A Conceptual Framework for the Co-Construction of Human–Dog Dyadic Relationship
Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
- Intrinsic motivation: doing an activity for its inherent interest and enjoyment (most autonomous).
- Extrinsic motivation: driven by external outcomes, further divided into four types, (integrated, identified, introjected and external), from most to least autonomous.
- Amotivation: a state of disengagement and lack of intention, representing non autonomous regulation.
- Autonomy: experiencing volition and self-direction.
- Competence: feeling effective and capable.
- Relatedness: feeling connected to others, accepted, and supported by peers.
- Dyadic relationships are co-constructed, with participation, cooperation, and SDT offering a viable framework for their conceptualization.
- Existing measurement approaches, although not originally designed to assess dyadic SDT, may serve as “proto-tools” that can be adapted and refined to study this relational process.
2. Materials and Methods
- The co-construction of dyadic interactions in relation to SDT;
- Tools and frameworks used to assess the fulfillment of basic psychological needs within dyadic contexts.
- Title and abstract relevance;
- Publication date (<10 years), ensuring the narrative review to reflect the most recent methodological and conceptual advances in a still emerging field;
- Language (French/English), as these options cover most of the relevant scientific literature, as appreciated by the authors.
3. Competence and Education
3.1. Theoretical Objectives and SDT
- Establishing, developing, adapting, and consolidating a foundational dyadic system of interspecific communication and interpretation;
- Identifying and (de)valuing (in)appropriate behaviors through inter-species (de)motivational techniques.
3.2. What Techniques for Achieving These Objectives: Links with SDT and Dyadic Impacts?
- Aversive techniques, based on negative reinforcement and positive punishment. Respectively, these involve removing a stimulus following a desired behavior or adding a negative stimulus following an undesirable one (e.g., electric collars, choke chains, prong collars, physical/verbal/visual punishments).
- Reward-based (positive) techniques, focused on positive reinforcement—where a desirable behavior is followed by a reward, and an undesirable behavior by the removal of a positive stimulus (e.g., treats, toys, praise, clicker training).
- Mixed techniques, which combines both aversive and reward-based methods.
- A higher risk of canine aggression [19].
- Neural coupling occurred in the frontal and parietal brain regions, associated with joint attention;
- Mutual gaze and physical touch independently enhanced this coupling, with their combination having a synergistic effect;
- This neural synchronization increased over time, plateauing by day seven.
- Dogs can understand certain words, tones, intentions, and human emotions;
- Humans can interpret canine vocalizations and emotional expressions.
- Sufficient autonomy to explore the other;
- Opportunities for self-expression;
- The ability to accumulate and interpret signals over time.
- The frustration of basic psychological needs, leading to extrinsic or amotivated perceptions of training, and possibly behavior problems that affect human well-being;
- A breakdown in canine communication that increases the risk of misunderstandings or accidents.
3.3. Intra-/Interspecific Factors Influencing This Psychological Need?
- Dogs exposed to humans early are more likely to understand pointing gestures;
- Older dogs may be less responsive to facial expressions;
- Brachycephalic dogs show more attention to human faces;
- Breed and work style influence gaze duration and task performance, although breed only accounts for a small portion of behavioral variation [23]. Behavioral expressions and predispositions are linked to a certain genetic selection but also to environment, utility, training, and experience.
3.4. Practical Feasibility of These Theoretical Objectives?
- Theoretical vs. practical gaps: While theoretically sound, these objectives are not always fully achievable in real-life contexts.
- Impact of educational techniques: The choice of an educational technique influences the fluidity and stability of dyadic co-construction.
- Effectiveness of positive methods: Reward-based methods tend to facilitate better fulfillment of fundamental psychological needs than aversive methods.
- Need for further research: Future studies should explore the interplay of biopsychosocial, experiential, and environmental factors in shaping dyadic competence over time.
4. Attachment
4.1. Can We Talk About Bilateral Relatedness Between Owners and Dogs?
- In the Middle Ages and early modern times, moralists, especially within the discourse of the Catholic Church, condemned the practice of keeping animals solely for companionship. However, in wealthier households and religious institutions, their presence was already considered normal in practice and everyday life, and their role could also include that of companionship.
- In the XVIth and XVIIth centuries, European aristocratic households kept a variety of species they considered companion animals, including dogs and exotic animals.
- In the XVIIIth century, the rise of the urban middle class contributed to the spread of companion animal ownership to broader segments of Western society. Factors behind this trend included the bourgeoisie’s desire to emulate the aristocracy and increased rural-to-urban migration.
- During the Victorian era, companion animal keeping became a family activity for the middle class. Companion animals were seen as having both social and educational benefits, especially for children.
- A high level of attachment anxiety refers to individuals who worry about the reliability or availability of those they are attached to.
- A high level of attachment avoidance refers to individuals who distrust or distance themselves from their attachment figures.
- Secure attachment, in contrast to these insecure styles, is characterized by confidence in the reliability and responsiveness of others, which promotes well-being.
- seeking proximity to their owners;
- displaying varying levels of distress during separation;
- actively initiating contact upon reunion.
4.2. What Are the Impacts of This Fundamental Psychological Need on Dyads and Their Participants in Relation to SDT?
- They show reduced asymmetry in rightward tail wagging in the presence of a stranger when their owner is near [80].
- Dogs aggressive toward strangers tended to have owners with lower levels of anxious attachment;
- Dogs aggressive toward their owners had owners with higher avoidant attachment and lower conscientiousness.
- most studies reported a positive or neutral correlation with depression;
- secure attachment was negatively associated with depression;
- insecure attachment (anxious or avoidant) was positively associated with depression;
- Still, causality remains hard to establish [68].
- Owners with secure attachment often had dogs with secure attachment.
- Owners with avoidant attachment tended to substitute human relationships with canine ones, and their dogs also displayed avoidant or anxious styles.
- Owners with anxious attachment preferred turning to their dog during stress, and their dogs often developed secure attachment.
- The quality of the bond between the dogs (friendly or parental; shared resources);
- The emotional state of the grieving owner.
4.3. Is This Psychological Need Influenced by Intra-/Interspecific Factors?
4.4. Is Attachment Within a Dyad Co-Constructed by Both the Human and the Dog?
- Dog’s attachment to humans in more depth;
- Influences of biopsychosocial factors, lived experiences, and environmental contexts;
- Longitudinal effects on dyadic development.
- Bilateral attachment: Attachment should no longer be viewed as a one-sided human experience, it is co-constructed.
- Attachment style and co-construction: Secure attachment fosters more positive dyadic co-construction than insecure styles.
- The example of grief: In the face of difficult events, humans and/or dogs seem to enter processes of deconstruction of the previously experienced co-construction and of emotional acceptance.
- Need for further research: Future studies must deepen the understanding of canine attachment as well as the various factors that shape dyads across time and space. Beyond the experience of grief, when faced with life’s challenges, are dyads capable of undergoing deco-construction and then reco-construction?
5. Autonomy
5.1. Theoretical Objectives and SDT
- Promote a safe environment for all members of the dyad [12].
5.2. What Techniques for Achieving These Objectives?
5.2.1. Relationship with the Physical and Social Environment: SDT-Relevant Influences (Impacts and Intra/Interspecific Factors)
- Analysis of the physical and social environment provided to the dog.
- The extent to which the dog appropriates this environment.
- The owners’ understanding of and responsiveness to their dog’s preferences.
- More time sniffing/exploring (p < 0.001) and digging intensively (p < 0.05).
- Less time seeking proximity or contact with their handler.
5.2.2. Negotiating Canine Consent: SDT-Relevant Influences
5.2.3. Perceptions of Human Autonomy in a Dyadic Context: SDT-Relevant Influences
5.3. Practical Feasibility of These Theoretical Objectives?
- Theoretical vs. practical tension: Despite its idealistic aims, the current literature remains limited.
- Dyadic interdependence: Dogs do not possess absolute autonomy. Instead, autonomy emerges through co-constructed interdependence with humans.
- Research gaps: Further studies are required to explore how human, canine, and dyadic actors relate to these theoretical goals. Future work should consider biopsychosocial factors, life experiences, and physical/social environments across timeframes.
6. Researching and Measuring Co-Construction
6.1. Engaging, Observing, Questioning
6.2. Exploration of the Nervous System
6.3. Measuring (Psychometrics and Behavioral Scales)
- Canine behavioral assessment & research questionnaire (C-BARQ): extensive (≈101 items) owner-reported measure covering multiple behavior domains, with established reliability and partial validity [114].
- Pet attachment questionnaire (PAQ) [81]: measures owner attachment (to a companion animal) style. Out of 26 items, testing anxious and avoidant attachment, 7-point Likert scale each, with acceptable reliability and partial validity.
- Lexington attachment to pets scale (LAPS) [125]: 23 items across factors of general attachment, substitution, and animal rights/welfare (4-point Likert scale), widely used in the literature.
6.4. Lessons from “Proto-Tools” and Paths Forward
- Develop bilateral instruments that simultaneously integrate owner and canine perspectives (e.g., matched owner-report and behavioral/physiological indices; dyadic questionnaires that capture shared routines, negotiated choices, and mutual influence).
- Adopt mixed-methods designs combining qualitative context, standardized behavioral probes, scalable psychometrics, and targeted neurophysiological measures. This triangulation would leverage the strengths of each approach.
- Standardize protocols for EEG/fMRI/HRV in dogs (e.g., electrode placement, preprocessing, training procedures, and anesthesia handling) to improve comparability and reproducibility.
- Prioritize longitudinal and naturalistic studies to capture co-construction as an evolving process and to disentangle causal pathways among technique, experience, environment, and dyadic outcomes.
- Report samples and contexts rigorously (e.g., breed, age, training history, owner demographics, environment) to allow appropriate stratification and meta-analytic synthesis.
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Foudriat, M. Définition et dimensions de la co-construction. In La Co-Construction: Une Alternative Managériale, 2nd ed.; Foudriat, M., Ed.; Politiques et interventions sociales; Presses de l’École des Hautes Études en Santé Publique: Rennes, France, 2019; pp. 15–36. ISBN 9782810908257. [Google Scholar]
- Siniscalchi, M.; d’Ingeo, S.; Minunno, M.; Quarenta, A. Communication in Dogs. Animals 2018, 31, 131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Range, F.; Kassis, A.; Taborsky, M.; Boada, M.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Wolves and dogs recruit human partners in the cooperative string-pulling task. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 17591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wallner Werneck Mendes, J.; Vindevogel, M.; van Peer, I.; Martínez, M.; Cimarelli, G.; Range, F. Dogs understand the role of a human partner in a cooperative task. Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 10179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Csepregi, M.; Gácsi, M. Factors Contributing to Successful Spontaneous Dog-Human Cooperation. Animals 2023, 13, 2390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodriguez, K.E.; Herzog, H.; Gee, N.R. Variability in Human-Animal Interaction Research. Front. Vet. Sci. 2021, 7, 619600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ryan, R.M. The Oxford Handbook of Self-Determination Theory, 1st ed.; Ryan, R.M., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2023; 1229p, ISBN 9780197600078. [Google Scholar]
- Skinner, B.F. (Ed.) Science And Human Behavior, 2nd ed.; B. F. Skinner Foundation: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014; 466p, ISBN -13 9780029290408. [Google Scholar]
- Staddon, J.E.R. Adaptive Behavior and Learning, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2016; 611p, ISBN 9781139998369. [Google Scholar]
- Healey, R.; Pepper, A. Interspecies justice: Agency, self-determination, and assent. Philos. Stud. 2021, 178, 1223–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fennell, D.A. Animal-informed consent: Sled dog tours as asymmetric agential events. Tour. Manag. 2022, 93, 104584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, N. Animal Ethics and the Autonomous Animal Self, 1st ed.; Thomas, N., Ed.; Palgrave Macmillan: London, UK, 2016; 190p, ISBN -13 9781137586841. [Google Scholar]
- Amiot, C.E.; Santerre-Bélec, L. Toward more equal and mutual human-pet relations: Insights and possible solutions based on social psychological theories. Front. Vet. Sci. 2022, 9, 1009267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bell Rizzolo, J.; Bradshaw, G. Nonhuman Animal Nations: Transforming Conservation into Wildlife Self-Determination. Soc. Anim. 2019, 29, 393–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barina-Silvestri, M.; Díaz-Videla, M.; Delgado-Rodríguez, R. Pet parenting: A systematic review of its characteristics and effects on companion dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2024, 76, 13–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henning, J.S.L.; Costa, A.G.; Fernandez, E.J. Shifting attitudes on animal ‘ownership’: Ethical implications for welfare research and practice terminology. Res. Ethics 2023, 19, 409–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Medina, L.; Dillenburger, K.; Hurtado-Parrado, C. We Don’t Train in Vain: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Human and Canine Caregiver Training. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2020, 23, 265–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Aniello, B.; Scandurra, A.; Alterisio, A.; Valsecchi, P.; Prato-Previde, E. The importance of gestural communication: A study of human-dog communication using incongruent information. Anim. Cogn. 2016, 19, 1231–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dodman, N.H.; Brown, D.C.; Serpell, J.A. Associations between owner personality and psychological status and the prevalence of canine behavior problems. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0192846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gobbo, E.; Zupan, M. Dogs’ Sociability, Owners’ Neuroticism and Attachment Style to Pets as Predictors of Dog Aggression. Animals 2020, 10, 315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, A.C.; Wynne, C.D.L. Training Dogs with Science or with Nature? An Exploration of Trainers’ Word Use, Gender, and Certification Across Dog-Training Methods. Anthrozoös 2023, 36, 35–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mikkola, S.; Salonen, M.; Puurunen, J.; Hakanen, E.; Sulkama, S.; Araujo, C.; Lohi, H. Aggressive behaviour is affected by demographic, environmental and behavioural factors in purebred dogs. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 9433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morrill, K.; Hekman, J.; Li, X.; McClure, J.; Logan, B.; Goodman, L.; Gao, M.; Dong, Y.; Alonso, M.; Carmichael, E.; et al. Ancestry-inclusive dog genomics challenges popular breed stereotypes. Science 2022, 376, eabk0639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Payne, E.M.; Arnott, E.R.; Early, J.B.; Bennett, P.C.; McGreevy, P.D. Dogmanship on the farm: Analysis of personality dimensions and training styles of stock dog handlers in Australia. J. Vet. Behav. 2015, 10, 471–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Payne, E.M.; Bennett, P.C.; McGreevy, P.D. DogTube: An examination of dogmanship online. J. Vet. Behav. 2017, 17, 50–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ren, W.; Yu, S.; Guo, K.; Lu, C.; Zhang, Y.Q. Disrupted Human–Dog Interbrain Neural Coupling in Autism-Associated Shank3 Mutant Dogs. Adv. Sci. 2024, 11, 2402493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vieira de Castro, A.C.; Fuchs, D.; Morello, G.M.; Pastur, S.; de Sousa, L.; Olsson, I.A.S. Does training method matter? Evidence for the negative impact of aversive-based methods on companion dog welfare. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0225023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stevens, J.R.; Wolff, L.M.; Bosworth, M.; Morstad, J. Dog and owner characteristics predict training success. Anim. Cogn. 2021, 24, 219–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Feuerbacher, E.N.; Wynne, C.D. Shut up and pet me! Domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer petting to vocal praise in concurrent and single-alternative choice procedures. Behav. Process. 2015, 110, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Correia-Caeiro, C.; Guo, K.; Mills, D.S. Visual perception of emotion cues in dogs: A critical review of methodologies. Anim. Cogn. 2023, 26, 727–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ziv, G. The effects of using aversive training methods in dogs—A review. J. Vet. Behav. 2017, 19, 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boch, M.; Huber, L.; Lamm, C. Domestic dogs as a comparative model for social neuroscience: Advances and challenges. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2024, 162, 105700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kotrschal, K. Wolf–Dog–Human: Companionship Based on Common Social Tools. Animals 2023, 13, 2729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bedossa, T.; Jeannin, S. Comportement et Bien-Être du Chien une Approche Interdisciplinaire, 1st ed.; Éducagri Éditions: Dijon, France, 2020; 500p, ISBN 9791027503438. [Google Scholar]
- Koutsopoulos, T. Dog Training: Theoretical Issues. In A New Approach to Dogs and Dog Training: Human-Canine Synergy in Theory and Practice, 1st ed.; Koutsopoulos, T., Ed.; Springer Nature: Cham, Switzerland, 2024; pp. 111–131. ISBN 978303154004. [Google Scholar]
- Gregg, J. Humanish: What Talking to Your Cat or Naming Your Car Reveals About the Uniquely Human Need to Humanize, 1st ed.; Little, Brown and Company: New York, NY, USA, 2025; 304p, ISBN -13 9780316577588. [Google Scholar]
- Arhant, C.; Bubna-Littitz, H.; Bartels, A.; Futschik, A.; Troxler, J. Behaviour of smaller and larger dogs: Effects of training methods, inconsistency of owner behaviour and level of engagement in activities with the dog. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 123, 131–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dale, A.R.; Podlesnik, C.A.; Elliffe, D. Evaluation of an aversion-based program designed to reduce predation of native birds by dogs: An analysis of training records for 1156 dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 191, 59–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deldalle, S.; Gaunet, F. Effects of 2 training methods on stress-related behaviors of the dog (Canis familiaris) and on the dog–owner relationship. J. Vet. Behav. 2014, 9, 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feuerbacher, E.N.; Wynne, C.D.L. Most domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer food to petting: Population, context, and schedule effects in concurrent choice. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2014, 101, 385–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haverbeke, A.; Laporte, B.; Depiereux, E.; Giffroy, J.M.; Diederich, C. Training methods of military dog handlers and their effects on the team’s performances. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 113, 110–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rooney, N.J.; Cowan, S. Training methods and owner–dog interactions: Links with dog behaviour and learning ability. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 132, 169–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Serpell, J.A.; Duffy, D.L. Aspects of Juvenile and Adolescent Environment Predict Aggression and Fear in 12-Month-Old Guide Dogs. Front. Vet. Sci. 2016, 3, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bender, Y.; Brauer, J.; Schweinberger, S.R. What makes a good dog-owner team?-A systematic review about compatibility in personality and attachment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2023, 260, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Groenewoud, D.; Enders-Slegers, M.J.; Leontjevas, R.; van Dijke, A.; de Winkel, T.; Hediger, K. Children’s bond with companion animals and associations with psychosocial health: A systematic review. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1120000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cimarelli, G.; Schindlbauer, J.; Pegger, T.; Wesian, V.; Virányi, Z. Secure base effect in former shelter dogs and other family dogs: Strangers do not provide security in a problem-solving task. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0261790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duranton, C.; Bedossa, T.; Gaunet, F. Pet dogs synchronize their walking pace with that of their owners in open outdoor areas. Anim. Cogn. 2018, 21, 219–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duranton, C.; Courby-Betremieux, C.; Gaunet, F. One- and Two-Month-Old Dog Puppies Exhibit Behavioural Synchronization with Humans Independently of Familiarity. Animals 2022, 12, 3356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duranton, C.; Bedossa, T.; Gaunet, F. Interspecific behavioural synchronization: Dogs exhibit locomotor synchrony with humans. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 12384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duranton, C.; Bedossa, T.; Gaunet, F. When walking in an outside area, shelter dogs (Canis familiaris) synchronize activity with their caregivers but do not remain as close to them as do pet dogs. J. Comp. Psychol. 2019, 133, 397–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lamontagne, A.; Legou, T.; Rauchbauer, B.; Grosbras, M.H.; Fabre, F.; Gaunet, F. Behavioural synchronization and social referencing of dogs and humans: Walking in dyad vs in group. Anim. Cogn. 2023, 26, 1021–1034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Le Roux, M.; Wright, S. The Relationship Between Pet Attachment, Life Satisfaction, and Perceived Stress: Results from a South African Online Survey. Anthrozoös 2020, 33, 371–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luhmann, M.; Kalitzki, A. How animals contribute to subjective well-being: A comprehensive model of protective and risk factors. J. Posit. Psychol. 2018, 13, 200–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martins, C.F.; Silva, L.; Soares, J.; Pinto, G.S.; Abrantes, C.; Cardoso, L.; Pires, M.A.; Sousa, H.; Mota, M.P. Walk or be walked by the dog? The attachment role. BMC Public Health 2024, 24, 684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGuire, B.; Fry, K.; Orantes, D.; Underkofler, L.; Parry, S. Sex of Walker Influences Scent-marking Behavior of Shelter Dogs. Animals 2020, 10, 632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meehan, M.; Massavelli, B.; Pachana, N. Using Attachment Theory and Social Support Theory to Examine and Measure Pets as Sources of Social Support and Attachment Figures. Anthrozoös 2017, 30, 273–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Northrope, K.; Ruby, M.B.; Howell, T.J. How Attachment to Dogs and to Other Humans Relate to Mental Health. Animals 2024, 14, 2773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rehn, T.; Beetz, A.; Keeling, L.J. Links between an Owner’s Adult Attachment Style and the Support-Seeking Behavior of Their Dog. Front Psychol. 2017, 8, 2059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ren, W.; Wei, P.; Yu, S.; Zhang, Y.Q. Left-right asymmetry and attractor-like dynamics of dog’s tail wagging during dog-human interactions. iScience 2022, 25, 104747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richards, E.M.; Silver, Z.A.; Santos, L.R. Impact of the Dog–Human Bond on Canine Social Evaluation: Attachment Predicts Preference toward Prosocial Actors. Animals 2023, 13, 2480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Riggio, G.; Gazzano, A.; Zsilák, B.; Carlone, B.; Mariti, C. Quantitative Behavioral Analysis and Qualitative Classification of Attachment Styles in Domestic Dogs: Are Dogs with a Secure and an Insecure-Avoidant Attachment Different? Animals 2021, 11, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sipple, N.; Thielke, L.; Smith, A.; Vitale, K.R.; Udell, M.A.R. Intraspecific and Interspecific Attachment between Cohabitant Dogs and Human Caregivers. Integr. Comp. Biol. 2021, 61, 132–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Uccheddu, S.; Ronconi, L.; Albertini, M.; Coren, S.; Da Graça Pereira, G.; De Cataldo, L.; Haverbeke, A.; Mills, D.S.; Pierantoni, L.; Riemer, S.; et al. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) grieve over the loss of a conspecific. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 1920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wanser, S.H.; MacDonald, M.; Udell, M.A.R. Dog–human behavioral synchronization: Family dogs synchronize their behavior with child family members. Anim. Cogn. 2021, 24, 747–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompkins, A.M.; Lazarowski, L.; Ramaiahgari, B.; Gotoor, S.S.R.; Waggoner, P.; Denney, T.S.; Deshpande, G.; Katz, J.S. Dog-human social relationship: Representation of human face familiarity and emotions in the dog brain. Anim. Cogn. 2021, 24, 251–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Solomon, J.; Beetz, A.; Schöberl, I.; Gee, N.; Kotrschal, K. Attachment security in companion dogs: Adaptation of Ainsworth’s strange situation and classification procedures to dogs and their human caregivers. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2019, 21, 389–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benz-Schwarzburg, J.; Monsó, S.; Huber, L. How Dogs Perceive Humans and How Humans Should Treat Their Pet Dogs: Linking Cognition With Ethics. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 584037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellis, A.; Hawkins, R.; Stanton, S.; Loughnan, S. The Association Between Companion Animal Attachment and Depression: A Systematic Review. Anthrozoös 2024, 37, 1067–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hughes, B.; Lewis Harkin, B. The Impact of Continuing Bonds Between Pet Owners and Their Pets Following the Death of Their Pet: A Systematic Narrative Synthesis. Omega 2022, 90, 1666–1684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Serpell, J.A. Companion animals. In Anthrozoology: Human-Animal Interactions in Domesticated and Wild Animals; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018; pp. 17–31. ISBN 9780198753629. [Google Scholar]
- Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss, 2nd ed.; Basic Book: New York, NY, USA, 1982; Volume 1, 326p, ISBN 0465005438. [Google Scholar]
- Kellert, S.R.; Wilson, E.O. The Biophilia Hypothesis, 1st ed.; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1993; 496p, ISBN 9781559631471. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, E.O. Biophilia—The Human Bond with Other Species, 2nd ed.; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1986; 176p, ISBN 978067074422. [Google Scholar]
- DeLoache, J.; Pickard, M.; LoBue, V. How very young children think about animals. In How Animals Affect Us: Examining the Influence of Human-Animal Interaction on Child Dev and Human Health, 1st ed.; McCardle, P., McCune, S., Griffin, J.A., Maholmes, V., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2011; pp. 85–99. ISBN 9781433808654. [Google Scholar]
- Walker-Meikle, K. Medieval Pets, 1st ed.; Boydell & Brewer: Woodbridge, UK, 2012; 179p, ISBN -13 9781843837589. [Google Scholar]
- Cook, P.F.; Spivak, M.; Berns, G.S. One pair of hands is not like another: Caudate BOLD response in dogs depends on signal source and canine temperament. Peer J. 2014, 2, e596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Horn, L.; Huber, L.; Range, F. The Importance of the Secure Base Effect for Domestic Dogs—Evidence from a Manipulative Problem-Solving Task. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e65296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mariti, C.; Ricci, E.; Zilocchi, M.; Gazzano, A. Owners as a secure base for their dogs. Behaviour 2013, 150, 1275–1294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGuire, B.; Song, A. Influence of Sex of Stranger on Responses of Shelter Dogs during Canine Behavioral Evaluations. Animals 2023, 13, 2461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Völter, C.J.; Starić, D.; Huber, L. Using machine learning to track dogs’ exploratory behaviour in the presence and absence of their caregiver. Anim. Behav. 2023, 197, 97–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zilcha-Mano, S.; Mikulincer, M.; Shaver, P.R. An attachment perspective on human–pet relationships: Conceptualization and assessment of pet attachment orientations. J. Res. Personal. 2011, 45, 345–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Völter, C.J.; Lonardo, L.; Steinmann, M.; Ramos, C.F.; Gerwisch, K.; Schranz, M.T.; Dobernig, I.; Huber, L. Unwilling or unable? Using three-dimensional tracking to evaluate dogs’ reactions to differing human intentions. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2023, 290, 20221621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gouvernement du Canada. Available online: https://agriculture.canada.ca/fr/commerce-international/renseignements-marches/rapports/analyse-tendances-du-marche-tendances-aliments-animaux-compagnie-au-canada (accessed on 29 April 2025).
- Fédération des Fabricants d’Aliments Pour Chiens, Chats, Oiseaux et Autres Animaux Familiers. Available online: https://www.facco.fr/chiffres-cles/les-chiffres-de-la-population-animale/ (accessed on 29 April 2025).
- American Veterinary Medical Association. Available online: https://www.avma.org/resources-tools/reports-statistics/us-pet-ownership-statistics?utm_source=chatgpt.com (accessed on 29 April 2025).
- Hess-Holden, C.L.; Monaghan, C.L.; Justice, C.A. Pet Bereavement Support Groups: A Guide for Mental Health Professionals. J. Creat. Ment. Health 2017, 12, 440–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Serpell, J.A. Pet-Keeping in Non-Western Societies: Some Popular Misconceptions. Anthrozoös 1987, 1, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, E. Constructing Canine Consent: Conceptualising and Adopting a Consent-Focused Relationship with Dogs, 1st ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2024; 120p, ISBN -13 9781032421599. [Google Scholar]
- Westgarth, C.; Christley, R.M.; Marvin, G.; Perkins, E. Functional and recreational dog walking practices in the UK. Health Promot. Int. 2021, 36, 109–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Westgarth, C.; Christley, R.M.; Jewell, C.; German, A.J.; Boddy, L.M.; Christian, H.E. Dog owners are more likely to meet physical activity guidelines than people without a dog: An investigation of the association between dog ownership and physical activity levels in a UK community. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 5704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diverio, S.; Menchetti, L.; Riggio, G.; Azzari, C.; Iaboni, M.; Zasso, R.; Di Mari, W.; Santoro, M.M. Dogs’ coping styles and dog-handler relationships influence avalanche search team performance. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 191, 67–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Comber, C.A.; Dayer, A.A. Understanding attitudes and norms of dog walkers to reduce disturbances to shorebirds. Hum. Dimens. Wildl. 2022, 27, 236–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merkouri, A.; Graham, T.M.; O’Haire, M.E.; Purewal, R.; Westgarth, C. Dogs and the Good Life: A Cross-Sectional Study of the Association Between the Dog-Owner Relationship and Owner Mental Wellbeing. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 903647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Townsend, L.; Gee, N.R. Recognizing and Mitigating Canine Stress during Animal Assisted Interventions. Vet. Sci. 2021, 8, 254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bensky, M.K.; Gosling, S.D.; Sinn, D.L. The World from a Dog’s Point of View: A Review and Synthesis of Dog Cognition Research. In Advances in the Study of Behavior; Brockmann, H.J., Roper, T.J., Naguib, M., Mitani, J.C., Simmons, L.W., Barrett, L., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2013; Volume 45, pp. 209–406. ISSN 00653454. [Google Scholar]
- Ducrot, C.; Fortané, N.; Paul, M. Approches Interdisciplinaires en Santé Animale: Dialogue Entre Sciences Sociales et Vétérinaires, 1st ed.; Éditions Quae: Versailles, France, 2024; 273p, ISBN 9782759239719. [Google Scholar]
- Herron, M.E.; Kirby-Madden, T.M.; Lord, L.K. Effects of environmental enrichment on the behavior of shelter dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2014, 244, 687–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siwak-Tapp, C.T.; Head, E.; Muggenburg, B.A.; Milgram, N.W.; Cotman, C.W. Region specific neuron loss in the aged canine hippocampus is reduced by enrichment. Neurobiol. Aging 2008, 29, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merola, I.; Prato-Previde, E.; Lazzaroni, M.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Dogs’ comprehension of referential emotional expressions: Familiar people and familiar emotions are easier. Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 373–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merola, I.; Prato-Previde, E.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Dogs’ Social Referencing towards Owners and Strangers. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e47653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merola, I.; Prato-Previde, E.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Social referencing in dog-owner dyads? Anim. Cogn. 2012, 15, 175–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowes, M.; Keller, P.; Rollins, R.; Gifford, R. The Effect of Ambivalence on On-Leash Dog Walking Compliance Behavior in Parks and Protected Areas. J. Park Recreat. Adm. 2017, 35, 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berns, G. Decoding the Canine Mind. Cerebrum 2020, 2020, cer-04-20. [Google Scholar] [PubMed Central]
- Teo, J.T.; Johnstone, S.J.; Römer, S.S.; Thomas, S.J. Psychophysiological mechanisms underlying the potential health benefits of human-dog interactions: A systematic literature review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2022, 180, 27–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Skierbiszewska, K.; Borowska, M.; Bonecka, J.; Turek, B.; Jasiński, T.; Domino, M. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Research on Dog Cognition: A Systematic Review. Appl. Sci. 2024, 14, 12028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, B.P.; Browne, M.; Mack, J.; Kontou, T.G. An Exploratory Study of Human–Dog Co-sleeping Using Actigraphy: Do Dogs Disrupt Their Owner’s Sleep? Anthrozoös 2018, 31, 727–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schöberl, I.; Beetz, A.; Solomon, J.; Wedl, M.; Gee, N.; Kotrschal, K. Social factors influencing cortisol modulation in dogs during a strange situation procedure. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 11, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosano, J.; Howell, T.; Conduit, R.; Bennett, P. Co-Sleeping between Adolescents and Their Pets May Not Impact Sleep Quality. Clocks Sleep 2021, 3, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Riggio, G.; Noom, M.; Gazzano, A.; Mariti, C. Development of the Dog Attachment Insecurity Screening Inventory (D-AISI): A Pilot Study on a Sample of Female Owners. Animals 2021, 11, 3381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patel, S.I.; Miller, B.W.; Kosiorek, H.E.; Parish, J.M.; Lyng, P.J.; Krahn, L.E. The Effect of Dogs on Human Sleep in the Home Sleep Environment. Mayo Clin. Proc. 2017, 92, 1368–1372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lenkei, R.; Alvarez Gomez, S.; Pongrácz, P. Fear vs. frustration—Possible factors behind canine separation related behaviour. Behav. Process. 2018, 157, 115–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koskela, A.; Törnqvist, H.; Somppi, S.; Tiira, K.; Kykyri, V.-L.; Hänninen, L.; Kujala, J.; Nagasawa, M.; Kikusui, T.; Kujala, M.V. Behavioral and emotional co-modulation during dog–owner interaction measured by heart rate variability and activity. Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 25201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hare, E.; Essler, J.L.; Otto, C.M.; Ebbecke, D.; Serpell, J.A. Development of a modified C-BARQ for evaluating behavior in working dogs. Front. Vet. Sci. 2024, 11, 1371630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Broseghini, A.; Guérineau, C.; Lõoke, M.; Mariti, C.; Serpell, J.; Marinelli, L.; Mongillo, P. Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ): Validation of the Italian Translation. Animals 2023, 13, 1254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Somppi, S.; Törnqvist, H.; Koskela, A.; Vehkaoja, A.; Tiira, K.; Väätäjä, H.; Surakka, V.; Vainio, O.; Kujala, M.V. Dog-Owner Relationship, Owner Interpretations and Dog Personality Are Connected with the Emotional Reactivity of Dogs. Animals 2022, 12, 1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hoffman, C.L.; Browne, M.; Smith, B.P. Human-Animal Co-Sleeping: An Actigraphy-Based Assessment of Dogs’ Impacts on Women’s Nighttime Movements. Animals 2020, 10, 278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vieira de Castro, A.C.; Araújo, Â.; Fonseca, A.; Olsson, I.A.S. Improving dog training methods: Efficacy and efficiency of reward and mixed training methods. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0247321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kulgod, A.; van der Linden, D.; França, L.G.S.; Jackson, M.; Zamansky, A. Non-invasive canine electroencephalography (EEG): A systematic review. BMC Vet. Res. 2025, 21, 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gazzaniga, M.S.; Ivry, R.B.; Mangun, G.R. Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 5th ed.; W.W. Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 2019; 655p, ISBN -13 9780393693170. [Google Scholar]
- Detweiler, D.K. The Dog Electrocardiogram: A Critical Review. In Comprehensive Electrocardiology; Macfarlane, P.W., van Oosterom, A., Pahlm, O., Kligfield, P., Janse, M., Camm, J., Eds.; Springer: London, UK, 2010; pp. 1861–1908. ISBN 9781848820456. [Google Scholar]
- Wright, H.; Mills, D.; Pollux, P. Development and Validation of a Psychometric Tool for Assessing Impulsivity in the Domestic Dog (Canis familiaris). Int. J. Comp. Psych. 2011, 24, 210–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, H.F.; Mills, D.S.; Pollux, P.M.J. Behavioural and physiological correlates of impulsivity in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Physiol. Behav. 2012, 105, 676–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowe, H.; Jarrin, D.C.; Noel, N.A.O.; Ramil, J.; McGrath, J.J. The curious incident of the dog in the nighttime: The effects of pet-human co-sleeping and bedsharing on sleep dimensions of children and adolescents. Sleep Health 2021, 7, 324–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ortmeyer, H.K. Effects of Proximity between Companion Dogs and Their Caregivers on Heart Rate Variability Measures in Older Adults: A Pilot Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 17, 2674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, T.P.; Garrity, T.F.; Stallones, L. Psychometric Evaluation of the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (Laps). Anthrozoös 1992, 5, 160–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amat, M.; Le Brech, S.; Camps, T.; Manteca, X. Separation-Related Problems in Dogs: A Critical Review. Adv. Small Anim. Care 2020, 1, 19207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cobb, M.L.; Iskandarani, K.; Chinchilli, V.M.; Dreschel, N.A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of salivary cortisol measurement in domestic canines. Domest. Anim. Endocrinol. 2016, 57, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gandenberger, J.; Flynn, E.; Moratto, E.; Wendt, A.; Morris, K.N. Molecular Biomarkers of Adult Human and Dog Stress during Canine-Assisted Interventions: A Systematic Scoping Review. Animals 2022, 12, 651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vasconcellos Ada, S.; Virányi, Z.; Range, F.; Ades, C.; Scheidegger, J.K.; Möstl, E.; Kotrschal, K. Training Reduces Stress in Human-Socialised Wolves to the Same Degree as in Dogs. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0162389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jean-Joseph, H.; Kortekaas, K.; Range, F.; Kotrschal, K. Context-Specific Arousal During Resting in Wolves and Dogs: Effects of Domestication? Front. Psychol 2020, 11, 568199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Main Theme | Path Used |
---|---|
Co-construction | (co-construction OR self-determination OR motivation OR partner OR coopera*1) AND (interspeci* OR animal OR dog OR canine) |
Competence & education | (education OR training OR behav* OR cognit* OR competence OR communication OR reinforcement OR avers* OR interaction) AND (dog OR canine) AND (owner OR human OR interspeci*) |
Relatedness (attachment) | (attachment OR bond OR relationship OR affiliation OR synchronization OR referenc* OR secur*) AND (dog OR canine) AND (owner OR human OR interspeci*) |
Autonomy | (autonomy OR consent OR liberty OR desire OR activit* OR environment* OR norm* OR regulation) AND (dog OR canine) AND (owner OR human OR interspeci*) |
Researching and measuring co-construction | (co-construction OR self-determination OR motivation OR partner OR coopera* OR education OR training OR reinforcement OR avers* OR interaction OR autonomy OR consent OR liberty OR desire OR activit* OR environment* OR norm* OR regulation OR attachment OR bond OR relationship OR affiliation OR synchronization OR referenc* OR secur*) AND (dog OR canine) AND (owner OR human OR interspeci*) AND (tool OR scale OR metric OR measure OR assessment OR observation OR marker OR physi* OR neuro* OR actigraphy OR perform* OR method* OR brain OR behav* OR cognit* OR competence OR communication) |
Main Theme | Number of References Drawn from Databases with Intra-Database Duplicates (n =) | Number of References Drawn from Databases Without Intra-Database Duplicates (n =) | Number of Cited References (n =) 2 |
---|---|---|---|
Co-construction | n = 19,613 | n = 14,184 | References found from the script: Original research articles: n = 3 [3,4,5] Others: n = 3 [10,13,14] Additional references: Books: n = 3 [1,7,12] Others: n = 3 [2,6,11] |
Competence & education | n = 12,252 | n = 8766 | References found from the script: Systematic review: n = 1 [17] Original research articles: n = 12 [18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29] Others: n = 6 [2,6,30,31,32,33] Additional references: Books: n = 3 [34,35,36] Original research articles: n = 7 [37,38,39,40,41,42,43] |
Relatedness (attachment) | n = 7041 | n = 5129 | References found from the script: Systematic reviews: n = 2 [44,45] Original research articles: n = 22 [20,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66] Others: n = 2 [2,67] Additional references: Systematic reviews or systematic narrative synthesis: n = 2 [68,69] Books: n = 6 [70,71,72,73,74,75] Original research articles: n = 7 [76,77,78,79,80,81,82] Others: n = 5 [83,84,85,86,87] |
Autonomy | n = 14,534 | n = 10,102 | References found from the script: Book: n = 1 [88] Original research articles: n = 5 [89,90,91,92,93] Others: n = 3 [11,13,94] Additional references: Books: n = 4 [12,36,95,96] Original research articles: n = 6 [97,98,99,100,101,102] Others: n = 3 [14,33,103] |
Researching and measuring co-construction | n = 16,199 | n = 11,749 | References found from the script: Systematic reviews, systematic literature reviews or meta-analysis: n = 2 [104,105] Original research articles: n = 17 [26,66,89,90,91,93,106,107,108,109,110,111,112,113,114,115,116] Other: n = 1 [117] Additional references: Systematic review: n = 1 [118] Books: n = 2 [119,120] Original research articles: n = 6 [81,121,122,123,124,125] Other: n = 1 [126] |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Martin, L.; Otis, C.; Lussier, B.; Troncy, E. A Conceptual Framework for the Co-Construction of Human–Dog Dyadic Relationship. Animals 2025, 15, 2875. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192875
Martin L, Otis C, Lussier B, Troncy E. A Conceptual Framework for the Co-Construction of Human–Dog Dyadic Relationship. Animals. 2025; 15(19):2875. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192875
Chicago/Turabian StyleMartin, Laurie, Colombe Otis, Bertrand Lussier, and Eric Troncy. 2025. "A Conceptual Framework for the Co-Construction of Human–Dog Dyadic Relationship" Animals 15, no. 19: 2875. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192875
APA StyleMartin, L., Otis, C., Lussier, B., & Troncy, E. (2025). A Conceptual Framework for the Co-Construction of Human–Dog Dyadic Relationship. Animals, 15(19), 2875. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192875