People Cheat on Task Performance When They Feel Bored: The Mediating Role of State Self-Efficacy
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. State Boredom
1.2. State Boredom vs. Trait Boredom
1.3. Boredom and Cheating Behaviors
1.4. State Self-Efficacy vs. Trait Self-Efficacy
1.5. Linking State Boredom and Cheating Behaviors: The Mediating Effect of State Self-Efficacy
1.6. Present Study
2. Study 1 Does Cheating Behavior Differ between High-State Boredom and Low-State Boredom?
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Measurement and Procedure
2.2. Results
2.3. Brief Discussion
3. Study 2 Is the Effect of State Boredom on Cheating Behaviors Mediated by State Self-Efficacy?
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Procedure
3.2. Results
3.3. Brief Discussion
4. General Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Fisher, C.D. Boredom at work: A neglected concept. Hum. Relat. 1993, 46, 395–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T.; Daniels, L.M.; Stupnisky, R.H.; Perry, R.P. Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring control-value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. J. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 102, 531–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fahlman, S.A.; Mercer-Lynn, K.B.; Flora, D.B.; Eastwood, J.D. Development and validation of the multi-dimensional boredom scale. Assessment 2011, 20, 68–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Retana, C. Consideraciones acerca del aburrimiento como emoción moral. Káñina 2011, 35, 179–190. [Google Scholar]
- Van Tilburg, W.A.; Igou, E.R. Can boredom help? Increased prosocial intentions in response to boredom. Self Identity 2016, 16, 82–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van Tilburg, W.A.; Igou, E.R. Boredom begs to differ: Differentiation from other negative emotions. Emotion 2016, 17, 309–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pekrun, R.; Elliot, A.J.; Maier, M.A. Achievement goals and discrete achievement emotions: A theoretical model and prospective test. J. Educ. Psychol. 2006, 98, 583–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pekrun, R.; Frenzel, A.C.; Goetz, T.; Perry, R.P. The control-value theory of achievement emotions: An integrative approach to emotions in education. In Emotion in Education; Elsevier Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2007; pp. 13–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pekrun, R.; Stephens, E.J. Academic emotions. In APA Educational Psychology Handbook; Volume 2: Individual Differences and Cultural and Contextual Factors; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2012; pp. 3–31. [Google Scholar]
- Chan, C.S.; Van Tilburg, W.A.; Igou, E.R.; Poon, C.; Tam, K.Y.; Wong, V.U.; Cheung, S.K. Situational meaninglessness and state boredom: Cross-sectional and experience-sampling findings. Motiv. Emot. 2018, 42, 555–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Neu, J. Boring from within: Endogenous versus reactive boredom. In Emotions in Psychopathology: Theory and Research; Flack, W.F., Laird, J.D., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1998; pp. 158–170. [Google Scholar]
- Todman, M. Boredom and psychotic disorders: Cognitive and motivational issues. Psychiatry 2003, 66, 146–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Farmer, R.; Sundberg, N.D. Boredom proneness: The development and correlates of a new scale. J. Personal. Assess. 1986, 50, 4–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barbalet, J.M. Boredom and social meaning. Br. J. Sociol. 1999, 50, 631–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fahlman, S.A.; Mercer, K.B.; Gaskovski, P.; Eastwood, A.E.; Eastwood, J.D. Does a lack of life meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and experimental analyses. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 2009, 28, 307–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frankl, V.E. Man’s Search for Meaning: An Introduction to Logotherapy; Pocket Books: New York, NY, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
- Sansone, C.; Weir, C.; Harpster, L.; Morgan, C. Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a self-regulatory mechanism. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 63, 379–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Tilburg, W.A.P.; Igou, E.R. On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motiv. Emot. 2012, 36, 181–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gerlach, P.; Teodorescu, K.; Hertwig, R. The truth about lies: A meta-analysis on dishonest behavior. Psychol. Bull. 2019, 145, 1–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kass, S.J.; Vodanovich, S.J.; Stanny, C.J.; Taylor, T.M. Watching the clock: Boredom and vigilance performance. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2001, 92 (Suppl. 3), 969–976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruursema, K.; Kessler, S.R.; Spector, P.E. Bored employees misbehaving: The relationship between boredom and counterproductive work behaviour. Work Stress 2011, 25, 93–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mozammel, S. The Effect of negative work outcomes on employee deviant behavior: Exploring the relationship between perceived injustice, boredom, moral disengagement and work withdrawal in training units. Rev. Int. Geogr. Educ. 2021, 11, 1453–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 191–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Fearful expectations and avoidant actions as coeffects of perceived self-inefficacy. Am. Psychol. 1986, 41, 1389–1391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control; Freeman: London, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Bong, M.; Skaalvik, E.M. Academic self-concept and self-efficacy: How different are they really? Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 15, 1–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schunk, D.H. Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educ. Psychol. 1991, 26, 207–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Linnenbrink, E.A. The role of affect in student learning: A multi-dimensional approach to considering the interaction of affect, motivation, and engagement. In Emotion in Education; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2007; pp. 107–124. [Google Scholar]
- Daniels, K.; Beesley, N.J.; Cheyne, A.J.T.; Wilmarisiri, V.P. Coping processes linking the demands-control-support model, affect and risky decisions at work. Hum. Relat. 2008, 61, 845–874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Putwain, D.; Sander, P.; Larkin, D. Academic self-efficacy in study-related skills and behaviours: Relations with learning-related emotions and academic success. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 83, 633–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tze, V.M.; Daniels, L.M.; Klassen, R.M.; Li, J.C.H. Canadian and Chinese university students’ approaches to coping with academic boredom. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2013, 23, 32–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Y.; Lu, Z. The relationship between academic self-efficacy and academic-related boredom: MAOA gene as a moderator. Youth Soc. 2017, 49, 254–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Folkman, S.; Lazarus, R.S. If it changes it must be a process: Study of emotion and coping during three stages of a college examination. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1985, 48, 150–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peixoto, F.; Sanches, C.; Mata, L.; Monteiro, V. “How do you feel about math?”: Relationships between competence and value appraisals, achievement emotions and academic achievement. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2017, 32, 385–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cizek, G.J. Cheating on Tests: How to Do It, Detect It, and Prevent It; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Murdock, T.B.; Hale, N.M.; Weber, M.J. Predictors of cheating among early adolescents: Academic and social motivations. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2001, 26, 96–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Finn, K.V.; Frone, M.R. Academic performance and cheating: Moderating role of school identification and self-efficacy. J. Educ. Res. 2004, 97, 115–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cohen, J. A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 112, 155–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kilduff, G.; Galinksy, A.; Gallo, E.; Reade, J. Whatever it takes to win: Rivalry increases unethical behavior. Acad. Manag. J. 2016, 59, 1508–1534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pierce, J.R.; Kilduff, G.J.; Galinsky, A.D.; Sivanathan, N. From glue to gasoline: How competition turns perspective takers unethical. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 1986–1994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hauser, D.J.; Ellsworth, P.C.; Gonzalez, R. Are manipulation checks necessary? Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Parrot, W.G.; Hertel, P. Research methods in cognition and emotion. In Handbook of Cognition and Emotion; Dalgleish, T., Power, M., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons Ltd: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1999; pp. 61–81. [Google Scholar]
- Alt, D. Assessing the connection between self-efficacy for learning and justifying academic cheating in higher education learning environments. J. Acad. Ethics 2015, 13, 77–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fritz, M.S.; MacKinnon, D.P. Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 18, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schwarzer, R.; Jerusalem, M. Generalized Self-Efficacy scale. In Measures in Health Psychology: A User’s Portfolio. Causal and Control Beliefs; Weinman, J., Wright, S., Johnston, M., Eds.; NFER-NELSON: Berkshire, UK, 1995; pp. 35–37. [Google Scholar]
- Podsakoff, P.M.; MacKenzie, S.B.; Lee, J.Y.; Podsakoff, N.P. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hayes, J.F. Modeling and Analysis of Computer Communications Networks; Springer Science & Business Media: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Wang, S.; Song, Q.; Cheng, J. Construction of path analysis model on influence factor of general self-efficacy in urban and rural elderly. Chin. J. Coal Ind. Med. 2016, 19, 1770–1774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panadero, E.; Jonsson, A.; Botella, J. Effects of self-assessment on self-regulated learning and self-efficacy: Four meta-analyses. Educ. Res. Rev 2017, 22, 74–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Y.; Xu, L.; Qin, W.; Zhang, J.; Xia, Y.; Jing, X.; Lu, L.; Jiao, A.; Li, Y. Gender difference in general self-efficacy among young-old elderly aged 60-74 in rural Shandong China: A Cross-Sectional Survey. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 5070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kaiser, C.R.; Vick, S.B.; Major, B. Prejudice expectations moderate preconscious attention to cues that are threatening to social identity. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 332–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hackett, G.; Campbell, N.K. Task self-efficacy and task interest as a function of performance on a gender-neutral task. J. Vocat. Behav. 1987, 30, 203–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Betz, N.E.; Hackett, G. The relationship of career-related self-efficacy expectations to perceived career options in college women and men. J. Couns. Psychol. 1981, 28, 399–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Post-Kammer, P.; Smith, P.L. Sex differences in career self-efficacy, consideration, and interests of eighth and ninth graders. J. Couns. Psychol. 1985, 32, 551–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Post-Kammer, P.; Smith, P.L. Sex differences in math and science career self-efficacy among disadvantaged students. J. Vocat. Behav. 1986, 29, 89–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elpidorou, A. The moral dimensions of boredom: A call for research. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2017, 21, 30–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lee, H.Y.; List, A. Examining students’ self-efficacy and perceptions of task difficulty in learning from multiple texts. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2021, 90, 20–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yam, K.C.; Chen, X.P.; Reynolds, S.J. Ego depletion and its paradoxical effects on ethical decision making. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2014, 124, 204–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Job, V.; Dweck, C.S.; Walton, G.M. Ego depletion—Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 1686–1693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Francis, Z.; Milyavskaya, M.; Lin, H.; Inzlicht, M. Development of a within-subject, repeated-measures ego-depletion paradigm: Inconsistent results and future recommendations. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 49, 271–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Milyavskaya, M.; Inzlicht, M.; Johnson, T.; Larson, M.J. Reward sensitivity following boredom and cognitive effort: A high-powered neurophysiological investigation. Neuropsychologia 2019, 123, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
High-Boredom Condition M (SD) | Low-Boredom Condition M (SD) | Range | p | Cohen’s d | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Boredom manipulation check | 4.96 (2.04) | 3.49 (1.76) | 1–7 | <0.001 | 0.77 |
Cheating behaviors | 1.47 (0.75) | 1.05 (0.74) | 0–2 | <0.01 | 0.56 |
Gender | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Age | 19.85 (1.33) | 19.89 (1.24) | 18–23 | 0.87 | 0.03 |
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
High-boredom condition (N = 47) | ||||
1. Boredom manipulation check | 1 | |||
2. Cheating behaviors | 0.10 | 1 | ||
3. Gender | 0.12 | −0.21 | 1 | |
4. Age | −0.07 | 0.12 | −0.001 | 1 |
Low-boredom condition (N = 57) | ||||
1. Boredom manipulation check | 1 | |||
2. Cheating behaviors | −0.16 | 1 | ||
3. Gender | −0.06 | −0.02 | 1 | |
4. Age | −0.18 | −0.05 | 0.06 | 1 |
M (SD) | Range | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. State boredom | 3.20 (1.01) | 1.15–6.40 | 1 | ||||
2. Sate self-efficacy | 3.60 (0.87) | 1.30–6.00 | −0.43 ** | 1 | |||
3. Cheating behaviors | 1.03 (0.82) | 0–2 | 0.20 * | −0.30 ** | 1 | ||
4. Age | 19.54 (0.89) | 18–22 | −0.17 * | −0.06 | 0.11 | 1 | |
5. Gender | -- | -- | 0.05 | −0.02 | 0.02 | −0.07 | 1 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Feng, C.; Liu, C.; Zhong, M. People Cheat on Task Performance When They Feel Bored: The Mediating Role of State Self-Efficacy. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 380. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100380
Feng C, Liu C, Zhong M. People Cheat on Task Performance When They Feel Bored: The Mediating Role of State Self-Efficacy. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(10):380. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100380
Chicago/Turabian StyleFeng, Chun, Chuanjun Liu, and Min Zhong. 2022. "People Cheat on Task Performance When They Feel Bored: The Mediating Role of State Self-Efficacy" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 10: 380. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100380
APA StyleFeng, C., Liu, C., & Zhong, M. (2022). People Cheat on Task Performance When They Feel Bored: The Mediating Role of State Self-Efficacy. Behavioral Sciences, 12(10), 380. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100380