Forensic Use of the Five Domains Model for Assessing Suffering in Cases of Animal Cruelty
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Changes in Conceptual Frameworks for Understanding Animal Welfare
2.1. Emergence of Two Key Conceptual Frameworks: Biological Functioning and Affective State
2.2. Integrating the Conceptual Frameworks of Biological Functioning and Affective State
2.3. Expert Opinions and Conceptual Frameworks for Understanding Animal Welfare
2.4. Expert Opinions That Incorporate Consideration of Affective States: Canadian Experience
2.5. Sentience and Up-To-Date Animal Welfare Understanding Are Well Aligned
3. Negative and Positive Affects That Sentient Animals Are Considered to Experience
- There are two major types of negative affective experiences: those that reflect imbalances or disturbances in the internal physical/functional state of the body, and those elicited from outside the body that contribute to an animal’s perception of its external circumstances.
- Internally generated negative affects include breathlessness, thirst, hunger, pain, nausea, dizziness, debility, weakness, and sickness. Each of these affects motivates animals to behave in particular ways that help to secure their survival. However, correction of the associated imbalance or disturbance, whether achieved by the animal unaided or with support from animal care staff, at best will usually only result in a neutral, not positive, affective outcome.
- Externally generated negative affects include anxiety, fear, panic, frustration, anger, helplessness, loneliness, boredom, and depression. These are mainly elicited by threatening, cramped, barren, and/or isolated circumstances, and will persist for as long as such conditions prevail. These are situation-related negative affects and human intervention is usually required to correct them.
- Providing animals with opportunities to engage in behaviours they find rewarding can replace situation-related negative affects with positive experiences. Such opportunities become available when social animals are kept with congenial others in spacious, stimulus-rich, and safe environments.
- Rewarding behaviours may arise when the key attributes of animals’ environments include, but are not limited to, the following: variability that provides a congenial balance between predictability and unpredictability; access to preferred sites for resting, thermal comfort, and voiding excrement; environmental choices that encourage exploratory and food acquisition behaviours which are enjoyable; availability of a variety of feeds having pleasurable tastes and textures; and circumstances that enable social species to engage in bonding and bond affirming activities and, as appropriate, other affiliative interactions such as maternal, paternal, or group care of young, play behaviour, and sexual activity. Expressed in general terms, the associated positive affects are likely to include various forms of comfort, pleasure, interest, confidence, and a sense of control.
4. Updated Understanding of the Concept of Suffering or Distress
5. The Five Domains Model for Animal Welfare Assessment
6. Examples of Cruelty or Neglect Interpreted by Reference to Affective Experiences in Dogs
7. Recommendations
- 1.
- Can the animal experience aversive events that could cause it to suffer?If the animal is sentient and was/is conscious when ill-treated, the answer may be “Yes” (see Section 2.2, Section 2.3, Section 2.4 and Section 2.5). The ability of an animal to experience various negative affects will vary between individuals, according to age, species, previous experience, and personality.
- 2.
- Does current legislation recognise suffering or equivalent negative welfare states in animals?The precise wording of national or regional laws or regulations in relation to the up-to-date understanding of suffering and related matters outlined in Section 4 should indicate what would be credible grounds for a prosecution in each country. Expert opinions need to be framed with this in mind, and, of course, experts must first evaluate whether a prosecution can be mounted at all given the particularities of the relevant laws or regulations.
- 3.
- Were conditions present that would cause the animal to suffer?Systematic evaluation of circumstances related to the nutritional, environmental, health, and behavioural domains of the Model will assist here (see Section 5). It is helpful to use a checklist for recording factual information about each domain. The checklists developed as part of a protocol strongly focused on suffering understood generically [90], which does not incorporate the detailed affective evaluations described here, would nevertheless be a good starting point.
- 4.
- Which affective state(s) would the animal likely experience?Reference to the Model helps when answering this question (see Section 5).
- 5.
- Is there physical and/or behavioural evidence that the animal actually did suffer or is suffering?This may be evaluated by direct observation of the animal supported by careful interpretation of other specific elements of the evidence including the animal’s precise circumstances (Section 6: e.g., Table 2), during and/or following the abusive or neglectful act. Use of detailed checklists such as those mentioned above [90] would be helpful.However, it is important to carefully consider possible alternative explanations for the occurrence of the observed physical and behavioural responses of the animal in the circumstances under review. For example, in the case of an animal that shows physiological and/or behavioural signs of fear, causes other than the circumstances imposed by the accused should also be considered.
- 6.
- How severe and protracted was or is the suffering?
- 7.
- Could the suffering have been avoided? Was it necessary?This refers to situations where there might have been reason to believe that the negative impacts were justified. Subsidiary questions that help to clarify these points are the following: Was a more humane option available? Could the overall benefit to the animal justify the negative impact of what was done?
- 8.
- Was the suffering inflicted willfully or recklessly?This question helps to distinguish purposeful cruelty from indifferent disregard, stupidity, ignorance, and naïveté, e.g., situations where the alleged perpetrator might not have anticipated the negative welfare impacts, or the possibility that they could occur.
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Webster, J. Assessment of animal welfare: The Five Freedoms. In Animal Welfare: A Cool Eye Towards Eden; Blackwell Science: Oxford, UK, 1994; pp. 10–14. [Google Scholar]
- Yeates, J.W.; Main, D.C.J. Assessment of positive welfare: A review. Vet. J. 2008, 175, 293–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fraser, D. Understanding Animal Welfare: The Science in its Cultural Context; Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Mellor, D.J.; Patterson-Kane, E.; Stafford, K.J. The Sciences of Animal Welfare; Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2009; Chapters 1–5; pp. 3–94. [Google Scholar]
- Green, T.C.; Mellor, D.J. Extending ideas about animal welfare assessment to include ‘quality of life’ and related concepts. N. Z. Vet. J. 2011, 59, 316–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J. Updating animal welfare thinking: Moving beyond the ‘Five Freedoms’ towards ‘A Life Worth Living’. Animals 2016, 6, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fraser, D.; Weary, D.M.; Pajor, E.A.; Milligan, B.N. A scientific conception of animal welfare that reflects ethical concerns. Anim. Welf. 1997, 6, 187–205. [Google Scholar]
- Mellor, D.J. Positive animal welfare states and reference standards for welfare assessment. N. Z. Vet. J. 2015, 63, 17–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barnett, J.L.; Hemsworth, P.H. Science and its application in assessing the welfare of laying hens. Aust. Vet. J. 2003, 81, 615–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fraser, D. Assessing animal welfare at the farm and group level: The interplay of science and values. Anim. Welf. 2003, 12, 433–443. [Google Scholar]
- Wemelsfelder, F. The scientific validity of subjective concepts in models of animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1997, 53, 75–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, I.J.H. Science-based assessment of animal welfare: Farm animals. Rev. Sci. Tech. 2005, 24, 483–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Panksepp, J. Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Conscious Cogn. 2005, 14, 30–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Webster, J. Zoomorphism and anthropomorphism: Fruitful fallacies? Anim. Welf. 2011, 20, 29–36. [Google Scholar]
- Duncan, I.J.H.; Petherick, J.C. The implications of cognitive processes for animal welfare. J. Anim. Sci. 1991, 69, 5017–5022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fraser, D.; Duncan, I.J.H. “Pleasures”, “pains” and animal welfare: Towards a natural history of affect. Anim. Welf. 1998, 7, 383–396. [Google Scholar]
- Gregory, N.G. Physiology and Behaviour of Animal Suffering; Blackwell Publishing: Oxford, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Wemelsfelder, F. How animals communicate quality of life: The qualitative assessment of behaviour. Anim. Welf. 2007, 16, 25–31. [Google Scholar]
- Broom, D.M. Cognitive ability and awareness in domestic animals and decisions about obligations to animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 126, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boissy, A.; Manteuffe, L.G.; Jensen, M.B.; Moe, R.O.; Spruijt, B.; Keeling, L.J.; Winckler, C.; Forkman, B.; Dimitrov, I.; Langbein, J.; et al. Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol. Behav. 2007, 92, 375–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rolls, E.T. Emotion Explained; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Mendl, M.; Burman, O.H.P.; Paul, E.S. An integrative and functional framework to the study of animal emotion and mood. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2010, 277, 2895–2904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J. Enhancing animal welfare by creating opportunities for ‘positive affective engagement’. N. Z. Vet. J. 2015, 63, 3–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Berridge, K.C. Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1996, 20, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Berridge, K.C.; Robinson, T.E. Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci. 2003, 9, 507–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, E.; Panksepp, J. Brain substrates of infant–mother attachment: Contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinepherine. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1998, 22, 437–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ikemoto, S.; Panksepp, J. The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: A unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Res. Rev. 1999, 31, 6–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfaff, D.W. Drive: Neurobiological and Molecular Mechanisms of Sexual Behavior; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Numan, M.; Insel, T.R. The Neurobiology of Parental Behavior; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp, J.; Zellner, M.R. Towards a neurologically based unified theory of aggression. Rev. Int. Psychol. Soc./Int. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 17, 37–61. [Google Scholar]
- Cushing, B.S.; Kramer, K.M. Mechanisms underlying epigenetic effects of early social experience: The role of neuropeptides and steroids. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005, 29, 1089–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fisher, H.E.; Aron, A.; Brown, L.L. Romantic love: A mammalian brain system for mate choice. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2006, 361, 2173–2186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Panksepp, J. Emotional endophenotypes in evolutionary psychiatry. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2006, 30, 774–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Newberry, R.C.; Swanson, J.C. Implications of breaking mother-young social bonds. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 110, 2–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dawkins, M.S. Through the eyes of animals: What behaviour tells us. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006, 100, 4–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirkden, R.D.; Pajor, E.A. Using preference, motivation and aversion tests to ask scientific questions about animals’ feelings. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006, 100, 29–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mason, G.; Rushen, J. Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Implications for Animal Welfare, 2nd ed.; CAB International: Wallingford, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Salter, R.E.; Hudson, R.J. Feeding ecology of feral horses in western Alberta. J. Range Manag. 1979, 32, 221–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hulet, C.V.; Anderson, D.M.; Smith, J.N.; Shupe, W.L.; Taylor, C.A., Jr.; Murray, L.W. Bonding of goats to sheep and cattle for protection from predators. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1989, 22, 261–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stolba, A.; Wood-Gush, D.G.M. The behaviour of pigs in a semi-natural environment. Anim. Sci. 1989, 48, 419–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wood-Gush, D.G.M.; Vestergaard, K. The seeking of novelty and its relation to play. Anim. Behav. 1991, 42, 599–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Day, J.E.L.; Kyriazakis, I.; Lawrence, A.B. The effect of food deprivation on the expression of foraging and exploratory behaviour in the growing pig. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1995, 42, 193–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scott, C.B.; Banner, R.E.; Provenza, F.D. Observations of sheep foraging in familiar and unfamiliar environments: Familiarity with environment influences diet selection. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1996, 49, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spinka, M.; Newberry, R.C.; Bekoff, M. Mammalian play: Training for the unexpected. Q. Rev. Biol. 2001, 76, 141–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Broad, K.D.; Curley, J.P.; Keverne, E.B. Mother-infant bonding and the evolution of mammalian social relationships. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2006, 361, 2199–2214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beaver, B.V. Ch 4, Canine social behavior; Ch7, Canine ingestive behavior. In Canine Behavior: Insights and Answers, 2nd ed.; Saunders Elsevier: St Louis, MO, USA, 2009; pp. 133–192, 223–242. [Google Scholar]
- Wirsing, A.J.; Cameron, K.E.; Heithaus, M.R. Spatial responses of predators vary with prey escape mode. Anim. Behav. 2010, 79, 531–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellor, D.J. Positive welfare states and promoting environment-focused and animal-to-animal interactive behaviours. N. Z. Vet. J. 2015, 63, 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J.; Beausoleil, N.J. Extending the ‘Five Domains’ model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Anim. Welf. 2015, 24, 241–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellor, D.J. Tail Docking of Canine Puppies: Reassessment of the Tail’s Role in Communication, the Acute Pain Caused by Docking and Interpretation of Behavioural Responses. Animals 2018, 8, 82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hemsworth, P.H.; Mellor, D.J.; Cronin, G.; Tilbrook, A. Scientific assessment of animal welfare. N. Z. Vet. J. 2015, 63, 24–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J.; Reid, C.S.W. Concepts of animal well-being and predicting the impact of procedures on experimental animals. In Improving the Well-Being of Animals in the Research Environment; Australian and New Zealand Council for the Care of Animals in Research and Teaching: Glen Osmond, Australia, 1994; pp. 3–18. [Google Scholar]
- Mellor, D.J. Operational details of the Five Domains Model and its key applications to the assessment and management of animal welfare. Animals 2017, 7, 60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Edgar, J.L.; Mullan, S.M.; Protchard, J.C.; McFarlane, U.J.C.; Main, D.C.J. Towards a ‘good life’ for farm animals: Development of a resource tier framework to achieve positive welfare for laying hens. Animals 2013, 3, 584–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beausoleil, N.J.; Mellor, D.J. Introducing breathlessness as an animal welfare issue. N. Z. Vet. J. 2015, 63, 44–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ledger, R.; Drever, E. Using ethology and animal welfare science to achieve successful prosecutions for suffering under the Criminal Code of Canada and the PCA Act. In Proceedings of the National Animal Welfare Conference, Canadian Federation of Humane Societies Annual Conference, Toronto, ON, Canada, 16–19 April 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Case 1, Regina v. Paulsen: R. v. Paulsen 2015, File Number 204679-1; British Columbia Provincial Court (Criminal Division) 45. Available online: https://www.canlii.org/en/bc/bcpc/doc/2015/2015bcpc45/2015bcpc45.html (accessed on 15 June 2018).
- Case 2, Regina v. Hague: R v. Hague 2015, File Number 237363-1. British Columbia Provincial Court (Criminal Division) online: National Centre for the Prosecution of Animal Cruelty. Available online: http://caselaw.ncpac.ca/?sfid=81&_sf_s=hague (accessed on 14 June 2018).
- Case 3, Viitre v. BC SPCA (May 18, 2016) online: BC Farm Industry Review Board. Available online: https://www2.gov.bc.ca/assets/gov/british-columbians-our-governments/organizational-structure/boards-commissions-tribunals/bc-farm-industry-review-board/animal-custody/pcaa-appeal-decisions/2016_may_18_viitre_v_bcspca_decision.pdf (accessed on 16 June 2018).
- Broom, D.M. Animal Welfare in the European Union. Report for the Directorate General of Internal Affairs. 2017. Available online: http://www.europarl.europa.eu/RegData/etudes/STUD/2017/583114/IPOL_STU(2017)583114_EN.pdf (accessed on 17 June 2018).
- The French National Assembly Recognises Animals as Sentient Beings; World Animal Protection 2015. Available online: https://www.worldanimalprotection.org/news/we-welcome-frances-recognition-animal-sentience (accessed on 17 June 2018).
- James, V. Recognising Animal Sentience: Including Minimum Standards for Opportunities to Display Normal Patterns of Behaviour in Codes of Welfare in New Zealand; LLM Research Paper Laws 544: Environmental Law, 2016; Available online: https://researcharchive.vuw.ac.nz/xmlui/bitstream/handle/10063/6335/paper_access.pdf?sequence=2 (accessed on 17 June 2018).
- Act to Improve the Legal Situation of Animals; Parliament of Quebec December 2015; Quebec Official Publisher: Quebec, QC, Canada, 2015. Available online: http://www2.publicationsduquebec.gouv.qc.ca/dynamicSearch/telecharge.php?type=5&file=2015C35A.PDF (accessed on 17 June 2018).
- Universal Declaration on Animal Welfare. 2014. Available online: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Universal_Declaration_on_Animal_Welfare (accessed on 17 June 2018).
- OIE Global Animal Welfare Strategy 2017; World Organisation for Animal Health: Paris, France, 2017. Available online: http://www.oie.int/fileadmin/Home/eng/Animal_Welfare/docs/pdf/Others/EN_OIE_AW_Strategy.pdf (accessed on 17 June 2018).
- Littlewood, K.; Mellor, D.J. Changes in the welfare of an injured working farm dog assessed using the Five Domains Model. Animals 2016, 6, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beausoleil, N.J.; Fisher, P.; Mellor, D.J.; Warburton, B. Ranking the negative impacts of wildlife control methods may help to advance the Three Rs. ALTEX 2012, 1, 481–485. [Google Scholar]
- Cuenya, L.; Fosacheca, S.; Mustaca, G.; Kamenetzky, G. Effects of isolation in adulthood on frustration and anxiety. Behav. Process. 2012, 90, 155–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Borchelt, P. Aggressive behavior of dogs kept as companion animals: Classification and influence of sex, reproductive status and breed. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1983, 10, 45–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, V.M.; Mellor, D.J.; Marbrook, J. Revision of a scale for assessing the severity of live animal manipulations. ALTEX 2006, 23, 163–169. [Google Scholar]
- Mellor, D.J.; Stafford, K.J. Integrating practical, regulatory and ethical strategies for enhancing farm animal welfare. Aust. Vet. J. 2001, 79, 762–768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J. Comprehensive assessment of harms caused by experimental, teaching and testing procedures on live animals. Altern. Lab. Anim. 2004, 32, 453–457. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Beausoleil, N.J.; Mellor, D.J. Complementary roles for systematic analytical evaluation and qualitative whole animal profiling in welfare assessment for Three Rs applications. ALTEX 2012, 1, 455–460. [Google Scholar]
- Mellor, D.J. Animal emotions, behaviour and the promotion of positive welfare states. N. Z. Vet. J. 2012, 60, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beausoleil, N.J.; Mellor, D.J. Advantages and limitations of the ‘Five Domains’ model for assessing animal welfare impacts associated with vertebrate pest control. N. Z. Vet. J. 2015, 63, 37–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sharp, T.; Saunders, G.A. Model for Assessing the Relative Humaneness of Pest Animal Control Methods; Australian Government Department of Agriculture, Fisheries and Forestry: Canberra, Australia, 2008.
- Sharp, T.; Saunders, G. A Model for Assessing the Relative Humaneness of Pest Animal Control Methods, 2nd ed.; Australian Department of Agriculture and Water Resources: Canberra, Australia, 2011. Available online: http://www.agriculture.gov.au/animal/welfare/aaws/humaneness-of-pest-animal-control-methods (accessed on 21 May 2017).
- Beausoleil, N.J.; Fisher, P.; Littin, K.E.; Warburton, B.; Mellor, D.J.; Dalefield, R.R.; Cowan, P. A systematic approach to evaluating and ranking the relative animal welfare impacts of wildlife control methods: Poisons used for lethal control of brushtail possums (Trichosurus Vulpecula) in New Zealand. Wildl. Res. 2016, 43, 553–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Portas, T. Achieving positive animal welfare outcomes in zoos and aquariums. In When Coping Is not Enough: Promoting Positive Welfare States in Animals, Proceedings of the 2013 RSPCA, Australia Scientific Seminar, Canberra, Australia, 26 February 2013; pp. 46–50. Available online: https://www.rspca.org.au/sites/ default/files/website/The-facts/Science/Scientific-Seminar/2013/SciSem_2013_Proceedings.pdf (accessed on 10 April 2017).
- Mellor, D.J.; Hunt, S.; Gusset, M. (Eds.) Caring for Wildlife: The World Zoo and Aquarium Animal Welfare Strategy; World Association of Zoos and Aquariums, Executive Office: Gland, Switzerland, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Baker, S.E.; Sharp, T.M.; Macdonald, D.W. Assessing animal welfare impacts in the management of European rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), European moles (Talpa europaea) and Carrion crows (Corvus corone). PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0146298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McGreevy, P.; Berger, J.; de Brauwere, N.; Doherty, N.O.; Anna Harrison, A.; Fiedler, J.; Jones, C.; McDonnell, S.; McLean, A.; Nakonechny, L.; et al. Using the five domains model to assess the adverse impacts of husbandry, veterinary, and equitation interventions on horse welfare. Animals 2018, 8, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cook, W.R.; Kibler, M. Behavioural assessment of pain in 66 horses, with and without a bit. Equine Vet. Educ. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spinka, M.; Wemelsfelder, F. Environmental challenge and animal agency. In Animal Welfare, 2nd ed.; Appleby, M.C., Mench, J.A., Olsson, I.A.S., Hughes, B.O., Eds.; CAB International: Wallingford, UK, 2011; pp. 27–43. [Google Scholar]
- Miklósi, Á. Dog Behaviour, Evolution and Cognition, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Serpell, J. (Ed.) The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Coren, S. How to Speak Dog; Simon and Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 2000; ISBN 9781471109416. [Google Scholar]
- Rugaas, T. On Talking Terms with Dogs: Calming Signals, 2nd ed.; Dogwise Publishing: Wenatchee, WA, USA, 2006; ISBN 1929242360. [Google Scholar]
- McMillan, D.F.; Duffy, D.L.; Zawistowski, S.L.; Serpell, J.A. Behavioral and psychological characteristics of canine victims of abuse. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2014, 18, 92–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hammerschmidt, J.; Molento, C.F.M. Protocolfor expert report on animal welfare in case of companion animal cruelty suspicion. Braz. J. Vet. Res. Anim. Sci. São Paulo 2014, 51, 282–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Negative Affective Experiences Generated within the Body | |
Breathlessness | An urgent compulsion to increase respiratory activity (e.g., breathing rate and depth; gasping), to overcome resistance to airflow due to obstructions in air passages (e.g., laboured breathing), or to escape from external impediments to breathing; can lead to anxiety, fear, and panic |
Thirst | A compulsion to seek and drink water |
Hunger | A compulsion to seek and eat energy-rich and other foods |
Pain | Noxious experiences associated with physical injury or the threat of such injury |
Nausea | A sensation of unease and discomfort in the upper stomach linked to an involuntary urge to vomit; it may precede vomiting, but can occur without it |
Dizziness | Impaired spatial perception and stability; feelings of disequilibrium or giddiness |
Weakness | A negative feeling associated with reduced strength, muscle tone, vigour, or fitness |
Exhaustion | A negative feeling associated with physical and/or mental fatigue linked to excessive metabolic demands (e.g., sustained strenuous exercise, repeated pregnancy/lactation) and/or persistently inadequate sleep/rest |
Debility | A negative feeling associated with physical weakness, especially associated with illness |
Sickness | Feelings of lethargy, depression, sleepiness, and reduced appetite associated with fever; can be associated with seeking isolation or comforting social contact |
Physical discomfort | Feelings of irritation such as itching from dust or allergens, joint stiffness due to cramped space, and/or the unpleasantness of being on hard or rough surfaces |
Thermal discomfort (too cold) | Distress of chilling caused by marked-to-severe cold, draughty, and/or wet conditions; severe cold may induce pain; when hypothermic cannot get or stay warm |
Thermal discomfort (too hot) | Distress of overheating caused by marked-to-severe radiant and/or ambient heat; radiant heat can lead to pain; when hyperthermic cannot get or stay cool; may progress to dizziness, fainting, weakness, and/or pain associated with organ shutdown |
Auditory discomfort | Distress experienced when the volume, pitch, and/or rhythm of sound is uncomfortable; may lead to pain and hearing impairment |
Olfactory discomfort | Distress experienced in the presence of unpleasant odours and/or irritants, e.g., smoke, urinary ammonia, fecal smells, dust, and carbon dioxide |
Visual discomfort | Distress experienced in the presence of light conditions that create eye strain, including dark, dim, and glaring light; can lead to pain |
Negative Affective Experiences that Reflect an Animal’s Perception of its External Circumstances | |
Anxiety | A negative feeling experienced in anticipation of a threat to safety |
Fear | A negative feeling experienced during a perceived threat to safety |
Panic | A sudden, uncontrollable, and intense level of anxiety or fear |
Frustration (general) | Exasperation at being delayed or thwarted in achieving an internally or externally motivated goal; can lead to anxiety and/or anger |
Frustration (sexual) | Exasperation at being delayed or thwarted in engaging in desired sexual experiences |
Anger | An intense emotion usually involving agitation and a strongly hostile response to a perceived provocation, challenge, or threat |
Helplessness | A negative feeling associated with an inability to cope or act effectively, due to repeated unsuccessful attempts to do so |
Loneliness | A negative feeling related to a lack of connection or communication with others |
Boredom | A specific mental state where a lack of stimulation leads to craving for relief |
Depression | A state of feeling sad, hopeless, helpless, and dejected, accompanied by a reduced ability to have pleasurable experiences |
Malaise | A vague or unfocused feeling of lethargy, weakness, or discomfort associated with unremitting constancy in barren environments |
Act of Cruelty or Neglect | Specific Situations | Behavioural Responses | Inferred Affective Experiences |
---|---|---|---|
* Suffocation: impeding breathing to kill or torture | Sealing the head or whole body in a plastic bag; blocking the nose and mouth by hand | Extreme struggling and escape attempts, accompanied by gasping Survivors may cower, withdraw, or show appeasement gestures in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place | Extreme breathlessness accompanied by anxiety, fear, and panic, and physical exhaustion Anxiety and fear may be expressed by survivors in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place, e.g., presence of the abuser, location where the abuse took place, exposure to equipment associated with the cruel act |
* Drowning: the use of water to impede breathing to kill, torture, or punish | Submerging in deep or turbulent water; restraining the animal under running water; hosing the face with water | Extreme struggling and escape attempts, accompanied by gasping Survivors may cower, withdraw, or show appeasement gestures in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place | Extreme breathlessness accompanied by anxiety, fear, and panic, and physical exhaustion Anxiety and fear may be expressed by survivors in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place, e.g., presence of the abuser, location where the abuse took place, exposure to equipment associated with the cruel act |
* Strangulation: obstructing breathing to kill or punish | Compressing the trachea by hand; suspending or forcefully swinging the dog by a neck rope or collar | Extreme struggling accompanied by gasping; attempts to vocalise; body may become limp Survivors may cower, withdraw ,or show appeasement gestures in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place | Extreme breathlessness, anxiety, fear, and panic, accompanied by pain from tight neck ligature; physical exhaustion. Anxiety and fear may be expressed by survivors in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place, e.g., presence of the abuser, location where the abuse took place, exposure to equipment associated with the cruel act |
* Injuring: inflicting blunt force trauma to punish or torture | Severe beating with fist, club, whip, or other solid object; kicking; throwing or swinging against solid objects | Loud distress calls, escape attempts, cowering; snarling or trying to attack abuser; subsequent guarding injured areas, reduced mobility, withdrawal, and vocalisations when manipulated, showing appeasing behaviours Survivors may cower, withdraw, or show appeasement gestures in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place | Extreme pain, fear, and panic during physical assaults; persistent subsequent marked pain from fractures and other serious injuries Anxiety and fear may be expressed by survivors in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place, e.g., presence of the abuser, location where the abuse took place, exposure to equipment associated with the cruel act |
* Shooting: | Shooting by bullet, pellet, arrow, or nail | If not immediately fatal, responses depend on effects of wound site(s) on the capacity to escape | Moderate-to-extreme pain, anxiety, and fear; fear extreme when escape is hindered by bone fractures, paralysis, blindness, or brain damage |
* Vehicular abuse: | Being dragged behind, thrown from, or run over by a vehicle | Loud distress calls, struggling, escape attempts, guarding injured areas, impaired mobility or paralysis | Extreme fear and panic plus pain during each type of assault and breathlessness if dragged by the neck; then persistent pain from fractures, areas excoriated to the bone, and other external or internal injuries, as well as fear and helplessness |
* Burning or boiling alive: | Applying accelerant and lighting it; throwing dog into a fire or boiling water; microwaving | Loud distress calls, vigorous escape attempts, writhing until death. Survivors withdrawn, immobile, vocalise when moved; may cower, withdraw, or show appeasement gestures in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place | Extreme pain, fear, and panic until death; in survivors, persistent marked-to-extreme pain and fear, plus extreme weakness Anxiety and fear may be expressed by survivors in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place, e.g., presence of the abuser, location where the abuse took place, exposure to equipment associated with the cruel act |
* Dog fights: | Pitting aggressive dogs against other aggressive or bait dogs in a confined space; fights end when one dog dies or is cowed or seriously injured | Aggressors: Growling, chasing, lunging, biting, wrestling, until one dog is incapable of continuing or is withdrawn. Victims: distress calls, retreating/escape attempts, defensive behaviour, appeasement gestures, cowering, trembling | Extreme anger, fear, and/or panic; subsequent extreme pain from serious bite and ripping injuries; potential frustration, helplessness, pain, and discomfort from lasting injury-induced disabilities such as partial blindness or crippling nerve, muscle, tendon and/or bone damage; potential for post-traumatic stress disorder Anxiety and fear may be expressed by survivors in circumstances similar to those in which the cruel act took place, e.g., presence of other dogs |
Extreme restraint: | Entire body with flexed legs bound tightly with nylon cord or adhesive tape for several days | Futile struggling; binding prevents virtually all movement; breathing impeded; unable to get to water or food | Extreme anxiety and fear at physical helplessness; breathlessness and panic due to restricted breathing; marked thirst and hunger; physical discomfort |
Excessive and prolonged confinement: | Isolated in coffin-like cage in low-light-intensity room for a prolonged period | Stands up and lies down but cannot turn around; self-grooming severely impeded; no exercise possible; self-mutilated forepaws gnawed to the bone | Extreme loneliness, depression, and anxiety from isolation; boredom and malaise from the barren constancy of the environment; helplessness; frustration and discomfort at inability to groom; severe pain from self-mutilated paws |
Close confinement: | Continuously kept alone in a small cage or kennel or on a very short tether with inadequate shelter and shade; resting area fouled by urine and faeces | Stereotypical pacing, jumping, and circling but mostly withdrawn and inactive; shivering when cold and panting when hot; self-grooming ineffective; coat unhygienic; suppurating skin sores | Extreme loneliness, depression, and anxiety, plus boredom and frustration in restricted environment; helplessness; thermal discomfort in very cold or hot weather; physical discomfort from hard lying areas; persistent itch and pain from skin sores |
Prolonged tethering | Kept on a tether outdoors, alongside other dogs | Stereotypical pacing; lunging to the end of the tether; vocalisation; self-mutilation; licking, chewing, and/or swallowing accessible non-nutritive material (e.g., bedding, rocks, tethers, fabric of shelters) | Intense helplessness, depression, and/or frustration at thwarted urges for physical contact with other dogs and for free-running exercise and play; anxiety and fear from inescapable threats; physical discomfort and neck pain from collar-induced lunging injuries |
Brutal tethers: | A tight collar, chain, rope, or wire used to tether a growing dog where the loop becomes deeply embedded in the neck | Laboured breathing due to tracheal compression; restlessness; frequent, prolonged scratching at the tightening loop and then the infected neck wound | Marked breathlessness and anxiety due to impeded breathing; increasingly severe pain and panic as the neck loop embeds more deeply and the skin breaches exposing raw and infected underlying tissue |
* Exposure to severe cold: | Exposure to unrelieved and extreme ambient cold, such as living in cold, wet, and/or windy conditions, or being confined on frozen ground | Seeking warmth, huddling with other animals, curling up to minimise heat loss, alternating which feet are placed onto frozen ground, intense shivering, hypothermic lethargy | Considerable chilling discomfort and misery, ultimately leading to unconsciousness; cold-induced pain |
* Exposure to severe heat: | Exposure to unrelieved and extreme radiant and/or ambient heat from sunshine, fires, overheated vehicles and other enclosures, and from contact with hot surfaces | Seeking shade and water; intense panting; ingestion of grass or other materials to alleviate nausea, then reduced panting, and convulsions | Considerable hyperthermic discomfort and pain; nausea; extreme thirst and panic; hyperthermic dizziness, lethargy, and weakness; physical exhaustion |
Abandonment: | Left confined or discarded with no provision for any necessities of life, e.g., water, food, shelter, shade, variety, company, and care | Distress calls; escape attempts from confinement; fruitlessly seeking companionship; seeking water and food, plus warm or cool, dry, comfortable, and hygienic resting areas; when the dog feels unsafe, hiding and reduced maintenance behaviour; sickness behaviours specific to developing pathologies | Moderate-to-very-severe loneliness, thirst, hunger, hypo- or hyper-thermic distress, physical discomfort, depression, anxiety, fear and/or panic; and, depending on the clinical pathology, breathlessness, pain, nausea, dizziness, debility, weakness, and/or sickness |
Prolonged restraint in a trap: | Caught in an unmonitored leg-hold jaw or snare trap for several days | Initially extreme and fruitless escape attempts, then inactive unless disturbed by other animals or events; no access to water, food, shelter, shade, or company; self-mutilation to facilitate escape | Extreme pain from the trap; severe anxiety, fear, and panic from inability to escape; physical discomfort; after some days, withdrawal, depression, lethargy, and loneliness compounded by marked-to-very-severe thirst and hunger, as well as potential hypo- or hyper-thermic distress; marked pain from self-mutilation |
Absent or delayed veterinary care: | No or slow provision of veterinary care for injuries or diseases that require it | Injury-specific and disease-specific behaviours | Potentially any or all of the following: marked-to-very-severe thirst, hunger, pains of different types, breathlessness, nausea, dizziness, debility, weakness, sickness, anxiety, fear, panic, helplessness, loneliness, depression, frustration, and anger |
Exposure to female(s) in oestrus: | Intact males in breeding facilities in close proximity to females in oestrus | Escape attempts; excessive barking; excessive pacing; excessive licking/chewing of genitals. Can lead to increased aggressive interactions between individuals. | Sexual frustration; anxiety; injury-induced pain from thwarted attempts to access females in oestrus; pain from self-mutilation |
Exposure to social threats without physical harm: | Interactions with humans that involve yelling, shouting, and other forms of intimidation, as may occur during training and restraint; threatening interactions with other dogs | Defensive behaviour: avoidance, retreating, cowering, trembling, appeasement gestures, tucked tail, distress calls | Anxiety, fear, and/or panic |
© 2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Ledger, R.A.; Mellor, D.J. Forensic Use of the Five Domains Model for Assessing Suffering in Cases of Animal Cruelty. Animals 2018, 8, 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8070101
Ledger RA, Mellor DJ. Forensic Use of the Five Domains Model for Assessing Suffering in Cases of Animal Cruelty. Animals. 2018; 8(7):101. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8070101
Chicago/Turabian StyleLedger, Rebecca A., and David J. Mellor. 2018. "Forensic Use of the Five Domains Model for Assessing Suffering in Cases of Animal Cruelty" Animals 8, no. 7: 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8070101
APA StyleLedger, R. A., & Mellor, D. J. (2018). Forensic Use of the Five Domains Model for Assessing Suffering in Cases of Animal Cruelty. Animals, 8(7), 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8070101