Next Article in Journal
Production of Cloned Bighorn Sheep Embryos Using ISCNT via HMC from Domestic Sheep Oocytes Treated with Resveratrol During IVM
Previous Article in Journal
Dog Guardian Interpretation of Familiar Dog Aggression Questions in the C-BARQ: Do We Need to Redefine “Familiar”?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

A Conceptual Framework for the Co-Construction of Human–Dog Dyadic Relationship

GREPAQ (Groupe de Recherche en Pharmacologie Animale du Québec), Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Université de Montréal, Saint-Hyacinthe, QC J2S 2M2, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2025, 15(19), 2875; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192875
Submission received: 15 September 2025 / Revised: 28 September 2025 / Accepted: 29 September 2025 / Published: 30 September 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Human-Animal Interactions, Animal Behaviour and Emotion)

Simple Summary

The relationship and interactions within owner–dog dyads may be shaped by a dynamic process known as co-construction, wherein each individual influences and adapts to each other. Although this concept is mentioned in the literature, it has yet to be clearly defined or fully conceptualized. This narrative review explores how self-determination theory can enhance our understanding of this phenomenon. This review examines how both owners and dogs may fulfill their three fundamental psychological needs: competence, autonomy, and relatedness. It also explores various methodological approaches for studying co-construction, highlighting their interests and limitations. Co-construction unfolds over time and across varied contexts. The benefits are not immediate; its expression appears to be influenced by biopsychosocial, experiential, and environmental factors unique to each dyad and its individual members. While the current literature presents several proto-tools, drawing on both qualitative and quantitative methods to explore dyadic co-construction, none directly measures the process in a bidirectional way. The development of more integrative, interactive tools would allow for a deeper and more nuanced understanding of the co-construction process within owner–dog relationships.

Abstract

Dyadic co-construction, the mutual adaptation that occurs between dogs and their owners, is often discussed in terms of cooperation and participation, yet it remains poorly defined and under-conceptualized in the literature. This review proposed that self-determination theory (SDT), with its three core psychological needs—autonomy, competence, and relatedness (attachment)—offers a valuable framework for understanding this phenomenon within a dyadic context. The objectives of this review were twofold: (1) to conceptualize co-construction in owner–dog interactions through the lens of SDT, and (2) to propose methodological approaches for studying this process, while acknowledging their current limitations. Dyadic co-construction emerges as a dynamic, evolving process of mutual influence, shaped by biopsychosocial factors, individual and shared experiences, and the physical and social environments of both human and dog, as well as the dyad as a unit. Depending on the nature of the interaction, co-construction can be beneficial or detrimental. Positive training practices and secure attachment patterns in both humans and dogs tend to foster more harmonious co-construction, whereas aversive methods and insecure attachment may hinder it. Although existing methodologies offer promising insights into this process, they often lack standardization, statistical robustness, and true bidirectionality. This review underscores the need for more integrative, longitudinal, and empirically grounded approaches to fully capture the complexity and clinical relevance of owner–dog dyadic co-construction.

1. Introduction

Co-construction generally refers to processes in which individuals jointly participate in the design, implementation, and execution of actions [1]. Although frequently cited, the concept remains under-defined and lacks a widely accepted theoretical framework [1]. When defined, it is typically framed as a combination of cooperation and individual participation. If we consider interaction as a form of action, then humans are not the only species capable of co-construction. Dogs, too, can engage in cooperative and participatory interactions, both within their own species and across species boundaries. In fact, humans and dogs appear able to hybridize their intraspecific communication systems, transforming them into mutually understandable, interspecific forms [2]. While multiple studies suggest that dogs can cooperate with humans [3,4,5], these investigations often exhibit limitations, including small sample sizes [3,4], minimal sociodemographic context [3,4], and a lack of attention to individual or shared (hybrid) experiences [3,4,5]. These gaps may contribute to variability in how dyadic co-construction is expressed [6]. Despite growing interest in cooperation and participation in human–dog interactions, there remains a lack of conceptual clarity regarding the very existence and definition of dyadic co-construction. Does co-construction exist in owner–dog relationships? If so, how can it be defined, and what frameworks can help to conceptualize it?
One promising lens is self-determination theory (SDT), a theory originally developed to understand human motivation and well-being [7]. SDT posits that individuals strive to fulfill three basic psychological needs—autonomy, competence, and relatedness (attachment)—and that satisfaction of these needs supports well-being and adaptive functioning; SDT classifies motivation along a self-continuum [7], ranging from:
  • Intrinsic motivation: doing an activity for its inherent interest and enjoyment (most autonomous).
  • Extrinsic motivation: driven by external outcomes, further divided into four types, (integrated, identified, introjected and external), from most to least autonomous.
  • Amotivation: a state of disengagement and lack of intention, representing non autonomous regulation.
Autonomous forms of motivation tend to promote psychological resilience, adaptive behavior, and overall well-being [7]. These motivations emerge when individuals can satisfy the three fundamental needs:
  • Autonomy: experiencing volition and self-direction.
  • Competence: feeling effective and capable.
  • Relatedness: feeling connected to others, accepted, and supported by peers.
When these needs are unmet or (actively) frustrated by the social environment, individuals may experience maladaptive outcomes [7].
While SDT was developed for humans, it has potential applications for non-human animals. Behaviorist perspectives emphasize external stimuli in shaping behavior [8], whereas evolutionary approaches highlight environmental pressures and fundamental needs as primary behavioral drivers [9]. SDT bridges these views by framing behavior as influenced by internal motivation and basic psychological needs, elements that may be relevant for animals, especially companion species like dogs and cats, who share human environments and social structures [10,11]. However, applying SDT to animals is complex. Animals differ from humans in their access to morality, rationality, legal rights, and cognitive communication tools [10,12]. Still, recent research supports the “animalization” of SDT, especially for companion animals living in anthropogenic environments where access to autonomy, competence, and relatedness is largely mediated by humans [10,11,12,13]. In such contexts, human (ir)responsibility can either support or hinder animals’ ability to fulfill their psychological needs, thereby affecting their well-being.
Considering these reflections, it becomes essential to ask whether SDT can truly be extended to dogs, and what the implications of such an extension would be for both owners and the dyadic relationship. Despite growing interest in animal SDT, few methodological tools exist to assess psychological and motivational needs across species in a systematic, bidirectional way [12]. Most studies remain anthropocentric or interpret animal behavior through human-centered lenses, perpetuating anthropomorphism and overlooking animal agency [11,14]. Although qualitative methods, such as behavioral observations, have proven valuable in studying participation and cooperation [3,4,5], it remains unclear whether current tools can adequately assess dyadic co-construction, especially in its relational, evolving, and bidirectional nature. From these reflections, we propose two key hypotheses:
  • Dyadic relationships are co-constructed, with participation, cooperation, and SDT offering a viable framework for their conceptualization.
  • Existing measurement approaches, although not originally designed to assess dyadic SDT, may serve as “proto-tools” that can be adapted and refined to study this relational process.
The primary objective of this narrative review was to conceptualize the co-construction of owner–dog interactions through the lens of SDT. The secondary objective was to examine existing methodological approaches that could be adapted to explore this process, and to highlight their respective limitations.
For reasons of clarity and readability, the term “dog owner” was chosen, even though it is more reductive than other concepts [15]. It refers to a basic human status in relation to companion animals, without presuming the perceptions of the animal [16] or suggesting a more favorable dyadic co-construction. Other terms such as “guardian”, “carer/caregiver/caretaker”, or even “parent” could be more appropriate.
This review was not intended to be exhaustive but rather to provide a conceptual and exploratory contribution to an underdeveloped field. The same structure was favored in each determinant of SDT, animated using questions. The latter were systematically followed by elements of response and avenues for reflection, thereby linking conceptual interrogations with constructive analysis.

2. Materials and Methods

This narrative review was developed using a comprehensive search strategy across several reference databases. Specialized databases in the social and natural sciences were consulted, including PsycINFO, PubMed, and CINAHL, alongside a multidisciplinary database (Web of Science). In addition, general search engines such as Google, Google Scholar, and Cairn were used to access complementary materials, including book chapters and gray literature. This combination of sources enabled a broad and integrated exploration of the key themes addressed in this review.
A thematic framework was developed to guide the search process. Two main thematic axes were identified; each associated with specific keyword combinations (see Table 1 and Table 2):
  • The co-construction of dyadic interactions in relation to SDT;
  • Tools and frameworks used to assess the fulfillment of basic psychological needs within dyadic contexts.
Searches were conducted using these keywords across all listed databases. To refine the selection, the following filters were applied:
  • Title and abstract relevance;
  • Publication date (<10 years), ensuring the narrative review to reflect the most recent methodological and conceptual advances in a still emerging field;
  • Language (French/English), as these options cover most of the relevant scientific literature, as appreciated by the authors.
However, older references were retained when they were deemed foundational or conceptually essential.
All references were managed using EndNote™ software (version 20.6, Clarivate, PA, USA). The selection process was led by L.M., who performed an initial screening of titles and abstracts. Only references that were fully accessible, demonstrated methodological rigor and strong conceptual relevance, and aligned with the objectives of the review were retained for in-depth analysis. Given the conceptual and reflective orientation of the narrative review, conceptual and theoretical articles were prioritized alongside original research papers and systematic reviews. L.M. synthesized the literature using detailed reading notes and thematic summaries. A draft synthesis plan was created, and each section was written by L.M., then discussed and refined collaboratively with E.T. and B.L., following an iterative review process until consensus was achieved.

3. Competence and Education

3.1. Theoretical Objectives and SDT

In recent years, canine education has progressively shifted from a model centered on human dominance over dogs toward a more integrative approach. While human well-being remains a central objective, other goals—focusing on canine welfare, and even bilateral benefits—are now emerging [34]. In theory, these objectives include [35]:
  • Establishing, developing, adapting, and consolidating a foundational dyadic system of interspecific communication and interpretation;
  • Identifying and (de)valuing (in)appropriate behaviors through inter-species (de)motivational techniques.
These objectives theoretically align with the development of dyadic SDT and its actors. The aim is to co-construct an evolving dialog, grounded in human, canine, and ultimately dyadic competence; all of which are dynamic and develop over time. Although this perspective emphasizes reciprocity, it must also acknowledge the asymmetry between dyadic actors. Domestication appears to have shaped dogs in such a way that cooperative and empathic leadership from their owners constitutes an important requirement for their health and well-being [33,36]. Are these theoretical objectives achievable in practice?

3.2. What Techniques for Achieving These Objectives: Links with SDT and Dyadic Impacts?

The most widely used training approaches are based on operant conditioning [35], a method that shapes canine behavior by teaching that actions have consequences. These techniques are divided into three categories, each aimed at increasing desirable behaviors and decreasing undesirable ones (noting that what constitutes a “desirable” behavior is often anthropocentrically defined) [35]:
  • Aversive techniques, based on negative reinforcement and positive punishment. Respectively, these involve removing a stimulus following a desired behavior or adding a negative stimulus following an undesirable one (e.g., electric collars, choke chains, prong collars, physical/verbal/visual punishments).
  • Reward-based (positive) techniques, focused on positive reinforcement—where a desirable behavior is followed by a reward, and an undesirable behavior by the removal of a positive stimulus (e.g., treats, toys, praise, clicker training).
  • Mixed techniques, which combines both aversive and reward-based methods.
The choice of technique has a significant influence on both the dyad and its individual members. It directly impacts the co-construction of the dyad and the expression of SDT’s core principles. For example, compared to positive methods, aversive techniques are associated with:
  • An increase in stress-related behaviors and postures in dog [27,38];
  • A higher risk of canine aggression [19].
While the causal link between canine training methods and aggression remains unclear [31], canine aggression itself is a complex behavior influenced by the dog’s (social) environment, experiences, and individual characteristics (see [20] for more details).
Conversely, exploratory studies suggest that positive training methods are associated with:
  • Increased frequency of dogs gazing at their owners during interaction [39];
  • Greater sociability with strangers and higher activity levels during play [42];
  • Improved focus and performance during professional tasks [41] and training [42].
Joint attention, facilitated by eye contact and physical touch, plays a vital role in both human and canine cognitive processing. For example, a study using non-invasive wireless electroencephalography (EEG) measured inter-cerebral neural coupling in human–dog dyads [26]. Results showed that:
  • Neural coupling occurred in the frontal and parietal brain regions, associated with joint attention;
  • Mutual gaze and physical touch independently enhanced this coupling, with their combination having a synergistic effect;
  • This neural synchronization increased over time, plateauing by day seven.
These findings suggest that sensory cues shared between species, such as gaze, voice, and touch, are crucial for enhancing interspecific interaction quality and reciprocity. Both dogs and humans possess competencies in directing attention, interpreting social cues, and adjusting behaviors accordingly [2]. From a neural perspective, these signals, as well as their accumulation over time, appear to be important for enhancing the outcomes and quality of an interspecific interaction [26]. For example [2]:
  • Dogs can understand certain words, tones, intentions, and human emotions;
  • Humans can interpret canine vocalizations and emotional expressions.
Thus, dyadic interaction appears to be co-constructed through both neural activity and communicative competencies. However, for this process to function positively, each individual must have:
  • Sufficient autonomy to explore the other;
  • Opportunities for self-expression;
  • The ability to accumulate and interpret signals over time.
Positive training methods appear to facilitate this process by promoting communicative competence and dynamic dyadic performance. In contrast, aversive methods impose rigid behavioral expectations, restricting exploration and self-expression. For instance, dogs trained with aversive methods look at their owners less frequently, potentially missing key social cues [32,39]. This could result in:
  • The frustration of basic psychological needs, leading to extrinsic or amotivated perceptions of training, and possibly behavior problems that affect human well-being;
  • A breakdown in canine communication that increases the risk of misunderstandings or accidents.
In such cases, neither the dyad nor its members fully benefit from the co-construction of shared competence.

3.3. Intra-/Interspecific Factors Influencing This Psychological Need?

Yes! Multiple intra- and interspecific factors influence how this need for competence is fulfilled. For instance, the coherence of communication (i.e., timing of reward, human posture, and the dog’s attention) affects learning success [25]. Key factors include:
  • Human adaptation to canine signals: In a two-choice task, dogs tend to respond better to human gestures than to voice commands [18].
  • Dog size: Owners of small dogs are less consistent in training and play than owners of large dogs, which correlates with lower obedience scores [37].
  • Reward type: Food is generally more effective than petting, which itself outperforms verbal praise [29,40]. However, preferences may vary by familiarity, environment, body area touched, and individual canine personality [2,40].
Regarding canine traits, factors such as skull shape, breed, sex, age, and training level influence social perception and behavior [30,34]. For example [30,34]:
  • Dogs exposed to humans early are more likely to understand pointing gestures;
  • Older dogs may be less responsive to facial expressions;
  • Brachycephalic dogs show more attention to human faces;
  • Breed and work style influence gaze duration and task performance, although breed only accounts for a small portion of behavioral variation [23]. Behavioral expressions and predispositions are linked to a certain genetic selection but also to environment, utility, training, and experience.
Among humans, gender and personality also seem to influence training preferences. One study found that female trainers were more likely to use non-aversive methods and be certified [21]. No recent study has established a link between the choice of training method and the gender of the owner. Personality and mental health of the owner play a role as well. Owners high in agreeableness were less likely to use physical or verbal corrections [24]. Neuroticism in owner was associated with dog aggression [20]. A study based on n = 1564 dyads, very weak evidence linked owner personality and dog behavior, mediated using aversive training methods [19]. But moderate depression in male owners correlated with a fivefold increase in aversive method use [19]. Some studies also suggest that the owner’s experience with dog is important in canine education [22,43]. Other factors include owner experience and cognitive abilities. For example, success in canine certification correlated with high owner cognitive skills, training frequency, and low levels of perceived canine disobedience [28]. In the context of canine education, the cognitive abilities of owners are not necessarily optimized. In fact, a systematic review (n = 29) explains that canine education is often based on oral instructions and advice [17]. This method seems moderately effective in teaching owners the necessary competence to train their dogs. To remain effective, it should last between 3 and 6 months, which is not always the case [17].
However, many of these studies are based on small, non-representative samples and diverse methodologies, limiting generalizability. While they were not designed with SDT in mind, they collectively highlight the need to account for individual and collective variability in dyadic interactions [6], a topic relevant to the next sections.

3.4. Practical Feasibility of These Theoretical Objectives?

These objectives are not always achievable in practice. Their success depends on the choice of training techniques, and biopsychosocial, experiential, and environmental variables specific to each dyad. These elements are interdependent, making causality difficult to establish. To better understand this complexity, longitudinal studies are needed.
Despite these challenges, dyadic co-construction consistently occurs, though some combinations of factors lead to more successful outcomes. Effective dyads enable exploration, error-making, and iterative improvement. They build upon individual strengths and motivations, which merge into shared dyadic motivation, reinforcing competence.
Key takeaways:
  • Theoretical vs. practical gaps: While theoretically sound, these objectives are not always fully achievable in real-life contexts.
  • Impact of educational techniques: The choice of an educational technique influences the fluidity and stability of dyadic co-construction.
  • Effectiveness of positive methods: Reward-based methods tend to facilitate better fulfillment of fundamental psychological needs than aversive methods.
  • Need for further research: Future studies should explore the interplay of biopsychosocial, experiential, and environmental factors in shaping dyadic competence over time.

4. Attachment

4.1. Can We Talk About Bilateral Relatedness Between Owners and Dogs?

In Western countries, dogs play an essential role in households. In 2024, the estimated number of dogs was 8.3 million in Canada [83], 9.9 million in France [84], and 89.7 million in the United States [85]. That same year, dogs ranked second to cats in pet ownership in Canada and France (8.9 million and 16.6 million, respectively) [83,84], while in the United States, dogs appeared to be the preferred companion animal, outnumbering cats (73.8 million) [85].
Across time, cultures, and societies, both (non-)Western, the role of companion animals—particularly dogs—has evolved and diversified across different functions [87]. Taking the example of Western European societies, it appears that with the rise in living standards, keeping a companion animal at home became increasingly popular [70,75,87]:
  • In the Middle Ages and early modern times, moralists, especially within the discourse of the Catholic Church, condemned the practice of keeping animals solely for companionship. However, in wealthier households and religious institutions, their presence was already considered normal in practice and everyday life, and their role could also include that of companionship.
  • In the XVIth and XVIIth centuries, European aristocratic households kept a variety of species they considered companion animals, including dogs and exotic animals.
  • In the XVIIIth century, the rise of the urban middle class contributed to the spread of companion animal ownership to broader segments of Western society. Factors behind this trend included the bourgeoisie’s desire to emulate the aristocracy and increased rural-to-urban migration.
  • During the Victorian era, companion animal keeping became a family activity for the middle class. Companion animals were seen as having both social and educational benefits, especially for children.
Since the XIXth century, companion animals have come to symbolize a mutual relationship [67], and many now consider them fully fledged family members [86]. From a SDT perspective, this evolution is particularly interesting. To fully understand the emergence, development, and current state of dyadic relationships, we must consider not only present-day interactions or recent life events but also cultural and historical dimensions. In this sense, traces of dyadic co-construction can likely be found as far back as the domestication of dogs.
From a theoretical standpoint, human attachment to companion animals is grounded in the same principles that govern infant–caregiver attachment [53,56,81]. Attachment theory [71] explains that are biologically predisposed to form close bonds with their caregivers, bond that can vary in quality and significantly affect development. In adults [57,68]:
  • A high level of attachment anxiety refers to individuals who worry about the reliability or availability of those they are attached to.
  • A high level of attachment avoidance refers to individuals who distrust or distance themselves from their attachment figures.
  • Secure attachment, in contrast to these insecure styles, is characterized by confidence in the reliability and responsiveness of others, which promotes well-being.
Beyond attachment, the biophilia hypothesis suggests that humans are genetically and evolutionarily programmed to be connected with nature and animals [72,73]. The love of animals seems almost innate: It has been demonstrated that human infants (4 to 12 months old) show greater visual attention and emotional engagement toward various animals than toward objects [74].
Do dogs form attachments to their owners? Several lines of research suggest they do. In separation–reunion tests, dogs show behaviors indicative of attachment [58], including:
  • seeking proximity to their owners;
  • displaying varying levels of distress during separation;
  • actively initiating contact upon reunion.
These behavioral observations are supported by neurological evidence. Functional MRI (fMRI) studies show that brain regions such as the caudate nucleus, hippocampus, and amygdala are activated when dogs view familiar, emotionally salient human faces [65]. These regions are associated with reward processing, memory, and emotion. Activation of the caudate nucleus, in particular, may reflect a learned association with human-mediated rewards [65] or general canine motivation [76]. Furthermore, dogs appear to develop attachment styles like those seen in human infants toward their caregivers [62,66]. This suggests that dyadic attachment involves not only human feelings and perceptions but also the canine brain, body, and emotions. Is attachment within a dyad co-constructed by both the human and the dog? (see below Section 4.4)

