Next Article in Journal
Effects of Dietary Potassium Diformate Supplementation on Growth Performance, Nutrient Digestibility, Gastrointestinal pH, Jejunal Morphology, Digestive Enzyme Activity, and Antioxidant Status in Weaned Piglets
Previous Article in Journal
Evaluation of Nasal Microbial Communities of Beef Calves During Pre-Weaning Outbreak of Bovine Respiratory Disease
Previous Article in Special Issue
Framework for Classification of Fattening Pig Vocalizations in a Conventional Farm with High Relevance for Practical Application
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

A Review of Assessment of Sow Pain During Farrowing Using Grimace Scores

by
Lucy Palmer
1,
Sabrina Lomax
1 and
Roslyn Bathgate
2,*
1
School of Life and Environmental Sciences, Faculty of Science, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia
2
Sydney School of Veterinary Science, Faculty of Science, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2025, 15(19), 2915; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192915
Submission received: 19 August 2025 / Revised: 22 September 2025 / Accepted: 29 September 2025 / Published: 7 October 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Animal Health and Welfare Assessment of Pigs)

Simple Summary

As the public demand for improved animal welfare in meat production systems continues to increase, better management of farm animal pain is essential. Farrowing, the process of giving birth in pigs, is known to be painful, yet the amount of pain experienced by sows that is considered “normal” and how to measure this pain is poorly understood. This review explores how pain during farrowing affects the health and welfare of both sows and their piglets, and why improved understanding and management of pain is important for animal welfare and farm productivity. A key focus was the use of facial grimace scales, a tool that can be used to assess pain by observing changes in facial expressions. The review examines the newly developed sow grimace scale that has been described as a simple and promising way to detect and score the pain experience during farrowing. Accurately identifying pain in sows could lead to improved care during birth and enhanced sow and piglet health and welfare, ultimately optimising productivity. Understanding and managing farrowing pain is not just important for ethical reasons but also benefits society by improving the productivity and sustainability of food production systems.

Abstract

Reproduction is one of the most important considerations for the livestock industry, presenting significant economic and animal health and welfare pressures for producers. Parturition, the process of giving birth, is known to be highly painful in many mammalian species, but the understanding of parturient pain in sows is limited. Farrowing, the process of parturition in pigs, is understudied compared to other livestock species, with very little research available specifically regarding pain. Pain can be detrimental to animal wellbeing; hence, it is vital for it to be reliably detected and managed in such a way that improves both sow and piglet health and welfare. Grimace scales have been developed as a method for pain detection and quantification in animals via observations of facial expression changes in response to painful stimuli. This presents a unique opportunity for improved pain assessment during farrowing, increasing the current understanding of farrowing dynamics and potentially enhancing farrowing management decisions to prioritise sow welfare. This review synthesises and critically analyses the current knowledge on sow parturient pain and the ability for the application of facial grimace scoring to measure pain severity. Grimace scoring was found to be an effective, simple and feasible method of pain assessment in a number of domestic species, and its recent application to farrowing is a promising development in the understanding and management of sow welfare during parturition.

1. Introduction

Pork consumption in Australia has been consistently increasing in the past two decades, recently having overtaken beef as the second most consumed meat in the country [1]. Profitable and effective reproductive management is essential to meet the growing consumer demand for pork. Livestock producers require the majority of female animals to have offspring in order to maintain productivity. Sows are expected to have, on average, 2.4 litters per year to sustain the Australian pork industry [2]. Parturition in sows, known as farrowing, is a crucial event for producers, with significant economic implications. Furthermore, gestation and parturition are deemed high risk periods, presenting a range of health and welfare concerns for the sow and piglets [3]. Animal welfare has become an important factor to consumers as shown through increasing public concern regarding animal use in science and the treatment of livestock in farms [4]. Optimising animal welfare is therefore not only important based on an individual’s moral or ethical beliefs, but also for meeting consumer requirements due to increased consumer concerns regarding animal welfare.
The process of parturition is associated with significant pain in humans [5] and has been recognised as painful in a number of domestic animals [6]. Despite this, parturient pain in sows is highly understudied and sometimes misunderstood [7], with the current literature lacking an understanding of what level of pain is considered “normal” [8]. Pain is highly aversive, triggering a negative affective state which can be detrimental to the welfare of the animal [9]. Hence, parturient pain must be acknowledged as a significant welfare issue experienced by sows. Although a degree of pain may be inevitable during parturition, recognising its impact on the animal’s wellbeing and applying practical strategies for its management is crucial for upholding animal welfare standards [10].
The lack of validated techniques for identification and assessment of pain in animals is a major barrier to understanding the pain experience [11] and hence the implementation of treatment and management strategies to minimise the negative impact of pain on welfare. In the past decade, facial grimace scoring has been introduced as a technique of pain detection and measurement in animals based on changes in facial expressions [11]. The development of the first sow grimace scale, by Navarro et al. [12], presents a unique opportunity for future research on the pain experience of farrowing sows and its influence on reproductive outcomes. This review aims to synthesise the current literature and identify gaps in knowledge of the pain associated with parturition in sows. Additionally, grimace scoring as a technique for pain assessment will be discussed, particularly in its utilisation with farrowing sows. The reviewed literature was selected by interrogating the Science Direct, PubMed and Web of Science databases with the search criteria including grimace scale, pain, pain score, sow, farrowing, parturition, facial expression. Exclusion criteria included articles not written in English and for which the full text was not available.

2. Physiology of Farrowing and Parturient Pain

2.1. Farrowing Physiology

Parturition is considered to be a highly painful experience but is understudied in sows compared to other domestic species. The anatomical and physiological similarities between humans and pigs allow for parallels to be drawn between the experience of parturition in women and sows [7], as has been recognised by many other authors [6,7,8,13]. In women, labour is often described as one of the most painful conditions [5]. Farrowing is hence considered to cause significant pain to the sow, as supported by behavioural [14] and physiological observations [15] and the positive effects of analgesics on sow behaviour post-farrowing [16]. In women, the intensity and location of pain during labour differs significantly between individuals [17]. It could be inferred that farrowing pain would likewise be highly variable between sows, but this has not yet been explicitly studied.
The onset of farrowing is characterised by physiological and physical changes to the cervix, including cervical ripening and dilation [18]. This is followed by myometrial contractions which manually move the foetuses caudally to the cervix [18]. This first stage of farrowing is mostly associated with visceral pain, which originates from internal organs, including the uterus, and is often experienced as widespread pain which can diffuse to nearby areas including the abdominal, lumbosacral and gluteal regions [7,19]. The second stage of farrowing is marked by the onset of strong abdominal contractions which function to expel the foetuses [18]. During this stage, somatic pain becomes dominant, resulting from the distention and stretching of the pelvic diaphragm and perineum [7]. Somatic pain originates from the musculoskeletal system and external soft tissues, with the pain considered sharp and well-localised [19]. The final stage of farrowing involves the expulsion of foetal membranes, but these are sometimes expelled between piglets [20]. During this stage, the strength of contractions decreases significantly [18] so pain could be assumed to likewise decrease in amplitude, as is common in women [21]. Throughout the parturition process, visceral and somatic pain-causing stimuli are detected by nociceptive neurons, triggering the perception of pain sensations by the sow [6].

2.2. ”Normal” and “Abnormal” Pain During Farrowing

Compared to what is considered normal, pain has been shown to often be extremely elevated in primiparous sows [6] as evidenced by concentration of pain markers in the blood [22] and in the instance of complications and dystocia [7]. Surveys have revealed that pig farmers and veterinarians believed farrowings classified as “difficult” are highly painful, reported in one study to occur in an estimated 4% of sows and 5% of gilts in the United Kingdom [23]. In sows, dystocia is defined as difficulty during the farrowing process, occurring due to weak myometrial contractions or factors that prevent the foetus’s movement through the birth canal, often requiring obstetrical assistance [8,20]. This condition is known to cause significantly higher levels of pain compared to normal parturition [7] and can be detrimental to both sow welfare and piglet survival [3]. Dystocia in sows is highly under-researched, with the current scientific literature lacking agreement on the frequency and classification of the condition in sows and gilts. Walls et al. [8] compiled the available definitions of dystocia, all of which typically relate to farrowing duration or inter-piglet interval, highlighting the inconsistencies between studies and lack of clear guidelines for pig producers. Furthermore, this study revealed the contentious nature of dystocia occurrence, with reported prevalence ranging from 0.25 to 47% across studies and definitions [8]. The limited knowledge of dystocia in sows highlights the overall inadequate understanding of the farrowing process, particularly in regard to sow health and welfare.

2.3. Implications of Parturient Pain

Perinatal pain can pose several consequences to the health and welfare of sows and piglets. Pain is aversive and distressing, causing a negative affective state [9] which can be detrimental to the welfare of the sow. Parturient pain may cause sows and gilts to become restless or agitated [7] resulting in more frequent postural changes and potentially increased incidences of piglet crushing [24,25]. Furthermore, sow restlessness can decrease time spent nursing [26] and has been reported to be associated with savaging behaviour in gilts [27]. Pain can also negatively affect the physical health of sows, with implications to body condition and lactation as it is not uncommon for sow feed and water intake to be depressed postpartum [28]. This could be related to pain, which is known to decrease appetite in pigs [29]. Inadequate feed and water intake results in weight loss and decreased milk production, which negatively impacts both sows and piglets [7]. Pain during and post-farrowing is hence not only a welfare issue but also a production and animal health concern.

2.4. Factors Affecting Parturient Pain

Factors that are believed to affect the degree of parturient pain in sows include sow factors; parity [22] and nutrition [29], piglet factors; litter size [3], piglet size [30] and presentation [31] and parturition factors; farrowing duration [23], inter-piglet interval [32] and dystocia [7]. Many of these factors have been shown to be associated with a longer farrowing duration, but not pain specifically, so their impact on pain is based on the assumption that extended farrowing durations cause greater pain to the sow. Farrowing duration and inter-piglet intervals are considered important factors impacting the ease of parturition [33]. The average farrowing duration has been found to be approximately 130 min, but this varies significantly between sows and is heavily influenced by breed, parity and litter size [34]. A number of studies have demonstrated that prolonged farrowing duration is directly correlated with the occurrence of stillborn piglets and can negatively impact sow health [29,31]. Dystocia occurrence is directly correlated with other complications including placenta retention and postpartum dysgalactia and can cause future reproductive failure [29]. The time between the expulsion of each piglet, known as the inter-piglet interval (IPI) is likewise highly variable, but the majority of the current literature state that an interval greater than 45 to 60 min is considered abnormal and can indicate dystocia [20,32,35]. Without a validated method of measuring pain in farrowing sows, the impact of these factors on pain cannot be properly explored.
The confinement of sows in farrowing crates is a common practice but has been demonstrated to cause stress to the sow, and prevent the expression of normal maternal behaviour, including nest-building [36]. How sow housing during farrowing impacts pain severity has not been investigated, but it could be that lower oxytocin levels observed in crated sows may result in prolonged farrowing duration [37] and potentially increased pain. Confined sows are often unable to perform nest-building behaviour and have been found to have increased blood cortisol levels, a common indicator of stress [37]. The experience of stress immediately prior to farrowing and the inability to perform natural behaviours could potentially impact the pain experience during farrowing, but this cannot be determined without appropriate techniques to measure pain. Investigation into the influence of housing during the periparturient period on parturient pain is recommended in order to improve the understanding of sow welfare in farrowing crates.

2.5. Treatment of Parturient Pain

Pain relief options for the effective and safe management of farrowing pain are limited. Farmers report sometimes administering Azaperone, a sedative drug, and non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs) during farrowing to minimise stress and pain [38]. Meloxicam, a common NSAID, has analgesic properties, but is not recommended to be administered pre-farrowing as it can inhibit prostaglandin production, prolonging farrowing duration [16]. Paracetamol has been demonstrated to be a safe analgesic for use in farrowing sows, but its efficacy as pain relief has not been studied [39,40]. Both meloxicam and paracetamol are transferred to the colostrum and milk so impacts of these drugs on piglets must also be considered [40]. Oxytocin is commonly administered to sows experiencing farrowing difficulties [38,41]. Whilst oxytocin is not an analgesic and therefore does not directly decrease pain, it stimulates uterine contractions, aiding in piglet expulsion and shortening farrowing duration and IPI [42]. Treatment with oxytocin therefore could be considered as a management option for farrowing pain, as the longer the farrowing duration, the more pain is experienced, particularly in cases of dystocia and breech presentation. Importantly, exogenous oxytocin administration has been associated with greater stillborn occurrence [42]. Unfortunately, there is currently no ideal analgesic for treatment of parturient pain in sows. Without a validated pain measurement technique for farrowing sows, the efficacy of analgesics cannot be accurately determined, which may be contributing to the lack of appropriate and safe pain relief available.
Despite evidence that farrowing can be an incredibly painful process, with significant impacts on production and welfare, the majority of the available literature on farrowing difficulties do not specifically address pain, highlighting a need for further research. The lack of consensus on what is considered “normal” pain during farrowing is a key obstruction to improving management and welfare of the farrowing sow. Furthermore, there is a lack of knowledge regarding whether it would be beneficial or feasible to treat or minimise “normal” or “abnormal” pain in sows, and how this could affect the outcome of parturition.

3. Measuring Pain in Domestic Animals

3.1. Pain as a Welfare Issue

Pain is a complex and undesirable sensory and affective experience which causes stress and evokes a negative mental state [9,43]. Due to the physiological similarities between vertebrate species, it is generally accepted that animals experience pain in a similar way to humans [44]. All domestic species have been confirmed to have the capacity to experience pain, as they are able to detect, react to, and respond to noxious stimuli [45]. With many animals regarded as sentient beings, pain is a significant welfare issue as it can be detrimental to an animal’s quality of life [46]. The Five Domains Model, often considered the gold standard guideline for assessing animal welfare, identifies pain as a negative experience which can be detrimental to affective state and hence welfare [47]. This model defines the welfare status of an animal as a long-term subjective state of being with biological functioning and affective components [10]. Affective state is the cumulative psychological, physiological and behavioural experience of an animal at a given time, involving emotional arousal and valence [48]. Pain is an unpleasant sensation, with negative valence, contributing to an overall negative affective state which can be observed through behavioural and physiological changes in the animal [9]. It is inevitable that animals will experience pain throughout their lives, whether that be from disease, injury, routine husbandry practices or parturition. Whilst pain can be unavoidable, it is important for the welfare of animals that it is acknowledged, and that effective and feasible methods of pain prevention or treatment are implemented to minimise the negative sensation [10]. Without accurate techniques for detecting and measuring pain, scientists, veterinarians and livestock producers are unable to ensure that an animal is not experiencing this negative affective state and hence cannot confirm the animal’s wellbeing.

3.2. Measuring and Quantifying Pain

In humans, pain is detected and measured via communication, most often verbal, but since animals are unable to communicate pain via language, different methods for detecting and measuring pain must be employed. Acute pain assessment in animals is generally categorised in two distinct methods; physiological and behavioural. Pain evokes a number of physiological changes via action of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA) and the autonomic nervous system [45]. Physiological pain assessment involves measuring physiological factors associated with distress, such as heart rate or blood concentrations of stress related hormones [49]. Quantifying pain using physiological changes can be challenging as they are influenced by several other stressors independent of pain, and the process of collecting data can cause more stress to the animal [46]. Behavioural methods of pain assessment rely on the fact that animals change their behaviour in certain ways in response to painful stimuli [50]. Pain and its associated negative affective state can hence be identified and potentially quantified via observations of postural and behavioural changes. These changes associated with pain are highly species specific [51], with each species requiring its own set of guidelines regarding what behaviours may be considered normal or abnormal.

3.3. Facial Grimace Scales

Charles Darwin was the first to suggest that non-human animals communicated emotions such as pain via facial expressions in a similar way to humans [52]. The Facial Action Coding System (FACS) was developed in 1978 to describe the range of facial muscle movements, known as facial action units (FAUs), in humans [53]. Affective states and pain intensity were found to be communicated through facial expressions. This led to the development of grimace scales, which were first applied to laboratory mice [54] and since then a number of other domestic species [12,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63], outlined in Table 1. Whilst pain related FAUs are species specific, there are a number which are common across species, including orbital tightening, changes in ear positioning, and tension of the facial muscles. For each FAU, grimace scales present an image for a score 0, indicating no pain, a score 1, indicating moderate pain, and a score 2, indicating severe pain. Scorers can then compare the animal’s face that they are observing to the images and descriptions on the scale. Application of grimace scoring is most well reported in rodents due to their use as human models in biomedical research, but there is increasing research on applications of grimace scales to livestock species [11].
A major limitation to knowledge about and hence treatment of pain in domestic animals has previously been a lack of validated methods of assessing pain [11]. Pigs, like other livestock species, are prey animals, having evolved to hide overt signs of pain to avoid predation [46]. This poses a major barrier to our understanding of the experience of pain in animals and highlights the need for techniques that can detect subtle indicators of pain, such as the facial grimace scale. The development of grimace scales for a range of species has provided individuals in the animal care, research and production industries unique opportunities to further the understanding of the experience of pain in animals. For example, the rat grimace scale has been used to assess the pain caused by different euthanasia methods [64] and the mouse grimace scale has been used to evaluate analgesic efficacy [65]. Unfortunately, grimace scoring is not yet routinely used outside of the research field, for example, in veterinary clinics or on farms [66]. This indicates that despite grimace scales being described as “easy to use” [46], there is still some barrier preventing the wide-scale employment of the technique. Facial grimace scoring can provide individuals with the means to accurately detect pain, which would in turn allow for improved methods for prevention and treatment of pain, but this requires further research and a push for individuals in the animal industry to implement such scales.

4. Measuring Pain in Farrowing Sows

4.1. Existing Methods of Pain Measurement During Farrowing

There has been little published research on pain measurement during farrowing. Some possible methods have been identified, including both behavioural and physiological measures. Pain indicators demonstrated relatively frequently by sows during active farrowing may include pawing, tail flicking, back arching, trembling and pulling in of the back leg [14]. These behavioural indicators require further validation to ensure they are direct responses to pain. Relying exclusively on such behaviours to detect pain is limiting as it does not allow for quantification and is difficult to validate. Vocalisations are also often clear indicators of pain in animals [67]. Sows produce distinct grunting sounds during farrowing but these vocalisations have not been found to be correlated with pain intensity or dystocia [68]. Physiological methods of measuring pain, such as blood cortisol concentration have also been explored in relation to the farrowing period in sows. Cortisol levels tend to increase during and immediately after farrowing [69], but this may be due to the generally stressful nature of parturition, rather than pain specifically [7]. This is one of the major limitations of physiological methods for pain measurement, particularly when used to study parturition, which involves many significant endocrine changes. A rise in foetal cortisol levels is the trigger of parturition in some species, and whilst it is observed in pigs, it has not been confirmed as the ultimate initiating factor, further complicating the use of cortisol to measure pain [36]. The limitations associated with the described behavioural and physiological methods of pain detection have restricted the current understanding of the farrowing process, highlighting the need for a technique which accurately quantifies the pain experience of sows.

4.2. Limitations in the Understanding of Pain in Sows

There are large disparities in the level of research and scientific understanding of parturition across species. When focusing on the pain associated with parturition, cows, ewes and mares typically receive more scientific attention than sows [18]. This is because parturient pain is often considered to be more significant, in terms of both production outcomes and animal welfare, in monotocous animals, such as cows and mares, compared to sows, which are polytocous [6]. The study by Mainau & Manteca [7], which aims to review parturient pain in cows and sows, predominantly focuses on cows, presenting much less information about sow parturition and pain. Dystocia is reported to occur most frequently in ewes, then mares and cows [70] hence it has received the most scientific and management-based attention in these species. In cows, dystocia is widely recognised as a major cause of postnatal complications and calf mortality [6]. Therefore, there is a larger focus on detection and management of calving dystocia and its associated pain as the condition is considered to be of greater importance in cows compared to sows. Despite the increased understanding of cow parturient pain and dystocia, there is no single widely accepted and implemented method for detecting and measuring pain during calving, with producers relying on complex behavioural and physiological factors, as is also the case for pig producers [7]. The validation and implementation of a reliable method for assessment of sow pain during farrowing could allow for the adaptation of the technique to other livestock species, where parturition dynamics are already understood to a deeper level.

4.3. The Sow Grimace Scale

Pain evokes a number of spontaneous reflexes in humans and animals including involuntary changes in facial expressions [53]. A range of pain-related facial actions units (FAUs) have been observed in animals, outlined in Table 1. Grimace scales are created using these facial expressions, which can then be used as a tool to detect the presence of pain and estimate its intensity. Grimace scales have been developed for piglets during tail docking and castration [60,61] and more recently for sows during farrowing [12]. The piglet grimace scale has been validated, refined and practically implemented in numerous studies [71,72,73] but nothing has been published on the sow grimace scale since its inception. Extensive literature reviews on the application of facial grimace scales to animals, such as those by Mogil et al. [11] and Evangelista et al. [74] discuss the piglet grimace scale but do not mention the sow grimace scale developed by Navarro et al. [12], which is yet to be validated by other studies.
The sow grimace scale involves five FAUs: tension above eyes, snout angle, neck tension, temporal tension and ear position, and cheek tension (Figure 1). These FAUs are ranked from 0 to 2, with 0 indicating no pain, 1 indicating moderate pain and 2 indicating severe pain. Images of sows exhibiting each FAU at each intensity level are presented on the scale, allowing individuals to recognise and grade facial expressions with ease.
In order to maintain validity for pain assessment, it is essential for each test subject to be observed without the presence of pain to establish a baseline [66]. Navarro et al. [12] used images of the sows 19 days post-farrowing as experimental controls, relying on the assumption that they were no longer in a state of pain. Images of sows faces during this time were collected and considered indicative of no pain, with a score of 0. To represent moderate pain, a score of 1, images were captured at each inter-piglet interval. To represent severe pain, a score of 2, images were selected immediately prior to each piglet expulsion. These assumptions are necessary due to the lack of a gold standard technique for measuring pain in sows, but they are supported by prior research which reported that the performance of behavioural pain indicators increased significantly during piglet expulsion [14]. These assumed pain scores were assigned by a single observer, and then eight blinded observers were given randomised facial images to rate according to the scoring system established by the initial observer to allow for the evaluation of reliability and validity [12].

4.4. Comparing Grimace Scales

Of the five FAUs depicted in Figure 1, all except “neck tension” have been demonstrated to be indicative of pain in the piglet grimace scale [60,61]. “Tension above eyes,” “temporal tension and ear position,” “snout angle,” and “cheek tension” have been identified as significant FAUs in a number of other species, as shown in Table 1. The majority of perinatal sows in Australia are housed in farrowing crates, providing a unique opportunity for ease in monitoring facial expressions as sows are confined in an enclosed area lying in lateral recumbency during farrowing. This has allowed the recent sow facial expression scale to introduce a new FAU, “neck tension” which has not previously been described in any species [12]. This study reported that all five FAUs had relatively high percentages of reliability and were statistically significant in relation to the moment each image was captured [12].
The “tension above eyes” FAU, which encompasses both orbital tightening and eyebrow expression, was found to be the most reliable FAU for pain assessment using the sow grimace scale [12]. This coincides with findings from grimace scoring in piglets [60] and other animals including the mouse [54], rat [55], rabbit [56], sheep [59], and ferret [62]. Whilst there is strong evidence supporting orbital tightening as a valid indicator of pain, it has been speculated that its reported superiority to other FAUs is because it is more obvious for observers to recognise [62]. It is also not possible to conclude that orbital tightening is specifically caused by pain rather than other stressful environmental or physiological factors triggering a negative affective state. For example, this FAU has been found to be associated with nausea in rats [75] and physical restraint in lambs [59]. This inability to differentiate between pain and other negative affective states is common across FAUs and species and represents a significant limitation to grimace scales [11].
The “cheek tension” FAU has been found to be statistically significant and highly reliable as an indicator of pain in both sows [12] and piglets [60]. In other grimace scales, such as the mouse [54] and ferret [62] cheek bulging was indicative of pain, but in rats [55], sheep [59], piglets [60] and sows [12], cheeks were observed to change in an opposing way, flattening when in pain, referred to as “cheek tension”. The differences in cheek FAUs across species grimace scales are shown in Table 2. This demonstrates the highly species-specific nature of pain assessment, and the need for reliable, validated grimace scales for as many species as possible.

4.5. Validity and Reliability of Grimace Scales

A meta-analysis on the available grimace scales for a number of domestic species to determine their validity and reliability across the current literature found that there is a significant range in the evidence of measurement properties for grimace scales across studies and species [74]. This highlights the need for further research and validation of grimace scales. Unfortunately, the lack of a gold standard method of detecting pain in animals presents a challenge to the validation of such scales [67]. For many species, including pigs, the limited number of studies on grimace scoring is a major limitation to its validation and hence adoption as a pain assessment technique [74].
Observers require substantial training and experience for the real-time detection of changes in facial expressions due to their fleeting nature [11]. For this reason, most of the current literature uses retrospective scoring, relying on images captured from video footage to perform grimace scoring. McLennan et al. [66] found that there is a difference in results between real-time and retrospective scoring, highlighting a gap in the current knowledge on the most accurate methodology for grimace scoring. Another key limitation in the literature on grimace scoring is that many published studies do not mention or describe the training undergone by individuals conducting the rating, hence reducing the reliability of the evidence for the effectiveness of grimace scoring [11].
Observer bias, which occurs when the individual conducting the pain scoring is influenced by their expectations or opinions, is a significant consideration regarding the validity of grimace scoring [76]. Assigning scores based on visual observations is inherently subjective, particularly concerning animal pain, which can trigger strong emotional reactions in some individuals. The personality, life experience, emotional intelligence and level of empathy the scorer feels towards the animal will most likely influence the score assigned [77]. This trend was demonstrated in a study which found that individuals with higher empathy towards either humans and/or animals on average assigned higher pain scores to cattle [78] and a similar study on dogs [79]. In addition to empathy, several other factors have been reported to influence the scoring of animal pain, including age [23], veterinary education and experience [80], family size [78], and emotional attachment to animals [78]. There is some evidence that observer gender impacts the perception of the intensity of pain, with women tending to assign higher pain scores than men [12,23,81]. This phenomenon has been refuted by other studies [82,83], hence requires further investigation. Artificial intelligence programs to assess facial expressions are currently being explored as a method for eliminating observer bias and hence increasing grimace scoring validity [84]. Recently, a study by Nie et al. [85] explored the use of computer monitoring and analysis of pig facial expressions to detect heat stress, highlighting the potential applications of grimace scoring beyond parturition, as well as the integration of technology to enhance such systems. Understaffing is a significant issue for Australian pork producers [86], so the capacity for grimace scoring to be automated using artificial intelligence presents a unique opportunity for the improvement of farrowing management.

4.6. Validation of the Sow Grimace Scale

The sow grimace scale has not yet been validated. Validation could be conducted via comparisons of pain scores with other pain measurement techniques. This could involve physiological indicators of pain, including elevated cortisol [69], C-reactive protein or haptoglobin [87], but it is important to consider that the process of collecting the necessary blood samples would cause further stress to the sow [46]. Less invasive methods of collecting physiological data, such as electroencephalogram (EEG) output has been demonstrated to change in the presence of acute pain in piglets [88] and could potentially be used to validate the sow grimace scale. Alternatively, pain scores could be validated via observations of the performance of pain related behaviours described by Ison et al. [14] or the ease of farrowing score developed by Mainau et al. [33]. What could be considered the most accurate methods of grimace scale validation is the controlled infliction of a painful stimulus, or the administration of analgesics when pain is known to be occurring [89]. As the sow grimace scale is developed for use during farrowing, controlled infliction of pain is not applicable for scale validation. Analgesics, such as NSAIDs could be administered to farrowing sows as a method of scale validation. Perceived pain should change in response to analgesics according to dose if the scores are valid [90]. This procedure could also determine if each FAU is specifically indicative of pain rather than an unrelated negative affective state. Important factors that must be considered include individual variation, farrowing dynamics and the potential impact of the drugs on piglet and sow health and farrowing dynamics.

4.7. On-Farm Implementation of the Sow Grimace Scale

Following further validation of the sow grimace scale, the feasibility and method of its implementation and use on-farm must be considered. The common practice of confinement of sows in farrowing crates would allow stockpersons to have a relatively unobstructed view of the face for scoring [12]. This may not be the case for all pen designs and farrowing rooms as some layouts may not allow clear vision of the face. Furthermore, many sows farrow at night, when staff may be limited or absent, meaning close observation and scoring may not be possible. The automation of grimace scoring using artificial intelligence would resolve this concern, but its implementation would be expensive. Using the grimace scale as a technique to decide if obstetric assistance is required may be the more feasible and cost-effective option for on-farm implementation. Further study is required to determine if pain score is directly related to farrowing difficulties, but if it is found to be, pain score could be used as an indication of manual assistance being necessary, in addition to conventional methods such as IPI and farrowing duration. Any on-farm use of the grimace scale would require stockpersons to be appropriately trained [11], which is another important factor to consider. Training and implementation of the sow grimace scale on farm would provide stockpersons with a relatively simple method of estimating pain presence and intensity, which would ultimately aid in the improvement of farrowing management.

5. Conclusions

The scientific knowledge of the pain experience and dynamics of farrowing sows is highly limited, hindering the implementation of appropriate management strategies to improve sow welfare during parturition. This review explored the current understanding of parturient pain in sows and the techniques in which it can be measured. The major barrier to improving welfare outcomes of farrowing is the lack of a reliable and easily implemented method of detecting and quantifying the severity of pain in sows. This review investigated the potential for grimace scoring to fill this gap. A sow facial grimace scale was found to be a promising tool for parturient pain assessment, but further research is required to validate the technique. It is recommended that further investigation be conducted to assess and compare farrowing pain using the sow grimace scale, specifically analysing the impact of factors such as farrowing duration, IPI, and piglet number and presentation. Assessment of the relationship between grimace score and the performance of pain-related behaviours or physiological pain measurements could be performed in order to further validate the sow grimace scale. Future studies utilising the grimace scoring method could significantly improve the understanding of farrowing dynamics and pain across stages of parturition and in instances of abnormal farrowing, potentially aiding in the development of improved management strategies and hence better production outcomes.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.L. and R.B.; writing—original draft preparation, L.P.; writing—review and editing, L.P., S.L. and R.B.; supervision, S.L. and R.B.; project administration, R.B.; funding acquisition, L.P. and R.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research and L.P. were funded by Australian Pork Limited, grant number 2025/0122.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created or analyzed in this study. Data sharing is not applicable to this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study or writing of the manuscript.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
FAUFacial Action Units
IPIInter-piglet interval
NSAIDNon-steroidal anti-inflammatory drug

References

  1. Dalgleish, M.; Whitelaw, A. State of the Industry Report 2021; Australian Pork Ltd.: Barton, ACT, Australia, 2021; Available online: https://australianpork.com.au/sites/default/files/2021-10/APLStateofIndustry-Report.pdf (accessed on 1 April 2025).
  2. Athorn, R.; Plush, K. Best Practice Gilt Management for Fertility and Longevity; Australian Pork Ltd.: Barton, ACT, Australia, 2019; Available online: https://australianpork.com.au/sites/default/files/2021-06/2019-09_Best_Practice_Gilt_Management_for_Fertility_and_Longevity.pdf (accessed on 10 April 2025).
  3. Oliviero, C.; Peltoniemi, O. Troubled process of parturition of the domestic pig. In Animal Reproduction in Veterinary Medicine; Aral, F., Payan-Carreira, R., Quaresma, M., Eds.; InTechOpen: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Alonso, M.E.; González-Montaña, J.R.; Lomillos, J.M. Consumers’ concerns and perceptions of farm animal welfare. Animals 2020, 10, 385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Niven, C.; Gijsbers, K. A study of labour pain using the MCGILL pain questionnaire. Soc. Sci. Med. 1984, 19, 1347–1351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Martínez-Burnes, J.; Muns, R.; Barrios-García, H.; Villanueva-García, D.; Domínguez-Oliva, A.; Mota-Rojas, D. Parturition in mammals: Animal models, pain and distress. Animals 2021, 11, 2960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Mainau, E.; Manteca, X. Pain and discomfort caused by parturition in cows and sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 135, 241–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Walls, A.; Hatze, B.; Lomax, S.; Bathgate, R. Defining “Normal” in pig parturition. Animals 2022, 12, 2754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Crook, A. Introduction: Pain: An issue of animal welfare. In Pain Management in Veterinary Practice; Egger, C.M., Love, L., Doherty, T., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-1-1189-9919-6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Mellor, D.J. Updating animal welfare thinking: Moving beyond the “Five Freedoms” towards “A life worth living”. Animals 2016, 6, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Mogil, J.S.; Pang, D.S.J.; Dutra, G.G.S.; Chambers, C.T. The development and use of facial grimace scales for pain measurement in animals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020, 116, 480–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Navarro, E.; Mainau, E.; Manteca, X. Development of a facial expression scale using farrowing as a model of pain in sows. Animals 2020, 10, 2113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Morton, D.B.; Griffiths, P.H.M. Guidelines on the recognition of pain, distress and discomfort in experimental animals and an hypothesis for assessment. Vet. Rec. 1985, 116, 431–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Ison, S.H.; Jarvis, S.; Rutherford, K.M.D. The identification of potential behavioural indicators of pain in periparturient sows. Res. Vet. Sci. 2016, 109, 114–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Gilbert, C.L.; Lawrence, A.B.; Forsling, M.L.; Goode, J.A.; McGrath, T.J.; McLean, K.A.; Petherick, J.C. Maternal plasma vasopressin, oxytocin and cortisol concentrations following foetal ejection in the pig. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 1996, 43, 137–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Mainau, E.; Ruiz-de-la-Torre, J.L.; Dalmau, A.; Salleras, J.M.; Manteca, X. Effects of meloxicam (Metacam®) on post-farrowing sow behaviour and piglet performance. Animal 2012, 6, 494–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Melzack, R.; Kinch, R.; Dobkin, P.; Lebrun, M.; Taenzer, P. Severity of labour pain: Influence of physical as well as psychologic variables. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 1984, 130, 579–584. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  18. Noakes, D.E.; Parkinson, T.J.; England, G.C.W.; Arthur, G.H. (Eds.) Arthur’s Veterinary Reproduction and Obstetrics, 8th ed.; Harcourt Publishers Limited: Hitchin, UK, 2001; ISBN 978-0-7020-2556-3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Lowe, N.K. The nature of labor pain. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 2002, 186, 16–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Cowart, R.P. Parturition and dystocia in swine. In Current Therapy in Large Animal Theriogenology, 2nd ed.; Youngquist, R.S., Threlfall, W.R., Eds.; Elsevier Inc: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2007; pp. 778–784. ISBN 978-0-7216-9323-1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Australian Government Department of Health and Aged Care. Pregnancy, Birth and Baby (PB&B). Giving Birth—Third Stage of Labour. Available online: https://www.pregnancybirthbaby.org.au/giving-birth-third-stage-of-labour (accessed on 1 April 2025).
  22. Ison, S.H.; Jarvis, S.; Hall, S.A.; Ashworth, C.J.; Rutherford, K.M.D. Periparturient behaviour and physiology: Further insight into the farrowing process for primiparous and multiparous sows. Front. Vet. Sci. 2018, 5, 122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ison, S.H.; Rutherford, K.M.D. Attitude of farmers and veterinarians towards pain and the use of pain relief in pigs. Vet. J. 2014, 202, 622–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Edwards, S.A.; Malkin, S.J.; Spechter, H.H. An analysis of piglet mortality with behavioural observations. In Proceedings of the British Society of Animal Production; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Haussmann, M.F.; Lay, D.C.; Buchanan, H.S.; Hopper, J.G. Butorphanol tartrate acts to decrease sow activity, which could lead to reduced pig crushing. J. Anim. Sci. 1999, 77, 2054–2059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Schmitt, O.; Baxter, E.M.; Boyle, L.A.; O’Driscoll, K. Nurse sow strategies in the domestic pig: II. Consequences for piglet growth, suckling behaviour and sow nursing behaviour. Animal 2019, 13, 590–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Ahlström, S.; Jarvis, S.; Lawrence, A.B. Savaging gilts are more restless and more responsive to piglets during the expulsive phase of parturition. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 76, 83–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Fraser, D.; Phillips, P.A. Lethargy and low water intake by sows during early lactation: A cause of low piglet weight gains and survival? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1989, 24, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Oliviero, C.; Heinonen, M.; Valros, A.; Peltoniemi, O. Environmental and sow-related factors affecting the duration of farrowing. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 2010, 119, 85–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Canario, L.; Cantoni, E.; Le Bihan, E.; Caritez, J.C.; Billion, Y.; Bidanel, J.P.; Foulley, J.L. Between-breed variability of stillbirth and its relationship with sow and piglet characteristics. J. Anim. Sci. 2006, 84, 3185–3196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. van Dijk, A.J.; van Rens, B.T.T.M.; van der Lende, T.; Taverne, M.A.M. Factors affecting duration of the expulsive stage of parturition and piglet birth intervals in sows with uncomplicated, spontaneous farrowings. Theriogenology 2005, 64, 1573–1590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Nam, N.H.; Sukon, P. Risk factors associated with dystocia in swine. Vet. World 2021, 14, 1835–1839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Mainau, E.; Dalmau, A.; Ruiz-de-la-Torre, J.L.; Manteca, X. A behavioural scale to measure ease of farrowing in sows. Theriogenology 2010, 74, 1279–1287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. van Rens, B.T.T.M.; van der Lende, T. Parturition in gilts: Duration of farrowing, birth intervals and placenta expulsion in relation to maternal, piglet and placental traits. Theriogenology 2004, 62, 331–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Zaremba, W.; Udluft, T.; Failing, K.; Bostedt, H. Analysis of the course of birth and the early postpartal period in pigs after hormonal partus induction with special consideration of complication rate. Anim. Vet. Sci. 2019, 7, 29–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Lawrence, A.B.; Petherick, J.C.; McLean, K.A.; Deans, L.A.; Chirnside, J.; Gaughan, A.; Clutton, E.; Terlouw, E.M.C. The effect of environment on behaviour, plasma cortisol and prolactin in parturient sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1994, 39, 313–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Oliviero, C.; Heinonen, M.; Valros, A.; Hälli, O.; Peltoniemi, O.A.T. Effect of the environment on the physiology of the sow during late pregnancy, farrowing and early lactation. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 2008, 105, 365–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Ison, S.H.; Jarvis, S.; Rutherford, K.M.D. A survey of sow management at farrowing in the UK. Anim. Welf. 2016, 25, 309–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Kuller, W.; Sietsma, S.; Hendriksen, S.; Sperling, D. Use of paracetamol in sows around farrowing: Effect on health and condition of the sow, piglet mortality, piglet weight and piglet weight gain. Porc. Health Manag. 2021, 7, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Schoos, A.; Chantziaras, I.; Vandenabeele, J.; Biebaut, E.; Meyer, E.; Cools, A.; Devreese, M.; Maes, D. Prophylactic use of meloxicam and paracetamol in perpartal sows suffering from postpartum dysgalactia syndrome. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 8, 603719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Björkman, S.; Grahofer, A. Tools and protocols for managing hyperprolific sows at parturition: Optimizing piglet survival and sows’ reproductive health. In Animal Reproduction in Veterinary Medicine; Aral, F., Payan-Carreira, R., Quaresma, M., Eds.; IntechOpen: London, UK, 2021; ISBN 978-1-83881-938-5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hill, S.V.; del Rocio Amezcua, M.; Ribeiro, E.S.; O’Sullivan, T.L.; Friendship, R.M. Defining the effect of oxytocin use in farrowing sows on stillbirth rate: A systemic review with a meta-analysis. Animals 2022, 12, 1795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Merskey, H. Pain terms: A list with definitions and a note on usage. Recommended by the International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) Subcommittee on Taxonomy. Pain 1979, 6, 249–252. [Google Scholar]
  44. Short, C.E. Fundamentals of pain perception in animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1998, 59, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Sneddon, L.U.; Elwood, R.W.; Adamo, S.A.; Leach, M.C. Defining and assessing animal pain. Anim. Behav. 2014, 97, 201–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. McLennan, K.M. Why pain is still a welfare issue for farm animals, and how facial expression could be the answer. Agriculture 2018, 8, 127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Mellor, D.J.; Reid, C.S.W. Concepts of animal well-being and predicting the impact of procedures on experimental animals. In Improving the Well-Being of Animals in the Research Environment; Baker, R.M., Jenkin, G., Mellor, D.J., Eds.; Australian and New Zealand Council for the Care of Animals in Research and Teaching (ANZCCART): Glen Osmond, SA, Australia, 1994; pp. 3–18. ISBN 978-0-6461-8116-5. [Google Scholar]
  48. Whittaker, A.L.; Marsh, L.E. The role of behavioural assessment in determining ‘positive’ affective states in animals. CABI Rev. 2019, 14, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Murrell, J.C.; Johnson, C.B. Neurophysiological techniques to assess pain in animals. J. Vet. Pharmacol. Ther. 2006, 29, 325–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Prunier, A.; Mounier, L.; Le Neindre, P.; Leterrier, C.; Mormède, P.; Paulmier, V.; Prunet, P.; Terlouw, C.; Guatteo, R. Identifying and monitoring pain in farm animals: A review. Animal 2012, 7, 998–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Anil, S.S.; Anil, L.; Deen, J. Challenges of pain assessment in domestic animals. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2002, 220, 313–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Darwin, C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1872; ISBN 978-1-1398-3381-3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ekman, P.; Friesen, W.V. Facial Action Coding System: Investigator’s Guide; APA PsycTests: Washington, DC, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Langford, D.J.; Bailey, A.L.; Chanda, M.L.; Clarke, S.E.; Drummond, T.E.; Echols, S.; Glick, S.; Ingrao, J.; Klassen-Ross, T.; LaCroix-Fralish, M.L.; et al. Coding of facial expressions of pain in the laboratory mouse. Nat. Methods 2010, 7, 447–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Sotocina, S.G.; Sorge, R.E.; Zaloum, A.; Tuttle, A.H.; Martin, L.J.; Wieskopf, J.S.; Mapplebech, J.C.S.; Wei, P.; Zhan, S.; Zhang, S.; et al. The rat grimace scale: A partially automated method for quantifying pain in the laboratory rat via facial expressions. Mol. Pain 2011, 7, 1744–8069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Keating, S.C.J.; Thomas, A.A.; Flecknell, P.A.; Leach, M.C. Evaluation of EMLA cream for preventing pain during tattooing of rabbits: Changes in physiological, behavioural and facial expression responses. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e44437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Gleerup, K.B.; Forkman, B.; Lindegaard, C.; Andersen, P.H. An equine pain face. Vet. Anaesth. Analg. 2015, 42, 103–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Gleerup, K.B.; Andersen, P.H.; Munksgaard, L.; Forkman, B. Pain evaluation in dairy cattle. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 171, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Guesgen, M.J.; Beausoleil, N.J.; Leach, M.; Minot, E.O.; Stewart, M.; Stafford, K.J. Coding and quantification of a facial expression for pain in lambs. Behav. Process. 2016, 132, 49–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Di Giminiani, P.; Brierley, V.L.M.H.; Scollo, A.; Gottardo, F.; Malcolm, E.M.; Edwards, S.A.; Leach, M.C. The assessment of facial expressions in piglets undergoing tail docking and castration: Toward the development of the piglet grimace scale. Front. Vet. Sci. 2016, 3, 100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Viscardi, A.V.; Hunniford, M.; Lawlis, P.; Leach, M.; Turner, P.V. Development of a piglet grimace scale to evaluate piglet pain using facial expressions following castration and tail docking: A pilot study. Front. Vet. Sci. 2017, 4, 51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Reijgwart, M.L.; Schoemaker, N.J.; Pascuzzo, R.; Leach, M.C.; Stodel, M.; de Nies, L.; Hendriksen, C.F.M.; van der Meer, M.; Vinke, C.M.; van Zeeland, Y.R. The composition and initial evaluation of a grimace scale in ferrets after surgical implantation of a telemetry probe. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0187986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Evangelista, M.C.; Watanabe, R.; Leung, V.S.Y.; Monteiro, B.P.; O’Toole, E.; Pang, D.S.J.; Steagall, P.V. Facial expressions of pain in cats: The development and validation of a feline grimace scale. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 19128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Domínguez-Oliva, A.; Olmos-Hernández, A.; Hernández-Ávalos, I.; Lecona-Butrón, H.; Mora-Medina, P.; Mota-Rojas, D. Rat grimace scale as a method to evaluate animal welfare, nociception, and quality of the euthansia method of Wistar rats. Animals 2023, 13, 3161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Matsumiya, L.C.; Sorge, R.E.; Sotocinal, S.G.; Tabaka, J.M.; Wieskopf, J.S.; Zaloum, A.; King, O.D.; Mogil, J.S. Using the mouse grimace scale to reevaluate the efficacy of postoperative analgesics in laboratory mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2012, 51, 42–49. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  66. McLennan, K.M.; Miller, A.L.; Dalla Costa, E.; Stucke, D.; Corke, M.J.; Broom, D.M.; Leach, M.C. Conceptual and methodological issues relating to pain assessment in mammals: The development and utilisation of pain facial expression scales. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 217, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Herskin, M.S.; Di Giminiani, P. Pain in pigs: Characterisation, mechanisms and indicators. In Advances in Pig Welfare; Špinka, M., Ed.; Woodhead Publishing: Cambridge, UK, 2017; pp. 325–355. ISBN 978-0-08-101012-9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Walls, A. An Investigation of Sow Parturition: Developing Tools, Technologies and Protocols to Improve Sow and Piglet Welfare. Unpublished. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia, 2024. [Google Scholar]
  69. Randall, G.C.B.; Kendall, J.Z.; Tsang, B.K.; Taverne, M.A.M. Endocrine changes following infusion of fetal pigs with corticotropin in litters of reduced numbers. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 1990, 23, 109–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Mota-Rojas, D.; Martínez-Burnes, J.; Napolitano, F.; Domínguez-Muñoz, M.; Guerrero-Legarreta, I.; Mora-Medina, P.; Ramírez-Necoechea, R.; Lezama-García, K.; Gonález-Lozano, M. Dystocia: Factors affecting parturition in domestic animals. CABI Rev. 2020, 15, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Viscardi, A.V.; Turner, P.V. Use of meloxicam or ketoprofen for piglet pain control following surgical castration. Front. Vet. Sci. 2018, 5, 299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Vullo, C.; Barbieri, S.; Catone, G.; Graïc, J.-M.; Magaletti, M.; Di Rosa, A.; Motta, A.; Tremolada, C.; Canali, E.; Dalla Costa, E. Is the piglet grimace scale (PGS) a useful welfare indicator to assess pain after cryptorchidectomy in growing pigs? Animals 2020, 10, 412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Lou, M.E.; Porter, S.T.; Massey, J.S.; Ventura, B.; Deen, J.; Li, Y. The application of 3D landmark-based geometric morphometrics towards refinement of the piglet grimace scale. Animals 2022, 12, 1944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Evangelista, M.C.; Monteiro, B.P.; Steagall, P.V. Measurement properties of grimace scales for pain assessment in nonhuman mammals: A systemic review. Pain 2022, 163, 697–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Yamamoto, K.; Tatsutani, S.; Ishida, T. Detection of nausea-like response in rats by monitoring facial expression. Front. Pharmacol. 2017, 7, 534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Tuyttens, F.A.M.; Stadig, L.; Heerkens, J.L.T.; Van laer, E.; Buijs, S.; Ampe, B. Opinion of applied ethologists on expectation bias, blinding observers and other debiasing techniques. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 181, 27–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Rutherford, K.M.D. Assessing pain in animals. Anim. Welf. 2002, 11, 31–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Norring, M.; Wikman, I.; Hokkanen, A.-H.; Kujala, M.V.; Hänninen, L. Empathic veterinarians score cattle pain higher. Vet. J. 2014, 200, 186–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Ellingsen, K.; Zanella, A.J.; Bjerkås, E.; Indrebø, A. The relationship between empathy, perception of pain and attitudes toward pets among Norwegian dog owners. Anthrozoös 2010, 23, 231–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Hellyer, P.W.; Frederick, C.; Lacy, M.; Salman, M.D.; Wagner, A.E. Attitudes of veterinary medical students, house officers, clinical faculty and staff toward pain management in animals. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 1999, 214, 238–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Huxley, J.N.; Whay, H.R. Current attitudes of cattle practitioners to pain and the use of analgesics in cattle. Vet. Rec. 2006, 159, 662–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Monteiro, B.P.; Lee, N.H.Y.; Steagall, P.V. Can cat caregivers reliably assess acute pain in cats using the feline grimace scale? A large bilingual global survey. J. Feline Med. Surg. 2023, 25, 1098612X221145499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Trindade, P.H.E.; Lopez-Soriano, M.; Merenda, V.R.; Tomacheuski, R.M.; Pairis-Garcia, M.D. Effect of the observer’s gender bias monitoring acute pain using a validated behaviour scale in castrated piglets. Res. Sq. 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Broomé, S.; Feighelstein, M.; Zamansky, A.; Carreira Lencioni, G.; Haubro Andersen, P.; Pessanha, F.; Mahmoud, M.; Kjellström, H.; Ali Salah, A. Going deeper than tracking: A survey of computer-vision based recognition of animal pain and emotions. Int. J. Comput. Vis. 2023, 131, 572–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Nie, L.; Li, B.; Jiao, F.; Shao, J.; Yand, T.; Liu, Z. ASPP-YOLOv5: A study on constructing pig facial expression recognition for heat stress. Comput. Electron. Agric. 2023, 214, 108346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Australian Pork Limited. Submission into the National Agricultural Workforce Strategy. August 2020. Available online: https://australianpork.com.au/sites/default/files/2022-02/APL%20NAWS%20Submission%20%281%29.pdf (accessed on 1 April 2025).
  87. Eckersall, P.D.; Saini, P.K.; McComb, C. The acute phase response of acid soluble glycoprotein, α1-acid glycoprotein. Ceruloplasmin, haptoglobin and C-reactive protein, in the pig. Vet. Immunol. Immunopathol. 1996, 51, 377–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Haga, H.A.; Ranheim, B. Castration of piglet: The analgesic effects of intratesticular and infrafunicular lidocaine injection. Vet. Anaesth. Analg. 2005, 32, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Onuma, K.; Watanabe, M.; Sasaki, N. The grimace scale: A useful tool for assessing pain in laboratory animals. Exp. Anim. 2024, 73, 234–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Weary, D.M.; Niel, L.; Flower, F.C.; Fraser, D. Identifying and preventing pain in animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006, 100, 64–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Sow grimace scale, from Navarro et al. [12].
Figure 1. Sow grimace scale, from Navarro et al. [12].
Animals 15 02915 g001
Table 1. Grimace scales currently published for domestic species, including the cause of pain for the animal and the Facial Action Units (FAUs) observed and scored in the study.
Table 1. Grimace scales currently published for domestic species, including the cause of pain for the animal and the Facial Action Units (FAUs) observed and scored in the study.
AnimalCause of PainFacial Action Units (FAUs)
Mouse [54]Injection of nociceptive compoundsOrbital tightening, nose bulge, cheek bulge, ear position, whisker change.
Rat [55]Injection of nociceptive compoundsOrbital tightening, nose/cheek flattening, ear changes, whisker change.
Rabbit [56]Ear tattooingOrbital tightening, cheek flattening, pointed nose, whisker change.
Horse [57]Tourniquet or topical capsaicinAngled eye, withdrawn and tense stare, asymmetrical/low ears, square-like nostrils, muzzle tension, tension of the mimic muscles.
Cow [58]Range of painful conditions Orbital tightening, tense stare, tense and backwards ears, tension of facial muscles, strained or dilated nostrils, tension of the lips.
Sheep [59]Tail dockingOrbital tightening, nose features, mouth features, cheek flattening, ear posture.
Piglet [60,61]Castration and tail dockingOrbital tightening, cheek tightening/nose bulge, ear position.
Ferrett [62]Intraperitoneal telemetry probe implantationOrbital tightening, nose bulging, cheek bulging, ear changes.
Cat [63]Admitted to veterinary hospital with abdominal painWhisker retraction, orbital tightening, muzzle tension, ear position, whisker position, head position.
Sow [12]FarrowingTension above eyes, snout angle, neck tension, temporal tension and ear position, cheek tension.
Table 2. Cheek Facial Action Units (FAUs) from published grimace scales with images showing the FAU in the absence of pain (pain score = 0), moderate pain (pain score = 1) and severe pain (pain score = 2). Images from [12,54,55,59,60,62].
Table 2. Cheek Facial Action Units (FAUs) from published grimace scales with images showing the FAU in the absence of pain (pain score = 0), moderate pain (pain score = 1) and severe pain (pain score = 2). Images from [12,54,55,59,60,62].
AnimalCheek FAUPain not PresentModerate PainSevere Pain
Mouse [54]“Cheek bulge”Animals 15 02915 i001Animals 15 02915 i002Animals 15 02915 i003
Ferret [62]“Cheek bulging”Animals 15 02915 i004Animals 15 02915 i005Animals 15 02915 i006
Rat [55]“Nose/cheek flattening”Animals 15 02915 i007Animals 15 02915 i008Animals 15 02915 i009
Sheep [59]“Cheek flattening”Animals 15 02915 i010Animals 15 02915 i011Animals 15 02915 i012
Piglet [60]“Cheek tension”Animals 15 02915 i013Animals 15 02915 i014Animals 15 02915 i015
Sow [12]“Cheek tension”Animals 15 02915 i016Animals 15 02915 i017Animals 15 02915 i018
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Palmer, L.; Lomax, S.; Bathgate, R. A Review of Assessment of Sow Pain During Farrowing Using Grimace Scores. Animals 2025, 15, 2915. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192915

AMA Style

Palmer L, Lomax S, Bathgate R. A Review of Assessment of Sow Pain During Farrowing Using Grimace Scores. Animals. 2025; 15(19):2915. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192915

Chicago/Turabian Style

Palmer, Lucy, Sabrina Lomax, and Roslyn Bathgate. 2025. "A Review of Assessment of Sow Pain During Farrowing Using Grimace Scores" Animals 15, no. 19: 2915. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192915

APA Style

Palmer, L., Lomax, S., & Bathgate, R. (2025). A Review of Assessment of Sow Pain During Farrowing Using Grimace Scores. Animals, 15(19), 2915. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192915

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop