Feeling One Thing and Doing Another: How Expressions of Guilt and Shame Influence Hypocrisy Judgment
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Implications of Guilt and Shame for Future Behaviors
1.2. Seeing Others’ Self-Conscious Emotions
1.3. The Present Study: Feeling One Thing and Doing Another
2. Study 1
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants and Design
2.1.2. Procedure and Materials
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Shame and Guilt Intensity
2.2.2. Change Expectation
2.2.3. Hypocrisy Judgment
2.3. Discussion
3. Study 2
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants and Design
3.1.2. Procedure and Materials
“I feel bad about making the decision to start the new program and causing her suffering. I would like to contact her right now and apologize for what I have done. I just want to tell her that I am sorry for what I have done, and that I would like to help make things right.”
“I feel bad about making the decision to start the new program and causing her suffering. I think I am a terrible person for what I have done, and I feel like I just want to hide right now.”
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Change Expectation
3.2.2. Hypocrisy Judgment
3.3. Discussion
4. General Discussion
Limitations and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Appendix A.1. Scenario in Study 1
Appendix A.2. Scenario in Study 2
References
- Tangney, J.P.; Dearing, R.L. Shame and Guilt; Guilford Publications, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Leary, M. How the self became involved in affective experience: Three sources of self-reflective emotions. In The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research; Tracy, J.L., Robins, R.W., Tangney, J.P., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 38–52. [Google Scholar]
- Sheikh, S.; Janoff-Bulman, R. The “shoulds” and “should nots” of moral emotions: A self-regulatory perspective on shame and guilt. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2010, 36, 213–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tracy, J.L.; Robins, R.W. Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical model. Psychol. Inq. 2004, 15, 103–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Stillwell, A.M.; Heatherton, T.F. Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychol. Bull. 1994, 115, 243–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jones, W.H.; Kugler, K.; Adams, P. You always hurt the one you love: Guilt and transgressions against relationship partners. In Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride; Tangney, J.P., Fischer, K.W., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995; pp. 301–321. [Google Scholar]
- Keltner, D.; Buswell, B.N. Evidence for the distinctness of embarrassment, shame, and guilt: A study of recalled antecedents and facial expressions of emotion. Cogn. Emot. 1996, 10, 155–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Niedenthal, P.M.; Tangney, J.P.; Gavanski, I. “If only I weren’t” versus “if only I hadn’t”: Distinguishing shame and guilt in counterfactual thinking. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 67, 585–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tracy, J.L.; Robins, R.W. Appraisal antecedents of shame and guilt: Support for a theoretical model. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2006, 32, 1339–1351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leith, K.P.; Baumeister, R.F. Empathy, shame, guilt, and narratives of interpersonal conflicts: Guilt-prone people are better at perspective taking. J. Personal. 1998, 66, 1–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Hooge, I.E.; Breugelmans, S.M.; Zeelenberg, M. Not so ugly after all: When shame acts as a commitment device. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 95, 933–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keltner, D.; Haidt, J. Social functions of emotions. In Emotions: Current Issues and Future Directions; Mayne, T.J., Bonanno, G.A., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2001; pp. 192–213. [Google Scholar]
- Hosser, D.; Windzio, M.; Greve, W. Guilt and shame as predictors of recidivism: A longitudinal study with young prisoners. Crim. Justice Behav. 2008, 35, 138–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tangney, J.P.; Stuewig, J.; Martinez, A.G. Two faces of shame: The roles of shame and guilt in predicting recidivism. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 25, 799–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ames, D.R.; Johar, G.V. I’ll know what you’re like when I see how you feel: How and when affective displays adjust behavior-based impressions. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 20, 586–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frijda, N.H. The Emotions; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson, C.; Thompson, L. Affect from the top down: How powerful individuals’ positive affect shapes negotiations. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2004, 95, 125–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Melo, C.; Carnevale, P.J.; Gratch, J. Reading people’s minds from emotion expressions in interdependent decision making. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 106, 73–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- van Kleef, G.A.; De Dreu, C.K.W.; Manstead, A.S.R. Supplication and appeasement in conflict and negotiation: The interpersonal effects of disappointment, worry, guilt, and regret. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 91, 124–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Echterhoff, G.; Higgins, E.T.; Levine, J.M. Shared reality: Experiencing commonality with others’ inner states about the world. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 4, 496–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, R.A.; Kamtekar, R.; Nichols, S.; Pizarro, D.A. “False positive” emotions, responsibility, and moral character. Cognition 2021, 214, 104770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giner-Sorolla, R.; Espinosa, P. Social cuing of guilt by anger and of shame by disgust. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 49–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proeve, M.; Tudor, S. Remorse: Psychological and Jurisprudential Perspectives; Ashgate: Farnham, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Silver, J.R.; Berryessa, C.M. Remorse, perceived offender immorality, and lay sentencing preferences. J. Exp. Criminol. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gold, G.J.; Weiner, B. Remorse, confession, group identity, and expectancies about repeating a transgression. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 22, 291–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lazare, A. On Apology; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Robinson, D.T.; Smith-Lovin, L.; Tsoudis, O. Heinous crime or unfortunate accident? The effects of remorse on responses to mock criminal confessions. Soc. Forces 1994, 73, 175–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, H.B. Shame and Guilt in Neurosis; International Universities Press: New York, NY, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Howell, A.J.; Turowski, J.B.; Buro, K. Guilt, empathy, and apology. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2012, 53, 917–922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stearns, D.C.; Parrott, W.G. When feeling bad makes you look good: Guilt, shame, and person perception. Cogn. Emot. 2012, 26, 407–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hareli, S.; Eisikovits, Z. The role of communicating social emotions accompanying apologies in forgiveness. Motiv. Emot. 2006, 30, 189–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barden, J.; Rucker, D.D.; Petty, R.E. “Saying one thing and doing another”: Examining the impact of event order on hypocrisy judgments of others. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2005, 31, 1463–1474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- O’Connor, K.; Effron, D.A.; Lucas, B.J. Moral cleansing as hypocrisy: When private acts of charity make you feel better than you deserve. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2020, 119, 540–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stone, J.; Fernandez, N.C. To practice what we preach: The use of hypocrisy and cognitive dissonance to motivate behavior change. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2008, 2, 1024–1051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monin, B.; Merritt, A. Moral hypocrisy, moral inconsistency, and the struggle for moral integrity. In The Social Psychology of Morality: Exploring the Causes of Good and Evil; Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P.R., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2011; pp. 167–184. [Google Scholar]
- Festinger, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance; Stanford University Press: Stanford, CA, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Knobe, J. Intentional action and side effects in ordinary language. Analysis 2003, 63, 190–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferguson, T.J.; Brugman, D.; White, J.E.; Eyre, H.L. Shame and guilt as morally warranted experiences. In The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research; Tracy, J.L., Robins, R.W., Tangney, J.P., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 330–348. [Google Scholar]
- Planalp, S. Communicating Emotion: Social, Moral, and Cultural Processes; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Trope, Y.; Liberman, N. Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 117, 440–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Haidt, J. The moral emotions. In Handbook of Affective Science; Davidson, R.J., Scherer, K.R., Goldsmith, H.H., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2003; pp. 852–870. [Google Scholar]
- Prinz, J.J.; Nichols, S. Moral emotions. In The Moral Psychology Handbook; Doris, J.M., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 111–146. [Google Scholar]
- Vaish, A. The prosocial functions of early social emotions: The case of guilt. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018, 20, 25–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harrel, W.A. The effects of alcohol use and offerder remorsefulness on sentencing decisions. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1981, 11, 83–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jordan, J.J.; Sommers, R. When does moral engagement risk triggering a hypocrite penalty? Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2022, 48, 101404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- ten Brinke, L.; MacDonald, S.; Porter, S. Crocodile tears: Facial, verbal and body language behaviours associated with genuine and fabricated remorse. Law Hum. Behav. 2012, 36, 51–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Giner-Sorolla, R.; Castano, E.; Espinosa, P.; Brown, R.J. Shame expressions reduce the recipient’s insult from outgroup reparations. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 519–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Halmesvaara, O.; Harjunen, V.J.; Aulbach, M.B.; Ravaja, N. How bodily expressions of emotion after norm violation influence perceivers’ moral judgments and prevent social exclusion: A socio-functional approach to nonverbal shame display. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0232298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Choi, H. Feeling One Thing and Doing Another: How Expressions of Guilt and Shame Influence Hypocrisy Judgment. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 504. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12120504
Choi H. Feeling One Thing and Doing Another: How Expressions of Guilt and Shame Influence Hypocrisy Judgment. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(12):504. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12120504
Chicago/Turabian StyleChoi, Hyeman. 2022. "Feeling One Thing and Doing Another: How Expressions of Guilt and Shame Influence Hypocrisy Judgment" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 12: 504. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12120504
APA StyleChoi, H. (2022). Feeling One Thing and Doing Another: How Expressions of Guilt and Shame Influence Hypocrisy Judgment. Behavioral Sciences, 12(12), 504. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12120504