4.2. What Are the Impacts of This Fundamental Psychological Need on Dyads and Their Participants in Relation to SDT?

On the canine side, research, especially using the strange situation procedure (SSP), offers valuable insights. Compared to insecure attachment, secure attachment in dogs appears to have numerous benefits for both the dog and the dyad:
  • Dogs with secure attachment increase exploratory behavior and proximity to humans, whether the human is a familiar owner or a stranger [61,66]. The effect of the owner, however, has a stronger effect [66].
  • They play more with toys when the owner is present, but not with a stranger [46,77,78].
  • They show reduced asymmetry in rightward tail wagging in the presence of a stranger when their owner is near [80].
The tail is a complex communication tool in dogs. Its asymmetry, position, amplitude, and wagging speed convey emotional states [2]. From a neurological perspective, tail wagging is influenced by cerebral lateralization. Positive stimuli, like seeing the owner, activate the left hemisphere, producing rightward wagging. Negative or withdrawal-related stimuli activate the right hemisphere, leading to leftward wagging [59,82].
Does the owner’s attachment style affect canine behavior? Yes. In a study involving 26 dogs with a history of aggression (13 toward humans and 13 toward other animals), Gobbo and Zupan found [20]:
  • Dogs aggressive toward strangers tended to have owners with lower levels of anxious attachment;
  • Dogs aggressive toward their owners had owners with higher avoidant attachment and lower conscientiousness.
From an SDT perspective, the first finding is intriguing—one might expect the opposite. Could this be due to owners being overconfident in their dog, leading to reduced vigilance and allowing aggressive behaviors to go unchecked? The second result is more intuitive: owners with strong avoidant attachment may struggle to meet their dog’s emotional needs, undermining the dog’s ability to see the owner a secure base [20], which can in turn lead to aggression. These findings support the idea of a co-construction process between human and canine attachment styles.
On the human side, a systematic review (n = 40) examining the link between attachment to companion animals and depression found [68]:
  • most studies reported a positive or neutral correlation with depression;
  • secure attachment was negatively associated with depression;
  • insecure attachment (anxious or avoidant) was positively associated with depression;
  • Still, causality remains hard to establish [68].
What happens when we combine human and canine attachment data? A systematic review (n = 6 studies) on attachment found that [44]:
  • Owners with secure attachment often had dogs with secure attachment.
  • Owners with avoidant attachment tended to substitute human relationships with canine ones, and their dogs also displayed avoidant or anxious styles.
  • Owners with anxious attachment preferred turning to their dog during stress, and their dogs often developed secure attachment.
In sum, insecure attachment, whether human or canine, can be detrimental to dyads and their fulfillment of SDT. Secure attachment, by contrast, fosters exploration, interaction, and psychological well-being. Owners with avoidant attachment appear to pose the greatest risk for insecure canine attachment and dysfunctional dyads [44]. In such cases, dogs may struggle to find in their owner a reliable attachment figure to refer to confidently [20,58].
What about grief? Most pet owners experience significant grief after a companion animal’s death. This sadness is unique to each individual and may be associated with anxiety, stress, shame, or trauma [69]. A narrative synthesis (n = 36) showed that maintaining emotional bond with deceased companion animal can intensify grief [69].
Dogs also appear to experience grief. In triads (an owner and two dogs) when one dog dies, the surviving dog exhibit fear or behavioral changes [63]. Predictors of this included [63]:
  • The quality of the bond between the dogs (friendly or parental; shared resources);
  • The emotional state of the grieving owner.
Interestingly, the length of the relationship did not influence these behaviors (in the surviving dog), only its intensity. This suggests that dyadic attachment persists beyond physical presence, requiring eventual deconstruction and emotional acceptance by both human and dog.

4.3. Is This Psychological Need Influenced by Intra-/Interspecific Factors?

Attachment strength is influenced by many factors [44]. On the dyadic level, interpersonal dyadic complementarity and relationship length are key [44,47,48,49,50,51,64]. The quality and content of shared activities also matter. Gender and familiarity may also shape interactions. In one SSP study, shelter dogs showed more concern toward unfamiliar men than women, possibly due to prior socialization patterns [79]. Similarly, another study found:
  • Male shelter dogs accompanied by unfamiliar men were more likely to adopt juvenile urinary postures and urinate less;
  • When walked by unfamiliar woman, dogs defecated less [55];
  • Female dogs showed similar patterns for defecation behaviors [55].
On the canine side, variables like age, breed, and dyadic history affect attachment [47,48,49,50,51,64]. On the human side, attachment strength is positively associated with being a woman [45,52,54,57]; having an agreeable personality; not having children; being the sole caregiver, positive interaction history and companion animal perception [44].

4.4. Is Attachment Within a Dyad Co-Constructed by Both the Human and the Dog?

Probably. However, most existing studies focused more on human attachment to dogs than the reverse [60]. Research is also limited in cultural, social, and environmental diversity, and often suffers from small samples and a lack of breed variety [47,48,49,50]. Despite these limitations, findings on secure attachment hint at benefits for both species’ psychological needs, including autonomy, relatedness, and competence. However, this connection has not yet been explicitly studied. Future research should explore:
  • Dog’s attachment to humans in more depth;
  • Influences of biopsychosocial factors, lived experiences, and environmental contexts;
  • Longitudinal effects on dyadic development.
Key takeaways:
  • Bilateral attachment: Attachment should no longer be viewed as a one-sided human experience, it is co-constructed.
  • Attachment style and co-construction: Secure attachment fosters more positive dyadic co-construction than insecure styles.
  • The example of grief: In the face of difficult events, humans and/or dogs seem to enter processes of deconstruction of the previously experienced co-construction and of emotional acceptance.
  • Need for further research: Future studies must deepen the understanding of canine attachment as well as the various factors that shape dyads across time and space. Beyond the experience of grief, when faced with life’s challenges, are dyads capable of undergoing deco-construction and then reco-construction?

5. Autonomy

5.1. Theoretical Objectives and SDT

Just as with the literature on attachment, that which addresses dyadic autonomy is incomplete (Table 2). As defined within the framework of SDT, autonomy has only recently attracted scholarly interest [13]. Available data suggest that autonomy should aim to:
  • Allow dogs to satisfy both their psychological and physiological needs while balancing the needs of both human and dog [89,93].
  • Respect canine choices and (dis)satisfactions [12,88].
  • Promote a safe environment for all members of the dyad [12].
These are theoretical objectives. They mark a departure from cartesian perspectives and human exceptionalism, promoting instead deeper recognition of canine identity [88]. Dogs are no longer regarded solely as individuals, but as beings whose identity must be respected for their well-being. Moreover, humans tend to perceive them as “persons” and to seek to understand them through anthropomorphism [33,36]. Although this interpretative approach is often criticized, Kotrschal proposes the idea of an enlightened anthropomorphism, supported by measured empathy [33]. Such an approach could foster a more harmonious co-construction, helping humans to respond more appropriately to their dog’s needs while allowing the latter to express themselves as an active social partner.
Thus, at the heart of this perspective lies the co-constructed balance between canine, human, and dyadic autonomy. To what extent are the theoretical objectives of this psychological need practically achievable?

5.2. What Techniques for Achieving These Objectives?

5.2.1. Relationship with the Physical and Social Environment: SDT-Relevant Influences (Impacts and Intra/Interspecific Factors)

Canine autonomy is closely tied to the quality (understood in canine terms) of the artificial environment in which dogs live. “Caninizing” the human environment allows dogs to engage in natural behaviors. Enrichment benefits not only dogs but also humans and their shared relationship. It can help regulate (un)desirable behaviors [97] and potentially support healthy aging [98]. According to SDT, and as discussed by Jones [88], the physical and social environment shapes the choices available to a dog. A highly enriched space supports autonomy by offering genuine opportunities for choice and self-expression. Conversely, overly restrictive or under-stimulating environments limit autonomy and can contribute to frustration or stress. The number, nature, and perceived value of choices matter. For instance, if a dog is offered only two options that hold little intrinsic interest, its experience of autonomy is likely to be diminished compared to having five options, including two that align with its preferences. As with the elephants studied by Bell Rizzolo and Bradshaw [14], dogs require a human partner who is genuinely invested in meeting their needs. Co-construction can be observed through:
  • Analysis of the physical and social environment provided to the dog.
  • The extent to which the dog appropriates this environment.
  • The owners’ understanding of and responsiveness to their dog’s preferences.
Dyadic performance can also reflect autonomy. In an observational study of 12 military avalanche search teams, dogs who performed best in simulated trials appeared more autonomous and less handler-dependent than those who failed [91]. Successful dogs spent:
  • More time sniffing/exploring (p < 0.001) and digging intensively (p < 0.05).
  • Less time seeking proximity or contact with their handler.
Handlers’ attentiveness also improved canine performance [91]. It is possible that these effects are less noticeable in dyads composed of ordinary owners and companion animals. However, companion dogs appear to regulate their behavior based on their owners’ emotional cues. They approach or move away from an object based on the positive or negative emotional message received from their owner, but do not react the same way to strangers’ message [99,100,101]. Autonomy, therefore, is not equivalent to independence or detachment.
These findings suggest that canine autonomy is co-constructed through individual characteristics, education, relational strength, and dyadic communication. Dogs that appear more autonomous may have internalized the actions they are performing, indicating intrinsic motivation and a sense of control over their body and environment. They may feel more comfortable exploring and successfully finding a solution to the given problem [88].

5.2.2. Negotiating Canine Consent: SDT-Relevant Influences

Dogs are sentient, emotional beings capable of reasoning [95,103]. This capacity implies a form of consent [11], whereby dogs can choose to participate, cooperate, or express their (dis)likes [88]. For this consent to be valid, human partners must perceive and respect it [88]. Can canine consent improve the course of a dyadic interaction? Does it seem to contribute to its co-construction? Cooperative care is a valuable model here [88]. Its goal is to allow dogs to exert control and actively participate in their care. For this, the dyad must develop a shared language that supports both the expression and interpretation of canine (non)consent. Through routine, redundancy, and mutual anticipation, both dog and human can better navigate interactions. From the SDT perspective, this exemplifies co-constructed autonomy in care contexts. Since each party understands what to expect, engagement, cooperation, and consent become easier. However, this dynamic can be undermined by relational quality, trust, comfort, and mutual knowledge [88]. The owner’s knowledge and willingness are also important [88]. Many humans struggle to correctly detect or interpret canine signals [11,94]; others take the excuse of canine autonomy to not assume their leadership, therefore dogs could take over and start controlling (as children) their parent(s) to gain security; and some consciously ignore them [11]. Environmental and contextual factors, including interaction duration and content, also influence a dog’s stress, and by extension, its autonomy [94].

5.2.3. Perceptions of Human Autonomy in a Dyadic Context: SDT-Relevant Influences

A dog’s illness or behavioral problems may reduce both canine autonomy and human autonomy, limiting owners’ ability to pursue daily activities, interests, or relationships [89,93]. A study of 1693 dyads found that many owners perceived meeting their dogs’ needs as burdensome [93], which impinges on their own autonomy. Since dogs depend entirely on humans, the latter bear full responsibility for fulfilling their needs [90].
The autonomy of human actors also depends on dyadic activities. For instance, recreational walks are perceived positively, while functional walks often feel like obligations, associated with guilt or duty rather than pleasure [89,93]. Regulations can also restrict autonomy. For example, permanent leash use is imposed in some jurisdictions, which may frustrate both humans and dogs [92,102]. Co-construction thus extends beyond the dyad. Public policies, legal frameworks, and various stakeholders also play roles while altering the predictability of dogs relationship with their parents (see [96]).

5.3. Practical Feasibility of These Theoretical Objectives?

In reality, dogs rarely experience full autonomy. What they experience might better be described as “artificial” or “perceived” autonomy [88]. The goal, therefore, is not independence but interdependence, an optimal, co-constructed balance over time and space [13]. Within this dynamic, both human and canine partners should ideally experience mutually respected freedom of action and expression. However, not all dyads achieve, or are even capable of achieving, such balance. As with education or competencies, dyadic autonomy depends on a complex interplay of biological, psychological, social, and environmental factors.
Key takeaways:
  • Theoretical vs. practical tension: Despite its idealistic aims, the current literature remains limited.
  • Dyadic interdependence: Dogs do not possess absolute autonomy. Instead, autonomy emerges through co-constructed interdependence with humans.
  • Research gaps: Further studies are required to explore how human, canine, and dyadic actors relate to these theoretical goals. Future work should consider biopsychosocial factors, life experiences, and physical/social environments across timeframes.

6. Researching and Measuring Co-Construction

6.1. Engaging, Observing, Questioning

The preceding sections indicate that dyadic co-construction can be approached with both qualitative and quantitative methodologies. Each family of methods has distinct strengths and limitations.
Interviews and open-ended questionnaires (commonly used to study human autonomy in dyads) are particularly powerful for accessing owners’ lived experience and contextualizing behaviors [89,90,93]. Their main strength lies in depth: they allow deconstruction of practices, meanings, and intentions. Their principal weakness is that they provide limited quantification, preventing the assignment of confidence weights, generalization, or systematic comparison across large samples.
Direct observation and standardized behavioral tests yield precise, controlled, and comparatively objective data [91,97]. Examples include task-based protocols [91,117] and adapted versions of SSP to assess attachment [66,107]. These methods excel at detailed, behavior-level analysis but are resource-intensive: they require trained observers, can be costly and time-consuming, and typically cannot be scaled easily to large, representative samples. They also often remove dyads from their natural context and are susceptible to reactivity and situational biases [114].
Taken together, these approaches underscore a key issue: studying dyadic SDT without situating findings within the actors’ individual and shared experiences risks producing fragmentary or non-comparable results. Many studies provide sparse sample descriptions and use heterogeneous protocols, which complicates synthesis and weakens external validity. Under such conditions, co-construction can only be explored as a snapshot rather than as an evolving, context-dependent process.

6.2. Exploration of the Nervous System

Neurophysiological tools (EEG, fMRI) and autonomic measures (ECG/heart-rate variability) offer complementary, objective windows into neural and physiological correlates of interaction, learning, emotion, and attachment [119]. fMRI captures hemodynamic responses reflective of regional brain activity; EEG indexes electrical activity with high temporal resolution; ECG-derived heart-rate variability (HRV) indexes autonomic balance and stress responsivity.
Recent systematic reviews summarize the scope of canine neurophysiological research: EEG studies (n = 22) have examined sleep, learning, sensory perception, language processing, social cognition, and emotions [118], while fMRI studies (n = 46) have mapped canine brain responses to resting-state and multi-sensory stimuli [105]. Studies integrating human and canine measures (e.g., simultaneous recordings) highlight reciprocal neural coupling during interaction and are promising for studying co-construction [26]. They enable the exploration, from a neurological perspective, of the well-being expression [104,124] and/or attachment [112,115] that occurs between a human and a dog. However, several methodological limitations constrain interpretation and generalizability: heterogeneous and often small samples with occasional dataset overlap [112,115]; variability in EEG electrode types, placement, and preprocessing [118]; sensitivity of HRV to context and individual physiology [104]; electrical signal baseline drifts, and electrical interferences; movement and muscle artifacts in awake recordings [120]; and inconsistent handling of anesthesia effects in fMRI/EEG studies (some studies include anesthetized subjects, others do not) [105]. Training dogs to tolerate equipment greatly improves data quality, but training protocols are inconsistent and underreported [105].
Notably, co-construction may extend into unconscious or off-task states (e.g., sleep). EEG research links sleep parameters to learning, memory consolidation, and social functioning in dogs [118]; actigraphy and dyadic co-sleep studies offer a lower-cost avenue to explore nocturnal or unconscious aspects of dyadic life [106,108,110,116,123].

6.3. Measuring (Psychometrics and Behavioral Scales)

Psychometric instruments have proliferated for human–animal interaction research. They are scalable, relatively inexpensive, and amenable to remote administration, which supports larger, more generalizable samples. Typical instruments target owner reports as proxies for dyadic states [114]. Here are some examples of instruments used in dyadic research:
  • Dog impulsivity assessment scale (DIAS) [121]: 18 items addressing behavioral regulation, aggression, and reactivity (5-point Likert). Shows promising validity; reliability needs further demonstration [121,122].
  • Canine behavioral assessment & research questionnaire (C-BARQ): extensive (≈101 items) owner-reported measure covering multiple behavior domains, with established reliability and partial validity [114].
  • Pet attachment questionnaire (PAQ) [81]: measures owner attachment (to a companion animal) style. Out of 26 items, testing anxious and avoidant attachment, 7-point Likert scale each, with acceptable reliability and partial validity.
  • Lexington attachment to pets scale (LAPS) [125]: 23 items across factors of general attachment, substitution, and animal rights/welfare (4-point Likert scale), widely used in the literature.
Crucially, no validated, widely accepted scale yet measures dogs’ attachment to owners; instruments such as the dog attachment insecurity screening inventory are still under development [109]. It is intended to assess the canine attachment style to its caregiver. Likewise, there is no validated scale that directly quantifies dyadic autonomy as a bilateral construct, and few instruments adequately capture canine motivation (though the WDC-BARQ explores working-dog motivations [113].
Limitations of owner-report scales include respondent bias, item misinterpretation, and the unilateral nature of the perspective: most scales capture what owners observe or feel, not the dog’s experience. Consequently, apparent behaviors (e.g., impulsivity on DIAS or problem behaviors on C-BARQ) may reflect unmet psychological needs, stress, or constrained autonomy rather than stable trait pathology [111,126]. Without concurrent canine-centered measures, interpretation remains ambiguous.

6.4. Lessons from “Proto-Tools” and Paths Forward

A variety of methodological approaches currently serve as starting points for documenting dyadic co-construction. These include subjective interviews and questionnaires, behavioral observations and standardized tests, neurophysiological methods, and multidimensional psychometric scales. However, they all have advantages and limitations. Qualitative methods provide depth and contextual richness but lack quantification and are difficult to generalize. Behavioral observations and standardized tests offer objective, fine-grained data but are resource heavy, often small-sample, and may disrupt natural contexts. Neurophysiological measures can objectify underlying processes (e.g., neural synchrony, autonomic regulation) but suffer from methodological heterogeneity, small samples, artifacts, and variable training/anesthesia protocols. Multidimensional psychometric scales are scalable and standardized but commonly adopt a unilateral (owner-centered) perspective and may miss canine agency and motivation.
Recommendations to advance measurements of co-construction:
  • Develop bilateral instruments that simultaneously integrate owner and canine perspectives (e.g., matched owner-report and behavioral/physiological indices; dyadic questionnaires that capture shared routines, negotiated choices, and mutual influence).
  • Adopt mixed-methods designs combining qualitative context, standardized behavioral probes, scalable psychometrics, and targeted neurophysiological measures. This triangulation would leverage the strengths of each approach.
  • Standardize protocols for EEG/fMRI/HRV in dogs (e.g., electrode placement, preprocessing, training procedures, and anesthesia handling) to improve comparability and reproducibility.
  • Prioritize longitudinal and naturalistic studies to capture co-construction as an evolving process and to disentangle causal pathways among technique, experience, environment, and dyadic outcomes.
  • Report samples and contexts rigorously (e.g., breed, age, training history, owner demographics, environment) to allow appropriate stratification and meta-analytic synthesis.
In short, current “proto-tools” can document elements of dyadic co-construction but remain largely unilateral and fragmented. Progress requires integrative, bidirectional, longitudinal approaches that respect the dyad as the primary unit of analysis.

7. Conclusions

From an SDT perspective, dyadic co-construction between humans and dogs is far more than simple participation or cooperation. It represents a dynamic, evolving process of interaction and mutual adaptation, in which each partner influences, and is influenced by, the other (Figure 1). At a minimum, these exchanges aim to meet each individual’s fundamental psychological needs, and in some cases, to actively support the partner’s needs as well. This perspective rejects unilateral control: interactions arise from a synergy shaped by bio-psycho-social, experiential, and environmental factors, with both actors contributing to the structure and evolution of the dyad.
Methodologically, dyadic co-construction can be examined through various approaches. Qualitative tools capture rich, detailed interactional snapshots but lack statistical generalizability. Mixed-method designs may help overcome this limitation, yet existing scales tend to adopt a unilateral lens. Advancing this field could benefit from techniques that explore nervous system activity, which may provide objective evidence of patterns already qualitatively described, and thus foster more balanced, bidirectional assessments. To date, however, no validated tool fully captures dyadic co-construction from both human and canine perspectives.
Although physiological and molecular biomarkers (e.g., heart rate, blood pressure, cortisol) are widely used in research related to SDT, health, and well-being, they were excluded from this review because they provide a more distal perspective on co-construction and yield inconsistent findings [104,127,128]. For instance, a systematic scoping review (n = 27) of animal-assisted interventions (AAI) [128] found that, while most human-focused studies (n = 18) reported at least one positive stress-related outcome, correlations between biomarkers and physiological measures were absent, and results between biomarkers and self-reported stress were inconsistent. Canine-focused studies (n = 9) also produced inconclusive results, particularly for non-certified dogs. Only three studies combined physiological and molecular markers, preventing meaningful correlation analyses
Practical challenges further limit biomarker use. Cortisol, for example, follows a circadian rhythm and is affected by physical activity, diet, and sampling methodology—variables that are often underreported [128]. In dogs [127], cortisol concentrations can also vary by age, sex, reproductive status, and even by the presence of the owner, yet socio-demographic details are rarely documented [128]. Moreover, to be optimal, cortisol levels interpretation needs parallel behavioral observations/measurements, considering its lack of specificity as biomarker for stress, anxiety, pain, or … positive engagement [128]. Interestingly, the greater cortisol modulation in interspecies social situations in dogs as compared to socialized wolves ultimately seems to serve the ability to adapt to the human-dominated environment—one of the essential results of dogification (domestication) [129,130]. This constitutes an underlying mechanism of dyadic co-construction.
This narrative review suggests that the relationship between dogs and their owners may be shaped by a dynamic process of co-construction, in which each partner continuously adapts to the other. Applying SDT to this context offers a valuable framework for understanding how the fundamental psychological needs of autonomy, competence, and attachment can be mutually fulfilled, or hindered, within the dyad. Co-construction appears to be influenced by a complex interplay of biopsychosocial factors, individual and shared experiences, and environmental conditions. While current methodological approaches provide useful insights, they fall short of capturing the bidirectional and evolving nature of this process in a standardized, statistically robust way. Advancing the study of dyadic co-construction will require the development of integrative, longitudinal, and multidimensional tools capable of simultaneously considering human and canine perspectives. Such advancements could contribute to evidence-based practices that enhance both canine welfare and the quality of human–dog relationships.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.L. and E.T.; investigation, L.M.; methodology, L.M., B.L. and E.T.; validation, L.M., B.L. and E.T.; formal analysis, L.M., B.L. and E.T.; resources, C.O., B.L. and E.T.; writing—original draft preparation, L.M., C.O., B.L. and E.T.; writing—review and editing, L.M., C.O., B.L. and E.T.; visualization, L.M. and E.T.; supervision, B.L. and E.T.; project administration, E.T.; funding acquisition, E.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded, in part, by Discovery grants (RGPIN #441651–2013 and #05512–2020; E.T.) for salaries, and a Collaborative Research and Development grant (RDCPJ #491953–2016; E.T.) supporting operations and salaries, from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all the Groupe de recherche en pharmacologie animale du Québec (GREPAQ) members for sharing their own perceptions and perspectives on the human—animal bond. A special thanks to Serge Marchand (Département de chirurgie, Faculté de médecine et des sciences de la santé, Université de Sherbrooke, QC, Canada), Eric Baratay (Faculté des Humanités, Lettres et Sociétés, Université Jean Moulin—Lyon 3, France) and Catherine Amiot (Département de psychologie, Université du Québec à Montréal, QC, Canada) who inspired this investigation, and to Marianne Kempeneers (Département de sociologie, Faculté des arts et des sciences, Université de Montréal, QC, Canada) who stimulated links between human and natural sciences.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Foudriat, M. Définition et dimensions de la co-construction. In La Co-Construction: Une Alternative Managériale, 2nd ed.; Foudriat, M., Ed.; Politiques et interventions sociales; Presses de l’École des Hautes Études en Santé Publique: Rennes, France, 2019; pp. 15–36. ISBN 9782810908257. [Google Scholar]
  2. Siniscalchi, M.; d’Ingeo, S.; Minunno, M.; Quarenta, A. Communication in Dogs. Animals 2018, 31, 131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Range, F.; Kassis, A.; Taborsky, M.; Boada, M.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Wolves and dogs recruit human partners in the cooperative string-pulling task. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 17591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Wallner Werneck Mendes, J.; Vindevogel, M.; van Peer, I.; Martínez, M.; Cimarelli, G.; Range, F. Dogs understand the role of a human partner in a cooperative task. Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 10179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Csepregi, M.; Gácsi, M. Factors Contributing to Successful Spontaneous Dog-Human Cooperation. Animals 2023, 13, 2390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Rodriguez, K.E.; Herzog, H.; Gee, N.R. Variability in Human-Animal Interaction Research. Front. Vet. Sci. 2021, 7, 619600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Ryan, R.M. The Oxford Handbook of Self-Determination Theory, 1st ed.; Ryan, R.M., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2023; 1229p, ISBN 9780197600078. [Google Scholar]
  8. Skinner, B.F. (Ed.) Science And Human Behavior, 2nd ed.; B. F. Skinner Foundation: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014; 466p, ISBN -13 9780029290408. [Google Scholar]
  9. Staddon, J.E.R. Adaptive Behavior and Learning, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2016; 611p, ISBN 9781139998369. [Google Scholar]
  10. Healey, R.; Pepper, A. Interspecies justice: Agency, self-determination, and assent. Philos. Stud. 2021, 178, 1223–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Fennell, D.A. Animal-informed consent: Sled dog tours as asymmetric agential events. Tour. Manag. 2022, 93, 104584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Thomas, N. Animal Ethics and the Autonomous Animal Self, 1st ed.; Thomas, N., Ed.; Palgrave Macmillan: London, UK, 2016; 190p, ISBN -13 9781137586841. [Google Scholar]
  13. Amiot, C.E.; Santerre-Bélec, L. Toward more equal and mutual human-pet relations: Insights and possible solutions based on social psychological theories. Front. Vet. Sci. 2022, 9, 1009267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bell Rizzolo, J.; Bradshaw, G. Nonhuman Animal Nations: Transforming Conservation into Wildlife Self-Determination. Soc. Anim. 2019, 29, 393–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Barina-Silvestri, M.; Díaz-Videla, M.; Delgado-Rodríguez, R. Pet parenting: A systematic review of its characteristics and effects on companion dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2024, 76, 13–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Henning, J.S.L.; Costa, A.G.; Fernandez, E.J. Shifting attitudes on animal ‘ownership’: Ethical implications for welfare research and practice terminology. Res. Ethics 2023, 19, 409–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Medina, L.; Dillenburger, K.; Hurtado-Parrado, C. We Don’t Train in Vain: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Human and Canine Caregiver Training. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2020, 23, 265–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. D’Aniello, B.; Scandurra, A.; Alterisio, A.; Valsecchi, P.; Prato-Previde, E. The importance of gestural communication: A study of human-dog communication using incongruent information. Anim. Cogn. 2016, 19, 1231–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Dodman, N.H.; Brown, D.C.; Serpell, J.A. Associations between owner personality and psychological status and the prevalence of canine behavior problems. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0192846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Gobbo, E.; Zupan, M. Dogs’ Sociability, Owners’ Neuroticism and Attachment Style to Pets as Predictors of Dog Aggression. Animals 2020, 10, 315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Johnson, A.C.; Wynne, C.D.L. Training Dogs with Science or with Nature? An Exploration of Trainers’ Word Use, Gender, and Certification Across Dog-Training Methods. Anthrozoös 2023, 36, 35–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Mikkola, S.; Salonen, M.; Puurunen, J.; Hakanen, E.; Sulkama, S.; Araujo, C.; Lohi, H. Aggressive behaviour is affected by demographic, environmental and behavioural factors in purebred dogs. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 9433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Morrill, K.; Hekman, J.; Li, X.; McClure, J.; Logan, B.; Goodman, L.; Gao, M.; Dong, Y.; Alonso, M.; Carmichael, E.; et al. Ancestry-inclusive dog genomics challenges popular breed stereotypes. Science 2022, 376, eabk0639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Payne, E.M.; Arnott, E.R.; Early, J.B.; Bennett, P.C.; McGreevy, P.D. Dogmanship on the farm: Analysis of personality dimensions and training styles of stock dog handlers in Australia. J. Vet. Behav. 2015, 10, 471–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Payne, E.M.; Bennett, P.C.; McGreevy, P.D. DogTube: An examination of dogmanship online. J. Vet. Behav. 2017, 17, 50–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Ren, W.; Yu, S.; Guo, K.; Lu, C.; Zhang, Y.Q. Disrupted Human–Dog Interbrain Neural Coupling in Autism-Associated Shank3 Mutant Dogs. Adv. Sci. 2024, 11, 2402493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Vieira de Castro, A.C.; Fuchs, D.; Morello, G.M.; Pastur, S.; de Sousa, L.; Olsson, I.A.S. Does training method matter? Evidence for the negative impact of aversive-based methods on companion dog welfare. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0225023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Stevens, J.R.; Wolff, L.M.; Bosworth, M.; Morstad, J. Dog and owner characteristics predict training success. Anim. Cogn. 2021, 24, 219–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Feuerbacher, E.N.; Wynne, C.D. Shut up and pet me! Domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer petting to vocal praise in concurrent and single-alternative choice procedures. Behav. Process. 2015, 110, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Correia-Caeiro, C.; Guo, K.; Mills, D.S. Visual perception of emotion cues in dogs: A critical review of methodologies. Anim. Cogn. 2023, 26, 727–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Ziv, G. The effects of using aversive training methods in dogs—A review. J. Vet. Behav. 2017, 19, 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Boch, M.; Huber, L.; Lamm, C. Domestic dogs as a comparative model for social neuroscience: Advances and challenges. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2024, 162, 105700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kotrschal, K. Wolf–Dog–Human: Companionship Based on Common Social Tools. Animals 2023, 13, 2729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Bedossa, T.; Jeannin, S. Comportement et Bien-Être du Chien une Approche Interdisciplinaire, 1st ed.; Éducagri Éditions: Dijon, France, 2020; 500p, ISBN 9791027503438. [Google Scholar]
  35. Koutsopoulos, T. Dog Training: Theoretical Issues. In A New Approach to Dogs and Dog Training: Human-Canine Synergy in Theory and Practice, 1st ed.; Koutsopoulos, T., Ed.; Springer Nature: Cham, Switzerland, 2024; pp. 111–131. ISBN 978303154004. [Google Scholar]
  36. Gregg, J. Humanish: What Talking to Your Cat or Naming Your Car Reveals About the Uniquely Human Need to Humanize, 1st ed.; Little, Brown and Company: New York, NY, USA, 2025; 304p, ISBN -13 9780316577588. [Google Scholar]
  37. Arhant, C.; Bubna-Littitz, H.; Bartels, A.; Futschik, A.; Troxler, J. Behaviour of smaller and larger dogs: Effects of training methods, inconsistency of owner behaviour and level of engagement in activities with the dog. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 123, 131–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Dale, A.R.; Podlesnik, C.A.; Elliffe, D. Evaluation of an aversion-based program designed to reduce predation of native birds by dogs: An analysis of training records for 1156 dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 191, 59–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Deldalle, S.; Gaunet, F. Effects of 2 training methods on stress-related behaviors of the dog (Canis familiaris) and on the dog–owner relationship. J. Vet. Behav. 2014, 9, 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Feuerbacher, E.N.; Wynne, C.D.L. Most domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer food to petting: Population, context, and schedule effects in concurrent choice. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2014, 101, 385–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Haverbeke, A.; Laporte, B.; Depiereux, E.; Giffroy, J.M.; Diederich, C. Training methods of military dog handlers and their effects on the team’s performances. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 113, 110–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Rooney, N.J.; Cowan, S. Training methods and owner–dog interactions: Links with dog behaviour and learning ability. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 132, 169–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Serpell, J.A.; Duffy, D.L. Aspects of Juvenile and Adolescent Environment Predict Aggression and Fear in 12-Month-Old Guide Dogs. Front. Vet. Sci. 2016, 3, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Bender, Y.; Brauer, J.; Schweinberger, S.R. What makes a good dog-owner team?-A systematic review about compatibility in personality and attachment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2023, 260, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Groenewoud, D.; Enders-Slegers, M.J.; Leontjevas, R.; van Dijke, A.; de Winkel, T.; Hediger, K. Children’s bond with companion animals and associations with psychosocial health: A systematic review. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1120000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Cimarelli, G.; Schindlbauer, J.; Pegger, T.; Wesian, V.; Virányi, Z. Secure base effect in former shelter dogs and other family dogs: Strangers do not provide security in a problem-solving task. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0261790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Duranton, C.; Bedossa, T.; Gaunet, F. Pet dogs synchronize their walking pace with that of their owners in open outdoor areas. Anim. Cogn. 2018, 21, 219–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Duranton, C.; Courby-Betremieux, C.; Gaunet, F. One- and Two-Month-Old Dog Puppies Exhibit Behavioural Synchronization with Humans Independently of Familiarity. Animals 2022, 12, 3356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Duranton, C.; Bedossa, T.; Gaunet, F. Interspecific behavioural synchronization: Dogs exhibit locomotor synchrony with humans. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 12384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Duranton, C.; Bedossa, T.; Gaunet, F. When walking in an outside area, shelter dogs (Canis familiaris) synchronize activity with their caregivers but do not remain as close to them as do pet dogs. J. Comp. Psychol. 2019, 133, 397–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Lamontagne, A.; Legou, T.; Rauchbauer, B.; Grosbras, M.H.; Fabre, F.; Gaunet, F. Behavioural synchronization and social referencing of dogs and humans: Walking in dyad vs in group. Anim. Cogn. 2023, 26, 1021–1034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Le Roux, M.; Wright, S. The Relationship Between Pet Attachment, Life Satisfaction, and Perceived Stress: Results from a South African Online Survey. Anthrozoös 2020, 33, 371–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Luhmann, M.; Kalitzki, A. How animals contribute to subjective well-being: A comprehensive model of protective and risk factors. J. Posit. Psychol. 2018, 13, 200–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Martins, C.F.; Silva, L.; Soares, J.; Pinto, G.S.; Abrantes, C.; Cardoso, L.; Pires, M.A.; Sousa, H.; Mota, M.P. Walk or be walked by the dog? The attachment role. BMC Public Health 2024, 24, 684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. McGuire, B.; Fry, K.; Orantes, D.; Underkofler, L.; Parry, S. Sex of Walker Influences Scent-marking Behavior of Shelter Dogs. Animals 2020, 10, 632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Meehan, M.; Massavelli, B.; Pachana, N. Using Attachment Theory and Social Support Theory to Examine and Measure Pets as Sources of Social Support and Attachment Figures. Anthrozoös 2017, 30, 273–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Northrope, K.; Ruby, M.B.; Howell, T.J. How Attachment to Dogs and to Other Humans Relate to Mental Health. Animals 2024, 14, 2773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Rehn, T.; Beetz, A.; Keeling, L.J. Links between an Owner’s Adult Attachment Style and the Support-Seeking Behavior of Their Dog. Front Psychol. 2017, 8, 2059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Ren, W.; Wei, P.; Yu, S.; Zhang, Y.Q. Left-right asymmetry and attractor-like dynamics of dog’s tail wagging during dog-human interactions. iScience 2022, 25, 104747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Richards, E.M.; Silver, Z.A.; Santos, L.R. Impact of the Dog–Human Bond on Canine Social Evaluation: Attachment Predicts Preference toward Prosocial Actors. Animals 2023, 13, 2480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Riggio, G.; Gazzano, A.; Zsilák, B.; Carlone, B.; Mariti, C. Quantitative Behavioral Analysis and Qualitative Classification of Attachment Styles in Domestic Dogs: Are Dogs with a Secure and an Insecure-Avoidant Attachment Different? Animals 2021, 11, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Sipple, N.; Thielke, L.; Smith, A.; Vitale, K.R.; Udell, M.A.R. Intraspecific and Interspecific Attachment between Cohabitant Dogs and Human Caregivers. Integr. Comp. Biol. 2021, 61, 132–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Uccheddu, S.; Ronconi, L.; Albertini, M.; Coren, S.; Da Graça Pereira, G.; De Cataldo, L.; Haverbeke, A.; Mills, D.S.; Pierantoni, L.; Riemer, S.; et al. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) grieve over the loss of a conspecific. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 1920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Wanser, S.H.; MacDonald, M.; Udell, M.A.R. Dog–human behavioral synchronization: Family dogs synchronize their behavior with child family members. Anim. Cogn. 2021, 24, 747–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Thompkins, A.M.; Lazarowski, L.; Ramaiahgari, B.; Gotoor, S.S.R.; Waggoner, P.; Denney, T.S.; Deshpande, G.; Katz, J.S. Dog-human social relationship: Representation of human face familiarity and emotions in the dog brain. Anim. Cogn. 2021, 24, 251–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Solomon, J.; Beetz, A.; Schöberl, I.; Gee, N.; Kotrschal, K. Attachment security in companion dogs: Adaptation of Ainsworth’s strange situation and classification procedures to dogs and their human caregivers. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2019, 21, 389–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Benz-Schwarzburg, J.; Monsó, S.; Huber, L. How Dogs Perceive Humans and How Humans Should Treat Their Pet Dogs: Linking Cognition With Ethics. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 584037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Ellis, A.; Hawkins, R.; Stanton, S.; Loughnan, S. The Association Between Companion Animal Attachment and Depression: A Systematic Review. Anthrozoös 2024, 37, 1067–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Hughes, B.; Lewis Harkin, B. The Impact of Continuing Bonds Between Pet Owners and Their Pets Following the Death of Their Pet: A Systematic Narrative Synthesis. Omega 2022, 90, 1666–1684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Serpell, J.A. Companion animals. In Anthrozoology: Human-Animal Interactions in Domesticated and Wild Animals; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018; pp. 17–31. ISBN 9780198753629. [Google Scholar]
  71. Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss, 2nd ed.; Basic Book: New York, NY, USA, 1982; Volume 1, 326p, ISBN 0465005438. [Google Scholar]
  72. Kellert, S.R.; Wilson, E.O. The Biophilia Hypothesis, 1st ed.; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1993; 496p, ISBN 9781559631471. [Google Scholar]
  73. Wilson, E.O. Biophilia—The Human Bond with Other Species, 2nd ed.; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1986; 176p, ISBN 978067074422. [Google Scholar]
  74. DeLoache, J.; Pickard, M.; LoBue, V. How very young children think about animals. In How Animals Affect Us: Examining the Influence of Human-Animal Interaction on Child Dev and Human Health, 1st ed.; McCardle, P., McCune, S., Griffin, J.A., Maholmes, V., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2011; pp. 85–99. ISBN 9781433808654. [Google Scholar]
  75. Walker-Meikle, K. Medieval Pets, 1st ed.; Boydell & Brewer: Woodbridge, UK, 2012; 179p, ISBN -13 9781843837589. [Google Scholar]
  76. Cook, P.F.; Spivak, M.; Berns, G.S. One pair of hands is not like another: Caudate BOLD response in dogs depends on signal source and canine temperament. Peer J. 2014, 2, e596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Horn, L.; Huber, L.; Range, F. The Importance of the Secure Base Effect for Domestic Dogs—Evidence from a Manipulative Problem-Solving Task. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e65296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Mariti, C.; Ricci, E.; Zilocchi, M.; Gazzano, A. Owners as a secure base for their dogs. Behaviour 2013, 150, 1275–1294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. McGuire, B.; Song, A. Influence of Sex of Stranger on Responses of Shelter Dogs during Canine Behavioral Evaluations. Animals 2023, 13, 2461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Völter, C.J.; Starić, D.; Huber, L. Using machine learning to track dogs’ exploratory behaviour in the presence and absence of their caregiver. Anim. Behav. 2023, 197, 97–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Zilcha-Mano, S.; Mikulincer, M.; Shaver, P.R. An attachment perspective on human–pet relationships: Conceptualization and assessment of pet attachment orientations. J. Res. Personal. 2011, 45, 345–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Völter, C.J.; Lonardo, L.; Steinmann, M.; Ramos, C.F.; Gerwisch, K.; Schranz, M.T.; Dobernig, I.; Huber, L. Unwilling or unable? Using three-dimensional tracking to evaluate dogs’ reactions to differing human intentions. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2023, 290, 20221621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Gouvernement du Canada. Available online: https://agriculture.canada.ca/fr/commerce-international/renseignements-marches/rapports/analyse-tendances-du-marche-tendances-aliments-animaux-compagnie-au-canada (accessed on 29 April 2025).
  84. Fédération des Fabricants d’Aliments Pour Chiens, Chats, Oiseaux et Autres Animaux Familiers. Available online: https://www.facco.fr/chiffres-cles/les-chiffres-de-la-population-animale/ (accessed on 29 April 2025).
  85. American Veterinary Medical Association. Available online: https://www.avma.org/resources-tools/reports-statistics/us-pet-ownership-statistics?utm_source=chatgpt.com (accessed on 29 April 2025).
  86. Hess-Holden, C.L.; Monaghan, C.L.; Justice, C.A. Pet Bereavement Support Groups: A Guide for Mental Health Professionals. J. Creat. Ment. Health 2017, 12, 440–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Serpell, J.A. Pet-Keeping in Non-Western Societies: Some Popular Misconceptions. Anthrozoös 1987, 1, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Jones, E. Constructing Canine Consent: Conceptualising and Adopting a Consent-Focused Relationship with Dogs, 1st ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2024; 120p, ISBN -13 9781032421599. [Google Scholar]
  89. Westgarth, C.; Christley, R.M.; Marvin, G.; Perkins, E. Functional and recreational dog walking practices in the UK. Health Promot. Int. 2021, 36, 109–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Westgarth, C.; Christley, R.M.; Jewell, C.; German, A.J.; Boddy, L.M.; Christian, H.E. Dog owners are more likely to meet physical activity guidelines than people without a dog: An investigation of the association between dog ownership and physical activity levels in a UK community. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 5704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Diverio, S.; Menchetti, L.; Riggio, G.; Azzari, C.; Iaboni, M.; Zasso, R.; Di Mari, W.; Santoro, M.M. Dogs’ coping styles and dog-handler relationships influence avalanche search team performance. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 191, 67–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Comber, C.A.; Dayer, A.A. Understanding attitudes and norms of dog walkers to reduce disturbances to shorebirds. Hum. Dimens. Wildl. 2022, 27, 236–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Merkouri, A.; Graham, T.M.; O’Haire, M.E.; Purewal, R.; Westgarth, C. Dogs and the Good Life: A Cross-Sectional Study of the Association Between the Dog-Owner Relationship and Owner Mental Wellbeing. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 903647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Townsend, L.; Gee, N.R. Recognizing and Mitigating Canine Stress during Animal Assisted Interventions. Vet. Sci. 2021, 8, 254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Bensky, M.K.; Gosling, S.D.; Sinn, D.L. The World from a Dog’s Point of View: A Review and Synthesis of Dog Cognition Research. In Advances in the Study of Behavior; Brockmann, H.J., Roper, T.J., Naguib, M., Mitani, J.C., Simmons, L.W., Barrett, L., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2013; Volume 45, pp. 209–406. ISSN 00653454. [Google Scholar]
  96. Ducrot, C.; Fortané, N.; Paul, M. Approches Interdisciplinaires en Santé Animale: Dialogue Entre Sciences Sociales et Vétérinaires, 1st ed.; Éditions Quae: Versailles, France, 2024; 273p, ISBN 9782759239719. [Google Scholar]
  97. Herron, M.E.; Kirby-Madden, T.M.; Lord, L.K. Effects of environmental enrichment on the behavior of shelter dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2014, 244, 687–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Siwak-Tapp, C.T.; Head, E.; Muggenburg, B.A.; Milgram, N.W.; Cotman, C.W. Region specific neuron loss in the aged canine hippocampus is reduced by enrichment. Neurobiol. Aging 2008, 29, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Merola, I.; Prato-Previde, E.; Lazzaroni, M.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Dogs’ comprehension of referential emotional expressions: Familiar people and familiar emotions are easier. Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 373–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Merola, I.; Prato-Previde, E.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Dogs’ Social Referencing towards Owners and Strangers. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e47653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Merola, I.; Prato-Previde, E.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Social referencing in dog-owner dyads? Anim. Cogn. 2012, 15, 175–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Bowes, M.; Keller, P.; Rollins, R.; Gifford, R. The Effect of Ambivalence on On-Leash Dog Walking Compliance Behavior in Parks and Protected Areas. J. Park Recreat. Adm. 2017, 35, 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Berns, G. Decoding the Canine Mind. Cerebrum 2020, 2020, cer-04-20. [Google Scholar] [PubMed Central]
  104. Teo, J.T.; Johnstone, S.J.; Römer, S.S.; Thomas, S.J. Psychophysiological mechanisms underlying the potential health benefits of human-dog interactions: A systematic literature review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2022, 180, 27–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Skierbiszewska, K.; Borowska, M.; Bonecka, J.; Turek, B.; Jasiński, T.; Domino, M. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Research on Dog Cognition: A Systematic Review. Appl. Sci. 2024, 14, 12028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Smith, B.P.; Browne, M.; Mack, J.; Kontou, T.G. An Exploratory Study of Human–Dog Co-sleeping Using Actigraphy: Do Dogs Disrupt Their Owner’s Sleep? Anthrozoös 2018, 31, 727–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Schöberl, I.; Beetz, A.; Solomon, J.; Wedl, M.; Gee, N.; Kotrschal, K. Social factors influencing cortisol modulation in dogs during a strange situation procedure. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 11, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Rosano, J.; Howell, T.; Conduit, R.; Bennett, P. Co-Sleeping between Adolescents and Their Pets May Not Impact Sleep Quality. Clocks Sleep 2021, 3, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Riggio, G.; Noom, M.; Gazzano, A.; Mariti, C. Development of the Dog Attachment Insecurity Screening Inventory (D-AISI): A Pilot Study on a Sample of Female Owners. Animals 2021, 11, 3381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Patel, S.I.; Miller, B.W.; Kosiorek, H.E.; Parish, J.M.; Lyng, P.J.; Krahn, L.E. The Effect of Dogs on Human Sleep in the Home Sleep Environment. Mayo Clin. Proc. 2017, 92, 1368–1372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Lenkei, R.; Alvarez Gomez, S.; Pongrácz, P. Fear vs. frustration—Possible factors behind canine separation related behaviour. Behav. Process. 2018, 157, 115–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Koskela, A.; Törnqvist, H.; Somppi, S.; Tiira, K.; Kykyri, V.-L.; Hänninen, L.; Kujala, J.; Nagasawa, M.; Kikusui, T.; Kujala, M.V. Behavioral and emotional co-modulation during dog–owner interaction measured by heart rate variability and activity. Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 25201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Hare, E.; Essler, J.L.; Otto, C.M.; Ebbecke, D.; Serpell, J.A. Development of a modified C-BARQ for evaluating behavior in working dogs. Front. Vet. Sci. 2024, 11, 1371630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Broseghini, A.; Guérineau, C.; Lõoke, M.; Mariti, C.; Serpell, J.; Marinelli, L.; Mongillo, P. Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ): Validation of the Italian Translation. Animals 2023, 13, 1254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Somppi, S.; Törnqvist, H.; Koskela, A.; Vehkaoja, A.; Tiira, K.; Väätäjä, H.; Surakka, V.; Vainio, O.; Kujala, M.V. Dog-Owner Relationship, Owner Interpretations and Dog Personality Are Connected with the Emotional Reactivity of Dogs. Animals 2022, 12, 1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Hoffman, C.L.; Browne, M.; Smith, B.P. Human-Animal Co-Sleeping: An Actigraphy-Based Assessment of Dogs’ Impacts on Women’s Nighttime Movements. Animals 2020, 10, 278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Vieira de Castro, A.C.; Araújo, Â.; Fonseca, A.; Olsson, I.A.S. Improving dog training methods: Efficacy and efficiency of reward and mixed training methods. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0247321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Kulgod, A.; van der Linden, D.; França, L.G.S.; Jackson, M.; Zamansky, A. Non-invasive canine electroencephalography (EEG): A systematic review. BMC Vet. Res. 2025, 21, 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Gazzaniga, M.S.; Ivry, R.B.; Mangun, G.R. Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 5th ed.; W.W. Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 2019; 655p, ISBN -13 9780393693170. [Google Scholar]
  120. Detweiler, D.K. The Dog Electrocardiogram: A Critical Review. In Comprehensive Electrocardiology; Macfarlane, P.W., van Oosterom, A., Pahlm, O., Kligfield, P., Janse, M., Camm, J., Eds.; Springer: London, UK, 2010; pp. 1861–1908. ISBN 9781848820456. [Google Scholar]
  121. Wright, H.; Mills, D.; Pollux, P. Development and Validation of a Psychometric Tool for Assessing Impulsivity in the Domestic Dog (Canis familiaris). Int. J. Comp. Psych. 2011, 24, 210–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Wright, H.F.; Mills, D.S.; Pollux, P.M.J. Behavioural and physiological correlates of impulsivity in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Physiol. Behav. 2012, 105, 676–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Rowe, H.; Jarrin, D.C.; Noel, N.A.O.; Ramil, J.; McGrath, J.J. The curious incident of the dog in the nighttime: The effects of pet-human co-sleeping and bedsharing on sleep dimensions of children and adolescents. Sleep Health 2021, 7, 324–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Ortmeyer, H.K. Effects of Proximity between Companion Dogs and Their Caregivers on Heart Rate Variability Measures in Older Adults: A Pilot Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 17, 2674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Johnson, T.P.; Garrity, T.F.; Stallones, L. Psychometric Evaluation of the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (Laps). Anthrozoös 1992, 5, 160–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Amat, M.; Le Brech, S.; Camps, T.; Manteca, X. Separation-Related Problems in Dogs: A Critical Review. Adv. Small Anim. Care 2020, 1, 19207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Cobb, M.L.; Iskandarani, K.; Chinchilli, V.M.; Dreschel, N.A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of salivary cortisol measurement in domestic canines. Domest. Anim. Endocrinol. 2016, 57, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Gandenberger, J.; Flynn, E.; Moratto, E.; Wendt, A.; Morris, K.N. Molecular Biomarkers of Adult Human and Dog Stress during Canine-Assisted Interventions: A Systematic Scoping Review. Animals 2022, 12, 651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Vasconcellos Ada, S.; Virányi, Z.; Range, F.; Ades, C.; Scheidegger, J.K.; Möstl, E.; Kotrschal, K. Training Reduces Stress in Human-Socialised Wolves to the Same Degree as in Dogs. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0162389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Jean-Joseph, H.; Kortekaas, K.; Range, F.; Kotrschal, K. Context-Specific Arousal During Resting in Wolves and Dogs: Effects of Domestication? Front. Psychol 2020, 11, 568199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Synthesis of dyadic co-construction in perspective with self-determination theory ***. This figure illustrates the process of dyadic co-construction through the three basic psychological needs associated with Self-Determination Theory. Each need is influenced by biopsychosocial, experiential, and environmental factors specific to humans, dogs, and dyads. Their satisfaction or frustration can generate effects that influence dyadic motivation, functioning, and well-being. Together, these dynamics determine the quality and stability of dyadic co-construction. * Positive expressions related to the satisfaction of the basic psychological need. ** Negative expressions related to the difficulties in satisfying the basic psychological need. *** Created in BioRender. Martin, L. (2025) https://BioRender.com/q9ncsm5, accessed on 28 September 2025.
Figure 1. Synthesis of dyadic co-construction in perspective with self-determination theory ***. This figure illustrates the process of dyadic co-construction through the three basic psychological needs associated with Self-Determination Theory. Each need is influenced by biopsychosocial, experiential, and environmental factors specific to humans, dogs, and dyads. Their satisfaction or frustration can generate effects that influence dyadic motivation, functioning, and well-being. Together, these dynamics determine the quality and stability of dyadic co-construction. * Positive expressions related to the satisfaction of the basic psychological need. ** Negative expressions related to the difficulties in satisfying the basic psychological need. *** Created in BioRender. Martin, L. (2025) https://BioRender.com/q9ncsm5, accessed on 28 September 2025.
Animals 15 02875 g001
Table 1. Pathways used in each reference bank according to main theme.
Table 1. Pathways used in each reference bank according to main theme.
Main ThemePath Used
Co-construction(co-construction OR self-determination OR motivation OR partner OR coopera*1) AND (interspeci* OR animal OR dog OR canine)
Competence & education(education OR training OR behav* OR cognit* OR competence OR communication OR reinforcement OR avers* OR interaction) AND (dog OR canine) AND (owner OR human OR interspeci*)
Relatedness
(attachment)
(attachment OR bond OR relationship OR affiliation OR synchronization OR referenc* OR secur*) AND (dog OR canine) AND (owner OR human OR interspeci*)
Autonomy(autonomy OR consent OR liberty OR desire OR activit* OR environment* OR norm* OR regulation) AND (dog OR canine) AND (owner OR human OR interspeci*)
Researching and measuring co-construction(co-construction OR self-determination OR motivation OR partner OR coopera* OR education OR training OR reinforcement OR avers* OR interaction OR autonomy OR consent OR liberty OR desire OR activit* OR environment* OR norm* OR regulation OR attachment OR bond OR relationship OR affiliation OR synchronization OR referenc* OR secur*) AND (dog OR canine) AND (owner OR human OR interspeci*) AND (tool OR scale OR metric OR measure OR assessment OR observation OR marker OR physi* OR neuro* OR actigraphy OR perform* OR method* OR brain OR behav* OR cognit* OR competence OR communication)
1 The use of asterisks in the keyword search strings made it possible to automatically retrieve all derived forms of a term from its root.
Table 2. Number of references retrieved for and used in each main theme (13 February 2025) 1.
Table 2. Number of references retrieved for and used in each main theme (13 February 2025) 1.
Main ThemeNumber of References Drawn from Databases with Intra-Database Duplicates (n =)Number of References Drawn from Databases Without Intra-Database Duplicates (n =)Number of Cited References (n =) 2
Co-constructionn = 19,613 n = 14,184References found from the script:
Original research articles: n = 3 [3,4,5]
Others: n = 3 [10,13,14]
Additional references:
Books: n = 3 [1,7,12]
Others: n = 3 [2,6,11]
Competence &
education
n = 12,252 n = 8766References found from the script:
Systematic review: n = 1 [17]
Original research articles: n = 12 [18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29]
Others: n = 6 [2,6,30,31,32,33]
Additional references:
Books: n = 3 [34,35,36]
Original research articles: n = 7 [37,38,39,40,41,42,43]
Relatedness
(attachment)
n = 7041 n = 5129References found from the script:
Systematic reviews: n = 2 [44,45]
Original research articles: n = 22
[20,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66]
Others: n = 2 [2,67]
Additional references:
Systematic reviews or systematic narrative synthesis: n = 2 [68,69]
Books: n = 6 [70,71,72,73,74,75]
Original research articles: n = 7 [76,77,78,79,80,81,82]
Others: n = 5 [83,84,85,86,87]
Autonomyn = 14,534 n = 10,102References found from the script:
Book: n = 1 [88]
Original research articles: n = 5 [89,90,91,92,93]
Others: n = 3 [11,13,94]
Additional references:
Books: n = 4 [12,36,95,96]
Original research articles: n = 6 [97,98,99,100,101,102]
Others: n = 3 [14,33,103]
Researching and measuring co-constructionn = 16,199n = 11,749References found from the script:
Systematic reviews, systematic literature reviews or meta-analysis: n = 2 [104,105]
Original research articles: n = 17 [26,66,89,90,91,93,106,107,108,109,110,111,112,113,114,115,116]
Other: n = 1 [117]
Additional references:
Systematic review: n = 1 [118]
Books: n = 2 [119,120]
Original research articles: n = 6 [81,121,122,123,124,125]
Other: n = 1 [126]
1 The main discussion was based on n = 122 references, specifically: n = 8 systematic reviews, systematic narrative syntheses, and meta-analyses; n = 77 original research articles; n = 16 review articles and conceptual analyses; n = 1 research protocol; n = 17 books; and n = 3 websites. Additionally, n = 8 references—namely, n = 3 systematic reviews and a systematic scoping review, n = 2 books, n = 2 original research article and n = 1 other—are unclassifiable [8,9,15,16,127,128,129,130]. They were included for contextual purposes. 2 If a reference was cited in multiple sections, it appears in all relevant sections.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Martin, L.; Otis, C.; Lussier, B.; Troncy, E. A Conceptual Framework for the Co-Construction of Human–Dog Dyadic Relationship. Animals 2025, 15, 2875. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192875

AMA Style

Martin L, Otis C, Lussier B, Troncy E. A Conceptual Framework for the Co-Construction of Human–Dog Dyadic Relationship. Animals. 2025; 15(19):2875. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192875

Chicago/Turabian Style

Martin, Laurie, Colombe Otis, Bertrand Lussier, and Eric Troncy. 2025. "A Conceptual Framework for the Co-Construction of Human–Dog Dyadic Relationship" Animals 15, no. 19: 2875. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192875

APA Style

Martin, L., Otis, C., Lussier, B., & Troncy, E. (2025). A Conceptual Framework for the Co-Construction of Human–Dog Dyadic Relationship. Animals, 15(19), 2875. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15192875

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop