Next Article in Journal
Dietary Supplementation with Oregano and Linseed in Autochthonous “Facciuta Lucana” Goats: Effects on Meat Quality Traits in Suckling Kids
Previous Article in Journal
Environmental and Anthropogenic Influences on Coliform Concentrations in the Octopus insularis Production Chain in the Veracruz Reef System, Gulf of Mexico
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Differences in Mother–Infant Bond and Social Behavior of African Elephant Calves Living In Situ and Ex Situ

by
Franziska Hoerner
1,*,
Jake Rendle-Worthington
2,
Arne Lawrenz
3,
Ann-Kathrin Oerke
4,
Karsten Damerau
5,
Santiago Borragán Santos
6,
Therese Hard
7 and
Gela Preisfeld
1
1
Department of Zoology, University of Wuppertal, 42119 Wuppertal, Germany
2
eleCREW, Victoria Falls XRH4+72, Zimbabwe
3
Zoo Wuppertal, 42117 Wuppertal, Germany
4
Endocrinology Laboratory, German Primate Centre, 37077 Goettingen, Germany
5
Department of Ecology, Europa-Universität Flensburg, 24943 Flensburg, Germany
6
Parque de Cabarceno, 39690 Obregón, Cantabria, Spain
7
Boras Djurpark, 506 31 Borås, Sweden
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2023, 13(19), 3051; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13193051
Submission received: 25 August 2023 / Revised: 21 September 2023 / Accepted: 27 September 2023 / Published: 28 September 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Zoo Animals)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

In the wild, African elephant calves must stay close to their mothers and the family unit as the African environment holds many threats. African elephant calves in zoos are raised in a protected environment. Therefore, we hypothesize that calves ex situ hold bigger distances and behave differently than in situ. Additionally, those differences are likely to increase with further zoo generations. This study used ethological research methods to compare the mother–calf bond of African elephant calves in situ and ex situ (first and second generation). The results showed that ex situ living calves of both generations maintain greater distances to their mothers and show a wider variation (positive and negative) in behavior than in situ. The detected differences indicate that calves ex situ can behave more freely as they are in a protected environment. Therefore, they can develop faster than in the wild, which agrees with similar findings on African elephant calf development and adult African elephants. The hypothesis that differences between in situ and ex situ increase with the zoo generations could not be verified. Hence, modifications in behavior under different environmental selection pressures may be adaptive.

Abstract

African zoo elephants live in safe environments with sufficient resources, are protected from threats, and have their health and body conditions cared for. Calves ex situ undergo the same developmental stages as in situ and are raised by the whole family unit. However, due to environmental differences, there might be behavioral modifications between calves in situ and ex situ. We hypothesize that these differences increase with ongoing generations. This ethological study compares social and general behavior and the distance calves kept to their mothers’ between calves of the first (F1) and second (F2) zoo generation and the wild. Using ethological methods, data were collected for ~90 in situ calves and 16 ex situ (8 F1, 8 F2) between the ages of 0.5 to 4 years (120 observation hours per group). Results showed that in situ calves spent significantly more time close to mothers than the F1 and the F2 zoo generations (F1/in situ: p = <0.001; F2/in situ: p = 0.007). The behaviors of eating, drinking, trunk movement, washing, and affiliative behaviors showed significant differences between in situ and ex situ calves. The amount and distribution of affiliative and agonistic behavior initiated and received by calves was displayed with a greater variety ex situ. Ex situ calves not only performed affiliative but, in contrast to the in situ, also agonistic behavior (F1/in situ: initiated p = 0.002, received p = 0.010; F2/in situ: initiated p = 0.050, received p = 0.037). The comparison of zoo generations suggests that differences did not increase with the generation. The more casual binding between mothers and offspring in zoos and the age-dependent improvement of social behavior of zoo-born calves are seen as a result of elephants’ adaptation to secure zoo conditions. The results of this study agree with the faster development of ex situ African elephants, like earlier puberty and more frequent breeding patterns, as known from the literature.

1. Introduction

African elephants (Loxodonta africana) are known for their complex and close social bonds. Calves are born into stable families and cared for by their mothers, other females (allomothers), and older siblings [1,2,3]. The death of a mother during the first 24 months of life will leave the calf with almost no chances of survival in situ [4,5,6] and even in captivity [7]. Tactile, visual, olfactory, and acoustic contact between mothers and calves is essential [4,5,6]. This can be seen in data from the Amboseli population, representing the most complete long-term dataset in the demography of wild African elephants [6]. There, calves spend about 56% of their time in close contact with their mothers during the first two years [6]. During this period, the close bond is maintained by both mother and calf, but will loosen after two years and is then pursued more by the mother as the mother is responsible for the calf’s safety and survival [6].
Besides the contact with their mothers, during the first two years, the calves were observed to spend approximately 20% of their time at a physical contact distance to the next family member [6]. Only about 10% of the time they were observed to be more than five meters away from their next neighbor [6]. Charif et al. [8] detected that close spatial bonds are even maintained by adult female elephants of the same bond groups (related family), which were found to have coordinated movement and preserve a distance of no more than 0.5 km for most of their time.
African elephant calves’ development is subdivided into seven stages, as listed in Table 1 [4,5,6,7,8,9,10].
Calves in zoos possibly undergo the same developmental stages and are described by Andrews et al. [11], Webber [12], and Freeman et al. [13]. However, social constellations are different from the wild, and zoo family units are not necessarily related, as in the wild. Due to the increase in captive breeding, zoos are now at the F2 generation (second generation of zoo-born elephants, both parent animals being of the F1 generation), with very few even reaching the F3 generation (third generation of zoo-born elephants) [14]. This aspect is essential as F1 calves (first generation of zoo-born calves) were born and raised by mothers imported from the wild, lacking their mother’s help and assistance. However, F2 calves are already born and raised by mothers who grew up in captivity and often in the presence of their grandmothers, which resembles the family structure from the wild [15].
It has been shown that elephants in zoos reach fertility at a younger age. Females start to show ovarian cycles at 6 to 7 years and can give birth for the first time at 8 to 9 years [16]. While data on the onset of the ovarian cycle in wild females are missing, the first births are reported in cows mostly between 12 and 16 years of age, the earliest being reported at 9 years old (± one month) [9,10,17,18]. Males in zoos must not show musth to be able to breed [19] and can sire offspring as young as 9 to 10 years old [19]. Whereas in the wild, males reach musth for the first time around the age of 12–14 years and were observed to be accepted as mating partners by cows only at the age of 25 years [10].
If we are to suppose that African elephants in zoos reach puberty much earlier than in the wild, in that case, it is possible that African elephant calves in zoos also develop faster than those in the wild, and most likely recreate faster. Preliminary data were collected by Hoerner et al. [under review], who found that calves living ex situ tended to maintain greater distances to their mothers than reported for their conspecifics in situ as known from the literature [4,6,8]. Calves in zoos were observed to spend up to 31% of their time at a distance of more than five meters from their mothers already at the age of three days [20,21,22]. This was observed in male and female calves and of the matriarch and sub-dominant cows. This spatial detachment was observed to increase with the age of the calves [20,21,22], whereas it is unclear if this feature will increase with future generations.
The new generations in the zoos (F2 and, most recently, F3) are no longer solely socialized by wild-born elephants but by zoo-born elephants. Additionally, the import of wild elephants is considered outdated [23]. Therefore, in situ-born elephants become less represented in zoos. Calves adapt the social and behavioral patterns of the relatives that raise them [9,24,25]. This results in our two hypotheses: (1) Captive elephant calves keep bigger distances to their mothers and show different social and general behavior than wild elephant calves. (2) Those differences between wild and captive elephant calves increase with the next zoo generations.
We tackled those hypotheses by combining ethological research in situ and ex situ to learn about possible differences in the behavior and distance keeping of calves brought up correspondingly. To investigate the second hypothesis, we collected data for ex situ calves from the F1 and F2 generations.

2. Material and Methods

2.1. Animals

Data were collected for a total number of ~106 elephant calves of three different groups: (I) F1 generation ex situ, (II) F2 generation ex situ, (III) in situ. Within Europe, data were collected in four zoos for 16 African elephants—8 of the F1 generation and 8 of the F2 generation. Calves from zoos had between 240 and 1315 m2/elephant of space. A possible impact of the varying enclosure size on the calves’ behavior was eliminated in a previous study [22].
The animals observed in situ were selected to reflect the same age group and distribution as the F1 and F2 groups. The age of in situ calves was estimated by body size and confirmed by knowledge of local rangers. Calves of all data sets were further sampled by age group according to developmental stages [4,5,6]. Data for this group were collected in the Jafuta Reserve, the Zambezi National Park, and Hwange National Park in Zimbabwe. The areas in which family units were observed were sparse miombo woodland with nearby water sources. As no register for the family units in the observation areas was available, it could not reliably be determined whether the same families were observed on several occasions. Family units were followed by vehicles. The age limit for the sample animals was drawn at approximately four years, as gender-related differences in behavior become significant from that age [6]. Family units of calves observed in situ had varying sizes between 4 and 26 animals, comprising family units with older daughter elephants, subadult males, and allomothers.
Table 2 displays the information of the animals, valid for the time of data collection.

2.2. Ethological Data Collection

The behavior of the calves was measured utilizing an ethogram extracted from Poole & Granli [26,27], listing 16 behavioral categories (Table 3), and two research methods: the Social Distance Method, Focal Animal Sampling [28,29,30].
The Social Distance Method measured the distance between calves and mothers, dividing the distance into five parameters: tactile contact, <1 m, 1–3 m, 3–5 m, and >5 m [28,30,31]. The distance was noted every 60 s using continuous sampling. This parameter was used to analyze the mother–calf relationship [6].
Utilizing the ethogram and Focal Animal Sampling, the general behavior of the calves was observed. These data were used to generate a behavioral profile of the calves of different origins [28,30,31,32,33]. Here again, the interval for data registration was 60 s using continuous recording.
Additionally, the calves’ social behavior within the family units was measured. Therefore, all affiliative and agonistic contact, either initiated or received by the calves, were measured. These data were collected using continuous recording [28,30,31,32,33].
Data collection in zoos took place between 2016 and 2021 and in the wild from February to March 2023. Each calf of the F1 and F2 ex situ generation was observed for 15 h, resulting in an observation time of 120 h for each sample group. For the in situ sample group, the observation time was also 120 h. However, here, calves were not observed for 15 h but ~3–4 h each, as the single individuals could not be tracked again reliably. Data in situ and ex situ were only collected when animals could behave freely without human interactions. During in situ data collection, observers held a big distance from the family units (at least 200 m). They used binoculars for a better view, ensuring that calves and family units were not influenced in their behavior by human presence.

2.3. Data Analysis

For data analysis, all data sets were classified numerically by summing up all data to a joined maximum of 100% [34,35,36]. For statistical analysis data for calves were additionally sampled according to age group [4,5,6]. As all sample groups were chosen to be age matched in sample size, we did not include age as a variable. Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS version 29 (IBM SPSS Statistics 29). All data sets were tested for distribution with the Shapiro–Wilk test and the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test [37]. As neither of the tests resulted in a homogenous or even distribution of the data sets, a graphical analysis of the Q-Q plots was used. All data sets were identified as non-parametric [38,39]. The Therefore, the Kruskal-Wallis calculation for non-parametric datasets was used to detected significant differences between the three sample groups (F1 ex situ, F2 ex situ, and in situ). In case of significant differences a Post hoc test was calculated to detect which sample groups showed differences [40,41]. For all calculations, the level of significance was set at p ≤ 0.05 (normal significance) and p ≤ 0.001 (strong significance) [42,43].

3. Results

3.1. Distance to Mother

Statistical data analysis on the distance between calves and their mothers with the Kruskal–Wallis test detected significant differences. We found that in situ living calves predominantly spend time in tactile contact with their mothers (M = 58.11%), in comparison to the ex situ calves of the F1 (M = 7.22%) and F2 generation (M = 15.27%) (Figure 1). Calves of the F1 and F2 zoo generations spent the majority of time in the distance category < 1 m to their mothers (F1: M = 35.05%, F2: M = 32.50%). It can also be seen that calves living in the wild barely spend time at a distance further than 1 m from their mothers.
Comparing the data of calves of the F1 zoo generation and the wild with the Post hoc test, significant differences were detected in the distance categories tactile, 1–3 m, 3–5 m, and >5 m. The difference between the F1 zoo generation and wild calves was not significant for the distance category <1 m. Calves of the F2 zoo generation and the wild also showed significant differences for the categories tactile, 1–3 m, 3–5 m, and >5 m. The comparison of the data on distance to the mother did not detect any significant differences between calves of the F1 and F2 zoo generations (see Table 4).

3.2. General Behavior

Calves of the F1 and F2 zoo generation mostly displayed the behavior eating (F1: M = 39.703, F2: M = 35.13), followed by the behavior affiliative contact (F1: M = 12.435, F2: M = 14.944) (Figure 2). In situ calves, on the other hand, mostly showed affiliative contact (M = 48.22). No other behavior was recorded as often as the three categories mentioned above.
The statistical analysis (Table 5) of the amount the behavioral categories were shown by calves of the F1 and F2 generations ex situ and living in situ (see Table 3) revealed significant differences between calves of the F1 generation and in situ, and also of the F2 generation and in situ in the five behavioral categories eat (in situ < ex situ), drink (in situ > ex situ), trunk movement (in situ < ex situ), wash (in situ > ex situ) and affiliative behavior (in situ > ex situ). For the behavioral categories suckle, walk, run, wash, sleep, social play, lone play, escape, seeking rescue, rescuing, and threatening, no significant differences were found between in situ and F1 ex situ, as well as in situ and F2 ex situ. Significant differences between the F1 and F2 generations were only detected for the behavioral category suckle (F1 < F2).

3.3. Social Behavior

The boxplots in Figure 3 demonstrate that calves of the F1 zoo generation initiated affiliative behavior of M = 91.41% and agonistic behavior of M = 8.59%. They received affiliative behavior of M = 83.65% and agonistic behavior of M = 13.35%. Calves of the F2 zoo generation initiated affiliative behavior of M = 94.61% and agonistic behavior of M = 5.39%. They received affiliative behavior of M = 85.78% and agonistic behavior of M = 14.22%. Calves living in the wild received and sent only affiliative behavior. The behavior initiated by in situ calves was 100% affiliative with no agonistic behavior. Additionally, they received affiliative behavior of M = 98.73%, with a single outlier.
The analysis of the social behavior of the calves of the three test groups also showed significant differences between calves living ex situ and in situ (Table 6). There were no significant differences between the F1 and F2 zoo generations. Significances were detected between the F1 generation and in situ calves in all four social behavior categories. Significant differences were found in received affiliative and agonistic behavior between the F2 generation and calves in situ.

4. Discussion

The results of the distances between calves and their mothers confirm the first hypothesis that there are significant differences between African elephant calves living in the wild and zoo environments. Calves in situ spending a majority of their time at a very close spatial distance to their mothers (tactile and <1 m: M = 92.78%), which was significantly higher than for the F1 zoo generation (tactile and <1 m: M = 42.27%) and the F2 zoo generation (tactile and <1 m: M = 46.78%), agrees with former observations of Webber [12]. She states that in situ calves stay almost continuously at a close spatial distance to their mothers and that this is not valid for calves born in zoos. However, while comparing African and Asian calves in situ and ex situ, Webber only found this difference in Asian elephant calves living in zoos. Berg [44] also observed captive African elephant calves up to six months of age and observed that they spend 70–75% of their time in body contact with other individuals. The data at hand first observed a spatial detachment for African calves living ex situ. A possible explanation for this spatial detachment between mothers and calves living in a zoo environment is the absence of possible threats (predators, losing the family unit, lack of water). In the wild, a close spatial bond with the mother elephant is crucial for the calf’s survival [2,6,45].
The second hypothesis of this study, stating that those differences might increase with the F2 zoo generation, was not confirmed by data on the distance kept by calves from their mothers. No significant differences between the distance keeping of the F1 and F2 generations were detected. The significance level even decreases from strong to normal with the generations. We interpret the increasing spatial detachment between calves and mothers observed in zoos not as an issue of concern regarding elephant breeding in zoos. Previous studies on adult F1 ex situ generation elephants detected species-specific social behavior and bonds that subsist over years and generations [46,47].
The data for the 16 general behavior categories for calves living in situ collected in this study resemble those described by other researchers [2,6,48], as do the data for the calves from zoos [12,49,50,51].
Other than the data on the distances between calves and mothers, the data on the general behavior of the calves do not display as many significant differences between the in situ and ex situ calves of both generations. The calves of the sample group in situ were observed to spend significantly more time drinking and washing. The observation spot can explain this, as data in situ were frequently collected close to a water hole when families moved out of dense bushes and could be easily observed. Hence, calves spent more time drinking and bathing on those occasions.
Calves in situ spent less time eating than calves of both generations ex situ. A possible reason is that animals must feel safe eating [2]. In the wild, calves were observed to be more anxious than in the zoos and, therefore, might spend less time eating than calves in zoos, which are constantly in a safe environment. Additionally, elephants in zoos have access to food almost continually and can eat without stress and fear [52,53]. In situ, elephants must feed in the open bush or grassland [17]. Ex situ calves of this study had constant access to food in the form of hay, branches, and occasionally fruits and vegetables. However, during our observation in the wild, calves also had constant access to food, such as grass and branches. As observations were made in March and April, the vegetation was dense due to the rainy season.
The behavioral category, trunk movement, was displayed significantly less frequently by calves living in the wild. A possible reason for calves living in zoos displaying this behavior more frequently is that calves in an ex situ environment have more time to train their trunk, instead of concentrating on following the mother and the family. It can be assumed that the protected environment leads to quicker development, as can also be noted by an earlier start of breeding [16,19]. The more leisure behavior observed from the calves in this study agrees with Webber et al.’s interpretations [54]. They observed that ex situ African and Asian elephant calves spend more time playing than in situ calves. They also conclude that this difference originates from the more peaceful zoo environment that gives calves more opportunities for playing behavior [54]. Another possible explanation for this difference is that calves in situ have other occupations besides playing. In the wild, they must gain ecological and social knowledge and specific skills to ensure survival [4,5,6,9]. This is not required in zoos.
The significantly higher affiliative behavior displayed by calves from the wild compared to the F1 and F2 generations in zoos indicates differing social behavior for calves living in these different environments. A possible reason for this is the ever-changing presence of other elephants, independent of the family, in the wild. Zoo elephants live in generally stable family units that change less frequently than in the wild. The number of changes in nearby animals likely impacts the affiliative behavior of calves in the wild, which depend on the care and positive reactions of other elephants and, therefore, almost solely displaying affiliative behavior [2,15].
Similar to the data for the distance between mother and calf, data on the general behavior of the calves showed no trend of an increase in the differences between ex situ and in situ calves with the next generation of zoo elephants.
The amount of affiliative behavior initiated and received by calves from the zoo in this study is significantly lower than that in the wild, where they are known to be treated with intense care and affiliative behavior by family members [2,17,45,55,56]. Also, the distribution of affiliative and agonistic behavior between ex situ and in situ calves differs in this study, as the in situ sample group was barely observed to initiate or receive agonistic behavior. Nevertheless, the ex situ calves also initiated and received significantly more affiliative than agonistic behavior, corresponding to the wild’s social behavior [2,45,55]. A possible reason for the lower amount of social behavior recorded for zoo calves is the enrichment and safety that the zoo environment supplies. While in the wild, calves must stay close, follow, and be in contact with their mothers almost constantly [2,17,45,55], a safe zoo environment allows them to devote themselves to other activities. This also enhances the faster development of elephants in zoos [16,19].
Also, affiliative behavior is less crucial for calves living in zoo environments as they live in rarely changing social groups. Many young animals display more affiliative than agonistic behavior in wild environments [57].
While many behavioral patterns of in situ and ex situ calves differ significantly in the study at hand, studies on adult African elephants of the F1 generation detected species-specific social behavior, with a strong majority of affiliative behavior initiated and received by family members, as stated before [46,47]. Hence, the question of whether differences in social behavior between ex situ and in situ living African elephants increase with the generations cannot be answered in this paper.

5. Conclusions

Despite the varying sample sizes and observation hours per animal, the present study found significant differences in the distance keeping and the general and social behavior of in situ and ex situ African elephant calves to their mothers and other family units or family members. These findings agree with former findings on Asian elephant calves in zoos by Webber [12]. However, they did not make the same observations for African elephant calves [12].
Calves living in a safe environment are not hesitant to separate earlier from their mothers as this involves less risk for them. Ex situ calves are less hesitant to contact other elephants with agonistic behavior. Additionally, instead of following their mothers and keeping social contact with the family members like in the wild, calves in a safe zoo environment have more time to observe, learn, play, adapt social behavior, eat, and compete and, therefore, can develop quicker. This faster development of ex situ calves corresponds with the earlier maturity and breeding of zoo elephants [16,19]. African elephant calves ex situ are more independent than in the wild and spend more time eating and interacting with others, following the faster growth rate and the general pattern of enhanced development rates ex situ [16,19].
If zoos continue to breed elephants to generate a self-sustaining population—which is necessary, as the import of wild elephants is considered outdated [23]—it needs to be ensured by ethological research that the elephants bred and socialized there show (social) behavior that does not indicate a negative impact on their wellbeing. Social interactions and touch in captive elephant calves are highly relevant during early development and are associated with prosocial behavior and elephant welfare [13]. Therefore, falsifying hypothesis two is essential as this is reassuring for the ex situ breeding program which seeks to establish an independent stock of zoo elephants living under the best welfare conditions [19].

Author Contributions

F.H.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, project administration, visualization, and writing—original draft; A.L.: resources, and writing—review, and editing; J.R.-W.: resources, and writing—review and editing; A.-K.O.: project administration, validation, and writing—review, and editing; S.B.S.: resources, and writing—review, and editing; T.H.: resources, and writing—review and editing; K.D.: formal analysis, and writing—review, and editing; G.P.: resources, supervision, validation, and writing—review and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

We acknowledge support from the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Wuppertal.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Animal welfare was not affected by data collection at any point during the study as the elephants were not affected by the behavioral observations. Not applicable as the study required no interaction with the animals.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Please contact [email protected] for data.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all persons involved in the study at the various zoos for the opportunity to collect the present data. We especially thank Beatriz Gallego Aldama, Dennis W. H. Müller, and the keepers of all elephant facilities for their help and cooperation during the whole project, as well as Yvonne Masarira and others from eleCREW in Zimbabwe for observing elephants in the wild.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the study’s design; in the collection, analysis, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Lee, P.C.; Moss, C.J. Early maternal investment in male and female African elephant calves. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1986, 18, 353–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Moss, C.J. The demography of an African elephant (Loxodonta africana) population in Amboseli, Kenya. J. Zool. 2001, 255, 145–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Wittemyer, G.; Getz, W.M. Hierarchical dominance structure and social organization in African elephants, Loxodonta africana. Anim. Behav. 2007, 73, 671–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Archie, E.A.; Moss, C.J.; Alberts, S.C. The ties that bind: Genetic relatedness predicts the fission and fusion of social groups in wild African elephants. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2006, 273, 513–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Archie, E.A.; Moss, C.J.; Alberts, S.C. Friends and Relations: Kinship and the Nature of Female Elephant Social Relationships. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C.J., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 238–245. [Google Scholar]
  6. Lee, P.C.; Moss, C. Calf Development and Maternal Rearing Strategies. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C.J., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 224–237. [Google Scholar]
  7. Lahdenperä, M.; Mar, K.U.; Lummaa, V. Short-term and delayed effects of mother death on calf mortality in Asian elephants. Behav. Ecol. 2016, 27, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Charif, R.A.; Ramey, R.R.; Langbauer, W.R.; Payne, K.B.; Martin, R.B.; Brown, L.M. Spatial relationships and matrilineal kinship in African savanna elephant (Loxodonta africana) clans. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2005, 57, 327–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Moss, C. Social circles. Wildl. News 1981, 16, 2–7. [Google Scholar]
  10. Poole, J.H. Mate guarding, reproductive success and female choice in African elephants. Anim. Behav. 1989, 37, 842–849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Andrews, J.; Mecklenborg, A.; Bercovitch, F.B. Milk intake and development in a newborn captive African elephant (Loxodonta africana). Zoo Biol. 2005, 24, 275–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Webber, C.E. A Comparison of Behavioural Development of Elephant Calves in Captivity and in the Wild: Implications for Welfare. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Stirling, Stirling, Scotland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  13. Freeman, P.T.; Anderson, E.L.; Allen, K.B.; O’Connell-Rodwell, C.E. Age-based variation in calf independence, social behavior and play in a captive population of African elephant calves. Zoo Biol. 2021, 40, 376–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bossy, S. Elefantenhaltung vor dem Umbruch; Pinguinal Magazien des Zoo-Vereins Wuppertal E.V.: Wuppertal, Germany, 2019; pp. 22–23. [Google Scholar]
  15. Lee, P.C. Allomothering among African elephants. Anim. Behav. 1987, 35, 278–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Oerke, A.-K. Monitoring sexual maturity in female elephants of the EEP population. EAZA News, 17 August 2004; pp. 26–27. [Google Scholar]
  17. Douglas-Hamilton, I.; Douglas-Hamilton, O. Unter Elefanten: Abenteuerliche Forschungen in der Wildnis Zentralafrikas; Gustav Lübbe Verlag GmbH: Bergisch Gladbach, Germany, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  18. Lee, P.C.; Fishlock, V.; Webber, C.E.; Moss, C.J. The reproductive advantages of a long life: Longevity and senescence in wild female African elephants. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2016, 70, 337–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. EAZA. Areas of Activity. EAZA. 2020. Available online: https://www.eaza.net/about-us/areas-of-activity/ (accessed on 11 April 2023).
  20. Hörner, F. Das Sozial- und Spielverhalten Afrikanischer Elefantenkälber in Menschlicher Obhut am Beispiel des Elefantenkalbs Tuffi im Zoologischen Garten Wuppertal. Bachelor’s Thesis, Wuppertal, Germany, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hörner, F. Soziale Interaktion des Elefantenkalbs Tuffi in Anbetracht der Trächtigkeit ihrer Mutter Sabie im Zoo Wuppertal—Eine Ethologische Studie. Entwicklung Eines Bilingualen Moduls zum Thema Sozialverhalten bei Afrikanischen Elefanten. Master’s Thesis, Wuppertal, Germany, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  22. Hoerner, F.; Oerke, A.-K.; Müller, D.W.; Azogu-Sepe, I.; Preisfeld, G. Behavior and social bonds of African elephant calves under different holding systems in European zoos. Zoo Biol. 2023. under review. [Google Scholar]
  23. CITES International Trade in Live Elephants. CITES. 2019. Available online: https://cites.org/eng/news/statement/international_trade_in_live_elephants (accessed on 11 April 2023).
  24. Moss, C.; Colbeck, M. Echo of the Elephants. The Story of an Elephant Family; William Morrow and Company, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  25. Ortolani, A.; Leong, K.; Graham, L.; Savage, A. Behavioral indices of estrus in a group of captive African elephants (Loxodonta africana). Zoo Biol. 2005, 24, 311–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Poole, J.; Granli, P. Signals, Gestures, and Behavior of African Elephants. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 109–124. [Google Scholar]
  27. Poole, J.; Granli, P. The Elephant Ethogram. Elephant Voices. 2021. Available online: https://elephantvoices.org/elephant-ethogram/ethogram-table/overview.html (accessed on 22 March 2023).
  28. Kappeler, P. Verhaltensbiologie; Springer Spektrum: Berlin, Germany, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  29. Klaus, R.-D. Methoden der Verhaltensforschung. Kölner Zoo. 2017. Available online: http://www.koelnerzoo.de/fileadmin/Downloads/zoo_schule/Beob.methoden_im_Zoo.pdf (accessed on 22 March 2023).
  30. Naguib, M.; Krause, E.T. Methoden der Verhaltensbiologie; Springer Spektrum: Berlin, Germany, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  31. Krull, H.-P. Beobachtungs- und Protokollmethoden für Verhaltensbeobachtungen. Zooschule Krefeld. 2000. Available online: https://docplayer.org/39459251-Beobachtungs-und-protokollmethoden-fuer-verhaltensbeobachtungen-zooschule-krefeld.html (accessed on 22 March 2023).
  32. Martin, P.; Bateson, P. Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide, 3rd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  33. Randler, C. Verhaltensbiologie; Haupt Verlag AG: Bern, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  34. Agresti, A. An Introduction to Categorical Data Analysis, 2nd ed.; Wiley Series in Probability and Statistics: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  35. American Psychological Association. Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, 6th ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  36. Bortz, J.; Döring, N. Forschungsmethoden und Evaluation: Für Human- und Sozialwissenschaftler; Springer-Medizin: Heidelberg, Germany, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  37. Razali, N.M.; Wah, Y.B. Power comparisons of Shapiro-Wilk, Kolmogorov-Smirnov, Lilliefors and Anderson-Darling tests. J. Stat. Model. Anal. 2011, 2, 21–33. [Google Scholar]
  38. Chambers, J.M. Graphical Methods for Data Analysis; Wadsworth International Group: Belmont, CA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  39. Fowlkes, E.B. A Folio of Distributions: A Collection of Theorical Quantite-Quantite Plots; M. Dekker: New York, NY, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  40. Benjamini, Y.; Hochberg, Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Methodol.) 1995, 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Hochberg, Y. A Sharper Bonferroni Procedure for Multiple Tests of Significance. Biometrika 1988, 75, 800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Fritz, C.O.; Morris, P.E.; Richler, J.J. Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2012, 141, 2–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Ryan, T.P. Sample Size Determination and Power; Wiley Series in Probability and Statistics; John Wiley & Sons Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  44. Berg, J.K. Report on the Behavioral Development of Three Captive Born African Elephant Calves. Elephant 1986, 2, 39–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Estes, R.D. The Behavior Guide to African Mammals: Including Hoofed Mammals, Carnivors, Primates; The University of California Press: Oxford, UK, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  46. Hörner, F.; Oerke, A.-K.; Müller, D.W.H.; Westerhüs, U.; Azogu-Sepe, I.; Hruby, J.; Preisfeld, G. Monitoring Behaviour in African Elephants during Introduction into a New Group: Differences between Related and Unrelated Animals. Animals 2021, 11, 2990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Hoerner, F.; Lawrenz, A.; Oerke, A.K.; Müller, D.W.; Azogu-Sepe, I.; Roller, M.; Damerau, K.; Preisfeld, A. Long-Term Olfactory Memory in African Elephants. Animals 2023, 13, 679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Lee, P.C. Early social development among African elephant calves. Natl. Geogr. Res. 1986, 2, 388–400. [Google Scholar]
  49. Kowalski, N.L.; Dale, R.H.I.; Mazur, C.L.H. A survey of the management and development of captive African elephant (Loxodonta africana) calves: Birth to three months of age. Zoo Biol. 2010, 29, 104–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Lundberg, U.; Szdzuy, K.; König, I. Beobachtungen zur Verhaltensontogenese Afrikanischer Elefanten (Loxodonta africana) im Tierpark Berlin-Friedrichsfelde. Milu Berlin 2001, 10, 392–408. [Google Scholar]
  51. Schulte, B.A. Social structure and helping behavior in captive elephants. Zoo Biol. 2000, 19, 447–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Olson, D. Elephant Husbandry Resource Guide; Allen Press: Lawrence, KS, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  53. VEASEY, J. Concepts in the care and welfare of captive elephants. Int. Zoo Yearb. 2006, 40, 63–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Webber, C.E.; Lee, P.C. Play in Elephants: Wellbeing, Welfare or Distraction? Animals 2020, 10, 305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Douglas-Hamilton, I. On the Ecology and Behaviour of the African Elephant. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK, 1972. [Google Scholar]
  56. Pinter-Wollman, N.; Isbell, L.A.; Hart, L.A. The relationship between social behaviour and habitat familiarity in African elephants (Loxodonta africana). Proc. Biol. Sci. 2009, 276, 1009–1014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kiley-Worthington, M.A. Comparative Study of Equine and Elephant Mental Attributes Leading to an Acceptance of Their Subjectivity & Consciousness. J. Anim. Res. Vet. Sci. 2019, 3, 119–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Percentage of time calves spend in a distance category to mother, depending on generation/environment.
Figure 1. Percentage of time calves spend in a distance category to mother, depending on generation/environment.
Animals 13 03051 g001
Figure 2. Percentage of time calves showed a certain behavior, depending on generation/environment. ° = outlier, * = extreme outlier.
Figure 2. Percentage of time calves showed a certain behavior, depending on generation/environment. ° = outlier, * = extreme outlier.
Animals 13 03051 g002
Figure 3. Percentage of affiliative and agonistic behavior initiated and received by calves, depending on generation/environment. * = extreme outlier.
Figure 3. Percentage of affiliative and agonistic behavior initiated and received by calves, depending on generation/environment. * = extreme outlier.
Animals 13 03051 g003
Table 1. Developmental stages of elephant calves.
Table 1. Developmental stages of elephant calves.
AgeDevelopment
0–6 months
-
Learn how to walk stable
-
Learn how to use the trunk for suckling
7–12 months
-
Learn how to use the trunk for foraging
-
Main nutrition shift from milk to solid food
13–24 months
-
First decrease in contact with mothers
-
Pick up on playing behavior
25–36 months
-
Start to show more agonistic behavior, which is relevant for learning to compete in rivalries
-
Increase in gender-specific differences
-
Peak in playing behavior (way of learning social behavior)
37–48 months
-
Classified as youngsters
-
Gender-specific differences in social behavior become more significant
49–60 months
-
Social play (that is, among other functions, intended to prepare youngsters for breeding behavior), such as climbing and chasing others, increases significantly
60-plus months
-
Classified as young adults
-
Males and females start to become fertile in situ, even though it still takes several years for them (especially the males) to mate successfully
-
Behavior shifts even stronger toward behavior related to breeding
-
Young bulls might already have left the natal family to socialize with bachelor groups
Table 2. List of elephants.
Table 2. List of elephants.
Generation/OriginElephantSexYear of BirthAge at Data CollectionNumber of PlaymatesTotal Number
F1/ex situTsM20206 months38
KuM20216 months3
GuM201912 months3
TuF201624 months3
JoM201436 months3
MajF201748 months4
ChF201748 months3
SaF201748 months3
F2/ex situKiF20206 months48
NeM20216 months1
ElF201912 months2
MarF201930 months2
ToraM201836 months4
ToriM201842 months4
TaF201648 months2
AyM201648 months1
in situ-M & F-3–48 months2–13~90
Table 3. Ethogram for the data collection on the general behavior of calves (extracted from Poole & Granli [26,27]).
Table 3. Ethogram for the data collection on the general behavior of calves (extracted from Poole & Granli [26,27]).
LabelBehavior
EatEating food using the trunk
DrinkDrinking water using the trunk
SuckleSuckling milk from the mother’s breast
WalkWalking at a slow pace, no more than one step per s, with the purpose to go somewhere
RunRunning in an enhanced space, more than one step per s, to run away from something or get somewhere fast for safety
Trunk movementMoving either the tip of the trunk or the whole trunk for practice or to search the ground for food/objects
WashingWashing the body with mud/water, sand bathing, rubbing the body on something to clean the skin, and protecting from mosquitos and sun
SleepSleeping or resting in a lying or standing position with the eyes closed
Social playPlaying with one or more other individuals
Lone playPlaying individually with oneself or an object
Affiliative behaviorBehaving positively with other individuals (e.g., touching with the trunk, helping behavior)
Agonistic behaviorBehaving negatively with other individuals (e.g., pushing with trunk, tusk, or body)
EscapingRunning from something while showing signs of fear (screaming, low tail, head high)
Seeking rescueRunning towards other individuals in fear (e.g., screaming, low tail, head high) and hiding under/behind them for protection
RescuingStanding over/in front of other individuals for protection, after that individual ran towards them to seek rescue (see above)
ThreateningPacing towards something, head, trunk, and ears high, sometimes trumpeting
Table 4. Kruskal–Wallis calculation and Post hoc test for the position of calves to their mothers, depending on generation/environment.
Table 4. Kruskal–Wallis calculation and Post hoc test for the position of calves to their mothers, depending on generation/environment.
Kruskal–WallisTactile<1 m1–3 m3–5 m>5 m
Kruskal–Wallis H15.4950.22215.08213.11313.995
df22222
Asymp. Sig.<0.0010.895<.0010.001<0.001
Monte Carlo Sig.Sig.<0.0010.897<.001<0.001<0.001
99% Confidence IntervalLower Bound0.0000.8890.0000.0000.000
Upper Bound0.0000.9050.0000.0010.001
Post hoc
Sig.F1/F20.2240.6510.8190.8870.570
F1/in situ<0.0010.895<0.0010.002<0.001
F2/in situ0.0070.7400.0010.0020.003
Table 5. Kruskal–Wallis calculation and Post hoc test for the general behavior of calves, depending on generation/environment.
Table 5. Kruskal–Wallis calculation and Post hoc test for the general behavior of calves, depending on generation/environment.
Kruskal–WallisEatDrinkSuckleWalkRunTrunk Move.WashSleepSocial PlayLone PlayAffiliativeAgonisticEscapeSeek Resc.RescueThreat
MF139.7031.4592.26410.1051.41511.9243.3352.6796.4957.12112.4350.4530.0580.1640.1040.236
F235.130.6166.17910.3981.04814.561.9552.474.886.86914.9440.311250.0040.0880.00.778
in situ4.5667.7134.38.0320.5361.9398.4313.2067.8842.22548.220.130.2251.2730.6060.653
Kruskal–Wallis H12.02614.4433.8790.9452.72014.4956.3351.1651.5650.66515.4053.5692.6584.6483.0631.682
df2222222222222222
Asymp. Sig.0.002<0.0010.1440.6230.257<0.0010.0420.5590.4570.717<0.0010.1680.2650.0980.2160.431
M.C. Sig.Sig.0.001<0.0010.1470.6470.267<0.0010.0390.5800.4740.733<0.0010.1560.2790.1010.3130.447
99% C.I.L.B.0.0000.0000.1380.6350.2560.0000.0340.5670.4610.7220.0000.1470.2670.0930.3010.434
U.B.0.0020.0000.1560.6600.2780.0000.0430.5930.4870.7440.0000.1650.2900.1090.3250.460
Post hoc
Sig.F1/F20.6200.3390.0490.7500.3270.7770.8600.4470.4370.6460.8320.2140.1030.9210.1000.269
F1/in situ0.0010.0070.2990.3400.1010.0020.0370.7770.6460.724<0.0010.0640.4540.0560.7650.254
F2/in situ0.006<0.0010.3520.5250.510<0.0010.0240.2970.2160.4160.0010.5410.3790.0690.1790.972
Table 6. Kruskal–Wallis calculation and Post hoc test for agonistic and affiliative behavior as initiator and recipient for calves, depending on generation/environment.
Table 6. Kruskal–Wallis calculation and Post hoc test for agonistic and affiliative behavior as initiator and recipient for calves, depending on generation/environment.
Kruskal–WallisAffiliative InitiatorAffiliative RecipientAgonistic InitiatorAgonistic Recipient
Kruskal–Wallis H6.1056.1057.5817.581
df2222
Asymp. Sig.0.0460.0440.0180.019
Monte Carlo Sig.Sig.0.0460.0440.0180.019
99% Confidence IntervalLower Bound0.0410.0390.0150.016
Upper Bound0.0510.0490.0220.023
Post hoc
Sig.F1/F20.6490.5760.6490.576
F1/in situ0.0220.0100.0220.010
F2/in situ0.0500.0370.0500.037
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hoerner, F.; Rendle-Worthington, J.; Lawrenz, A.; Oerke, A.-K.; Damerau, K.; Borragán Santos, S.; Hard, T.; Preisfeld, G. Differences in Mother–Infant Bond and Social Behavior of African Elephant Calves Living In Situ and Ex Situ. Animals 2023, 13, 3051. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13193051

AMA Style

Hoerner F, Rendle-Worthington J, Lawrenz A, Oerke A-K, Damerau K, Borragán Santos S, Hard T, Preisfeld G. Differences in Mother–Infant Bond and Social Behavior of African Elephant Calves Living In Situ and Ex Situ. Animals. 2023; 13(19):3051. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13193051

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hoerner, Franziska, Jake Rendle-Worthington, Arne Lawrenz, Ann-Kathrin Oerke, Karsten Damerau, Santiago Borragán Santos, Therese Hard, and Gela Preisfeld. 2023. "Differences in Mother–Infant Bond and Social Behavior of African Elephant Calves Living In Situ and Ex Situ" Animals 13, no. 19: 3051. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13193051

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop