Moral Foundations would significantly predict attitudes and behaviours towards refugees. To be more specific, based on patterns observed in the literature, we expected that moral values of care and fairness significantly predict favourable attitudes and behaviours towards refugees. Second, we expected that frames that were congruent and morally aligned would be most effective in eliciting pro-refugee attitudes. Third, we expected that congruent and morally aligned frames will be most effective in eliciting pro-refugee behaviours.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Experimental Design

This study utilised a one-way between subjects’ experimental design with levels (control, care, fair, sanctity and loyalty message frame). In addition, participants provided demographic information (age, gender, racial group and religious affiliation) and political demographic information (political party alignment, political ideology, and position on Brexit).

2.2. Control Group

With the benefit of hindsight, the control group employed by this experiment, when compared to the treatment groups, might have been significantly flawed. Relying on the work of Kaufman [14], the passage used for the treatment groups is arguably contentious and scientifically noisy, evoking themes of inclusion and exclusion relating to migrants and refugees (Appendix ATable A2). The control group read a significantly different passage that contained none of these themes, leading to a potentially critical difference between the treatment groups and the control groups that was not intended. Accordingly, the control group was excluded from most analyses reported.

2.3. Stimuli

We used the Moral Foundations Theory Questionnaire-30 (MFQ-30), a 32-item measure of individual values (30 items, plus two attention checks) to measure participants on their level of five morals. The treatment conditions were devised by way of infusing three written passages with morally laden wording. The passages were threefold: a statement on immigration in the UK (Figure 1), an excerpt from a speech of a fictional politician (Figure 2) and a policy proposal (Figure 3). The treatment groups were five: a control group, as well as four different morally framed conditions. All moral frames were infused with wording that stemmed from each moral value (Table A1 and Table A2). Rather than the statement on immigration, the control group received a passage on the role of lobbyists. While the excerpt from the politician centred on refugees for four of the treatment groups, for the control group, the fictional politician spoke on voting systems.

2.4. Measures

Attitudinal measurement was threefold. Participants were presented with a statement regarding finding the number of refugees entering the UK as being of concern or alarming and were asked to agree (1) or disagree (0) with it. Based on the work of Voelkel and Willer [19] participants were asked to provide the likelihood of their support for a pro-refugee political candidate (on a 5-point Likert scale). Finally, participants were asked to either provide support (1) or not (0) for a policy that would see refugee numbers increase in the UK by 5%.
The behaviour measures employed were twofold. One involved an invitation to sign a petition in support of, or against, increasing refugee numbers. The second behavioural measure was an invitation to donate 20% of participation in experiment earnings (£0.20) towards a fictional UK-based refugee charity. While this amount might seem insignificant, it represented 20% of the total incentive offered to participants to partake in this study. It can fairly be deduced that for an individual motivated by £1, one-fifth of that amount would carry value.

2.5. Participants

1076 British participants (Age: M = 36.8, range: 18–84; 641 females—Table 1 below). All were recruited through the Prolific platform.

2.6. Consent

Participants actively consented to participation in the study by way of opting into the experiment and indicating their consent.

2.7. Sample Size

To determine the sample size required to adequately carry out this experiment, a G-power calculation was conducted, using the following parameters:
Effect size (d)—0.35; Power—0.95; Alpha (p)–0.05.
An effect size of 0.35 was selected due to being as close to a strong effect (0.25) as possible, while not exceeding available resources. This yielded n of 214 per treatment group, with a total n of 1070.

2.8. Recruitment

Participants were recruited in October 2019 using Prolific, deemed a sound recruitment tool that avoids some of the problematic aspects that arise on MTurk [31]. Participants were reimbursed for their time at a rate of £6.43/hour. Participants were instructed they would be ‘answering a series of questions about their values and attitudes, and taking a number of measures relating to the population of the UK’.

2.9. Ethical Considerations

Participants were informed that they would be free to withdraw from the study at any point. They were also provided with the email address of the LSE’s Research Governance Manager and invited to contact them should any concerns or complaints arise. Participant data was anonymized and all data was treated securely, being accessible only to the researcher and their supervisor. This study was ethically self-approved according to the LSE’s Psychological and Behavioural Science ethical guidelines.

2.10. Procedure

Participants were instructed to complete some of the demographic measures before they were instructed to read the first framed item carefully and state their agreement/disagreement with the first attitudinal measure. They were then instructed to read the second framed item and state their likelihood of voting for the fictional political candidate. Participants then read the framed policy proposal and stated their support or opposition to it. After the attitudinal measures, participants completed the two behavioural measures. Firstly, participants were invited to sign a petition in support of, or a petition against, increasing the number of refugees allowed into the UK. Secondly, participants were invited to donate £0.20, or any other amount, from their participation earnings.
Participants were then asked to carry out the MFQ-30, followed by providing the remaining demographic information. This was purposely set to be done after the main attitudinal and behavioural measures, as it was determined these were the most important measures of the experiment, hence this was done to protect them from participant fatigue. On completion, participants were shown the participant debrief page.

2.11. Exclusion Criteria and Process

The MFQ-30 has two attention-check questions. Q6 comes within the first section, in which participants are determining how relevant the items are to their decision of whether or not something is right or wrong, The item of Q6 states: ‘Whether or not someone was good at math’. Q22, in the second section, in which participants are stating their agreement or disagreement with the items, states: ‘It is better to do good than to do bad’.
According to the authors of the MFQ-30, any positive response to question six and any negative response to 22 should disqualify the participant. Eight participants violated both conditions and were removed from the data set. 18 participants violated Q22 and were removed from the data set. 159 participants violated Q6. On further enquiry, it was thought that this position could be challenged (This was challenged by two participants who were flagged for potential disqualification). As such, a case could be made that utilitarianism, and consequentialism as a whole, are based on the idea that maths and ethics are related. Accordingly, an individual’s level of maths might be a minor factor in processing morality. A case could thus be made, very much equivocally, that intelligence and ethics can be related; certainly, consequentialist ethics relies on mathematical valuation. In light of this thinking, participants who violated Q6 were only removed if they completed the questionnaire in less than four minutes. 29 participants were removed based on the amended criteria.

2.12. Materials

Stata (15.0); Qualtrics (www.qualtrics.com); Prolific (www.prolific.co).

2.13. Pre-Registration

This experiment was pre-registered with the Centre for Open Science (OSF). Areas of focus and expected findings were listed and described here (https://osf.io/7tvj6/).

3. Results

3.1. The Relationship between Moral Foundations and Attitudes towards Refugees

In order to probe potential relationships between moral foundations and the combined attitude measures towards refugees, an Ordered Logit regression was executed. The regression analysis, with a Pseudo R2 of 0.089, showed that pro-refugee attitudes were significantly predicted by all moral value measures, barring sanctity (z = −0.26, p < 0.80, OR = 0.979 95% CI [0.836–1.147]). Care (z = 4.21, p < 0.001, OR = 1.55, 95% CI [1.26–1.90]), fairness (z = 7.32, p < 0.001, OR = 2.28 95%CI [1.83–2.85]), loyalty (z = −2.26, p = 0.024, OR = 0.81 95% CI [0.666–0.971]) and authority (z = −6.20, p < 0.001, OR = 0.527, CI [0.43–0.645]), were all found to be significantly related to attitude towards refugees—fairness and care positively so, loyalty and authority negatively so (See Figure 4A). As predicted, care and fairness are the strongest predictors of positive attitudes towards refugees.

3.2. The Effect of Moral Foundational Framing on Shifting Attitudes

To assess the effectiveness of framing conditions on observed attitudes, we ran the same Ordered Logit regression, including the effect of framing conditions in two blocks: first as a main effect and second with interaction effects. The model fit did not significantly improve (X2(4) = 7.36, p = 0.118) by adding a main effect (Pseudo R2 of 0.091). The model did significantly improve by adding main and interaction effects (X2(20) = 55.96, p < 0.001; Pseudo R2 of 0.101). This model displays a main effect of the care frame (Z = −4.07, p < 0.001, OR = 0.007, 95% CI = 0–0.08) and sanctity relative to the control frame (Z = −2.05, p = 0.041, OR = 0.14, 95% CI = 0.02–0.09) relative to the control frame.
Despite the unexpected directionality of the main effects (due to previously explained issues with the control group), we also note a number of interaction effects: those who scored higher on authority were less likely to respond to the care (Z = −2.70, p = 0.007, OR = 0.4, 95% CI = 0.2–0.78), fair (Z = −2.97, p = 0.003, OR = 0.48, 95% CI = 0.2–0.72) or loyalty (Z = −2.89, p = 0.004, OR = 0.4, 95% CI = 0.20–0.73) frame than the control. Those who scored higher on loyalty were more likely to respond to the care frame than to the control (Z = 1.986, p = 0.047, OR = 1.85, 95% CI =1.01–3.42), which was most consistent with our prediction. Those who scored higher on for care also responded better to the care frame (Z = 2.29, p = 0.022, OR = 2.12, 95% CI = 1.12–4.05).
We repeated this analysis without the control group (care frame as a reference). This yielded similar results: there were no main effects of frames if these were added alone but there were when interaction effects were included (Supplementary Material for the complete analyses). We reject the null hypothesis: not only is no one frame well suited to all participants, but some predispositions are also predictive of the frame type that may direct their attitude toward more prosocial behaviour.

3.3. The Relationship between Moral Foundations on Pro-Refugee Behaviour

Did differences carry through to behaviours? We reviewed the relationships between moral foundations and behaviours practised towards refugees—two regressions models were executed. In relation to signing the pro-refugee petition, we ran a Ordered Logit regression (Pseudo R2 of 0.032): fairness was a significant positive [Z = 4.03, p < 0.001, OR = 1.62, 95% CI = 1.29–2.06], and authority was a significant negative predictor [Z = −3.56, p < 0.001, OR = 0.68, 95% CI = 0.55–838] of pro-refugee signing (see Figure 4B).
In relation to pro-refugee donations, we ran a binomial logistic regression model (Pseudo R2 of 0.096). We found that care [Z = 5.40, p < 0.001, OR = 2.04, 95% CI = 1.58–2.65] and fairness [Z = 2.07, p = 0.038, OR = 1.33, 95% CI = 1.01–1.73] were significantly positive and authority [Z = −3.75, p < 0.001, OR = 0.63, 95% CI = 0.50–0.80] was a significant negative predictor of donating (see Figure 4B).
In sum, we reject the null hypothesis: the foundation of care predicts pro-refugee behaviour, whereas authority does so negatively. Interestingly, differences were less pronounced with monetary donations (odds ratio around 4) relative to the signing of the petition (odds ratio around 2).

3.4. The Effect of Moral Foundational Framing on Pro-Refugee Behaviour

To review the effects of framing on pro-refugee behaviour, we added framing conditions to the Ordered Logit model for pro-refugee petitions and the logistic regression model for pro-refugee donating.
For pro-refugee petitions, the predictive power of the model did not significantly improve with added effects of condition (X2(4) = 1.97, p = 0.741; Pseudo R2 of 0.033), nor when interaction effects between framing and moral foundation were added (X2(20) = 22.1, p = 0.334; Pseudo R2 of 0.044). Upon review of the two models, there were no main interaction effects. Findings did not differ when the control group was removed from the analysis (Supplementary Material).
For pro-refugee donations, we used the same approach. We also observed no changes in predictive power of the model when the framing condition was added (X2(4) = 4.12, p = 0.39; Pseudo R2 of 0.099), nor when interaction effects between framing and moral foundation were added (X2(20) = 22.16, p = 0.332; Pseudo R2 of 0.1147). Upon review of the two models, there were no main of interaction effects. For this measure too, findings did not differ when the control group was removed from the analysis (Supplementary Material).

4. Discussion

In this study, we find first that moral foundations significantly predict both attitudes and behaviours practised towards refugees—fairness and care positively so, and sanctity and loyalty negatively so. Second, we find that moral foundational framing was successfully deployed to influence attitudes towards refugees, amongst some groups, most consistently in relation to the care frame and at times for the fairness frame. Third, we find that moral foundational framing had no impact on shifting pro-refugee behaviour.
Our results also demonstrate what type of attitudes are likely to shift by means of moral framing. Presenting individuals with care and fare frames evoked stronger support for voting intention for a pro-refugee political candidate. No such shifts were found for being not concerned over refugee numbers or refugee policy. These findings show that tailoring messaging based on an individual’s morals may hold potential in particular politicized domains [19,20,28]. This impact is weakened by the inability to find significance between framing and two of the three attitudinal measures.
When we take a closer look at the differential effect frames, the morals of care and fairness were associated with positive attitudes towards refugees, while authority was negatively associated. This finding had been reported previously [29]. The replication of this result highlights the level of significance that might be offered by framing. It might serve as a useful influencer of opinion and behaviour; however, the impact of moral values is more robust. Notably, this does not automatically make it useful for shifting attitudes: moral values are not likely to be changed by way of one experiment, but rather, are formed over the course of an individual’s psychological and moral development.
These findings do offer further support for the possible uses of moral foundational framing in garnering support and aiding in persuasion. Critically, support was not found in overturning moral impulses by way of framing; individuals who scored highly in binding values (sanctity and loyalty), who would typically not be forthcoming in support for refugees, were not persuaded by the sanctity and loyalty framed messaging. Our results thus failed to replicate findings by Feygina, Goldsmith and Jost [20], and Voelkel and Willer [19], who were able to shift moral positions based on this form of framing.
The predictive power of the moral foundations (rather than the frame) might offer a candid indication of where effective change lies: attempts at shaping core morals might hold more potential than adjusting the framing they encounter, especially as frames had no impact on behaviour, even if they shifted attitudes. An interesting finding relating to the discrepancy between attitudes and behaviours is found in the study’s secondary focus. When asked if they would support a hypothetical policy that would see more refugees brought into the UK, 65.24% of participants said they would. When they were asked to sign a petition in support of this goal, this number halved to 32.59%, thus again highlighting the discrepancy between changing attitudes and changing behaviours.
The inability of this study to evoke a change in moral applications might of course be due to ineffective deployment of moral framing: the moral frames used might not have been sufficiently congruent. Another possibility is that the loyalty and sanctity-framed texts might have served to prime individuals to feel morals of which they were already in possession. If this were true, it would highlight one of the potential difficulties of using framing in this way. Another possible limitation is the extent to which real behaviour can be captured online. In addition, the sampled population group, as is typical of online populations, was distinctly left-wing, as highlighted by the more than 2:1 ratio of Labour to Conservative voters amongst participants. This might be addressed by means of field experiments. An addressable weakness was identified in the measure that asked participants whether they would support a policy to increase refugee numbers by 5%: could responses to this have been influenced by the differing degrees of what 5% more refugees in the UK would actually mean in real-world numbers? A simple follow-up question could be employed in future to account for this.
The most pressing future avenue of enquiry will continue to be testing how and when framing can be catalysing enough so as to overturn a pre-held belief. This study only showed that framing can increase the strength of attitudes. While this is an important finding in and of itself, were the circumstances under which framing might overturn an attitude discovered, a significantly different range of possibilities will open to those invested in changing attitudes and behaviours towards the oppressed, marginalized and disenfranchised. The robust relationship found between moral values and attitudes towards refugees makes salient the importance of actively shaping moral foundations, in turn shaping attitudes and behaviours towards refugees.

Supplementary Materials

The following data analysis tables are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/bs12050118/s1. Analysis S1 Logit Regression Attitude with Control Group. Analysis S2 Logit Regression Attitude Without Control Group. Analysis S3 Logit Regression Petition with Control Group. Analysis S4 Logit Regression Petition Without Control Group. Analysis S5 Binomial Regression Donation with Control Group. Analysis S6 Binomial Regression Donation Without Control Group. References [32,33,34] are cited in the supplementary materials.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.M.; Data curation, T.M.; Formal analysis, T.M.; Investigation, T.M. and J.G.S.; Methodology, T.M. and J.G.S.; Project administration, T.M.; Resources, T.M.; Supervision, J.G.S.; Validation, J.G.S.; Visualization, T.M.; Writing—original draft, T.M.; Writing—review & editing, T.M. and J.G.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the London School of Economics and Political Sciences’ Ethical Committee, as part of the department of Psychological and Behavioural Science; the study was self-assessed as carrying no significant ethical concerns and proceeded as per the LSE’s ethical guidelines.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data collected as part of this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Framing Terms.
Table A1. Framing Terms.
Framing Words and Terms
Word Count
CareFairnessSanctityLoyaltyControl
Frame 1145142141146181
Frame 2140131130137107
Frame 32019221911
Total305292293302299
Framing Phrases
Frame 1welcoming the
stranger
justice…for allrooted in…decencylooking after one
another
n/a
treating the
neglected
equality…for allrooted in…purityLoyalty to our nationn/a
giving sheltergranting equality(does not) compromisee
thnic…integrity
(does not) compromise
the loyalty
n/a
compassionate
thing to do
just thing to do(does not) compromise
racial…integrity
…for one anothern/a
Phrases4444
Total Words12101014
Moral Words77810
Frame 2caring for allfairness for allpromoting purityloyalty to our
great nation
n/a
preventing harmpreventing injusticepreventing
infringements
on sanctity
ensuring we excel
at upholding
n/a
letting down a
vulnerable
group
denying basic
principle
of justice
sullying the
greatness
great namen/a
putting them in
harm’s way
free from injusticebelieves in the
inviolability
does not compromise
this country
n/a
free from danger,(free from)
oppression
ensure the purity of
this country
…enhances itn/a
…violence(free from)
inequality
is upheldour great peoplen/a
not coloured by
harm
not coloured
by injustice
is uncompromisedleading nations…n/a
Phrases7777n/a
Total Words23232324
Moral Words12101212
Frame 3increasing careincreasing fairnessprotecting pure valuesmaintaining
country’s greatness
n/a
Table A2. Treatment passages.
Table A2. Treatment passages.
TreatmentCareFairnessSanctityLoyaltyControl
Vignette1. Please read the following statement carefully:
“This country’s great heritage is rooted in welcoming the stranger and treating the neglected. These values are central to our proud British tradition. According to Professor Eric Kaufmann of the University of London, a large share of the children of immigrants have become White British.

Immigration has risen and fallen over time, but, like the English language, Britain’s culture is only superficially affected by foreign influence.

Taking refugees into our country, when well managed, does not risk threatening the ethnic or cultural make up of Britain. Indeed, Britain has absorbed many populations of refugees and migrants, French, Irish, Jews and black people, all of whom eventually melted into the white majority. Britain shapes its migrants, migration doesn’t shape Britain.

Giving shelter to victims of conflict, war and oppression is both a compassionate thing to do, and one that is of value to the indigenous society.”
“This country’s great heritage is rooted in justice and equality for all. These values are central to our proud British tradition. According to Professor Eric Kaufmann of the University of London, a large share of the children of immigrants have become White British.

Immigration has risen and fallen over time, but, like the English language, Britain’s culture is only superficially affected by foreign influence.

Taking refugees into our country, when well managed, does not risk threatening the ethnic or cultural make up of Britain. Indeed, Britain has absorbed many populations of refugees and migrants, French, Irish, Jews and black people, all of whom eventually melted into the white majority. Britain shapes its migrants, migration doesn’t shape Britain.

Granting equality to victims of conflict, war and oppression is both a just thing to do, and one is of value to the indigenous society.”
“This country’s great heritage is rooted in decency and purity. These values are central to our proud British tradition. According to Professor Eric Kaufmann of the University of London, a large share of the children of immigrants have become White British.

Immigration has risen and fallen over time, but, like the English language, Britain’s culture is only superficially affected by foreign influence.

Taking refugees into our country, when well managed, does not risk threatening the ethnic or cultural make up of Britain. Indeed, Britain has absorbed many populations of refugees and migrants, French, Irish, Jews and black people, all of whom eventually melted into the white majority.
Britain shapes its migrants, migration doesn’t shape Britain.

Taking in victims of conflict, war and oppression is the right thing to do, and does not compromise the ethnic and racial integrity of this country.”
“This country’s great heritage is rooted in looking after one another. Loyalty to our nation is central to our proud British tradition. According to Professor Eric Kaufmann of the University of London, a large share of the children of immigrants have become White British.

Immigration has risen and fallen over time, but, like the English language, Britain’s culture is only superficially affected by foreign influence.

Taking refugees into our country, when well managed, does not risk threatening the ethnic or cultural make up of Britain. Indeed, Britain has absorbed many populations of refugees and migrants, French, Irish, Jews and black people, all of whom eventually melted into the white majority. Britain shapes its migrants, migration doesn’t shape Britain.

Taking in victims of conflict, war and oppression is the right thing to do, and does not compromise the loyalty we have for one another, or our country.”
Policy formation is one of the key aspects to modern government. Policies address key areas of both day-to-day citizen life, as well as the direction of the country as whole. A bad policy can cost a government power, where the electorate respond at the next election. Policies can be both partisan and bipartisan, but typically require a parliamentary majority to be enacted, in countries that utilise a parliamentary democracy. At times, a major policy decision will be decided by way of a referendum. Referenda are an example of direct democracy, wherein the people make a decision directly. Switzerland is often cited as an example of a country that regularly utilises referenda.
The role of lobbying groups in influencing government policy is an area of the workings of modern government that is deemed as undermining democracy. Lobbying groups and lobbyists do the work of those who pay them, allowing those with greater access to money, greater ability to hire forces for lobbying. Recent moves have been taken in countries such as the US and the UK to limit the influence of lobbyists.
Speech2. Below is an excerpt from the speech of politician, Candidate X, who is running in the upcoming election. This part of the speech relates to his refugee policy.2...This part of the speech relates to his views
about election reform.
“My vision for this country is based on the principle of caring for all and preventing harm to those most in need of protection.

I think it is regrettable that this great country only accepted 10,000 refugees in 2018—we are letting down a vulnerable group of human beings, and putting them in harm’s way. I believe we are ignoring one of this country’s great values and should strive to rectify this immediately.

Every human being is born with the right to live a life that is free from danger and violence Seeing to it that refugees are given this right should be a priority for all of us. Our policies should reflect this goal.

Vote for me, and I will fight for the right of every human being to live a life that is not coloured by harm.”
“My vision for this country is based on the principle of fairness for all and preventing injustice to those most in need of protection.

I think it is regrettable that this great country only accepted 10,000 refugees in 2018—we are denying the basic principle of justice for all, and ignoring one of this country’s great values and should strive to rectify this immediately.

Every human being is born with the right to live a life that is free from injustice, oppression and inequality. Seeing to it that refugees are given this right should be a priority for all of us. Our policies should reflect this goal.

Vote for me, and I will fight for the right of every human being to live a life that is not coloured by injustice.
“My vision for this country is based on the principles of promoting purity and preventing infringements on the sanctity of our society.

I think it is regrettable that this great country only accepted 10,000 refugees in 2018—we are sullying this country’s greatness by failing to live up to what we could be and should strive to rectify this immediately.

Every human being is born with the right to live a life that allows them to strive towards their potential. Seeing to that that refugees are given their rights should be a priority for anyone who believes in the inviolability of this country. Our policies should reflect this goal.

Vote for me, and I will fight to ensure the purity of this country and its vision is upheld and uncompromised.”
“My vision for this country is based on the principle of loyalty to our great nation and ensuring we excel at upholding its great name.

I think it is regrettable that this great country only accepted 10,000 refugees in 2018—we are letting down a vulnerable group of human beings, and putting them in harm’s way. I believe we are ignoring one of this country’s great values and should strive to rectify this immediately.

Protecting fellow humans from danger, violence and threat, does not compromise this country, in fact, it enhances it. Seeing to it that refugees are given this right should be a priority for all of us. Our policies should reflect this goal.

Vote for me, and I will fight to ensure this our great people continue to be among the leading nations on earth.”
“There is a need for a rethinking of the way in which elections in this country are judged. For too long, the First-Past-the-Post system has disadvantaged certain voices while granting too much weight to other voices. While the thinking of using this system might have made sense in the past, the UK is in need of modernising.
By switching to a Proportional Representation (PR) system, this country will take a step in the right direction. The PR system is already being used by many other countries around the world, the system allows for a much realer realisation of the fundamental principle underlying democracy, “one person, one vote”.
PolicyIn the interest of increasing the care of refugees,
Candidate X will increase refugee settlements in this country by 5%.
In the interest of increasing fairness towards refugees, Candidate X will increase refugee settlements in this country by 5%.In the interest of protecting the pure
values of this country, Candidate X will increase refugee settlements in this country by 5%.
In the interest of maintaining our
country’s greatness Candidate X will increase refugee settlements in this country by 5%.
Candidate X will increase refugee
settlements in this country by 5%.

References

  1. UNHCR. Asylum in the UK. 2019. Available online: https://www.unhcr.org/asylum-in-the-uk.html (accessed on 10 June 2020).
  2. Podesta, J. The Climate Crisis, Migration, and Refugees. The Brookings Institute. 2019. Available online: https://www.brookings.edu/wp-content/uploads/2019/07/Brookings_Blum_2019_climate.pdf (accessed on 15 August 2020).
  3. Blinder, S. Imagined Immigration: The Impact of Different Meanings of ‘Immigrants’ in Public Opinion and Policy Debates in Britain. Political Stud. 2013, 63, 80–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Getmansky, A.; Sınmazdemir, T.; Zeitzoff, T. Refugees, xenophobia, and domestic conflict. J. Peace Res. 2018, 55, 491–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Facchini, G.; Margalit, Y.; Nakata, H. Countering public opposition to immigration: The impact of information campaigns. Eur. Econ. Rev. 2021, 141, 103959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Steele, L.G.; Abdelaaty, L. Ethnic diversity and attitudes towards refugees. J. Ethn. Migr. Stud. 2018, 45, 1833–1856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Alesina, A.; Miano, A.; Stantcheva, S. Immigration and Redistribution; National Bureau of Economic Research: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Azarnert, L.V. Refugee resettlement, redistribution and growth. Eur. J. Political Econ. 2018, 54, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Grigorieff, A.; ROTH, C.; Ubfal, D. Does Information Change Attitudes Towards Immigrants? Representative Evidence from Survey Experiments. SSRN Electron. J. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Stangor, C.; Sechrist, G.B.; Jost, J.T. Changing Racial Beliefs by Providing Consensus Information. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 27, 486–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Dobbs, M.; Crano, W.D. Outgroup Accountability in the Minimal Group Paradigm: Implications for Aversive Discrimination and Social Identity Theory. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 27, 355–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Blanchard, F.A.; Lilly, T.; Vaughn, L.A. Reducing the Expression of Racial Prejudice. Psychol. Sci. 1991, 2, 101–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Paluck, E.L. Peer Pressure Against Prejudice: A Field Experimental Test of a National High School Prejudice Reduction Program; Working Paper; Harvard University: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  14. Kaufmann, E. Assimilation and the Immigration Debate: Shifting People’s Attitudes. The London School of Economics and Political Science. 2016. Available online: https://blogs.lse.ac.uk/politicsandpolicy/assimilation-and-the-immigration-debate-shifting-peoples-attitudes/ (accessed on 10 June 2020).
  15. Galinsky, A.D.; Moskowitz, G.B. Perspective-taking: Decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 78, 708–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Lecheler, S.; De Vreese, C.H. How Long Do News Framing Effects Last? A Systematic review of Longitudinal Studies. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 2016, 40, 3–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Druckman, J.N.; McDermott, R. Emotion and the Framing of Risky Choice. Politics Behav. 2008, 30, 297–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Druckman, J.N.; Nelson, K.R. Framing and Deliberation: How Citizens’ Conversations Limit Elite Influence. Am. J. Politics Sci. 2003, 47, 729–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Voelkel, J.G.; Willer, R. Resolving the Progressive Paradox: Conservative Value Framing of Progressive Economic Policies Increases Candidate Support. 2019. Available online: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3385818 (accessed on 10 June 2020).
  20. Feygina, I.; Jost, J.T.; Goldsmith, R.E. System Justification, the Denial of Global Warming, and the Possibility of “System-Sanctioned Change”. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 36, 326–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Graham, J.; Haidt, J.; Koleva, S.; Motyl, M.; Iyer, R.; Wojcik, S.; Ditto, P. Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 47, 55–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Iyer, R.; Koleva, S.; Graham, J.; Ditto, P.; Haidt, J. Understanding Libertarian Morality: The Psychological Dispositions of Self-Identified Libertarians. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e42366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Fisk, K. Refugee Geography and the Diffusion of Armed Conflict in Africa. Civ. Wars 2014, 16, 255–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sherif, M. Experimental Study of Positive and Negative Intergroup Attitudes between Experimentally Produced Groups: Robbers Cave Study; University of Oklahoma Press: Norman, OK, USA, 1954. [Google Scholar]
  25. Yitmen, Ş.; Verkuyten, M. Feelings toward refugees and non-Muslims in Turkey: The roles of national and religious identifications, and multiculturalism. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2017, 48, 90–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Haidt, J.; Graham, J.; Joseph, C. Above and Below Left–Right: Ideological Narratives and Moral Foundations. Psychol. Inq. 2009, 20, 110–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Thornhill, R.; Fincher, C.L. The Parasite-Stress Theory of Values and Sociality: Infectious Disease, History and Human Values Worldwide; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Nath, L.; Pedriana, N.; Gifford, C.; Mcauley, J.W.; Fülöp, M. Examining Moral Foundations Theory through Immigration Attitudes. Athens J. Soc. Sci. 2022, 9, 9–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Captari, L.E.; Shannonhouse, L.; Hook, J.N.; Aten, J.D.; Davis, E.B.; Davis, D.E.; Van Tongeren, D.; Hook, J.R. Prejudicial and Welcoming Attitudes toward Syrian Refugees: The Roles of Cultural Humility and Moral Foundations. J. Psychol. Theol. 2019, 47, 123–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bryan, C.J.; Tipton, E.; Yeager, D.S. Behavioural science is unlikely to change the world without a heterogeneity revolution. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021, 5, 980–989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Palan, S.; Schitter, C. Prolific.ac—A subject pool for online experiments. J. Behav. Exp. Finance 2018, 17, 22–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. The Jamovi Project. Jamovi. (Version 1.2) [Computer Software]. 2020. Available online: https://www.jamovi.org (accessed on 10 June 2020).
  33. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. (Version 3.6). 2019. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/ (accessed on 10 June 2020).
  34. Ripley, B.; Venables, W.; Bates, D.M.; Hornik, K.; Gebhardt, A.; Firth, D. MASS: Support Functions and Datasets for Venables and Ripley’s MASS. [R package]. 2018. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=MASS (accessed on 10 June 2020).
Figure 1. Morally framed passage (care).
Figure 1. Morally framed passage (care).
Behavsci 12 00118 g001
Figure 2. Morally framed political speech excerpt (care).
Figure 2. Morally framed political speech excerpt (care).
Behavsci 12 00118 g002
Figure 3. Morally framed policy proposal (care).
Figure 3. Morally framed policy proposal (care).
Behavsci 12 00118 g003
Figure 4. Displaying moral foundation scores by (A) the average attitude toward refugees and (B) pro-refugee behaviour.
Figure 4. Displaying moral foundation scores by (A) the average attitude toward refugees and (B) pro-refugee behaviour.
Behavsci 12 00118 g004
Table 1. Internal Validity of Randomization.
Table 1. Internal Validity of Randomization.
Demographic InformationCare (n = 210)Fair (n = 215)Loyalty (n = 217)Sanctity (n = 216)Control (n = 218)
Male8385898284
Female126128125132130
White178184189176189
Ethnic Minority3231284029
Bachelor’s Degree and Higher105102104109112
Less Than Bachelor’s105113113107106
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mobayed, T.; Sanders, J.G. Moral Foundational Framing and Its Impact on Attitudes and Behaviours. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 118. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12050118

AMA Style

Mobayed T, Sanders JG. Moral Foundational Framing and Its Impact on Attitudes and Behaviours. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(5):118. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12050118

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mobayed, Tamim, and Jet G. Sanders. 2022. "Moral Foundational Framing and Its Impact on Attitudes and Behaviours" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 5: 118. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12050118

APA Style

Mobayed, T., & Sanders, J. G. (2022). Moral Foundational Framing and Its Impact on Attitudes and Behaviours. Behavioral Sciences, 12(5), 118. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12050118

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop