Next Article in Journal
Equid Assessment, Research and Scoping (EARS): The Development and Implementation of a New Equid Welfare Assessment and Monitoring Tool
Next Article in Special Issue
Domestic Foal Weaning: Need for Re-Thinking Breeding Practices?
Previous Article in Journal
KA-1002, a Novel Lysophosphatidic Acid Signaling Antagonist, Alleviates Bovine Tracheal Cell Disruption and Inflammation
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Retrospective Survey of Factors Affecting the Risk of Incidents and Equine Injury During Non-Commercial Transportation by Road in the United Kingdom
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Indicators of Horse Welfare: State-of-the-Art

by
Clémence Lesimple
Univ Rennes, Normandie Univ, CNRS, EthoS (Éthologie Animale et Humaine)—UMR 6552, F-35380 Paimpont, France
Animals 2020, 10(2), 294; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10020294
Submission received: 2 December 2019 / Revised: 10 February 2020 / Accepted: 10 February 2020 / Published: 13 February 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Horse Welfare)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Scientific interest in animal welfare was initially driven by popular emotional, ethics and political concerns. It has now become an important societal question and all the stakeholders agree on the necessity to rely on unambiguous scientific evidence for evaluation and making decisions. Animal welfare is defined as a chronic state reflecting an animal’s subjective perception of its situation indicated by behavioural, postural and physiological parameters. Because of their multiple statuses (as farm, leisure, sport or pet animals), horses experience a large variety of more or less adequate environmental conditions that present risks of impairing their welfare. The aim of this review is to disentangle welfare parameters and to differentiate reliable animal-based indicators of horses’ welfare from potential signals of acute sickness, discomfort, temporary states of pain, stress or emotion that are not based on popular beliefs in order to provide the equine industry with appropriate guidelines and recommendations.

Abstract

Animal welfare is defined as a chronic state reflecting an individual’s subjective perception of its situation. Because it is possible to be in a good welfare state and nevertheless experience acute fear or pain, and conversely, short-term positive emotions can be experienced during impaired welfare states, welfare as a chronic state has to be clearly distinguished from temporary states related to emotions, pain or stress. The evaluation of non-verbal individuals’ welfare state, particularly in interspecific situations, is a real challenge that necessarily implies animal-based measures and requires multidisciplinary scientifically validated measures. In the last decade, studies investigating horses’ welfare flourished together with new measures that were not always scientifically tested before being used. At a time were legal decisions are made on animal welfare, it is crucial to rely on reliable welfare indicators in order to prevent false evaluation. The aim of this review is to identify the scientifically tested and reliable indicators of horses’ welfare (e.g., body lesions, apathy, aggressiveness, stereotypic behaviours) from signals of temporary states related to acute pain emotions or stress and from popular beliefs, in order to give the scientific community and the horse industry accurate evaluation tools.

1. Introduction

1.1. From Animal Protection to Animal Welfare

The first major measure of animal protection legislation appeared in the 1800s in the United Kingdom, when parliament ratified the Cruel Treatment of Cattle Act [1] and made it an offence, punishable by fines up to five pounds or two months imprisonment, to “beat, abuse, or ill-treat any horse, mare, gelding, mule, ass, ox, cow, heifer, steer, sheep or other cattle”. Similarly, the United States [2] and France [3] included the offense of cruelty to animals (including, but not only, killing, wounding and maiming) in their laws. However, only in 1976 did the French law recognize that animals are sentient beings that should be kept under conditions fitting their biological needs [4]. However, despite a century of amendments and law modifications, change was slow, as the notions of cruelty, abuse and ill-treatment are vague and highly subjective.
The current concept of “animal welfare” emerged from popular emotional reactions elicited by Ruth Harrison’s book [5], which prompted the British government to create a commission to investigate the breeding conditions of intensively farmed animals. Under the direction of Professor F.W.R. Brambell, the commission gathered information about the practices inherent to intensive animal production in the UK, by visiting consenting facilities and collecting the oral/written testimonies of various publics (15 days). This inquiry resulted in a report [6] aiming at “examining the conditions in which livestock are kept under systems of intensive husbandry and to advise whether standards ought to be set in the interests of their welfare, and if so what they should be” in the UK. Their conclusions were limited, according to the authors, in domain (intensive breeding only, excluding non-intensive breeding, transport and slaughtering conditions or privately kept animals), geographic area (Great Britain only, excluding Northern Ireland) and time (“what appears tolerable today may come to be considered intolerable in the future”) [6]. It stated that animals should at least be free to stand up, lie down, turn around, stretch limbs and interact with conspecifics. In 1979, the Farm Animal Welfare Council (FAWC, UK) used this report as a basis to institute the concept of the Five Freedoms principle to regulate farm animals’ welfare, while “encouraging alternative systems of livestock husbandry which are ethically acceptable to the concerned public and economically competitive with the existing system of intensive production” [7]. Both the Brambell report and the Five Freedoms principle were based more on observations of environmental conditions (mainly spatial organization) than on the animal itself and were presented not as scientific guarantees of preserved welfare, but as minimal requirements without which the animals’ welfare is necessarily impaired [6,7,8]. The Five Freedoms concept hence stems from emotional awareness, political urgency and ethical issues rather than from real scientific concerns about animal welfare and have been updated in order to meet popular and ethical expectations [8]. Interestingly, even though the European Community agrees on the necessity of scientific-based evidence concerning animal welfare, its current legislation in this domain is still based on the Five Freedoms [9].

1.2. Scientific Approaches of Animal Welfare

Unlike most scientific areas that emerged from the interest and curiosity of pioneer scientists, research on animal welfare emerged from emotional and ethical societal concerns (e.g., [9,10]). In the dictionary, welfare has been defined as a “chronic pleasant state resulting from both body needs’ satisfaction and calmness of the mind” [11]. Although satisfaction of body needs can be assessed relatively easily, evaluation of the “calmness of the mind” of non-verbal individuals is more challenging. The scientific community has developed several concepts to characterize and appreciate animal welfare. Behavioural approaches state that animals should live accordingly to their nature [12] and should be able to perform (or at least not prevented in the long term from performing) all necessary behaviours without aversion or deprivation (e.g., [13]). However, perceptions of what a “necessary behaviour” is differ and some authors consider it as the possibility to express the animal’s entire natural behavioural repertoire (e.g., [14]), while other authors consider it as the ability to perform what an animal “desires” (subjective feelings) or needs (biological needs) without supplementary effort (e.g., [15]). Since animals have been recognized as sentient beings, the presence/absence of emotions has been included in animal welfare considerations. Thus, in order to ensure good welfare, animals should be protected from having negative emotions (e.g., fear, pain), and should be able to experience positive emotions [16,17].
Physiological approaches based on the concepts of homeostasis and allostasis have been developed. Homeostasis is defined as an approximately constant state which varies only within tolerable limits [18], whereas allostasis is defined as “stability through changes” [19]. The homeostasis model suggests that an animal’s welfare can be insured only by protecting it from environmental changes (e.g., [18]), but the allostasis principle is based on an organism’s constant internal and external changes aiming to maintain a homeostatic state. In both cases, welfare is ensured when the organism is in a long-term stable state, including (allostasis) or excluding (homeostasis) necessary variations. These different approaches emphasize the chronic nature of animal welfare as well as its complexity and multi-facetted essence.

1.3. Welfare, Emotions and Pain

As stated above, the term welfare refers to the persistent state of an individual over time (i.e., chronic) and can be either positive or negative according to its subjective perception of its environment. On the contrary, emotions (positive or negative) or pain (unpleasant sensation felt in some part of the body, [11]) are mainly related to acute and temporary events, hence restricted in time (except for chronic pains that are mainly considered as disabilities [20]). Feeling pain or negative emotions (e.g., fear) on a regular basis over time can lead to a negative welfare state, whilst a succession of positive emotions is most certainly a necessary precursor of a positive welfare state. However, an individual in a negative welfare state can experience positive emotions, and conversely, an individual in a positive welfare state can experience negative emotions. Thus, living under naturalistic conditions does not imply that an animal is free from constraints (e.g., thermal, feeding). However, experiencing too hot/cold temperatures or lack of food, as long as these deprivations are within the animals’ adaptation possibilities, induces only occasional or temporary perturbations (positive or negative) rather than welfare impairment [9]. When investigating welfare conditions, especially those of non-verbal individuals, it is crucial to adopt a chronic perspective and to evaluate the long-lasting situation of the organism rather than its temporary state, to prevent drawing unfounded conclusions.
Whatever the approach adopted, authors commonly accept that welfare is a complex multifaceted concept reflecting an organism’s subjective perception of a chronic situation.

2. Notion of Welfare Indicator

Assessing the welfare of a non-verbal individual, in particular, in an interspecific context, is a real challenge and requires the identification of reliable visible indicators reflecting an individual’s subjective perception of its chronic situation. An indicator can be defined as “a thing that indicates the state or level of something” [11]. A potential animal welfare indicator must include the following principles: validity (i.e., be meaningful for animal welfare and measure what it is supposed to), reliability (i.e., produce consistent results when used by different observers) and feasibility (i.e., easy to use in the field) [21,22]. Thus, before being considered as true welfare indicators, potential candidates have to be clearly related to an individual’s actual welfare state and have to be proven to be consistent and reliable in the field.
In order to respect the “validity” principle (it measures what it is meant to), welfare indicators have to yield information about an individual’s subjective perception of its situation, and consequently, have to be based on the animal itself rather than on its environment. Indeed, description of an animal’s environment can only indicate risks of welfare impairment, not the actual welfare state of an animal [23,24] and thus, cannot be considered as a welfare indicator. According to Veasey [25], it is mandatory, when working on animal welfare, to be able to refer to a standard population, living in optimal conditions and presenting a positive welfare state, in order to make appropriate comparisons. Finally, one must keep in mind that the welfare state is a subjective perception, leading to potential differences between two conspecifics in the same situation; if the many expressions of welfare improvement/impairment are not considered, the choice of indicators might lead to extensive discrepancies between evaluations of the same situation (e.g., [26]). To evaluate an animal’s welfare state accurately, it is thus essential to rely on multiple scientifically validated indicators, covering all the facets of welfare (behavioural, psychological and physiological) and reflecting an individual’s chronic state (e.g., [27]). These indicators have to be based on the characteristics and needs of the species (welfare indicators are not necessarily interchangeable between species) and reflect the animal’s perception (free from human anthropomorphic considerations) [13]. Finally, when evaluating the welfare of a group of animals (an entire facility), the proportion of individuals presenting welfare impairment signals must be taken into account (see Hausberger et al, same issue).
As they are considered as livestock, work, sport, leisure and pet animals, horses are submitted to a large array of more or less adapted environmental conditions (inducing more or less important impacts on their welfare). Consequently, to improve horses’ welfare, reliable welfare indicators reflecting horses’ chronic states must be identified and differentiated from potential indicators of temporary states, such as pain, emotion or acute stress. The aim here is to review the signals currently used to assess horse welfare, to identify reliable indicators, and consequently, to provide the equine industry with consistent welfare assessment tools. Thus, if all the signals and indicators included in the current welfare evaluation protocols as well as the ones currently used in the literature about horses’ welfare are evaluated here, the relevance of these protocols is not at stake here and will not be discussed in this review (see Hausberger et al., same issue).
The term “indicators” is used only for reliable scientifically established measures (scientifically related to other welfare impairment signs and providing consistent results between observers) concerning horses’ welfare. The term “signal” is used for other measures (related to acute states, ambiguous or vague descriptions). As mentioned above, as the only way to evaluate an individual’s welfare state is to consider the individual itself, environmental measures will not be taken into consideration here. We focus first on health-related, physiological, postural and behavioural welfare indicators, and then present signals potentially related to welfare states, but that require further investigation. Finally, we discuss parameters used for welfare assessment based on subjective evaluations and popular beliefs that are only vaguely described.

3. Indicators of Horses’ Welfare

This part focuses on reliable indicators of horses’ welfare, i.e., the signals revealing horses’ chronic state that have been formerly scientifically related to other signals and have been proven as consistent between observations (Table 1).

3.1. Health-Related Indicators

Physical injury is considered as one of the primary causes of welfare impairment, as it affects the individual in its integrity. First of all, the presence of work-related body lesions or marks of former lesions (naked or white hair areas) located at places of friction with working equipment (e.g., harness, bit, bridle, saddle), which reflects chronic use of poor-fitting equipment or work overload (e.g., [28]), is related to poor body condition scores, apathy and systemic health abnormalities including diarrhoea and dehydration [28,29,30], revealing welfare impairment. Using a classical 3-point scale (superficial: hair loss, broken skin, deep/swelling wounds, e.g., [28,30,62,63,64]), Burn et al. [65] found moderate to excellent agreement between six observers concerning the presence and severity of work-related body lesions on 80 horses. Therefore, the presence of work-related body lesions indicates an impaired welfare.
With the worldwide expansion of overweight horses [31,66] and the poor ability of owners to assess their horses’ condition correctly [32,66], it is important to have a practical tool to evaluate their body condition. The Body Condition Score (BCS) is based on visual evaluation of body fat by palpation of various areas of the animal’s body in order to evaluate its “physical state”, ranging from extreme thinness to extreme obesity. Two different scales are currently used, the first in 5 points (1–5, [67]) and the second in 9 points (1–8, e.g., [33]). The 9-point scale is used more frequently and is positively correlated with horses’ body-fat percentage [32,33], and blood variables (including leptin, insulin, glucose and triglyceride rates). Equines with high BCS (7 < X < 8, considered obese) are more prone to suffer physiological disorders (e.g., hyperinsulinemia [31], insulin resistance: [34]). Recent research highlighted differences between BCS and body weight ranging from 2.2% to 4.9% of the horse’s total body weight (3 to 39 kg) according to the breed [66]. However, the aim of BCS is to evaluate a horse’s body condition rather than its actual weight, with both evaluations reflecting different factors. Although BSC has proven to be repeatable when used by experienced assessors [68], its scoring is less reliable when used by non-experienced assessors, in particular, for the higher scores [32]. BCS has proven to assess horses’ body condition reliably and thus potential welfare impairments when used by experienced observers, but requires adequate suitable training to ensure its efficiency.
Elementary visual examination of a horse’s body can alert an observer to potential welfare deterioration when work-related lesions are present and BC scores are extreme (low or high).

3.2. Postural Indicators

Body postures are long used to assess animals’ acute emotional and chronic states [69]. In some cases, chronic health disorders are related to typical postures, such as laminitis with an arched body that takes the weight off the fore feet [20]. Ear positions are good indicators: if ears pointing backwards are associated with negative states, including pain (e.g., [20] for a review) or agonistic social interactions [70], they are also associated with chronically restricted conditions—the time spent with ears backwards in homogenous conditions (i.e., foraging) is related to chronically restricted living conditions (strict social isolation [35]) and chronic welfare impairment (e.g., vertebral disorders, stereotypic behaviours [36,37]), while horses under naturalistic conditions are almost never observed with such ear positions. Although time spent with ears backwards while foraging is not related to one particular context, it is a reliable indication of impaired welfare. Finally, a recent study showed that the prevalence of vertebral disorders evaluated by sEMG (see below) was related to a particular neck shape [38]: markers were placed along the spine in a sagittal plane, and located at musculoskeletal cues, enabling consistent reproduction of positioning, and horses were photographed when standing and walking in hand. The evaluations of the horses’ neck postures were based on the measurement of the angles between the markers. The values of these angles were significantly correlated to the prevalence of vertebral disorders: the hollower the neck, the more the spine was affected. On the contrary, horses with a rounded neck had no vertebral disorders [38,39].
Here again, visual examination of a horse’s body yielded reliable information concerning that horse’s welfare state and potential injuries.

3.3. Physiological Indicators

Changes in welfare state, either in a positive or in a negative way, can affect the physiology of an individual, but physiological signals of welfare are difficult to apprehend in a chronic perspective as: (1) it can take a long time before a modification of the environment is reflected by physiological parameters (i.e., blood count), it is difficult to know for sure what environmental conditions are effective, and (2) short-term internal changes (e.g., hormonal) can reflect chronic as well as acute situations. However, the presence of chronic vertebral disorders, related to lameness [71,72,73], changes in gait [71,72] or asymmetry of the pelvic bony prominences [72,74], is commonly considered as one of the most widespread causes of impairment of working horses’ welfare [39,72,74,75,76] and can induce muscular hypertonicity along the spine that can be assessed by manual palpation [36,38,40,76,77]) or static electromyography (sEMG) [38]. Evaluations of back pain/vertebral disorders presented a strong agreement between practitioners regardless of their occupational background (e.g., [38,40,77]), as well as between methods [38]. Although muscular activity cannot be estimated by simple visual examination, its evaluation constitutes a reliable indicator of horses’ welfare [39].

3.4. Behavioural Indicators

Sub-optimal living conditions are known to have an impact on animals’ behaviour, including modification of their time-budget (i.e., time spent in different activities) and on their behavioural repertoire (apparition or loss of behaviours).

3.4.1. Modifications of Horses’ Behavioural Repertoire

One of the most common and recognized behavioural indicators of welfare impairment is the presence of stereotypic or abnormal repetitive behaviours (SB/ARB). SB/ARB are invariant repetitive behavioural sequences, performed with no obvious goal or function [41], that appear under sub-optimal living conditions [41,42]. The development of horses’ SB/ARB has been widely investigated and, although genetic aspects are most certainly involved (e.g., [43]), these behaviours only emerge in inappropriate environments. Management during a horse’s early development may increase (or on the contrary prevent) risks of SB/ARB. Thus, early weaning and social isolation facilitate SB/ARB, while late weaning and group housing (especially in the presence of adults) have protective effects [78]. SB/ARB can appear as soon as 3 weeks after stabling [44]. Inappropriate management of adult horses has also been shown to facilitate SB/ARB emergence (social isolation [44,45]; spatial restriction [43], visual horizon [46,47], feeding [48,49], working conditions [50,51]); whereas efforts to provide conditions closer to natural conditions [47,79] or to decrease potential frustration [47,48] decreased their prevalence. Expression of SB/ARB is related to physiological and health disorders [46,54,80,81]. Following the coping hypothesis, SB/ARB would be a way for horses (and other animals) to tolerate their environment better (e.g., [82]). Despite the fact that having to “cope” with the environment means that the environment is inappropriate, the results concerning the coping efficiency of SB/ARB are not clear: although the acute stress state of stereotypic horses seemed to worsen when they were prevented from stereotyping [83], cortisol concentrations of stereotypic and non-stereotypic horses did not differ significantly [84,85,86,87,88], suggesting that stereotypic horses do not cope better with sub-optimal environments than their non-stereotypic counterparts. Evaluation of the presence and prevalence of SB/ARB was consistent over time [89]; therefore, reliably indicating impaired welfare of horses [35,42,90]. However, although some ARB are well known (e.g., weaving, cribbing) and their recordings present good inter-observer agreement [62], other ARB are more subtle and are more difficult to identify [91]. Many studies of horses focus on “traditionally” recognized stereotypic behaviours (i.e., cribbing, windsucking, weaving, pacing, pawing, head tossing/nodding), but it is crucial to take into account other more subtle ARB (e.g., compulsive licking/biting/kicking, other head movements, tongue movements, complex sequences, see also [35,42,91]) to ensure a reliable evaluation. The expression of SB/ARB by stereotypic horses seems to be strongly context-dependent (in individual stalls rather than in pasture, with a large outside visual horizon rather than in indoor stalls, mostly before meals [42,46,47]), highlighting the importance of choosing the right time to assess their prevalence.
When under sub-optimal conditions, horses can develop an apathetic state, related to depressed-like syndromes [28,29,52,65,92], characterized by sensorial withdrawal (visual and tactile [28,92]) and a general flattened posture [92]. This syndrome, first identified for horses working in developing countries [28,30], has also been identified in riding school horses in France [35,52,92], and is related to the occurrence of health (work-related wounds [29,30], low BCS and lameness [28]), physiological (depressed cortisol levels [92]) and behavioural/psychological (anhedonia: [52]) disorders. However, Burn et al. [65] found only a moderate inter-observer agreement for their horse population. An apathetic state is a reliable indicator of impaired welfare but it requires suitable training for assessors to identify it consistently.

3.4.2. Modification of the Horses’ Time Budget

Observations of feral horse populations enabled the construction of the natural “time-budget” of this species, i.e., the percentage of time devoted to diverse activities [70]. Modifications of this time-budget occur in inappropriate environments and may reveal welfare impairment. Under natural conditions, horses spend most of their time grazing/foraging (67%–75% of their time), resting (including sleeping: 15%–25%), observing their environment (6%–10%) or moving (8%), the remaining time being occupied by maintenance activities, social interactions and reproduction [70]. When horses are placed under inappropriate living conditions, this time budget may be modified. Thus, horses living under inappropriate environmental conditions were observed to decrease time resting (including sleep) [47,55,81], increase their aggressiveness towards conspecifics [53,55,92] and increase their active locomotion patterns (including active walk, trot, canter, [53,55,56]). Lack of rest/sleep in situations favouring the expression of stereotypic behaviours [47] was related to an increase of pathological behaviours [59], whereas horses with deprived social competencies increased their aggressiveness and their reproduction abilities decreased [57,93].

3.4.3. Other Behavioural Indicators

Behavioural reactions towards humans, either in human–horse relation tests [58] or during daily interactions (e.g., [28,29,30]) also yield information concerning the horses’ welfare. Horses living under constraining environmental conditions can become aggressive towards humans [44,60] and this has been related to higher rates of vertebral disorders [36]. On the contrary, horses were less aggressive towards the experimenter when their back was healthy [36]. Finally, a cognitive bias was shown to reflect horses’ welfare state. Thus, a recent study [37] showed that horses in a poor welfare state (evaluation based on the expression of SB/ARB, ear position, aggressive reactions to humans and vertebral disorders) were also more pessimistic when in the presence of an ambiguous stimulus, whereas horses in a good welfare state were clearly optimistic.
Observations of a horse’s behaviour either in its usual situations or during behavioural experiments testing particular reactions offer a reliable insight of its current welfare state. A frequent criticism of behavioural observation is the risk of an observer-related bias, which indeed exists, but is minimized when the observers have been trained correctly, the behavioural patterns are described precisely, inter-observer reliability is checked and the methods used are rigorous and validated [61].

3.5. Conclusions

Thus, reliable indicators of horses’ welfare exist, with most of them being detectable by visual examination of a horse’s body or by observation of their behaviour. However, in order to prevent mistaken or false evaluations, and consequently, detrimental management decisions, the observers have to be adequately trained to identify and to recognize real welfare alterations.

4. Signals Potentially Related to Welfare State

As stated above, when evaluating the welfare state of an animal, its chronic (long-term) state must be taken into consideration and the reliability of an evaluation must be guaranteed (do we measure what we are supposed to?). The signals discussed below can be related to both chronic and acute contexts and may be included in the scope of welfare evaluation if coupled with reliable indicators, but their reliability for assessing welfare on their own is not ensured (Table 2).

4.1. Health-Related Signals

Lameness, potentially related to acute pain or discomfort [94], can reveal chronic problems [73,75] and thus become a welfare issue. Clinical evaluation of lameness is generally based on visual scoring. However, the scales and methodologies used vary greatly between studies, involving different detection scales including evaluation points that are more or less well described [124,125]. Apart from overt lameness (i.e., when the horse is unable to use its leg or has a clear unbalanced neck movement), inter-observer agreement varied from poor [124,125] to good [126] between reports depending on the evaluation method, on the speed, on the type of movement (straight or in a circle) and on the experience of the observers. In addition to being potentially related to acute pain or discomfort and not chronic welfare, the presence of lameness seems to be difficult to assess reliably. Given its non-specificity and its rather complicated evaluation, its presence signals a potential problem, but cannot be considered a welfare indicator per-se. Prolapses (uterine, anal or vaginal) are included in most welfare evaluation protocols and considered as welfare impairment signals. Little scientific literature on this subject exists, most of it reporting case studies (e.g., [95]). The presence of prolapses whatever the type, reveals a severe violation of the individual’s health and rather than welfare impairment, constitutes a clear veterinary emergency.
In the same way, the evaluation of hoof condition is often included in welfare evaluation protocols, together with the presence of “medical” symptoms such as coughing or discharges (e.g., ocular, nasal, genital), all likely to occur in both acute and chronic contexts, and can even not relate to any impact on the animal’s welfare. Inappropriate hoof condition is likely to increase the incidence of foot and limb problems as well as lameness and may be related to unsuitable housing, feeding or laminitis episodes (e.g., [96] for a review). However, the evaluation of hoof condition has to be used cautiously: the horses’ hoof wall is particularly well-adapted to its functional role and has internal properties leading to natural fractures when walking on hard soils, thus inducing an uneven aspect of the hooves that does not reflect any welfare impairment [97]. On the contrary, inadequate trimming or shoeing may be visually imperceptible but alter a horse’s integrity and induce musculoskeletal injuries [127]. Moreover, most studies were based on clinical assessment methods that cannot be used practically in the field [128]. Except for extreme visible cracks, long wall or overt lameness, it is difficult to determine whether the condition of a horse’s hooves reveals its welfare state. Fluid discharges and coughing can be related to either acute (e.g., [98]) or chronic (e.g., [99]) disorders. Easy to assess, their presence cannot be considered a reliable welfare indicator, as they might reflect temporary discomfort. However, in the case of high prevalence inside a stable or continuous expression by one individual, coughing and fluid discharges point to potential veterinary issues.
Finally, the prevalence of gastric ulceration is very important in domestic horses [129,130]. As it is difficult to assess, it remains often untreated, leading to chronic abdominal pain and subsequent welfare impairment. However, apart from behavioural modifications (see Section 3.4.1), gastric ulcerations assessment requires performing invasive or expansive examination of the gastro-intestinal tract, and no clear indicators exist.
Broadly speaking, if the presence of wounds (other than work-related), marks of former lesions and overt signals of pain, pathology or health-related issues, in general, may be related to temporary states, it has still to alert on potential welfare issues. As such, it should elicit a thorough welfare inspection of the animal, using reliable welfare indicators.

4.2. Postural Signals

Amongst the signals used more and more to assess horses’ welfare emerge the evaluation of facial expressions. Used for a long time to identify humans’ emotional states, evaluation of facial expression is based on the description of facial movements due to underlying musculature [100]. Previous reports showed that an animal’s face is not necessarily the best place to look when assessing its emotional state and could even lead to misleading results [101]. Attempts have been made to characterize horses’ facial expressions when in acute pain either induced [102], following veterinarian interventions [63] or at work [108]. In addition, tests have been made to use horses’ facial expressions to evaluate their welfare [62]. Although the accuracy of facial expressions to assess horses’ pain is not clear (see [20]), its relevance in case of chronic states (welfare) has never yet been tested: the different patterns used in the horse facial expression scale were never scientifically related to actual welfare impairment signals and further studies are clearly needed.

4.3. Physiological Signals

Hormonal assays have been developed to investigate horses’ welfare. In particular, cortisol concentrations (the so called “stress hormone”) are commonly used, through blood, faecal or more recently, hair sampling. Plasma cortisol levels, currently used to investigate the effect of acute stressors because of its quick release peak after stimulation, are used to evaluate chronic situations. In most cases, plasma cortisol levels of horses presenting potential welfare impairments (chronic health disorders: e.g., [103]; chronic stress: e.g., [44]; depressed-like horses [92]) are low. However, no differences were found between the cortisol levels of stereotypic and non-stereotypic horses [85,86,87,88] or stereotyping and non-stereotyping phases [87], but the cortisol levels of horses with chronic laminitis were higher than those of controls [104]. In addition to these contradictory results, blood plasma analyses necessitate sampling by venipuncture that could have an impact on cortisol levels. More recently, non-invasive cortisol assays by faecal, saliva and hair sampling have been developed for horses, avoiding any potential sampling effects, but they yielded contradictory results. Pawluski et al. [103] found that horses with impaired welfare had lower faecal cortisol levels (e.g., vertebral disorders, ears backwards, abnormal cell counts), but no differences were found between horses presenting SB/ARB [84] or gastric ulceration [105] and their healthy counterparts, or between socially isolated and group-housed horses [78]. Basal salivary cortisol concentrations of stereotypic horses were lower [106] or were similar [85] to those of non-stereotypic horses. Moreover, Harewood [107] showed that although social isolation could elicit strong behavioural reactions, salivary cortisol levels did not increase. Results concerning hair cortisol may be biased due to temporal changes in hair growth as well as possible effects of local cortisol production in the hair follicles [131], and may change with the hair and according to its body location [132]. Whatever the sampling methodology, results concerning cortisol assays in relation to the assessment of horses’ welfare are contradictory, and although cortisol levels likely reflect temporary unpleasant states, they do not seem reliable to evaluate welfare. Recent analyses of serotonin and oxytocin levels related to positive (oxytocin increase [133]; serotonin increase [109]) or negative (serotonin decrease [110]) emotional states were developed for assessing horse welfare. However, results are contradictory: some studies report lower basal serotonin levels for horses with chronic health disorders than for healthy horses (chronic diseases, laminitis [104]) and lower for cribber than for non-cribber horses [87]. On the other hand, no differences were found between cribbing and non-cribbing phases for cribber horses [87], or between stereotypic and non-stereotypic horses (all SB/ARB included [88]). In other species, hormonal profiles were shown to vary according to a wide range of factors, including, but not only, genetic [111,112] and gender [112,134] effects, which could explain such discrepancies. Here again, hormonal levels do not seem to inform reliably about horses’ welfare and appear to be related more to intermittent states.
Haematological parameters, and in particular, white cell counts, are affected by chronic diseases (ex: chronic laminitis [113]) and by prolonged stressful conditions (transportation [118]). A recent study of over 2000 horses showed that horses’ welfare was negatively correlated with the neutrophil/lymphocytes (N/L) ratio [114]: horses with lower welfare scores presented higher N/L ratios. However, the horses’ welfare score used in this study included both animal-based indicators and environmental observations (reflecting potential and not actual welfare alterations). Further investigations including only animal-based scores are necessary to confirm that the N/L ratio is a reliable indicator of welfare impairment.
Finally, attempts have been made to use heart rate (HR) and heart rate variability (HRV), first developed for the assessment of emotional situations (e.g., [135]), to evaluate potential welfare alterations. However, although HR and HRV vary in emotionally challenging situations, the results regarding welfare assessment were more controversial. Thus, HR and HRV of stereotypic horses were either lower than [106] or did not differ from those of [86] their non-stereotypic counterparts; and Lebelt et al. [87] found that cribber horses’ HR decreased during cribbing periods. On the other hand, social and spatial restriction did not influence HR [107].
Scientific evidence shows thus that hormonal and heart rate scores can reflect potentially intermittent unpleasant states but they cannot be used reliably to evaluate welfare. Abnormal blood cell counts seem to be an interesting lead, but the results for the N/L ratio have to be confirmed.

4.4. Behavioural Signals

Some behaviours may be expressed in positive, negative and more ambiguous situations, and for this reason, have to be considered carefully together with their context of emission, and cannot necessarily be considered as welfare indicators. Yawning, often considered as a relaxation signal and used as a positive welfare expression, can be expressed in contexts of motor relaxation, but may also be triggered by stress and negative contexts [136]. Increased rates of yawning by horses were found following the administration of soothing products leading to muscular relaxation [137], but also in ambiguous or frustrating situations [115] or chronic diseases (e.g., [116]) and have even been related to the occurrence of SB/ARB [138]. In the same way, play has long been considered as an indicator of positive welfare [117], but increasing evidence shows a potential relationship with inappropriate living conditions (see [139] for review). Young horses express play exclusively [70] under favourable conditions (e.g., [119]), but play can also be triggered by social isolation [140]. Adult horses in a wild and feral population are hardly ever seen playing [70], while domestic horses are regularly reported to play. Also, locomotor rebound happens in animals under spatial restriction when they have access to larger spaces [56,141], and this is regularly considered as play. Thus, the presence of play, although potentially related to immediate short-term positive emotions, also raises questions concerning potential chronic restrictions in a horse’s life and hence, its welfare. Reports recently related horses’ attentional state to the prevalence of vertebral disorders. Calm observation of their environment, reflecting a positive attentional state [70], was less frequent by horses presenting vertebral disorders than by controls [120], and horses with impaired welfare were more unresponsive to their environment [28,30]. Although horses with impaired welfare seem to neglect their environment, over-attention—including vigilance—often associated with increased locomotion, is not at stake here. Vacuum chewing, sometimes defined as a relaxation signal in horses, is considered a displacement behaviour when in stressful situations for other species [121] and appears in contexts frustrating (e.g., [122,136]) for horses. All these behaviours are likely to reflect both liberation from chronic constraints and potential positive temporary states. As a consequence, they cannot be considered as welfare indicators, but their occurrence, in particular, when repeated over time can indicate welfare impairment.

4.5. Acoustic Signals

Horses’ acoustic communication has not been thoroughly investigated, and although whinnies are known to encode both social [123] and emotional information [142], information carried by non-vocal communication (snore, snort, blows) are less clear. Because descriptions are imprecise, snorts and snores have been confused for a long time, both were considered as indicative of high emotional states [143,144]. Recent reports seem to show that snorts are expressed more in positive contexts [145,146]. Additionally, the rates of snort production decreased significantly when the horses’ welfare state was impaired [146]. The production of snorts could hence inform about the valence of a horse’s perception of its environment.

4.6. Conclusions

Most of the signals described in the section above are not specific of welfare impairment and can indicate acute and chronic states. Thus, their presence cannot be considered as signals of welfare impairment but should lead to further examination of the animal. Clear identification of the methodologies and context of observation must be provided.

5. Signals Lacking Scientific Validation

The use of signals that are scientifically validated, based on popular beliefs and not sufficiently described to be reliably identified can lead to inappropriate evaluations and mistaken conclusions, and are thus not helpful. The signals described below, although they might be related at some point to welfare impairment, have not yet been sufficiently characterized and require additional scientific testing before being used.

5.1. Health-Related Elements

The fact that animal welfare concerns are related to emotional and ethical matters [9,10] means that substantial risks related to subjectivity and tradition are likely to become involved when evaluating welfare, and this is at the expense of scientific-based evidence, thus potentially leading to incorrect conclusions. The signals described in this section might be related to welfare impairment or discomfort, but are mostly based on feelings and lack precise description and/or scientific validation.
Because they are external signals that can be recognized by rapid visual examination, several health-related signals are commonly used to evaluate horses’ welfare, even if they have not been described precisely or shown to be reliably related to any chronic situation. The presence of swollen joints, abnormal breathing or excessive sweating or shivering for example, are often considered as signals of impaired welfare. However, these elements are only vaguely described (“exaggerate effort to breathe”, “profuse sweating”, [62]) or only defined in comparison to the other limb for the swollen joint. Moreover, rather than signals of chronic welfare impairment, these elements seem more informative about acute pathological states. In the same vein, the assessment guidelines concerning hair coat condition, also described as a signal of potentially impaired welfare, are imprecise (“local alteration”, “changing coat” [62]) and have not previously been related to any physiological, behavioural or health-related welfare-impairment indicator.

5.2. Behavioural and Postural Elements

Elements used by humans to describe a horse’s behaviour or attitude (annoyed, at ease, happy, undesirable behaviours) as well as vaguely described, and acute state-related behavioural patterns (tail swishing, fear behaviours, pain behaviours) are sometimes included in the evaluation of a horse’s welfare [62,63]. Such vague evaluations, based on an assessor’s subjective perception of an animal’s state, never related to welfare impairment indicators, are likely to lead to incorrect conclusions.

5.3. Physiological Elements

When an individual is in acute stress, a process of sympathetically mediated vasoconstriction induces an almost immediate drop of its skin and body temperatures [147]. Thus, these last few years, animals’ body temperature, measured in particular at the level of the eyes, has been used increasingly to assess the emotional valence and intensity of acute events (see [148] for a review). Increase of horses’ eye temperature is related to other physiological measures of acute stress (blood and saliva cortisol levels [149]). The impact of acute environmental conditions [150], fear [151] and pain (tighten noseband: e.g., [152]) on eye temperature has been investigated. However, although links between acute stress and skin/body temperature seem clear [149] and can be explained physiologically [147], currently there is no evidence of an impact of a chronic (positive or negative) situation. Thus, body temperature should be considered as an indicator of acute modification but not as an indicator of welfare.

5.4. Conclusions

Although some of these elements may reflect inadequate management of horses and consequently, welfare impairment, the lack of scientific precision and validation makes them difficult to be used reliably. In order to ensure reliable evaluation and to prevent incorrect conclusions/decisions, references to subjective human perceptions, poorly described elements or popular beliefs must be avoided. Further research has to ensure the validity and reliability of these elements before using them as welfare indicators.

6. General Conclusions

Animal welfare opens a very interesting but a very difficult research area, where strong emotional and popular beliefs [9,10] risk overriding scientific evidence. Horses’ particular status elicits strong emotional reactions and although scientists and legislators recognize the necessity to rely on scientific evidence, old habits and popular beliefs die hard. The main aim of all the research on horses’ welfare is to ensure that every decision will lead to an improvement of their living conditions. Taking this into consideration, and to prevent making premature decisions that could be harmful, it is crucial to conform to scientific standards and to the definition of welfare.
Thus, when investigating horses’ welfare, one should ensure that (1) the signals recorded truly reflect a chronic state; (2) the signals are meaningful for animal welfare and measure what they are supposed to; (3) the definition and description of the signals are clear and can be identified beyond question [21,22]; (4) that the signals are scientifically validated and related to other welfare indicators. An animal can experience temporary pain or fear although its welfare is good and, on the contrary, it can experience a positive mood temporarily although its welfare is impaired. The assessment of fear, pain or emotional level has to be clearly differentiated from welfare evaluation. Reliable and visible indicators of horses’ welfare do exist, and these should be exclusively used when evaluating horses’ welfare. Of course, additional less specific signals could be included in welfare evaluation, and could alert to potential impairment, but further research is needed in order to ensure their reliability. Finally, subjective human perception should clearly be removed from welfare evaluations, as it introduces emotional biases with a high risk of drawing incorrect conclusions and making harmful decisions. Before presenting or publicizing novel indicators of horse welfare, their reliability and validity must be more effectively checked [21,22] so as to avoid making incorrect evaluations and incorrect decisions [26,102].

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to my colleagues Martine Hausberger and Séverine Henry for the helpful discussions, to the reviewers for their help improving the manuscript and to Ann Cloarec for improving the language.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. 1822: 3 George 4 c.71: Cruel Treatment of Cattle Act. Available online: http://statutes.org.uk/site/the-statutes/nineteenth-century/1822-3-george-4-c-71-cruel-treatment-of-cattle-act/ (accessed on 3 October 2019).
  2. New York Revised Statutes 1829: Title 6: Section 26. Available online: https://www.animallaw.info/statute/new-york-revised-statutes-1829-title-6-section-26 (accessed on 3 October 2019).
  3. Loi Du 2 Juillet 1850 Dite Grammont Sur Les Mauvais Traitements Envers Les Animaux Domestiques. Available online: https://www.legifrance.gouv.fr/affichTexte.do?cidTexte=JORFTEXT000000332380&categorieLien=id (accessed on 3 October 2019).
  4. Préaubert, C. La Protection Juridique De L’animal en France. Ph.D. Thesis, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  5. Harrison, R. Animal Machines: The New Factory Farming Industry, 1st ed.; Vincent Stuart Ltd.: London, UK, 1964; p. 186. [Google Scholar]
  6. Brambell, F.W.R. Report of the Technical Committee to Equire into the Welfare of Animals kept under Intensive Livestock Husbandry Systems, 1st ed.; AW & Co Ltd.: Hemel Hempstead, UK, 1965; p. 85. [Google Scholar]
  7. Farm Animal Welfare Council, Press Statement 1979. Available online: https://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20121010012427/http://www.fawc.org.uk/freedoms.htm (accessed on 3 October 2019).
  8. Farm Animal Welfare Council. Farm Animal Welfare in Great Britain: Past, Present and Future, 1st ed.; FAWC: London, UK, 2009; p. 57. [Google Scholar]
  9. Korte, S.M.; Olivier, B.; Koolhaas, J.M. A new animal welfare concept based on allostatsis. Physiol. Behav. 2007, 92, 422–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Ohl, F.; van der Staay, F.J. Animal welfare: At the interface between science and society. Vet. J. 2012, 192, 13–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Larousse. Le grand Larousse illustré 2018; 2018 Bicentenaire; Larousse: Paris, France, 2019; p. 2106. [Google Scholar]
  12. Rollin, B.E. Animal welfare, science, and value. J. Agric. Environ. Ethic. 1993, 6, 44–50. [Google Scholar]
  13. Dawkins, M.S. Behavioural deprivation: A central problem in animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1988, 20, 209–225. [Google Scholar]
  14. Kiley-Worthington, M. Ecological, ethological, and ethically sound environments for animals: Toward symbiosis. J. Agric. Ethics 1989, 2, 323–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Fraser, D. Science, values and animal welfare: Exploring the inextricable connection. Anim. Welf. 1995, 4, 103–117. [Google Scholar]
  16. Fraser, D. Assessing animal well-being: Common sense, uncommon science. In Proceedings of the Conference on Food Animal Well-Being, West Lafayette, IN, USA, 13–15 April 1993; Purdue University Office of Agricultural Research Programs: West Lafayette, IN, USA; pp. 37–54. [Google Scholar]
  17. Dawkins, M.S. A user’s guide to animal welfare science. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2006, 21, 77–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Broom, D.M.; Johnson, K.G. Stress and Animal Welfare, 1st ed.; Kluwer Academic Publishers: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1993; p. 211. [Google Scholar]
  19. Sterling, P.; Eyer, J. Handbook of life stress, cognition and health. In Allostasis: A New Paradigm to Explain Arousal Pathology; Fisher, S., Reason, J., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1988; pp. 629–649. [Google Scholar]
  20. Hausberger, M.; Fureix, C.; Lesimple, C. Detecting horses’ sickness: In search of visible signs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 175, 41–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Rushen, J.; Chapinal, N.; de Passillé, A.M. Automated monitoring of behavioural based animal welfare indicators. Anim. Welf. 2012, 21, 339–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Phythian, C.J.; Michalopoulou, E.; Jones, P.H.; Winter, C.J.; Clarkson, M.J.; Stubbings, L.A.; Grove-White, D.; Cripps, P.J.; Duncan, J.S. Validating indicators of sheep welfare through a consensus of expert opinion. Animal 2011, 5, 943–952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Blokhuis, H.J.; Jones, R.B.; Geers, R.; Miele, M.; Veissier, I. Measuring and monitoring animal welfare: Transparency in the food product quality chain. Anim. Welf. 2003, 12, 445–455. [Google Scholar]
  24. Whay, H.; Main, D.; Green, L.E.; Webster, A. Assessment of the welfare of dairy cattle using animal-based measurements: Direct observations and investigation of farm records. Vet. Rec. 2003, 153, 197–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Veasey, J.S.; Waran, N.K.; Young, R.J. on comparing the behaviours of zoo housed animals with wild conspecifics as a welfare indicator. Anim. Welf. 1996, 5, 13–24. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lindelow, M. Sometimes more equal than others: How health inequalities depend on the choice of welfare indicator. Health Econ. 2006, 15, 163–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Sneddon, L.U.; Elwood, R.W.; Adamo, S.A.; Leach, M.C. Defining and assessing animal pain. Anim. Behav. 2014, 97, 201–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Pritchard, J.C.; Lindberg, A.C.; Main, D.C.J.; Whay, H.R. Assessment of the welfare of working horses, mules and donkeys, using health and behaviour parameters. Prev. Vet. Med. 2005, 69, 265–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Popescu, S.; Diugan, E.A. The relationship between behavioral and other welfare indicators of working horses. J. Equine Vet. Sci. 2013, 33, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Burn, C.; Dennison, T.; Whay, H. Relationships between behavior and health in working horses, donkeys, and mules in developing countries. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 126, 109–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Carter, R.A.; Geor, R.J.; Staniar, W.B.; Cubitt, T.A.; Harris, P.A. Apparent adiposity assessed by standardized scoring systems and morphometric measurements in horses and ponies. Vet. J. 2009, 179, 204–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Jensen, R.B.; Danielsen, S.H.; Tauson, A.H. Body condition score, morphometric measurements and estimation of body weight in mature islandic horses in Denmark. Acta Vet. Scand. 2016, 58, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Henneke, D.R.; Potter, G.D.; Kreider, J.L.; Yeates, B.F. Relationship between condition score, physical measurements and body fat percentage in mares. Equine Vet. J. 1983, 15, 371–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Frank, N.; Elliott, S.B.; Brandt, L.E.; Keisler, D.H. Physical characteristics, blood hormone concentrations and plasma lipid concentrations in obese horses with insulin resistance. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2006, 228, 1383–1390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Lesimple, C.; Poissonnet, A.; Hausberger, M. How to keep your horse safe? An epidemiological study about management practices. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 181, 105–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Fureix, C.; Menguy, H.; Hausberger, M. Partners with bad temper: Reject or cure? A study of chronic pain and aggression in horses. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e12434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Henry, S.; Fureix, C.; Rowberry, R.; Bateson, M.; Hausberger, M. Do horses with poor welfare show ‘pessimistic’ cognitive biases? Sci. Nat. 2017, 104, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lesimple, C.; Fureix, C.; De Margerie, E.; Sénèque, E.; Menguy, H.; Hausberger, M. Towards a postural indicator of back pain in horses (Equus caballus). PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e44604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Lesimple, C.; Fureix, C.; Aubé, L.; Hausberger, M. Detecting and measuring back disorders in nonverbal individuals: The example of domestic horses. Anim. Behav. Cognit. 2016, 3, 159–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lesimple, C.; Fureix, C.; Biquand, V.; Hausberger, M. Comparison of clinical evaluation of back disorders and human’s evaluation of back pain in riding school horses. BMC Vete. Res. 2013, 9, 209–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Mason, G.J. Stereotypy: A critical review. Anim. Behav. 1991, 41, 1015–1038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Mills, D.S. Repetitive movement problems in the horse. In The Domestic Horse, The Origins, Development and Management of Its Behaviour; Mills, D.S., McDonnell, S.M., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2005; pp. 212–227. [Google Scholar]
  43. Vecchiotti, G.; Galanti, R. Evidence of heredity of cribbing, weaving and stallwalking in thoroughbred horses. Livest. Prod. Sci. 1986, 14, 91–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Visser, E.K.; Ellis, A.D.; Van Reenen, C.G. The effect of two different housing conditions on the welfare of young horses stabled for the first time. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 114, 521–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. McAfee, L.M.; Mills, D.S.; Cooper, J.J. The use of mirrors for the control of stereotypic weaving behaviour in the stabled horse. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 159–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Cooper, J.J.; McDonald, L.; Mills, D.S. The effect of increasing visual horizons on stereotypic weaving: Implications for the social housing of stabled horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2000, 69, 67–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lesimple, C.; Gautier, E.; Benhajali, H.; Rochais, C.; Lunel, C.; Bensaïd, S.; Khalloufi, A.; Henry, S.; Hausberger, M. Stall architecture influences horses’ behaviour and the prevalence and type of stereotypies. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 21, 104833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Houpt, K.A.; McDonnell, S. Equine stereotypies. Comp. Contin. Educ. Pract. 1993, 15, 1265–1272. [Google Scholar]
  49. Nicol, C.J. Understanding equine stereotypies. Equine Vet. J. Suppl. 1999, 28, 20–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Hausberger, M.; Gautier, E.; Biquand, V.; Lunel, C.; Jégo, P. Could Work Be a Source of Behavioural Disorders? A Study in Horses. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e7625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Christie, J.L.; Hewson, C.J.; Riley, C.B.; McNiven, M.A.; Dohoo, I.R.; Bate, L.A. Management factors affecting stereotypies and body condition score in nonracing horses in Prince Edward Island. Can. Vet. J. 2006, 47, 136–143. [Google Scholar]
  52. Fureix, C.; Meagher, R.K. What can inactivity (in its various forms) reveal about affective states in nonhuman animals? A review. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 171, 8–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Benhajali, H.; Richard-Yris, M.A.; Leroux, M.; Ezzaouia, M.; Charfi, F.; Hausberger, M. A note on the time budget and social behaviour of densely housed horses - A case study in Arab breeding mares. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 112, 196–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Mills, D.S.; Davenport, K. The effect of a conspecific versus the use of a mirror for the control of stereotypic weaving behavior in the stabled horse. Anim. Sci. Glasg. 2002, 74, 95–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Benhajali, H.; Richard-Yris, M.A.; Ezzaouia, M.; Charfi, F.; Hausberger, M. Foraging opportunity: A crucial criterion for horse welfare? Animal 2009, 3, 1308–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Lesimple, C.; Fureix, C.; LeScolan, N.; Richard-Yris, M.A.; Hausberger, M. Housing conditions and breed are associated with emotionality and cognitive abilities in riding school horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 129, 92–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bourjade, M.; de Bpoyer des Roches, A.; Hausberger, M. Adult-young ratio, a major factor regulating social behaviour of young: A horse study. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e4888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Hausberger, M.; Roche, H.; Henry, S.; Visser, E.K. A review of the human-horse relationship. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 109, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Bertone, J.J. Excessive drowsiness secondary to recumbent sleep deprivation in two horses. Vet. Clin. Equine 2006, 22, 157–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Rivera, E.; Benjamin, S.; Nielsen, B.; Shelle, J.; Zanella, A.J. Behavioral and physiological responses of horses to initial training: The comparison between pastured versus stalled horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 235–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Burghardt, G.M.; Bartmess-LeVasseur, J.N.; Browning, S.A.; Morrison, K.E.; Stec, C.L.; Zachau, C.E.; Freeberg, T.M. Perspectives—Minimizing observer bias in behavioral studies: A review and recommendations. Ethology 2012, 118, 511–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Czycholl, I.; Büttner, K.; Klingbell, P.; Krieter, J. An indication of reliability of the two level approach of the AWIN welfare assessment protocol for horses. Animals 2018, 8, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Dalla Costa, E.; Dai, F.; Lebelt, D.; Scholz, P.; Barbien, S.; Canali, E.; Zanella, A.J.; Minero, M. Welfare assessment of horses: The AWIN approach. Anim. Welf. 2016, 25, 481–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. DuBois, C.; DeVries, T.; Halye, D.B.; Lawlis, P.; Merkies, K. Putting an on farm welfare assessment tool into practice in the canadian equine industry—A pilot study. J. Equine Vet. Sci. 2018, 63, 35–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Burn, C.C.; Pritchard, J.C.; Whay, H.R. Observer reliability for working equine welfare assessment: Problems with high prevalence of certain results. Anim. Welf. 2009, 18, 177–187. [Google Scholar]
  66. Catalano, D.N.; Coleman, R.J.; Hathaway, M.R.; McCue, M.E.; Rendahl, A.K.; Martinson, K.L. Estimation of actual and ideal bodyweight using morphometric measurements and owner guessed bodyweight of adult draft and Warmblood horses. J. Equine Vet. Sci. 2016, 39, 38–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Leighton Hardman, A.C. Equine nutrition, 1st ed.; Pelham Books: London, UK, 1980; p. 112. [Google Scholar]
  68. Winckler, C.; Willen, S. The reliability and repeatability of a lameness scoring system for use as an indicator of welfare in dairy cattle. Acta Agric. Scand. A An. Suppl. 2001, 30, 103–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Darwin, C. L’expression Des Emotions Chez L’homme Et Les Animaux, 2nd ed.; Comité des travaux historiques et scientifiques: Paris, France, 1872; p. 222. [Google Scholar]
  70. Waring, G. Horse Behavior: The Behavioural Traits and Adaptations of Domestic and Wild Horses, Including Ponies, 2nd ed.; Noyes Publications; William Andrew Publishing: New York, NY, USA, 2003; p. 456. [Google Scholar]
  71. Fonseca, B.P.A.; Alves, A.L.G.; Nicoletti, J.L.M.; Thomassian, A.; Hussni, C.A.; Mikail, S. Thermography and ultrasonography in back pain diagnosis of equine athletes. J Equine Vet Sci. 2006, 26, 507–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Goff, L.M.; Jeffcott, L.B.; Jasiewicz, J.; McGowan, C.M. Structural and biomechanical aspects of equine sacroiliac joint function and their relationship to clinical disease. Vet. J. 2008, 176, 281–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Landman, M.A.A.; de Blaauw, J.A.; van Weeren, P.R.; Hofland, L.J. Field study of the prevalence of lameness in horses with back problems. Vet. Rec. 2004, 155, 165–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Haussler, K.K. The lower back and pelvis of performance horses receive a closer look. J. Equine Vet. Sci. 1996, 16, 279–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Jeffcott, L.B. Disorders of the thoracolumbar spine of the horse. A survey of 443 cases. Equine Vet. J. 1980, 12, 197–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Haussler, K.K. Application of chiropractic principles and techniques to equine practice. In Proceedings of the 43rd Annual Convention of the AAEP, Phoenix, AZ, USA, 7–10 December 1997. [Google Scholar]
  77. Lesimple, C.; Fureix, C.; Menguy, H.; Hausberger, M. Human direct actions may alter animal welfare: A study on horses (Equus caballus). PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e10257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Heleski, C.R.; Shelle, A.C.; Nielsen, B.D.; Zanella, A.J. Influence of housing on weanling horse behavior and subsequent welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 291–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Goodwin, D.; Davidson, H.P.; Harris, P. Foraging enrichment for stabled horses: Effects on behaviour and selection. Equine Vet. J. 2002, 34, 686–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Nicol, C.J.; Davidson, H.P.D.; Harris, P.A.; Waters, A.J.; Wilson, A.D. Study of crib-biting and gastric inflammation and ulceration in young horses. Vet. Rec. 2002, 151, 658–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Ninomiya, S.; Sato, S.; Sugawara, A. Weaving in stabled horses and its relationship to other behavioural traits. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 106, 134–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Mason, G.; Clubb, R.; Latham, N.; Vickery, S. Why and how should we use environmental enrichment to tackle stereotypic behaviour? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 163–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Nagy, K.; Bodó, G.; Bárdos, G.; Harnos, A.; Kabai, P. The effect of a feeding stress-test on the behavior and heart rate variability of control and crib-biting horses (with or without inhibition). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 121, 140–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Fureix, C.; Benhajali, H.; Henry, S.; Bruchet, A.; Prunier, A.; Ezzaouia, M.; Coste, C.; Hausberger, M.; Plame, R.; Jego, P. Plasma cortisol and faecal cortisol metabolites concentrations in stereotypic and non-stereotypic horses: Do stereotypic horses cope better with poor environmental conditions? BMC Vet. Res. 2013, 9, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Pell, S.M.; McGreevy, P.D. A study of cortisol and beta-endorphin levels in stereotypic and normal Thoroughbreds. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1999, 64, 81–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Clegg, H.A.; Buckley, P.; Friend, M.A.; McGreevy, P. The ethological and physiological characteristics of cribbing and weaving horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 109, 68–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Lebelt, D.; Zanella, A.J.; Unshelm, J. Physiological correlates associated with cribbing behavior in horses: Changes in thermal threshold, heart rate, plamsa B-endorphin and serotonin. Equine Vet. J. Suppl. 1998, 27, 21–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Omidi, A.; Jafari, R.; Nazifi, S.; Parker, M.O. Potential role for selenium in the pathophysiology of crib-biting behavior in horses. J. Vet. Behav. 2018, 23, 10–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Hausberger, M.; Gautier, E.; Müller, C.; Jego, P. Lower learning abilities in stereotypic horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 107, 299–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Mills, D.; Alston, R.; Rogers, V.; Longford, N. Factors associated with the prevalence of stereotypic amongst Thoroughbred horses passing through auctioneer sales. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 115–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Lesimple, C.; Hausberger, M. How accurate are we at assessing others’ well-being? The example of welfare assessment in horses. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Fureix, C.; Jego, P.; Henry, S.; Lansade, L.; Hausberger, L. Towards an ethological model of depression? A study on horses. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e39280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Wells, S.M.; Goldschmidt-Rothschild, B. Social behavior and relationships in a herd of Camargue horses. Z. Tierpsychol. 1979, 49, 363–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Ross, M.W.; Dyson, S.J. The lameness score: Quantification of lameness severity. In Diagnosis and Management of Lameness in the Horse; Ross, M.W., Dyson, S.J., Eds.; W.B. Saunders: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2003; pp. 66–67. [Google Scholar]
  95. Hewes, C.A.; Johnson, A.K.; Kivett, L.E.; Stewart, A.J.; Weisman, J.L.; Caldwell, F.J. Uterine prolapse in a mare leading to metritis systemic inflammatory response syndrome, septic shock and death. Equine Vet. J. 2011, 23, 273–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Moyer, W. Hoof wall defects: Chronic hoof wall separations and hoof wall cracks. Vet. Clin. Equine 2003, 19, 463–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Bertram, J.; Gosline, J.M. Fracture toughness design in horse hoof keratin. J. Exp. Biol. 1986, 125, 29–47. [Google Scholar]
  98. Santos, M.P.; Gutierrez-Nibeyro, S.D.; Stewart, A.A.; Hyde, R.M.; Rodgerson, D.H. Identification of a periorbital wooden foreign body as the cause of chronic ocular discharge in a horse. Aust. Vet. J. 2012, 90, 84–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Ramseyer, A.; Gaillard, C.; Burger, D.; Straub, R.; Jost, U.; Boog, C.; Marti, E.; Gerber, V. Effects of genetic and environmental factors on chronic lower airway disease in horses. J. Vet. Intern. Med. 2008, 2, 149–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Ekman, P.; Friesen, W.V. Facial Action Coding System: Investigator’s Guide; Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1978; p. 527. [Google Scholar]
  101. Leach, M.; Coulter, C.; Richardson, C.; Flecknell, P. Are we looking at the wrong place? Implications for behavioural-based pain assessment in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculi) and beyond? PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e13347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Gleerup, K.; Forkman, B.; Lindegaard, C.; Andersen, P. An equine pain face. Vet. Anaesth. Analg. 2015, 42, 103–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Pawluski, J.; Jego, P.; Henry, S.; Bruchet, A.; Palme, R.; Coste, C.; Hausberger, M. Low plasma cortisol and fecal cortisol metabolite measures as indicators of xompromised welfare in domestic horses (Equus caballus). PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0182257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Ayala, I.; Martos, N.F.; Silvan, C.; Gutierrez-Panizo, C.; Clavel, J.G.; Illera, J.C. Cortisol, adrenocorticotropic hormone, serotonin, adrenaline and noradrenaline serum concentrations in relation to disease and stress in the horse. Res. Vet. Sci. 2012, 93, 103–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Malmkvist, J.; Moller Poulsen, J.; Luthersson, N.; Palme, R.; Christensen, J.W.; Sondergaard, E. behaviour and stress responses in horses with gastric ulceration. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 142, 160–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Fejsakova, M.; Kottferova, J.; Dankulincova, Z.; Haladova, E.; Matos, R.; Mino, I. Some possible factors affecting horse welfare assessment. Acta Vet. Brno 2013, 82, 447–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Harewood, E.J.; McGowan, C.M. Behavioral and physiological responses to stabling in naïve horses. J. Equine vet. Sci. 2005, 25, 164–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Dyson, S.; Berger, J.; Ellis, A.; Mullard, J. Can the presence of musculoskeletal pain be determined from the facial expression of ridden horses? J. Vet. Behav. 2017, 19, 78–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Alberghina, D.; Rizzo, M.; Piccione, G.; Giannetto, C. An explanatory study about the association between serum serotonin concentration and canine-human social interactions in shelter dogs (Canis familiaris). J. Vet. Behav. 2017, 18, 96–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Cakiroglu, D.; Meral, Y.; Sancak, A.; Cifti, G. Relationship between the serum concentration of serotonin and lipids in aggression in dogs. Vet.Rec. 2017, 161, 59–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Amat, M.; Le Brech, S.; Camps, T.; Torrente, C.; Mariotti, V.; Ruiz, J.; Manteca, X. Differences in serotonin serum concentration between aggressive English cocker spaniels and aggressive dogs of other breeds. J. Vet. Behav. 2013, 8, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Kovácsa, K.; Kis, A.; Pogány, A.; Koller, D.; Topál, J. Differential effects of oxytocin on social sensitivity in two distinctbreeds of dogs (Canis familiaris). Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016, 74, 212–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  113. Steelman, S.; Johnson, D.; Wagner, B.; Stokes, A.M.; Chowdhary, B.P. Cellular and humoral immunity in chronic equine laminitis. Vet. Immunol. Immunopathol. 2013, 153, 217–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Popescu, S.; Diugan, E. The relationship between the welfare quality and stress index in working and breeding horses. Res. Vet. Sci. 2017, 115, 442–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Rochais, C.; Henry, S.; Hausberger, M. Hay-bags and Slow feeders: Testing their impact on horse behaviour and welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018, 198, 52–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Pearson, E.G. Liver disease in the mature horse. Equine vet. Educ. 1999, 11, 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Fagen, R. Animal Play Behavior; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1981; p. 684. [Google Scholar]
  118. Padalino, B.; Lee Raidal, S.; Carter, N.; Celi, P.; Muscatello, G.; Jeffcott, L.; de Sikva, K. Immunological, clinical, haematological and oxidative responses to long distance transportation in horses. Res. Vet. Sci. 2017, 115, 78–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Henry, S.; Zanella, A.; Sankey, C.; Richard-Yris, M.A.; Marko, A.; Hausberger, M. Adults may be used to alleviate weaning stress in domestic foals (Equus caballus). Physiol. Behav. 2012, 106, 428–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Rochais, C.; Fureix, C.; Lesimple, C.; Hausberger, M. Lower attention to daily environment: A novel cue for detecting chronic horses’ back pain? Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 20117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  121. Tinbergen, N. Derived activities; their causation, biological significance, origin and emancipation during evolution. Q. Rev. Biol 1952, 27, 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Cooper, J.J.; Mason, G. The identification of abnormal behaviour and behavioural problems in stabled horses and their relationship to horse welfare: A comparative review. Equine Vet. J. 2010, 30, 5–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Lemasson, A.; Boutin, A.; Boivin, S.; Blois-Heulin, C.; Hausberger, M. Horse (Equus caballus) whinnies: A source of social information. Anim. Cognit. 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Starke, S.D.; Raistrick, K.J.; May, S.A.; Pfau, T. The effect of trotting speed on the evaluation of subtle lameness in horses. Vet. J. 2013, 197, 245–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Hammarberg, M.; Egenvall, A.; Pfau, T.; Rhodin, M. Rater agreement of visual lameness assessment in horses during lungeing. Equine Vet. J. 2016, 48, 78–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  126. Keegan, K.G.; Dent, E.V.; Wilson, D.A.; Janicek, J.; Kramer, J.; Lacarrubba, A.; Walsh, D.M.; Cassells, M.W.; Esther, T.M.; Schiltz, P.; et al. Repeatability of subjective evaluation of lameness in horses. Equine Vet. J. 2010, 42, 92–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. O’Grady, S.E.; Poupard, D.A. Physiological horseshoeing: An overview. Equine vet. Educ. 2003, 13, 330–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Hemsworth, L.M.; Jongman, E.; Coleman, G. Rercreational horse welfare: The relationships between recreational horse owner attributes and recreational horse welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 165, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Banse, H.; MacLeod, H.; Crosby, C.; Windeyer, C. Prevalence of and risk factors for equine glandular and squamous gastric disease in polo horses. Can. Vet. J. 2018, 59, 880–884. [Google Scholar]
  130. Sykes, B.; Hewetson, M.; Hepburn, R.; Luthersson, N.; Tamzali, Y. European College of Equine Internal Medicine Consensus Statement—Equine Gastric Ulcer Syndrome in Adult Horses. J. Vet. Intern. Med. 2015, 29, 1288–1299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Salaberger, T.; Millard, M.; El Makarem, S.; Möstl, E.; Grünberger, V.; Krametter-Frötscher, R.; Wittek, T.; Palme, R. Influence of external factors on hair cortisol concentrations. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 2016, 233, 73–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Duran, M.C.; Janz, D.M.; Waldner, C.L.; Campbell, J.R.; Marques, F.J. hair cortisol concentration as a stress biomarker in horses: Associations with body location and surgical castration. J. Equine Vet. Sci. 2017, 55, 27–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Mistui, S.; Yamamoto, M.; Nagasawa, M.; Mogi, K.; Kikusui, T.; Ohtani, N.; Otha, M. Urinary oxytocin as a noninvasive biomarker of positive emotion in dogs. Horm. Behav. 2011, 60, 239–243. [Google Scholar]
  134. Ozsoy, S.; Esel, E.; Kula, M. Serum oxytocin levels in patients with depression and the effects of gender and antidepressant treatment. Psychiatry Res. 2009, 169, 249–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Visser, E.K.; Van Reenen, C.G.; Van Der Werf, J.T.N.; Schilder, M.B.H.; Knaap, J.H.; Barneveld, A.; Blokhuis, H.J. Heart rate and heart rate variability during a novel object test and a handling test in young horses. Physiol. Behav. 2002, 76, 289–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Walusinski, O.; Deputte, B.L. Le bâillement: Phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie. Revue Neurol. 2004, 160, 1011–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Kammerling, S.; Weckman, T.; Donahoe, J.; Tobin, T. Dose related effect of the kappa agonist U-50, 488H on behaviour, nociception and autonomic response in the horse. Equine Vet. J. 1988, 20, 114–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Fureix, C.; Gorecka-Bruzda, A.; Gautier, E.; Hausberger, M. Cooccurrence of yawning and stereotypic behaviour in horses (Equus caballus). ISRN Zool. 2011, 2011, 71209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  139. Blois-Heulin, C.; Rochais, C.; Camus, S.; Fureix, C.; Lemasson, A.; Lunel, C.; Bézard, E.; Hausberger, M. Animal welfare: Could adult play be a false friend? Anim. Behav. Cognit. 2015, 2, 156–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Christensen, J.; Ladewig, J.; Sondergaard, E.; Malmkvist, J. Effects of individual versus group stabling on social behaviour in domestic stallions. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 75, 233–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Dawkins, M.S. Battery hens name their price: Consumer demand theory and the measurement of ethological needs. Anim. Behav. 1983, 31, 1195–1205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Briefer, E.; Mandel Maigrot, A.L.; Briefer Freymond, S.; Bachmann, I.; Hillman, E. Perception of emotional valence in horse whinnies. Front. Zool. 2017, 14, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  143. Wolff, A.; Hausberger, M.; LeScolan, N. Experimental tests to assess emotionality in horses. Behav. Process. 1997, 40, 209–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Visser, K.; van Dierendonck, M.; Ellis, A.; Rijksen, C.; van Reenen, C.G. A comparison of sympathetic and conventional training methods on responses to initial horse training. Vet. J. 2009, 181, 48–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  145. Dyson, S.; Berger, J.; Ellis, A.; Mullard, J. Behavioural observations and comparisons of nonlame horses and lame horses before and after resolution of lameness by diagnostic analgesia. J. Vet. Behav. 2018, 26, 64–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. Stomp, M.; Leroux, M.; Cellier, M.; Henry, S.; lemasson, A.; Hausberger, M. An unexpected acoustic indicator of positive emotions in horses. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0197898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  147. Marks, A.; Vianna, D.M.; Carrive, P. Nonshivering thermogenesis without interscapular brown adipose tissue involvement during conditioned fear in the rat. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2009, 296, R1239–R1247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  148. Church, J.S.; Cook, N.; Schaefer, A. Recent Applications of Infrared Thermography for Animal Welfare and Veterinary Research: Everything from Chicks to Elephants; InfraMation: North Billerica, USA, 2009; p. 10. [Google Scholar]
  149. Cook, W.R. Damage by the bit to the equine interdental space and second lower premolar. Equine Vet. Educ. 2001, 23, 355–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Bartolomé, E.; Sanchez, M.; Molina, A.; Schaefer, A.L.; Cervantes, I.; Valera, M. using eye temperature and heart rate for stress assessment in young horses competing in jumping competitions and its possible influence on sport performance. Animal 2013, 7, 2044–2053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Dai, F.; Cogi, N.; Heinzl, E.; Dalla Costa, E.; Canali, E.; Minero, M. Validation of a fear test in sport horses using infrared thermography. J. Vet. Behav. 2015, 10, 128–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. McGreevy, P.; Warren-Smith, A.; Guisard, Y. The effect of double bridles and jaw-clamping crank nosebands on temperature of eyes and facial skin of horses. J. Vet. Behav. 2012, 7, 142–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Indicators of horses’ welfare. X: specifies whether the indicator may reveal a temporary state or a veterinary issue as well as welfare. (X): indicates whether signal may be a predictor of chronic welfare state, temporary state or a veterinary issue when combined with other signals. References are given in italics for each signal.
Table 1. Indicators of horses’ welfare. X: specifies whether the indicator may reveal a temporary state or a veterinary issue as well as welfare. (X): indicates whether signal may be a predictor of chronic welfare state, temporary state or a veterinary issue when combined with other signals. References are given in italics for each signal.
Type of SignalWelfare StateTemporary State
Health RelatedBody lesion [28,29,30] xx
Body Condition Score [31,32,33,34]xx
Specific postures [20]xx
PosturalEar position [35,36,37]xx
Neck shape [38,39]x(x)
PhysiologicalMuscular hypertonicity [39,40,41]xx
BehaviouralBehavioural repertoireStereotypic/Abnormal Repetitive Behaviours (SB/ARB) [42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51]xx
Apathy [28,29,30,52,53]x(x)
Time-budgetRest [48,54,55]xx
Aggressiveness [52,55,56,57,58]xx
Active locomotion [55,56,59]x
OtherReaction to humans [28,29,30,36,45,60,61]xx
Cognitive bias [37]xx
Table 2. Signals potentially related to horses’ welfare. X: indicates whether signal reveals a chronic welfare state, temporary state or a veterinary issue. (X): indicates whether signal may be a predictor of chronic welfare state, temporary state or a veterinary issue when combined to other signals. References are given in italics for each signal.
Table 2. Signals potentially related to horses’ welfare. X: indicates whether signal reveals a chronic welfare state, temporary state or a veterinary issue. (X): indicates whether signal may be a predictor of chronic welfare state, temporary state or a veterinary issue when combined to other signals. References are given in italics for each signal.
Type of SignalChronic Welfare StateTemporary State
Health-RelatedLameness [73,75,94](x)x
Prolapse [95] x
Hoof condition [96,97](x)x
Cough/Discharges [98,99](x)
PosturalFacial expressions [20,62,63,100,101] x
PhysiologicalCortisol (faecal, blood, hair, saliva) [44,78,84,85,86,87,88,102,103,104,105,106,107](x)x
Serotonin/oxytocin [87,88,108]
White cell count [109,110,111](x)x
HR/HRV [86,87,104,105,112] x
BehaviouralYawning [113,114](x)x
Play [70,115,116](x)x
Attentional state [28,30,70,117](x)x
Vacuum chewing [118,119](x)x
AcousticSnort [120,121,122,123](x)x

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lesimple, C. Indicators of Horse Welfare: State-of-the-Art. Animals 2020, 10, 294. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10020294

AMA Style

Lesimple C. Indicators of Horse Welfare: State-of-the-Art. Animals. 2020; 10(2):294. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10020294

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lesimple, Clémence. 2020. "Indicators of Horse Welfare: State-of-the-Art" Animals 10, no. 2: 294. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10020294

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop