Next Article in Journal
Effects of Exercise Combined with Undenatured Type II Collagen on Endurance Capacity, Antioxidant Status, Muscle Lipogenic Genes and E3 Ubiquitin Ligases in Rats
Previous Article in Journal
Effects on Synchronization and Reproductive Efficiency of Delaying the Removal of the Intravaginal Progesterone Device by 24 h in the 5d Co-Synch Protocol in Heifers
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Activity Time Budgets—A Potential Tool to Monitor Equine Welfare?

1
Anaesthesiology and Perioperative Intensive Care Medicine Unit, Department of Companion Animals and Horses, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Veterinaerplatz 1, 1210 Vienna, Austria
2
Equine Surgery Unit, University Equine Hospital, Department of Companion Animals and Horses, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Veterinaerplatz 1, 1210 Vienna, Austria
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2021, 11(3), 850; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11030850
Submission received: 25 February 2021 / Revised: 14 March 2021 / Accepted: 15 March 2021 / Published: 17 March 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Equids)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Horses’ behavior is a good indicator of their welfare status. However, its complexity requires objective, quantifiable, and unambiguous evidence-based assessment criteria. As healthy, stress-free horses exhibit a highly repetitive daily routine, horses’ time budget (amount of time in a 24 h period spent on specific activities) can assist in equine welfare assessment. A systematic review of the literature yielded 12 papers that assessed equine time budgets for eating, resting and movement for a minimum of 24 continuous hours. A total of 144 horses (1–27 years old), 59 semi-feral and 85 domesticated horses, are included in this review. The reported 24 h time budgets for eating ranged from 10% to 66.6%, for resting from 8.1% to 66%, for lying from 2.7% to 27.3%, and for movement from 0.015% to 19.1%. The large variance in time budgets between studies can largely be attributed to differences in age and environmental conditions. Management interventions (free access to food, increased space, decreased population density) in domesticated horses yielded time budgets similar to semi-feral horses. The data support the importance of environmental conditions for horses’ well-being and the ability of time budgets to assist in monitoring horses’ welfare.

Abstract

Horses’ behavior can provide valuable insight into their subjective state and is thus a good indicator of welfare. However, its complexity requires objective, quantifiable, and unambiguous evidence-based assessment criteria. As healthy, stress-free horses exhibit a highly repetitive daily routine, temporal quantification of their behavioral activities (time budget analysis) can assist in equine welfare assessment. Therefore, the present systematic review aimed to provide an up-to-date analysis of equine time budget studies. A review of the literature yielded 12 papers that fulfilled the inclusion criteria: assessment of equine time budgets for eating, resting and movement for a minimum of 24 continuous hours. A total of 144 horses (1–27 years old), 59 semi-feral and 85 domesticated horses, are included in this review. The 24 h time budgets for foraging or eating (10–6.6%), resting (8.1–66%), lying (2.7–27.3%), and locomotion (0.015–19.1%) showed large variance between studies, which can largely be attributed to differences in age and environmental conditions. Management interventions in domesticated horses (ad libitum access to food, increased space, decreased population density) resulted in time budgets similar to their (semi-)feral conspecifics, emphasizing the importance of environmental conditions and the ability of time budgets to assist in monitoring horses’ welfare.

1. Introduction

Animal welfare is a multifaceted, continuously evolving concept at the interface of science and society, influenced by ethical, economic, cultural, and political concerns [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Its multidimensionality, both from a scientific and societal perspective, requires objective, quantifiable, and unambiguous evidence-based parameters to assess animal welfare and inform guidelines and policies [4,5,6,7,8,9,10].
As welfare is a subjective individual experience, the animal welfare scientific community has moved progressively from input or resource-based measures, such as housing type or the amount of food provided to the animal, toward more output or animal-based assessments that not only evaluate the quality of the environment in which an animal is kept but also its physical and psychological condition and its ability to express the full repertoire of species-appropriate behaviors [6,9,11]. Therefore, behavior is increasingly used as an indicator of welfare because an animal’s behavior can provide valuable insight into its subjective state [6,12,13]. The behavior of horses is defined by their niche as a prey species and highly social grassland dwellers with strong group fidelity who, in their natural habitat, are continual grazers with ultradian activity patterns roaming areas of land up to 78 km2 [14,15,16,17]. Healthy, stress-free horses divide their time between activities that allow them to satisfy their basic requirements of food, movement, and rest, and exhibit a highly repetitive, individual, daily routine with almost identical time patterns of behavior from day to day [15,18,19]. Accordingly, the amount of time an animal engages in behavioral activities (time budget) is considered a very informative welfare indicator [6,9,15,20,21,22,23,24]. To this end, domesticated horses are often compared to feral or wild conspecifics. Thereby, feral horses can be used as a benchmark for comparison, not as gold standard for optimum welfare [6,9,15,20,21,22,23,24]. Indeed, domestic horses, given the opportunity to display species-appropriate behavior and an environment sufficiently reflecting a natural habitat, display time budgets similar to those of wild horses [4,15,23,25]. However, domestic horses are kept in a variety of housing systems that offer more or less adequate environmental conditions with differing levels of physical freedom, often with regimental feeding and limited foraging and social opportunities [14,15]. Thus, differences in the time budgets of domesticated horses compared to feral or wild conspecifics are currently used as a measure of compromised welfare [6,15,23,24,26,27].
As equine behavioral activity displays 24 h and circadian variation, measuring time budgets requires detailed surveillance over several days [9,17,28,29]. Traditionally, this has been done by direct observation which was prone to observer bias, often limited to daylight hours and too time and resource-intensive to be feasible for welfare assessment. However, recent advances in biotelemetry and artificial intelligence (AI) based on sensor (mostly accelerometer) or video analysis provide increased objectivity and enable remote recording of behavioral data, longer observation periods, and quantification of behaviors with higher accuracy and temporal resolution than the human eye allows [30,31,32]. Hence, time budget analysis, as an objective, quantitative measure of behavior, now has the potential to become a useful, reliable tool for on-farm assessment of equine welfare and comparison of welfare under different environmental conditions. Therefore, the present review aimed to provide an up-to-date analysis of equine time budget studies and synthesize evidence relating to the effect of housing and management systems on horses’ behavioral time budgets.

2. Materials and Methods

This review was carried out in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [33].

2.1. Data Sources and Searches

Scientific peer-reviewed articles were identified through a systematic search in the PubMed (National Institutes of Health. PubMed (Database). Bethesda, MD, USA: National Library of Medicine; https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov, accessed on 25 February 2021) and Scopus (Elsevier, Amsterdam, The Netherlands; https://www.scopus.com, accessed on 25 February 2021) electronic databases searching for the terms “((“time budget” OR “activity budget” OR “activity tracking”) OR ((sleep OR sleeping OR resting OR lying OR eating OR foraging) AND (behaviour OR behavior OR time OR video OR sensor OR gyroscope OR accelerometer))) AND (horse OR equine OR pony OR horses OR ponies OR “equus caballus”)“ in title or abstract, with no restriction on publication date, in November 2020. The study selection process was carried out by the first and last author following the procedure detailed in Figure 1, excluding papers that upon closer inspection, did not study the time horses spend on specific activities or did not include a minimum of 24 h uninterrupted observations. Any disagreement between the authors on the studies to be included in the review was resolved during a consensus meeting.
All quantitative or qualitative randomized controlled trials, observational studies, and case series focused on equine time budgets or activity quantification written in English or German were included. The following exclusion criteria were set: (a) non-peer-reviewed publication, (b) conference/seminar abstract only published, (c) dissertation, thesis, review, commentary, or single case report, (d) the article was not written in English or German.

2.2. Data Extraction and Risk of Bias Assessment

Information on the population, intervention, comparison, outcome, and study design (PICOS) was retrieved from the articles, and the risk of bias of selected studies was assessed using a modification of the Evidence Project risk-of-bias tool [34,35,36].

3. Results

A total of 286 articles were identified in PubMed, 723 additional papers in Scopus, and another 66 based on references, yielding a total of 1075 articles. After removing duplicates, reviews, commentaries, single case reports, books and non-English or German articles, 646 papers remained. Following the exclusion of papers that did not focus on equine activity tracking or time budgets, but on different species or other behavioral observations or used an observation period of less than 24 h per day, 12 articles remained included in the qualitative synthesis [18,28,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46]. A total of 6 (50% of the total) articles were classified as observational studies [18,28,37,38,39,45], 5 articles (41.6% of the total) as prospective, non-blinded, non-randomized case series [41,42,43,44,46], 1 article (8%) as prospective, non-blinded, non-randomized controlled trial [40] and none as randomized controlled trials (Table 1).
Risk-of-bias assessment (Table 1) revealed the lack of a control or comparison group (only 8.3% of the articles fulfilled this criterion), random assignment of participants to intervention (8.3% of the articles fulfilled this criterion), random selection of participants for assessment (none of the articles fulfilled this criterion), as the most critical concerns. Further limitations of the papers included in this review are the small sample sizes (4–22 horses) and the variable observation methods which were restricted to manual observation in the field in 33% [18,37,38,39] and manual behavior scoring from video in 50% [40,41,42,43,45,46]. Only 25% used biotelemetry devices (25%) [28,44,45]. Manual quantification of observed behavior was carried out in 83.3% (10/12 studies) of the studies using scan sampling in 33% (4/12 studies) [18,37,38,46], focal sampling [39,43], and instantaneous sampling in 16.6% (2/12 studies) [40,41] each, and ad libitum sampling [45] and unspecified methodology [42] in one paper. In addition, the ethogram, based on which behaviors were categorized, was not provided in 25% [39,41,42], and the video analysis software was not detailed in 41.6 % [40,42,44,45,46] of the articles. Data analysis was limited to descriptive statistics in the observational studies [18,28,37,38,39,45] and the other 50% of the included papers detailed further statistics [40,41,42,43,44,46]. The large variance in study methodology and data presentation did not allow combining the data for meta-analysis.
A total of 144 horses are included in the present systematic review comprising ages between 1 and 27 years (Table 2) [18,28,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46]. Three papers (25% of the total) studied free-ranging (300–335 ha area) (semi-)feral horses [18,37,38], two (17% of the total) Przewalski horses that were bred in zoos and living in a 44 ha semi-reserve [28,39], and seven (58% of the total) domesticated horses [40,41,42,43,44,45,46]. All studies determined the 24 h time budgets for foraging or eating (10–66.6%), resting (8.1–66%) and locomotion (0.015–19.1%), but only 9 (75% of the total) specifically quantified lying (2.7–27.3%) (Table 3) [18,37,38,39,40,42,43,45,46]. Two of the three studies on free-ranging horses [18,37] were focused on the effect of age and sex on time budgets and one on environmental and seasonal influences [38].
The time budget of free-ranging horses was divided between 13–66.6% eating or foraging (weaned, grazing horses: 50.82–66.6%), 8.1–29.3% resting, 2.7–15.5% lying, and 4.3–13.4% locomotion [18,37,38]. Age had a significant effect on the time budgets of free-ranging horses, with foals until weaning spending significantly more time sleeping while lying flat, while expectedly, after weaning, the time spent foraging increased [18]. Stallions spent more time standing alert and moving rapidly but less time foraging than mares [37].
Przewalski horses kept under more confined conditions (44 ha semi-reserve) dedicated 29.8–46.4% of their time budget to eating or foraging, 36.4–36.6% to resting, 5.3% to lying, 7.4% to locomotion, and 10.2% to other behaviors such as drinking, self-grooming, and playing (Table 3) [28,39]. Behavioral analysis by automated tracking revealed a complex diurnal and ultradian rhythmicity and seasonal variations of activity patterns [28].
Four of the seven studies on domesticated horses [41,42,43,45] studied the effect of different feeding regimes on equine time budgets, and one study evaluated the effect of postoperative pain [40], stocking density [46] and turn-out management together with paddock size [44]. The time budget of domesticated horses was divided between 10–64% eating or foraging, 15.6–68% resting, 3–27.3% lying, 0.015–19.3% (in horses not confined to a stable: 2.5–19.3%) locomotion and 2–11.5% other behaviors such as drinking and self-grooming (Table 3) [40,41,42,43,44,45,46]. The large variance in activity time budgets can in part be explained by the age range of the horses included in the various studies, as young horses are resting more in a recumbent position than adults [43,46].
The feeding regime significantly affected the time budget of horses [41,45]. Regular feeding times and coordinated rations led to a significant reduction in the time spent on feeding. Offering the hay ration in hay bags increased the time spent feeding but to a lesser extent than ad libitum feeding [41,45]. The diurnal and ultradian rhythmicity of feeding of wild and feral horses could also be observed in domestic horses with more time spent feeding during daytime [42,44]. Paddock size and stocking density had no influence on feeding time but affected time for resting and locomotion with a decreased stocking density showing a positive correlation with locomotion, playing, and self-grooming [46] and a smaller paddock size being associated with decreased locomotion time [44].

4. Discussion

Aiming to provide an up-to-date analysis of equine time budget studies, this review included all studies that determined equine time budgets for eating, resting and locomotion over an observation period of at least 24 h [18,28,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46]. The reported time budgets of (semi-)feral and domesticated horses show large variances (Table 3). In (semi-)feral horses, the variation in time budgets was primarily attributed to age [18], sex [37] and seasonal influences with correspondingly variable food availability, temperatures, and insect pest densities [28,38]. At this point, it is important to take into consideration that the papers included in this review studied domesticated and semi-feral but not wild horses and hence may not entirely reflect the natural behavior of their wild ancestors.
The differences in the average time budgets between (semi-)feral and domesticated adult horses were especially evident for eating (semi-feral: 50.82–66.6% versus domesticated: 10–64%) and resting (semi-feral: 12.9–29.3% versus domesticated: 15.6–66%, Table 3) [18,28,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46]. However, while the limitations of feeding opportunities and feed availability have different causes in wild, (semi-)feral and domesticated horses, offering ad libitum food to domesticated horses increased their time spent eating to levels similar to their (semi-)feral conspecifics (Table 3) [41,42,45], highlighting the importance of management interventions for equine welfare. Furthermore, horses kept in small paddocks or densely populated group pens exhibited significantly increased resting times (Table 3) [44,46]. Decreasing stocking density reduced the high resting times to levels approximating the time budgets seen in other studies (Table 3) and increased locomotion, playing, and self-grooming [46]. This supports the utility of time budgets for monitoring interventions aimed at improving horses’ welfare.

4.1. Time Budget Measurement: Methods and Use

Unfortunately, even though Berger et al. already used a telemetric system in 1999 to monitor a group of horses over one year [28], many studies included in this review have not made optimal use of available technology (Table 1). Instead, they used direct observation or video recordings with scan or focal sampling which does not provide a continuous assessment of behavior and thus potentially misses or underrepresents important, infrequent behaviors. Using automated tracking methods instead of visual observation reduces observer bias, improves data resolution, and allows long-term tracking of larger group sizes. This may help to reveal behavioral patterns over more extended periods and determine the influence of environmental conditions on time budgets. Currently, most biotelemetry systems are not yet able to differentiate reliably between different gaits (walk, trot, canter) and between walking and static movements (e.g., stamping, twitching to ward off pests) and thus may determine erroneous gait patterns or too high movement values [32,47]. However, automated tracking methods have been validated in other species (seals, goats, pigs, birds) [30,31,48,49,50] and show great promise for application in equine studies [28,44].
The time budgets of several welfare relevant behaviors, such as foraging, resting, and lying, can already be accurately determined with commercially available sensors and can be used as welfare indicators to identify welfare problems and monitor the success of interventions [32,44]. Furthermore, real-time analysis of equine behavior may also facilitate early detection of health problems, such as colic, lameness or other painful conditions and accelerate therapeutic interventions [4,40,51,52,53,54,55]. Indeed, time budgets for specific behavior have been identified as more sensitive indicators of equine discomfort than repeated direct observation of specific events and postures and thus could facilitate rapid detection of painful conditions and objective, quantitative monitoring of the success of therapeutic interventions [40,54,55].

4.2. Time Budget for Feeding and Foraging

Horses’ digestive physiology and anatomy have adapted to their natural diet that is rich in fiber and low in starch and energy [27,54,56]. Free-ranging horses devote the majority of their time to the search for and consumption of food, spending up to 18 h a day foraging, and rarely fast voluntarily for more than 2 to 4 h at a time [27,57]. Thus, the time budget for foraging in grazing (semi-)feral horses ranges from 50.82% to 66.6%, with circadian and seasonal variations depending on climatic conditions and food quality but also differences between daylight and darkness (Table 3) [18,28,37,38,58]. However, many domestic horses have limited access to roughage and are fed restricted amounts of hay and a commercial feed with higher caloric density. Thus, the determined time budget for foraging in the adult domestic horse varies greatly from 16% with rationed feeding [45] to 64% with ad libitum access to hay (Table 3) [41]. One of the papers included in this systematic review also found postoperative pain to reduce feeding time [40] but did not separate the effects of pain from those of general anesthesia. In contrast, another recent study investigating the influence of an iatrogenically-induced acute septic osteoarthritis on behavior found no effect of pain on feeding behavior [50].
Feeding time budgets are relevant for equine welfare because the reduction of the time spent foraging may induce health problems such as gastric inflammation and ulceration. Insufficient eating times have also been associated with the emergence of stereotypes and abnormal behavior, such as increased time spent active walking [6,27,54,57,59,60,61,62]. In contrast, management interventions providing increased foraging opportunities have shown to decrease abnormal behavior [24,59,61,63,64,65,66] and yield time budgets for eating analogous to their wild conspecifics [41,42,45], further confirming that reduced opportunities for foraging may be a source of stress and poor welfare for domestic horses [27,41,45,54,59,60,61,62,66,67,68,69,70,71].

4.3. Time Budget for Resting and Sleeping

Adult feral horses spend 12.9–29.3% of their day resting standing and 4.2–15.5% lying in a polyphasic pattern in multiple shorter periods (Table 3) [37,38,72]. However, the time budget for resting, which includes periods of inactivity and sleep, cannot be compared beyond doubt between studies, as there is disagreement whether vigilant standing is part of the resting behavior or activity behavior or forms an independent behavior category. Resting behavior can occur while standing as well as lying down in sternal or lateral recumbency and is age-dependent. Adult horses spend 80% of their resting time standing and only a relatively small proportion of their 24 h time budget recumbent, while foals under three months of age lie down for 70–80% of their resting time [18,37,38,52,73,74]. Young domestic horses are lying down approximately 25% of the day [43,46], which is significantly more than adults that are recumbent for approximately 5% of the day (Table 3) [40,42]. Domesticated horses, analogous to wild horses, lie down mostly between midnight and four in the morning [39,46].
Measuring lying behavior is an essential component of equine welfare assessment because horses usually fall asleep shortly after lying down; thus, recumbency is a reliable indicator of sleep. Four stages of horses’ sleep-wake rhythm are typically differentiated and defined by specific cortical electronic activity and movement patterns: wakefulness, drowsiness, slow-wave-sleep, and paradoxical or rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, with the majority of sleep occurring between midnight and 5:00 am [39,44,46,60,72,75,76]. While sleep in horses is not uniquely associated with recumbency as horses can sleep standing, recumbency (sternal or lateral) is required for rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, which is vital for many physiological and cognitive functions [53,72]. Indeed, in other species including humans, REM-sleep deprivation has been linked to hyperalgesia and persistent chronic pain [53,77,78].
In horses, decreased lying time budgets were associated with inappropriate environmental conditions, stress, and painful musculoskeletal issues [4,40,50,52,53]. In contrast, increased lying times were observed in animals with higher social rank [79], larger stall size [80], straw bedding compared to shavings [81], and following administration of analgesics to horses suffering from orthopedic pain [4,52,53].

4.4. Time Budget for Locomotion and Movement

The time budget dedicated to movement or activity ranges from 4.3% to 13.4% in feral horses, with walk as the predominant gait and less movement at night than during the day (Table 3) [37,38,39]. In stallions, a greater need for rapid movement (trot, canter) has been reported [37]. The time budget for movement also includes, in addition to locomotion in the different gaits, play behavior which is most commonly observed in foals, and stereotypical movements such as weaving, crib-biting, and stall-walking. Horses with insufficient foraging opportunities and horses living in too densely stocked conditions show increased active locomotion patterns (Table 3) [4,26,27,42,45], confirming the value of locomotor activity as reliable indicators for horse welfare [82]. As horses also move while foraging, albeit slowly and less linearly, a clear distinction between foraging and movement behavior is required, but unfortunately this was not evident in all studies; thus, the time budgets for movement have to be interpreted with caution [9,15].

5. Conclusions

Activity time budgets allow an objective, quantitative on-farm welfare assessment and comparison of different management, feeding, and housing systems. In addition, changes in time budgets can be used to identify painful conditions and monitor the success of management interventions to improve equine welfare. However, the diversity of the horse populations, the environment the horses were living in, and the measurement methods leads to a great heterogeneity of the studies and is reflected in the highly variable time budgets. Thus, further studies of larger horse groups that live in clearly defined housing and management conditions, using modern observational technologies, such as biotelemetry or AI-based video analysis, are needed to further validate and establish the use of time budgets as a reliable indicator of equine welfare.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: U.A. and F.J.; methodology, literature search and analysis: U.A., V.E., F.J.; risk of bias assessment: U.A. and Z.K.; writing—original draft preparation: U.A., F.J.; writing—review and editing: U.A., Z.K., F.J.; All authors have read and agreed to the submitted version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statements

Data is contained within the article.

Acknowledgments

Open Access Funding by the University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Fraser, D. Understanding animal welfare. Acta Veter Scand. 2008, 50, S1–S7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Ohl, F.; Van Der Staay, F. Animal welfare: At the interface between science and society. Veter J. 2012, 192, 13–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Green, T.C.; Mellor, D.J. Extending ideas about animal welfare assessment to include ‘quality of life’ and related concepts. N. Z. Veter J. 2011, 59, 263–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Lesimple, C. Indicators of Horse Welfare: State-of-the-Art. Animals 2020, 10, 294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Lesimple, C.; Reverchon-Billot, L.; Galloux, P.; Stomp, M.; Boichot, L.; Coste, C.; Henry, S.; Hausberger, M. Free movement: A key for welfare improvement in sport horses? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2020, 225, 104972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Hausberger, M.; Lerch, N.; Guilbaud, E.; Stomp, M.; Grandgeorge, M.; Henry, S.; Lesimple, C. On-Farm Welfare Assessment of Horses: The Risks of Putting the Cart before the Horse. Animals 2020, 10, 371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Broom, D.M. Animal Welfare: The Concept of the Issues. In Attitudes to Animals; Dollins, F., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999; pp. 129–142. ISBN 9780511608476. [Google Scholar]
  8. McGreevy, P.; Berger, J.; De Brauwere, N.; Doherty, O.; Harrison, A.; Fiedler, J.; Jones, C.; McDonnell, S.; McLean, A.; Nakonechny, L.; et al. Using the Five Domains Model to Assess the Adverse Impacts of Husbandry, Veterinary, and Equitation Interventions on Horse Welfare. Animals 2018, 8, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Wolfensohn, S. Too Cute to Kill? The Need for Objective Measurements of Quality of Life. Animals 2020, 10, 1054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lesimple, C.; Gautier, E.; Benhajali, H.; Rochais, C.; Lunel, C.; Bensaïd, S.; Khalloufi, A.; Henry, S.; Hausberger, M. Stall architecture influences horses’ behaviour and the prevalence and type of stereotypies. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 219, 104833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Waran, N.; Randle, H. What we can measure, we can manage: The importance of using robust welfare indicators in Equitation Science. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 190, 74–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Asher, L.; Collins, L.M.; Ortiz-Pelaez, A.; Drewe, J.A.; Nicol, C.J.; Pfeiffer, D.U. Recent advances in the analysis of behavioural organization and interpretation as indicators of animal welfare. J. R. Soc. Interface 2009, 6, 1103–1119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Hockenhull, J.; Whay, H.R. A review of approaches to assessing equine welfare. Equine Veter Educ. 2014, 26, 159–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Goodwin, D. The importance of ethology in understanding the behaviour of the horse. Equine Veter J. 2010, 31, 15–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Yarnell, K.; Hall, C.; Royle, C.; Walker, S.L. Domesticated horses differ in their behavioural and physiological responses to isolated and group housing. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 143, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Green, N.F.; Green, H.D. The Wild Horse Population of Stone Cabin Valley, Nevada: A Preliminary Report. In Proceedings of the National Wild Horse Forum, Reno, NV, USA, 5–7 April 1977. [Google Scholar]
  17. Murphy, B.A. Circadian and Circannual Regulation in the Horse: Internal Timing in an Elite Athlete. J. Equine Veter Sci. 2019, 76, 14–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Boy, V.; Duncan, P. Time-Budgets of Camargue Horses I. Developmental Changes in the Time-Budgets of Foals. Behavior 1979, 71, 187–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Berger, A.; Scheibe, K.-M.; Michaelis, S.; Streich, W.J. Evaluation of living conditions of free-ranging animals by automated chronobiological analysis of behavior. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 2003, 35, 458–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Veasey, J.; Waran, N.; Young, R. On comparing the behaviour of zoo housed animals with wild conspecifics as a welfare indicator. Anim. Welf. 1996, 5, 13–24. [Google Scholar]
  21. Waran, N.K. Can studies of feral horse behaviour be used for assessing domestic horse welfare? Equine Veter J. 1997, 29, 249–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Yeates, J. Naturalness and Animal Welfare. Animals 2018, 8, 53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Flannigan, G.; Stookey, J. Day-time time budgets of pregnant mares housed in tie stalls: A comparison of draft versus light mares. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 125–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sarrafchi, A.; Blokhuis, H.J. Equine stereotypic behaviors: Causation, occurrence, and prevention. J. Veter Behav. 2013, 8, 386–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Heleski, C.; Shelle, A.; Nielsen, B.; Zanella, A. Influence of housing on weanling horse behavior and subsequent welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 291–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Benhajali, H.; Richard-Yris, M.-A.; Leroux, M.; Ezzaouïa, M.; Charfi, F.; Hausberger, M. A note on the time budget and social behaviour of densely housed horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 112, 196–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Benhajali, H.; Richard-Yris, M.-A.; Ezzaouia, M.; Charfi, F.; Hausberger, M. Foraging opportunity: A crucial criterion for horse welfare? Animals 2009, 3, 1308–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Berger, A.; Scheibe, K.-M.; Eichhorn, K.; Scheibe, A.; Streich, J. Diurnal and ultradian rhythms of behaviour in a mare group of Przewalski horse (Equus ferus przewalskii), measured through one year under semi-reserve conditions. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1999, 64, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Martin, A.-M.; Elliott, J.A.; Duffy, P.; Blake, C.M.; Ben Attia, S.; Katz, L.M.; Browne, J.A.; Gath, V.; McGivney, B.A.; Hill, E.W.; et al. Circadian regulation of locomotor activity and skeletal muscle gene expression in the horse. J. Appl. Physiol. 2010, 109, 1328–1336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Ladds, M.A.; Salton, M.; Hocking, D.P.; McIntosh, R.R.; Thompson, A.P.; Slip, D.J.; Harcourt, R.G. Using accelerometers to develop time-energy budgets of wild fur seals from captive surrogates. PeerJ 2018, 6, e5814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Lind, N.M.; Vinther, M.; Hemmingsen, R.P.; Hansen, A.K. Validation of a digital video tracking system for recording pig locomotor behaviour. J. Neurosci. Methods 2005, 143, 123–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Eerdekens, A.; Deruyck, M.; Fontaine, J.; Martens, L.; De Poorter, E.; Joseph, W. Automatic equine activity detection by convolutional neural networks using accelerometer data. Comput. Electron. Agric. 2020, 168, 105139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G.; Prisma Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Methley, A.M.; Campbell, S.; Chew-Graham, C.; McNally, R.; Cheraghi-Sohi, S. PICO, PICOS and SPIDER: A comparison study of specificity and sensitivity in three search tools for qualitative systematic reviews. BMC Health Serv. Res. 2014, 14, 579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Kennedy, C.E.; Fonner, V.A.; Armstrong, K.A.; Denison, J.A.; Yeh, P.T.; O’Reilly, K.R.; Sweat, M.D. The Evidence Project risk of bias tool: Assessing study rigor for both randomized and non-randomized intervention studies. Syst. Rev. 2019, 8, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Villafaina-Domínguez, B.; Collado-Mateo, D.; Merellano-Navarro, E.; Villafaina, S. Effects of Dog-Based Animal-Assisted Interventions in Prison Population: A Systematic Review. Animals 2020, 10, 2129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Duncan, P. Time-Budgets of Camargue Horses Ii. Time-Budgets of Adult Horses and Weaned Sub-Adults. Behavior 1980, 72, 26–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Duncan, P. Time-Budgets of Camargue Horses Iii. Environmental Influences. Behavior 1985, 92, 188–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Boyd, L.E.; Carbonaro, D.A.; Houpt, K.A. The 24-hour time budget of Przewalski horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1988, 21, 5–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Price, J.; Catriona, S.; Welsh, E.M.; Waran, N.K. Preliminary evaluation of a behaviour–based system for assessment of post–operative pain in horses following arthroscopic surgery. Veter Anaesth. Analg. 2003, 30, 124–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Elia, J.B.; Erb, H.N.; Houpt, K.A. Motivation for hay: Effects of a pelleted diet on behavior and physiology of horses. Physiol. Behav. 2010, 101, 623–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Aristizabal, F.; Nieto, J.; Yamout, S.; Snyder, J. The effect of a hay grid feeder on feed consumption and measurement of the gastric pH using an intragastric electrode device in horses: A preliminary report. Equine Veter J. 2013, 46, 484–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Sartori, C.; Guzzo, N.; Normando, S.; Bailoni, L.; Mantovani, R. Evaluation of behaviour in stabled draught horse foals fed diets with two protein levels. Animals 2017, 11, 147–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Maisonpierre, I.N.; Sutton, M.A.; Harris, P.; Menzies-Gow, N.; Weller, R.; Pfau, T. Accelerometer activity tracking in horses and the effect of pasture management on time budget. Equine Veter J. 2019, 51, 840–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Correa, M.G.; e Silva, C.F.R.; Dias, L.A.; Junior, S.D.S.R.; Thomes, F.R.; Lago, L.A.D.; Carvalho, A.D.M.; Faleiros, R.R. Welfare benefits after the implementation of slow-feeder hay bags for stabled horses. J. Veter Behav. 2020, 38, 61–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Raspa, F.; Tarantola, M.; Bergero, D.; Nery, J.; Visconti, A.; Mastrazzo, C.M.; Cavallini, D.; Valvassori, E.; Valle, E. Time-Budget of Horses Reared for Meat Production: Influence of Stocking Density on Behavioural Activities and Subsequent Welfare. Animals 2020, 10, 1334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Rose-Meierhöfer, S.; Klaer, S.; Ammon, C.; Brunsch, R.; Hoffmann, G. Activity Behavior of Horses Housed in Different Open Barn Systems. J. Equine Veter Sci. 2010, 30, 624–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Bonneau, M.; Vayssade, J.-A.; Troupe, W.; Arquet, R. Outdoor animal tracking combining neural network and time-lapse cameras. Comput. Electron. Agric. 2020, 168, 105150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Nathan, R.; Spiegel, O.; Fortmann-Roe, S.; Harel, R.; Wikelski, M.; Getz, W.M. Using tri-axial acceleration data to identify behavioral modes of free-ranging animals: General concepts and tools illustrated for griffon vultures. J. Exp. Biol. 2012, 215, 986–996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Egan, S.; Kearney, C.M.; Brama, P.A.; Parnell, A.C.; McGrath, D. Exploring stable-based behaviour and behaviour switching for the detection of bilateral pain in equines. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021, 235, 105214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pritchett, L.C.; Ulibarri, C.; Roberts, M.C.; Schneider, R.K.; Sellon, D.C. Identification of potential physiological and behavioral indicators of postoperative pain in horses after exploratory celiotomy for colic. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2003, 80, 31–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Dubois, C.; Zakrajsek, E.; Haley, D.B.; Merkies, K. Validation of triaxial accelerometers to measure the lying behaviour of adult domestic horses. Animals 2015, 9, 110–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Clothier, J.; Small, A.; Hinch, G.; Barwick, J.; Brown, W.Y. Using Movement Sensors to Assess Lying Time in Horses With and Without Angular Limb Deformities. J. Equine Veter Sci. 2019, 75, 55–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hausberger, M.; Fureix, C.; Lesimple, C. Detecting horses’ sickness: In search of visible signs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 175, 41–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Ashley, F.H.; Waterman-Pearson, A.E.; Whay, H.R. Behavioural assessment of pain in horses and donkeys: Application to clinical practice and future studies. Equine Veter J. 2010, 37, 565–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Houpt, K.A. Ingestive Behavior. Veter Clin. N. Am. Equine Pr. 1990, 6, 319–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Harris, P. Nutrition, behaviour and the role of supplements for calming horses: The veterinarian’s dilemma. Veter J. 2005, 170, 10–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Mayes, E.; Duncan, P. Temporal Patterns of Feeding Behaviour in Free-Ranging Horses. Behavior 1986, 96, 105–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Thorne, J.; Goodwin, D.; Kennedy, M.; Davidson, H.; Harris, P. Foraging enrichment for individually housed horses: Practicality and effects on behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005, 94, 149–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Ellis, A.D.; Fell, M.; Luck, K.; Gill, L.; Owen, H.; Briars, H.; Barfoot, C.; Harris, P. Effect of forage presentation on feed intake behaviour in stabled horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 165, 88–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Ellis, A.D.; Redgate, S.; Zinchenko, S.; Owen, H.; Barfoot, C.; Harris, P. The effect of presenting forage in multi-layered haynets and at multiple sites on night time budgets of stabled horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 171, 108–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Rochais, C.; Henry, S.; Hausberger, M. “Hay-bags” and “Slow feeders”: Testing their impact on horse behaviour and welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018, 198, 52–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Christie, J.L.; Hewson, C.J.; Riley, C.B.; McNiven, M.A.; Dohoo, I.R.; Bate, L.A. Management factors affecting stereotypies and body condition score in nonracing horses in Prince Edward Island. Can. Veter J. Rev. Veter Can. 2006, 47, 136–143. [Google Scholar]
  64. Hothersall, B.; Nicol, C. Role of Diet and Feeding in Normal and Stereotypic Behaviors in Horses. Veter Clin. N. Am. Equine Pr. 2009, 25, 167–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Hothersall, B.; Casey, R. Undesired behaviour in horses: A review of their development, prevention, management and association with welfare. Equine Veter Educ. 2011, 24, 479–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Hanis, F.; Chung, E.L.T.; Kamalludin, M.H.; Idrus, Z. The Influence of Stable Management and Feeding Practices on the Abnormal Behaviors among Stabled Horses in Malaysia. J. Equine Veter Sci. 2020, 94, 103230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Waters, A.J.; Nicol, C.J.; French, N.P. Factors influencing the development of stereotypic and redirected behaviours in young horses: Findings of a four year prospective epidemiological study. Equine Veter J. 2010, 34, 572–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Ninomiya, S.; Kusunose, R.; Sato, S.; Terada, M.; Sugawara, K. Effects of feeding methods on eating frustration in stabled horses. Anim. Sci. J. 2004, 75, 465–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Burla, J.-B.; Ostertag, A.; Patt, A.; Bachmann, I.; Hillmann, E. Effects of feeding management and group composition on agonistic behaviour of group-housed horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 176, 32–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Zupan, M.; Štuhec, I.; Jordan, D. The Effect of an Irregular Feeding Schedule on Equine Behavior. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2019, 23, 156–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Baumgartner, M.; Boisson, T.; Erhard, M.H.; Zeitler-Feicht, M.H. Common Feeding Practices Pose A Risk to the Welfare of Horses When Kept on Non-Edible Bedding. Animals 2020, 10, 411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Burla, J.-B.; Rufener, C.; Bachmann, I.; Gygax, L.; Patt, A.; Hillmann, E. Space Allowance of the Littered Area Affects Lying Behavior in Group-Housed Horses. Front. Veter Sci. 2017, 4, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Arnold, G. Comparison of the time budgets and circadian patterns of maintenance activities in sheep, cattle and horses grouped together. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1984, 13, 19–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Sweeting, M.P.; Houpt, C.; Houpt, K.A. Social Facilitation of Feeding and Time Budgets in Stabled Ponies. J. Anim. Sci. 1985, 60, 369–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Ogilvie-Graham, T. Time Budget Studies in Stalled Horses. 1994. Available online: https://era.ed.ac.uk/handle/1842/29924 (accessed on 2 January 2021).
  76. Greening, L.; Downing, J.; Amiouny, D.; Lekang, L.; McBride, S. The effect of altering routine husbandry factors on sleep duration and memory consolidation in the horse. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021, 236, 105229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Wei, H.; Zhao, W.; Wang, Y.-X.; Pertovaara, A. Pain-related behavior following REM sleep deprivation in the rat: Influence of peripheral nerve injury, spinal glutamatergic receptors and nitric oxide. Brain Res. 2007, 1148, 105–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. May, M.E.; Harvey, M.T.; Valdovinos, M.G.; Kline, R.H.; Wiley, R.G.; Kennedy, C.H. Nociceptor and age specific effects of REM sleep deprivation induced hyperalgesia. Behav. Brain Res. 2005, 159, 89–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Fader, C.; Sambraus, H. The Resting Behaviour of Horses in Loose Housing Systems. Tierärztliche Umsch. 2004, 59, 320–327. [Google Scholar]
  80. Raabymagle, P.; Ladewig, J. Lying behavior in horses in relation to box size. J. Equine Veter Sci. 2006, 26, 11–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Pedersen, G.R.; Søndergaard, E.; Ladewig, J. The influence of bedding on the time horses spend recumbent. J. Equine Veter Sci. 2004, 24, 153–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Burla, J.-B.; Ostertag, A.; Westerath, H.S.; Hillmann, E. Gait determination and activity measurement in horses using an accelerometer. Comput. Electron. Agric. 2014, 102, 127–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow chart illustrating the selection of studies included in the review.
Figure 1. Flow chart illustrating the selection of studies included in the review.
Animals 11 00850 g001
Table 1. List of the included articles, their study design, observation method, the observation method(s), and the numbers of observation days.
Table 1. List of the included articles, their study design, observation method, the observation method(s), and the numbers of observation days.
Author(s) and
Publication Year
Study DesignControl GroupInterventionObservation Method(s)Observation: Number of Days
Boy & Duncan 1979 [18]Observational study (field)nonomanual, scan sampling 5-min blocks24 h for two day in two weeks each month
Duncan 1980 [37]Observational study (field)nonomanual, scan sampling 5-min blocks24 h for two day in two weeks each month
Duncan 1985 [38]Observational study (field)nonomanual, scan sampling 5-min blocks24 h two day in two weeks each month
Boyd et al. 1988 [39]Observational study (field)nonomanual, focal sampling24 h
Berger et al. 1999 [28]Observational study (field) nonotelemetry system ETHOSYSone year
Price et al. 2003 [40]Prospective non-blinded, non-randomized controlled trialyeselective arthroscopyCVI, time lapse video recorder, manual, instantaneous sampling continuously 72 h
Elia et al. 2010 [41]Prospective non-blinded, non-randomized case seriesnofeeding testvideo time-lapse mode, manual, instantaneous sampling, Observer TM program24 h 3 days per week
Aristizabal et al. 2014 [42]Prospective non-blinded, non-randomized case seriesnotwo feeding devicesvideo, manual24 h
Sartori et al. 2017 [43]Prospective non-blinded, non-randomized case seriesnotwo isoenergetic dietsvideo, manual, focal sampling continuous2 days
Maisonpierre et al. 2019 [44]Prospective non-blinded, non-randomized case seriesnonoaccelerometer (Actigraph)24 h, 20 days
Correa et al. 2020 [45]Observational study (stable)noadditional hay bagsvideo, manual, ad libitum sampling, pedometer24 h
Raspa et al. 2020 [46]prospective non-blinded, non-randomized case seriesnothree stocking density’svideo, manual, scan sampling at 15 min intervals3 consecutive days (72 h)
Table 2. Signalment of horses included in the study. Depending on the data available in the respective papers, ages are provided as range, median (plus range), or mean ± standard deviation. Similarly, the gender is detailed depending on the information provided in the papers.
Table 2. Signalment of horses included in the study. Depending on the data available in the respective papers, ages are provided as range, median (plus range), or mean ± standard deviation. Similarly, the gender is detailed depending on the information provided in the papers.
Author(s) and
Publication Year
Horses (n)Feral/Semiferal/
Domesticated
BreedGenderAge
Boy & Duncan 1979 [18]11semiferal, free-rangingCamargueFoalsn.a.
Duncan 1980 [37]18semiferal, free-rangingCamargue4 males, 9 mares, 5 yearlings n.a.
Duncan 1985 [38]18semiferal, free-rangingCamargue9 males, 9 maresn.a.
Boyd et al., 1988 [39]8semiferal, semi-reservePrzewalski1 stallion, 6 mares, 1 foaln.a.
Berger et al. 1999 [28]4semiferal, semi-reservePrzewalskimaresn.a.
Price et al. 2003 [40]12domesticatedmixed breed6 geldings, 6 mares9 (4–15) years
Elia et al. 2010 [41]8domesticatedmixed breed8 mares(6–14) years
Aristizabal et al. 2014 [42]10domesticatedmixed breed4 geldings, 6 mares23 (20–27) years
Sartori et al. 2017 [43]20domesticatedItalian draft horse10 males, 10 mares12–18 months
Maisonpierre et al. 2019 [44]6domesticatedmixed breed4 geldings, 2 mares14 (4–22) years
Correa et al. 2020 [45]7domesticatedBrazilian jumper3 males, 4 mares10 ± 5 years
Raspa et al. 2020 [46]22domesticatedComtois19 males, 3 mares22 ± 2 months
Table 3. Time budgets for standing/resting, lying, eating, movement/locomotion and other behaviors determined in the 12 studies included in this review, provided as range, or mean ± standard deviation.
Table 3. Time budgets for standing/resting, lying, eating, movement/locomotion and other behaviors determined in the 12 studies included in this review, provided as range, or mean ± standard deviation.
Author(s) and
Publication Year
TB in % Standing or RestingTB in %
Lying
TB in %
Eating
TB in %
Movement/Locomotion
TB in %
Other Behaviour e.g. Drinking, Playing
Comments
Boy & Duncan 1979 [18]8.1–11.8%2.7–15%13–62% Data of foals during developing
Duncan 1980 [37]12.9–19.52%4.25–13.76%50.82–63.89%5.45–9.3%n.a.TB ranges based on the TB detailed in Table 9 of the publication, TB variation due to sex and age
Duncan 1985 [38]13.4–29.3%4.2–15.5%60.8–66.6%4.3–13.4%n.a.TB ranges based on the TB detailed in Table 8 of the publication; TB depending on season and gender
Boyd et al. 1988 [39]36.6% ± 5.4%5.3% ± 2.5%46.4% ± 5.9%7.4% ± 1%10.2% ± 0.5%Variation during daytime and season for feeding and standing
Berger et al. 1999 [28]36.4% ± 15.7%
winter: 48.4% ± 15%
summer: 30.7% ± 29%
n.a.29.8% ± 13%n.a.n.a.Fluctuation over 24 h for feeding behaviour
Price et al. 2003 [40]54 ± 9: control group
66 ± 12: post-surgery
8 ± 6: control group
4 ± 6: post-surgery
34 ± 6: control group
20 ± 9: post-surgery
0.015 ± 0.005: control group
3 ± 2: post-surgery
2% ± 0%: control
2% ± 1%: post-surgery
Values 0–24 h post-surgery, horses are housed in a stable
Elia et al. 2010 [41]in stall: pellet-fed group: 58%
in stall: hay-fed group: 36.6%
paddock: pellet-fed group: 47.5 %
paddock: hay-fed group: 32.4%
n.a.In stall: pellet-fed group: 10%
In stall: hay-fed group: 64%
paddock: pellet-fed group:12.3% paddock: hay-fed group 19.1%searching:
in stall: pellet-fed group: 11.5%
in stall: hay-fed group: 1.2%
TB are provided separately for the time spent in the stable resp. the paddock for both groups
Aristizabal et al. 2014 [42]ground feeding: 68% ± 8.6%.
feeder: 65% ± 8.2%
ground feeding: 3% ± 5.5%
feeder: 5% ± 6.68%
ground feeding: 28% ± 5.5%
feeder: 31% ± 8.4%
n.a.n.a.Increased hay intake during daytime
Sartori et al. 2017 [43]15.58% ± 5.02%25.72% ± 4%32.47% ± 3.75%15.32% ± 2.37%11.31 ± 3.32%Details for gender and diet
Maisonpierre et al. 2019 [44]33% (27.5–31.1) daytime51% (47.1–55.2) night-time
36% (33.3–39.2) standard paddock
42.9% (36.6–47.1) small paddock
n.a.60.8% (58.2 65–4) daytime
46.8% (43.3–50.2) night-time
50.8% (47.9–55) standard paddock
48.6% (42.9–56.7) small paddock
4.6% (3.7–6.9) daytime2.4% (0.8–3.4) night-time
4.1% (3.1–5.8) standard paddock
2.5% (1.9–4.2) small paddock
n.a.TBs per paddock size were calculated based on the hours spent for each activity provided in the publication
Correa et al. 2020 [45]Basal: 62.7%
Hay bag: 65%
Basal: 10.7%
Hay bag: 9.9%
Basal: 12.5%Hay bag: 15.9% Abnormal behaviour:
Basal: 9%
Hay bag: 5.9%
Leisure combines movement, standing, investigation
Raspa et al. 2020 [46]30.56% ± 6.56%27.33% ± 2.05%30.55% ± 3.59%4.07% ± 1.06%<2%reduced stocking density increased locomotion and playing, this change in TB was not quantified
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Auer, U.; Kelemen, Z.; Engl, V.; Jenner, F. Activity Time Budgets—A Potential Tool to Monitor Equine Welfare? Animals 2021, 11, 850. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11030850

AMA Style

Auer U, Kelemen Z, Engl V, Jenner F. Activity Time Budgets—A Potential Tool to Monitor Equine Welfare? Animals. 2021; 11(3):850. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11030850

Chicago/Turabian Style

Auer, Ulrike, Zsofia Kelemen, Veronika Engl, and Florien Jenner. 2021. "Activity Time Budgets—A Potential Tool to Monitor Equine Welfare?" Animals 11, no. 3: 850. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11030850

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop