Next Article in Journal
Activity Profile, Heart Rate, Technical Involvement, and Perceived Intensity and Fun in U13 Male and Female Team Handball Players: Effect of Game Format
Next Article in Special Issue
Short-Term Cardiac Autonomic Recovery after a Repeated Sprint Test in Young Soccer Players
Previous Article in Journal
Cross-Sectional Investigation of Stress Fractures in German Elite Triathletes
Previous Article in Special Issue
Effects of 7-Week Hip Thrust Versus Back Squat Resistance Training on Performance in Adolescent Female Soccer Players
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Working Memory Training in Professional Football Players: A Small-Scale Descriptive Feasibility Study—The Importance of Personality, Psychological Well-Being, and Motivational Factors

1
Department of Neuropsychology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, 6229 ER Maastricht, The Netherlands
2
Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Alzheimer Center Limburg, Maastricht University, 6229 ER Maastricht, The Netherlands
3
Hogeschool van Arnhem en Nijmegen, Sport en Bewegen, 6503 GL Nijmegen, The Netherlands
4
Department of Medical Psychology, Maastricht University Medical Center, Maastricht and Adelante, Rehabilitation Center, 6432 CC Hoensbroek, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sports 2019, 7(4), 89; https://doi.org/10.3390/sports7040089
Submission received: 28 October 2018 / Revised: 9 December 2018 / Accepted: 11 April 2019 / Published: 18 April 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Training Process in Soccer Players)

Abstract

:
Background: Working memory training (WMT) programs can improve working memory (WM). In football players, this could lead to improved performance on the pitch. Method: Eighteen professional football players of Maatschappelijke Voetbal Vereniging Maastricht (MVV) participated and followed an online, computerized WMT program. Neuropsychological performance, psychological wellbeing, self-efficacy, and football skills (Loughborough Soccer Passing Test; LSPT) were assessed at three time points, before and after WMT and at three-month follow-up. Descriptive data are reported. Results: Baseline characteristics were roughly similar for both groups. Participants performed better on the trained WM tasks, but performance for other neuropsychological test measures or the LSPT did not change. Low compliance rates were observed, showing differences in personality and well-being between compliers and non-compliers. Conclusions: WMT is not a feasible and effective strategy to improve non-trained cognitive measures and football performance. However, this study indicates that it is important to take individual characteristics into account.

1. Introduction

Working memory (WM) is the cognitive skill that allows us to hold information in mind for just long enough to use it [1]. Information is held in mind as active neural traffic until it is forgotten, erased by the next job, or consolidated in long-term memory. WM tasks are those that require goal-oriented active monitoring or manipulation of information or behaviors in the face of interfering processes and distractions. WM can be seen as the “cognitive workbench” of the human brain, as it requires goal-oriented active monitoring or manipulation of information or behaviors in the face of interfering processes and distractions. The more people can station in their WM, the larger their cognitive ‘capacity’ and, as a consequence, their success in multi-tasking. WM—an important aspect of executive functioning (EF)—is associated with a wide range of complex cognitive behaviors, such as planning, impulse control, and reasoning [2,3], and is recently recognized as a predictor of success in top-football players [4]. WM forms the basis for learning, planning, linguistic, mathematical, and learning skills, organizing, staying focused, knowledge acquisition, impulse control, and reasoning [2,3]. EF includes the abilities of goal formation, planning, carrying out goal-directed plans, and effective performance [5]. EF skills are viewed as crucial developmental building blocks in cognitive and social abilities, with a focus on three core EF skills: working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive or mental flexibility [6]. An excellent football player could be characterized by excellent cognitive functions, such as spatial attention, divided attention, inhibition, and WM. The player must be able to quickly adapt, change strategy, and inhibit responses in the ongoing, complex, and quickly changing stream of information during a soccer match. This is also referred to as “game intelligence” [4]. The paper of Vestberg et al. [4] suggests that both High Division (HD) and Lower Division (LD) football players had significantly better measures of executive functions in comparison to a norm group, for both men and women. Moreover, the HD players outperformed the LD players in these tests. In the second prospective part of the study, a partial correlation test showed a significant correlation between the result from the executive test and the numbers of assists and goals the players had scored two seasons later. The results from this study strongly suggest that cognitive test performance predicts the success of ball sport players [4].
WM can directly be trained by cognitive training programs, as has recently been shown in a variety of populations, for instance children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and aging adults [7,8]. Klingberg et al. [9] developed a home-based computerized working memory training (WMT) program, resulting in better concentration, more control over impulsive behavior, and better complex reasoning skills. However, methodological shortcomings in many WMT studies have led to criticism on the use of WMT [6,10,11]. Only near-transfer, short-term improvements on verbal and nonverbal WM tasks were found, meaning that these effects are not generalizable to other functions or activities and are not sustained for a longer period of time. Even though there is critique on the WMT program, it is exciting to investigate the possibility of expanding WM capacity in football players through such program. If it is possible to improve the working memory function in football players, this could lead to improved performance on the pitch.
In the present study, we aimed to assess the feasibility of WMT in a group of professional football players. Primarily, the effect of WMT was investigated on trained WM tasks, non-trained WM tasks, and other cognitive measures. In addition, the generalizability of the effect of WMT to everyday life, in this case football performance, was investigated. In addition, non-cognitive measures, like self-efficacy and mental health, were included in this study. In previous research, it was shown that in professional sports, symptoms of common mental disorders, like distress, anxiety/depression, and sleep disturbance can negatively influence sport performance [12]. Since it was suspected that both psychological wellbeing and self-efficacy play an important role in WMT feasibility, these factors were examined as well. That is, self-efficacy induces people to strive, choose a certain activity, persevere and not give up, overcome temporary difficulties, and control the events that affect their lives, so that they can achieve their goals [13,14]. Also, the current literature suggests that competitive sport may contribute to poor mental health and that there is a stigma on mental health in athletes [15]. Finally, personality factors were included, as some, e.g., impulsivity, have been related to WM functioning [16].

2. Methods

2.1. Design and Population

Eighteen professional football players from Maatschappelijke Voetbal Vereniging Maastricht (MVV) in the southern part of The Netherlands participated in this study (all males; mean age = 23.78, SD = 4.08; range 17–31). All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. At baseline, motivation to participate was evaluated via an interview, followed by a neuropsychological assessment. For this study, the WMT program Cogmed was used, supervised by a certified Cogmed coach. Directly after WMT and three months after, all players were assessed with the neuropsychological test measures and Loughborough Soccer Passing Test (LSPT). For this study a waitlist control design was used (see Figure 1). Subjects were randomly assigned to the experimental group (N = 8, mean age = 22.63, SD = 2.86, range = 20–29) or waitlist control group (N = 6, mean age = 25.00, SD = 5.40, range: 17–31).

2.2. Intervention

WMT (Cogmed) consisted of 25 computerized training sessions. Each session (duration of 30–45 min) consisted of a selection of various tasks that targeted the different aspects of WM. The participants were not directly trained on forwards or backwards digit recall, but on near-transfer tasks, e.g., memorizing which numbers lightened up on a screen. The training program was five-week-long with five sessions every week. The training was led by a qualified coach who worked with the user to provide structure, motivation, and feedback on the progress. The coach planned and structured the sessions with every individual, provided advice on how to get the most out of the training, gave personal feedback by mail every week on the training, and was available for questions. Further, if necessary, individual appointments intended to increase motivation were planned. However, no football players requested an additional appointment. Therefore, every football player received the same amount of attention/information. Both the user and the coach were able to review and monitor the results of each day’s training, using the online system. For this study, the coaches were two experienced neuropsychologists who completed the Cogmed coach program. The neuropsychological assessment was administered by trained psychologists.

2.3. Measurements

First, the neuropsychological assessment covered various cognitive domains. Intelligence was measured by the standard abbreviated form of the Groninger Intelligence Test-2 (GIT), consisting of several subtests: word list (woordenlijst), verbal abstract reasoning (matrijzen), puzzles (legkaarten), discovering of figures (figuur ontdekken), arithmetic (cijferen), fluency (professions and animals) [17]. Sufficient reliability, good concept validity, and sufficient criterion validity were reported for the overall GIT [18].
Short-term memory was assessed by the WAIS-III Digit span subtest (forward) and the Corsi Block-Tapping Task (CBTT); working memory was assessed by the WAIS-III Digit span subtest (backward) [19,20]. These tests were used as outcome measures of near-transfer effects. The digit span subtest of the WAIS consists of both a forward and a backward (reverse-order) recitation task, in which digit sequences are read aloud by an examiner. The sequences start with two digits that increase in length each trial. When subjects fail to accurately report back the sequence, testing is ceased [20]. Good reliability, sufficient concept validity, and insufficient criterion validity were reported for the overall WAIS-III [21]. During the CBTT, the participant had to mimic the researcher as he/she tapped a sequence of up to nine identical spatially separated blocks. It is a visual analogue of the WAIS-III Digit Span [22] which measures visuo-spatial short-term working memory. Because of the many variations and modifications of the task parameters, there is a paucity of assessments of its psychometric properties [23].
To evaluate learning capacity, efficiency of storage, and retrieval processes of newly learned verbal material, the Verbal Learning Test (VLT-15) was used [24]. Four parallel versions were used to avoid learning effects through rehearsal. In this test, 15 words were read out loud in a fixed order. After each trial, the patient was asked to reproduce the memorized words (immediate recall). Twenty minutes after the last trial, the patient was asked again to reproduce the set of words (delayed recall). Following the delayed recall, a list of 30 words was read out lout, and the subjects had to state whether or not the presented word was on the learning list (recognition). Acceptable reliability was reported for total and delayed recall scores [25].
The Stroop Color–Word Test (SCWT), the Concept Shifting Test (CST), and the Zoo map from the Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) were used to measure EF, such as mental flexibility and vulnerability to interference [26,27,28]. In the SCWT, the interference of an automatic process (reading) with a more effort-demanding, controlled process task (naming colors) was investigated [27]. The SCWT consists of three subtasks: color word naming (I), color naming (II), and naming of color words printed in a different color (inference task III). The time needed to complete each card was scored [28]. The SCWT has been shown to have moderate to high reliability and validity [29]. The CST consists of three stimulus cards, and, on each test sheet, the participant is asked to cross out 16 circles in a certain order. All three stages are preceded by practice trails that contain six circles. In the first stage, CSTA, the trial is in numerical order, and in the second stage, CSTB, it is in alphabetical order. On the last stage, CSTC, both letters and numbers are shown, and circles need to be crossed out in number–letter–number–letter order, i.e., 1, A, 2, B, 3, C, etc. After this, a page with empty circles is shown (CST-0), and the participant has to cross them out, as fast as possible to compute the motor speed. Although many studies acknowledge the usefulness of the CST to assess concept shifting and related functions, its validity is still to be established [30]. The Zoo map subtest is a planning test that provides information about the ability to plan a route to visit 6 of possible 12 locations in a zoo. Reliability and validity have not been researched [31].
In addition to these objective instruments, the Cognitive Failure Questionnaire (CFQ; [32,33] and the Dutch version of the Working Memory Questionnaire (WMQ) were assessed [34]. Both questionnaires are self-report, with the CFQ consisting of 25 items (five-point scale, 0—never, 4—very often) measuring the frequency of everyday cognitive failures in the past 6 months, and the WMQ consisting of 30 items (7-point scale, 0—not at all, 5—extremely, 6—not relevant) measuring three dimensions of WM: short-term storage, attention, and executive control. Good test–retest and internal reliability of the CFQ were reported [35]. Good internal consistency and sensitivity were reported for the WMQ, and concurrent validity with the CFQ [34].
Secondly, differences in football skill performance in football players was assessed with the LSPT [36]. The LSPT requires players to complete 16 passes as quickly as possible, to listen to the test leader, memorize a color, decide where to pass the ball (four color options), and pass the ball. The time needed to complete 16 passes was used as the outcome measure. To improve accuracy, the LSPT was videotaped, and time was measured afterwards. The LSPT was reported to be a valid and reliable way to assess differences in football skill performance [36].
Thirdly, general psychological wellbeing was screened with the Symptom Check List-90 (SCL-90) [37], and self-efficacy, which is defined as the belief that one is capable of performing in a certain manner to attain certain goals, was assessed with the Dutch General Self-efficacy Scale (GSE) [38]. Personality was assessed with the NEO-Five-Factor Inventory (FFI), measuring five core personality traits (neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness) [39]. For the SCL-90, good internal consistency, construct validity, and divergent validity were reported [37]. For the GSE, high internal consistency and item-total correlations were reported, indicating that it is a reliable scale [40]. For the NEO-FFI, the foremost manner to test the validity is to replicate its factor structure, which the majority of studies have done, and the psychometric properties of the NEO-FFI are robust [41].
Finally, as for subject characteristics, age was used as a continuous variable. Educational level was indexed on an eight-point ordinal scale, ranging from primary to university education [24]. We used the “WMT start index” and “WMT index improvement” from the Cogmed software program as outcome measures of the effect on the trained aspects of WM. The WMT improvement index was calculated by subtracting the WMT start index from the WMT max index (i.e., mean of the three best trials of the two best training days).

2.4. Statistical Considerations

The neuropsychological test results of the complete football team were compared with the performance of healthy individuals of similar age and educational level by using normative data from the Maastricht Aging Study [42]. Descriptive statistics were performed to summarize sample characteristics. Given our small sample size, no statistical analyses were performed.

3. Results

The average WMT start index was 97.27 (SD = 8.87). Performance on the LSPT was 43.44 (SD = 2.07), which is comparable to earlier reported performance times of an elite football team (M = 43.6, SD = 3.8, [36]). The z-scores and c-scores of all neuropsychological tests indicated an overall average performance of this football team, reflecting cognitive performance within the normal range (Table 1). The experimental and waitlist control group showed to be roughly similar in terms of age, mean intelligent quotient (IQ) score, WMT start index, and performance on LPST (Table 1). Further, no clear change with regard to neuropsychological performance (Table 1), psychological well-being, subjective cognitive functioning, and self-efficacy was observed (Table 2).

Feasibility

Of 18 football players who participated in this study, 14 started with WMT. Of these, 11 players completed WMT. Only 5 out of 14 players met the compliance criteria (defined as ≥20 training days within 5 weeks) [9]. Three subjects trained for 25 days, and two for 24 days. Overall, one subject did not meet the improvement index criterion (≥17), scoring 13.68. WMT improvement index ranged from 13.68 to 53.87. Of those who met the compliance criteria, the improvement index ranged from 31.75 to 53.87. Differences between compliers and non-compliers were observed for personality and well-being. For example, at t1, compliers scored lower on the NEO-FFI scale neuroticism (M = 21.67, SD = 1.52, versus M = 27.43, SD = 3.26) and higher on the scale extraversion (M = 52.33, SD = 3.21, versus M = 43.71, SD = 5.82) than non-compliers. Further, compliers scored lower on psychological wellbeing, i.e., psychoneuroticism, at t1 (M = 103.50, SD = 10.47, versus M = 114.33, SD = 15.20). We also observed drop-outs to be younger (M = 20.33, SD = 2.89 versus M = 24.55, SD = 4.03) and to have lower IQ scores (M = 83.67, SD = 22.75 versus M = 91.73, SD = 15.14) as compared to those who remained in the study.

4. Discussion

For the first time, this small-scale feasibility study evaluated the effects of WMT in professional football players. The WMT start index at baseline was similar for both the experimental and the control group. The WMT improvement index indicated that players performed better on the trained WM tasks (i.e., Cogmed task); however, no change on measures of neuropsychological tests, i.e., attention, verbal and visual WM, and planning, was observed over the study period, for both the experimental and the control group. Thus, we were not able to demonstrate a positive effect of WMT on non-trained neuropsychological measures, including measures of verbal and visual WM (i.e., the test battery as listed below Section 2.3). In addition, no change in football performance was found over the course of the WMT. Internal consistency with regard to LSPT scores was good, as mean LSPT scores found in this study resemble those reported in prior studies that assessed soccer performance in adolescents and young elite players [36]. It is important to note that at baseline, the experimental and control group were similar with respect to demographic variables (e.g., age, level of education, or measures of intelligence), personality, psychiatric symptoms, subjective cognitive functioning, and self-efficacy.
Interestingly, we observed that non-compliers were emotionally more instable and experienced more psychological complaints than compliers. This seems to indicate that the individuals who experienced less (cognitive) problems were more willing and able to commit to the WMT program. Also, it has to be noted that drop-outs were on average younger and had a lower IQ score.
A strength of the current study is that it was conducted in a natural setting and used a random assignment of football players to the different groups. The major limitation of the current study is the small sample size, because of which we were not able to follow through with our planned statistical testing. The most important observation made during the study was a lack of motivation of the football players. Only 5 out of 14 players met the compliance criteria (defined as ≥20 training days within 5 weeks). This was disappointing and somewhat unexpected, as participation was stimulated by trainers and medical staff of MVV. Furthermore, the coaches were present every week for questions, problems, and to improve motivation and were available for questions through mail. However, previous studies in the adult population also encountered difficulties with recruitment and retention, such as in subjects with ADHD [43]. High attrition rates were attributed to the fact that the WMT did not meet subjects’ levels of acceptability or practicality. Further, the program was experienced as far more time-consuming than adverted [43]. The authors reasoned that the difference with respect to the efficacy rates seen in children might be explained by the fact that the latter were closely monitored and scaffolded by clinicians and caregivers [43]. We hypothesize that football players did not experience enough cognitive complaints as shown by the results of the CFQ. In daily life practice, it seemed difficult for football players to make the translation between cognitive exercises and performance on the pitch, since there was no direct reward. This could indicate that low internal motivation was present for intensive WMT, and, therefore, the need to incorporate the WMT in their daily routine was not recognized as such. It is important to note that our study was initially meant as a small-scale feasibility study, and the fact that we were able to examine factors that we hypothesized to influence compliance rates (e.g., personality traits) in an objective manner can therefore be considered as a strength.
The main point of criticism regarding WMT is that improvements (if at all) are not found to be generalizable to other functions or activities [8,9]. It is worth noting that improvements in memory tasks after intensive practice could be the result of task-specific strategies, not of the improvement of memory itself. Some even argued that WMT programs are based on a naïve “physical–energetic” model: training WM (process, e.g., such a exercising a muscle) will not necessarily result in improvement in that process (e.g., strengthening that muscle) [10].

5. Clinical Implications

The results of this small study show that WMT is not a feasible and effective strategy to improve WM and football performance. Further, it is essential to characterize the psychological wellbeing of the subjects before starting an intensive computerized WMT. In this study, we did not focus on individual patterns of WMT benefits. It is likely that some football players will benefit more than others from WMT. Future research should investigate individual patterns, like personality, cognitive failures, and psychological wellbeing and their influence on WMT programs.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.I.d.B., R.P.; Methodology, D.I.d.B., K.D., R.P.; Software, NA; Validation, D.I.d.B., K.D., R.P.; Formal Analysis, L.B.; Investigation, D.I.d.B., K.D., T.K.; Resources, D.I.d.B., R.P.; Data Curation, D.I.d.B., K.D., T.K.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, D.I.d.B., L.B.; Writing—Review & Editing, D.I.d.B., K.D., L.B., R.P.; Visualization, L.B.; Supervision, R.P.; Project Administration, D.I.d.B., K.D., R.P.; Funding Acquisition, R.P.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Baddeley, A. Working Memory (Oxford Psychology Series No. 11); Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  2. Miyake, A.; Shah, P. Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  3. Phye, G.D.; Pickering, S.J. Working Memory and Education; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  4. Vestberg, T.; Gustafson, R.; Maurex, L.; Ingvar, M.; Petrovic, P. Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer players. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e34731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Lezak, M.D. Neuropsychological Assessment; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  6. Diamond, A.; Ling, D.S. Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 18, 34–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Beck, S.J.; Hanson, C.A.; Puffenberger, S.S.; Benninger, K.L.; Benninger, W.B. A controlled trial of working memory training for children and adolescents with ADHD. J. Clin. Child Adoles. Psychol. 2010, 39, 825–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Borella, E.; Carretti, B.; Riboldi, F.; De Beni, R. Working memory training in older adults: Evidence of transfer and maintenance effects. Psychol. Aging 2010, 25, 767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Klingberg, T.; Fernell, E.; Olesen, P.J.; Johnson, M.; Gustafsson, P.; Dahlström, K.; Gillberg, C.G.; Forssberg, H.; Westerberg, H. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD-a randomized, controlled trial. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2005, 44, 177–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Melby-Lervåg, M.; Hulme, C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 49, 270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Shipstead, Z.; Hicks, K.L.; Engle, R.W. Cogmed working memory training: Does the evidence support the claims? J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2012, 1, 185–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Gouttebarge, V.; Hopley, P.; Kerkhoffs, G.; Verhagen, E.; Viljoen, W.; Wylleman, P.; Lambert, M.A. 12-month prospective cohort study of symptoms of common mental disorders among professional rugby players. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2018, 18, 1004–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Bandura, A. Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived self-efficacy. Dev. Psychol. 1989, 25, 729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Gencturk, A.; Memis, A. An. Investigation of Primary School Teachers’ Teacher Efficacy and Job Satisfaction in Terms of Demographic Factors. Ilkogr. Online 2010, 9, 1037. [Google Scholar]
  15. Bauman, N.J. The Stigma of Mental Health in Athletes: Are Mental Toughness and Mental Health Seen as Contradictory in Elite Sport? BMJ Publishing Group Ltd and British Association of Sport and Exercise Medicine: London, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  16. Cools, R.; Sheridan, M.; Jacobs, E.; D’Esposito, M. Impulsive personality predicts dopamine-dependent changes in frontostriatal activity during component processes of working memory. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 5506–5514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Luteijn, F.; Van der Ploeg, F.A. Groninger Intelligentie Test.: Handleiding [Groninger Intelligence Test.: Manual]; Swets, Zeitlinger BV: Lisse, The Netherlands, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  18. Egberink, I.J.L.; Vermeulen, C.S.M. COTAN Beoordeling 2006, Groninger Intelligentie Test 2, GIT-2 [COTAN Review 2006, GIT-2]. Available online: www.cotandocumentatie.nl (accessed on 29 November 2018).
  19. Kessels, R.P.; Van Zandvoort, M.J.; Postma, A.; Kappelle, L.J.; De Haan, E.H. The Corsi block-tapping task: Standardization and normative data. Appl. Neuropsychol. 2000, 7, 252–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Wechsler, D. WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale: Administration and Scoring Manual; Sychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  21. Egberink, I.J.L.; Vermeulen, C.S.M. COTAN Beoordeling 2006, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III, WAIS-III [COTAN Review 2006, WAIS-III]. Available online: www.cotandocumentatie.nl (accessed on 29 November 2018).
  22. Kessels, R.P.; van Den Berg, E.; Ruis, C.; Brands, A.M. The backward span of the Corsi Block-Tapping Task and its association with the WAIS-III Digit Span. Assessment 2008, 15, 426–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Berch, D.B.; Krikorian, R.; Huha, E.M. The Corsi block-tapping task: Methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain Cognit. 1998, 38, 317–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Brand, N.; Jolles, J. Learning and retrieval rate of words presented auditorily and visually. J Gen. Psychol. 1985, 112, 201–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Van der Elst, W.; Van Boxtel, M.P.; Van Breukelen, G.J.; Jolles, J. Detecting the significance of changes in performance on the Stroop Color.-Word Test., Rey’s Verbal Learning Test., and the Letter Digit. Substitution Test.: The regression-based change approach. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2008, 14, 71–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Burgess, P.W.; Alderman, N.; Evans, J.; Emslie, H.; Wilson, B.A. The ecological validity of tests of executive function. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1998, 4, 547–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Stroop, J.R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1992, 121, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Van der Elst, W.; Van Boxtel, M.P.; Van Breukelen, G.J.; Jolles, J. The Stroop color-word test: Influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment 2006, 13, 62–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Strauss, E.; Sherman, E.M.; Spreen, O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary; American Chemical Society: Washington, DC, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  30. Van der Elst, W.; Van Boxtel, M.P.; Van Breukelen, G.J.; Jolles, J. The Concept Shifting Test.: Adult normative data. Psychol. Assess. 2006, 18, 424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Egberink, I.J.L.; Vermeulen, C.S.M. COTAN Beoordeling 1999, Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexcutive Syndrome, BADS [COTAN Review 1999, WAIS-III]. Available online: www.cotandocumentatie.nl (accessed on 29 November 2018).
  32. Broadbent, D.E.; Cooper, P.F.; FitzGerald, P.; Parkes, K.R. The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1982, 21, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ponds, R.; Van Boxtel, M.; Jolles, J. De Cognitive Failure Questionnaire als maat voor subjectief cognitief functioneren. Tijdschr. Neuropsychol. 2006, 2, 37–45. [Google Scholar]
  34. Vallat-Azouvi, C.; Pradat-Diehl, P.; Azouvi, P. The Working Memory Questionnaire: A scale to assess everyday life problems related to deficits of working memory in brain injured patients. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2012, 22, 634–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Bridger, R.S.; Johnsen, S.Å.K.; Brasher, K. Psychometric properties of the cognitive failures questionnaire. Ergonomics 2013, 56, 1515–1524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Ali, A.; Williams, C.; Hulse, M.; Strudwick, A.; Reddin, J.; Howarth, L.; Eldred, J.; Hirst, M.; McGregor, S. Reliability and validity of two tests of soccer skill. J. Sports Sci. 2007, 25, 1461–1470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Buckelew, S.P.; Burk, J.P.; Brownlee-Duffeck, M.; Frank, R.G.; DeGood, D. Cognitive and somatic aspects of depression among a rehabilitation sample: Reliability and validity of SCL-90—R research subscales. Rehabil. Psychol. 1988, 33, 67–75. [Google Scholar]
  38. Teeuw, B.; Schwarzer, R.; Jerusalem, M. Dutch Adaptation of the General Self-Efficacy Scale. Berlin, Germany, 1994. Available online: https://www.fysiotherapiewetenschap.com/bestanden/45.pdf (accessed on 6 July 2018).
  39. Costa, P.T.; MacCrae, R.R. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources, Incorporated: Lutz, FL, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  40. Scholz, U.; Doña, B.G.; Sud, S.; Schwarzer, R. Is general self-efficacy a universal construct? Psychometric findings from 25 countries. Eur. J. psychol. Assess. 2002, 18, 242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Costa, P.; McCrae, R.R. The Five-Factor Model and the NEO Inventories. In Oxford Handbook of Personality Assessment; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 299–322. [Google Scholar]
  42. Jolles, J.; Houx, P.J.; Van Boxtel, M.P.J.; Ponds, R.W.H.M. The Maastricht Aging Study. Determinants of Cognitive Aging; Neuropsych Publishers: Maastricht, The Netherlands, 1995; p. 192. [Google Scholar]
  43. Marcelle, E.T.; Ho, E.J.; Kaplan, M.; Adler, L.A.; Castellanos, F.X.; Milham, M. Cogmed Working Memory Training Presents Unique Implementation Challenges in Adults With ADHD. Front. Psychiatry 2018, 9, 388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Study Design. a, b, c and d indicate changes in outcome variables; with a and d indicating an intervention effect; in case a significant effect on a is found, these can be compared to b, with the direction of change indicating whether there is an intervention effect (i.e., no change or positive change) or a temporary effect (negative change).
Figure 1. Study Design. a, b, c and d indicate changes in outcome variables; with a and d indicating an intervention effect; in case a significant effect on a is found, these can be compared to b, with the direction of change indicating whether there is an intervention effect (i.e., no change or positive change) or a temporary effect (negative change).
Sports 07 00089 g001
Table 1. Subject characteristics pre- and post-working memory training (WMT).
Table 1. Subject characteristics pre- and post-working memory training (WMT).
Standardized Scores *Unstandardized Scores **
Total GroupExperimental GroupWaitlist Control
Pre-WMTPre-WMTPre-WMT (t1)Post-WMT (t2)Pre-WMT (t2)Post-WMT (t5)
(N = 18)(N = 18)(N = 8)(N = 5)(N = 6)(N = 6)
Age 23.78 (4.08)22.63 (2.83)NA25.00 (5.40)NA
Cogmed start index 97.27 (8.87)97.18 (10.01)NA97.38 (8.12)NA
LSPT 43.44 (2.07)42.93 (2.30)39.88 (4.64)40.66 (0.68)38.20 (2.67)
GIT
IQ 89.06 (16.63)86.25 (12.03)NA95.00 (21.00)NA
Wordlist4.33 (2.52)11.00 (3.82)11.00 (3.42)NA11.50 (3.39)NA
Puzzles4.67 (2.09)12.56 (3.75)11.63 (3.81)NA13.33 (4.27)NA
Figures4.17 (3.19)11.44 (5.09)9.87 (4.76)NA11.50 (6.38)NA
Arithmic4.72 (2.27)8.56 (3.28)9.63 (2.62)NA9.33 (3.88)NA
Verbal Abstract Reasoning4.00 (1.57)11.61 (2.38)11.13 (2.17)NA12.33 (3.01)NA
Fluency – Animals3.83 (2.12)22.50 (6.20)22.13 (7.79)NA24.33 (5.35)NA
SCWT
Interference0.42 (0.81)32.06 (11.04)29.57 (2.56)31.36 (5.08)24.45 (8.11)22.63 (6.86)
CST
Interference-0.15 (1.07)6.99 (5.96)5.96 (3.32)6.50 (3.85)5.32 (3.37)8.94 (10.80)
WAIS-III digit span
Forward 9.56 (2.04)9.88 (1.46)11.60 (1.82)10.50 (2.88)11.33 (3.56)
Backward 6.83 (2.66)7.13 (2.59)9.60 (2.19)10.00 (2.28)10.33 (2.87)
Corsi
Total score 9.78 (1.59)10.50 (1.07)10.60 (1.67)10.00 (1.10)11.00 (2.45)
Longest sequence 6.39 (0.78)6.75 (0.46)6.40 (0.89)6.50 (0.55)6.50 (1.22)
BADS Zoo 3.17 (0.92)3.13 (0.64)3.00 (1.00)3.00 (1.26)3.83 (0.41)
VLT
Immediate Recall0.99 (1.06)57.44 (8.03)57.00 (8.81)64.20 (6.91)64.50 (4.46)66.33 (4.13)
Delayed Recall0.74 (0.83)12.83 (1.98)12.88 (1.89)13.60 (1.67)14.17 (1.17)13.83 (1.83)
Note. LSPT is Loughborough Soccer Passing Test; GIT is Groninger Intelligence Test-2; IQ is intelligence quotient; SCWT is Stroop Color–Word Test; CST is Concept Shifting Test; WAIS-III is Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; Corsi is Corsi block-tapping task; BADS zoo is Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome, Zoo map; VLT is Verbal Learning Test-15. ** LSPT Experimental group (N = 5) and Waitlist control (N = 5). * standardized scores, i.e., computed c-scores for the GIT and computed z-scores (using normative means) for the SCWT, CST, and VLT; ** unstandardized scores, i.e., raw test scores.
Table 2. Baseline measures of personality, psychiatric symptoms, subjective cognitive functioning, and self-efficacy.
Table 2. Baseline measures of personality, psychiatric symptoms, subjective cognitive functioning, and self-efficacy.
Total GroupExperimental GroupWaitlist Control
M (SD)M (SD)M (SD)
NEO-FFIN = 13N = 7N = 3
Neuroticism25.92 (3.97)27.00 (3.65)22.67 (3.06)
Extraversion45.69 (6.33)43.86 (5.98)52.00 (3.61)
Openness to Experience30.54 (4.68)28.86 (5.08)33.67 (3.51)
Agreeableness44.39 (5.49)42.00 (4.73)48.67 (7.57)
Conscientiousness48.08 (5.66)48.57 (3.55)52.00 (8.89)
SCL-90N = 11N = 6N = 4
Psychoneuroticism109.82 (13.30)112.83 (14.48)105.75 (14.10)
N = 11N = 6N = 4
GSE30.46 (3.21)29.17 (3.19)33.00 (1.83)
CFQ69.91 (12.26)70.33 (11.76)75.50 (4.65)
WMQ48.73 (12.92)49.00 (14.95)11.00 (7.35)
Note. NEO-FFI is NEO Five-Factor Inventory; SCL-90 is Symptom Check List—90; GSE is Dutch General Self-Efficacy Scale; CFQ is Cognitive Failures Questionnaire; WMQ is Working Memory Questionnaire. Data are presented as raw test scores.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

In de Braek, D.; Deckers, K.; Kleinhesselink, T.; Banning, L.; Ponds, R. Working Memory Training in Professional Football Players: A Small-Scale Descriptive Feasibility Study—The Importance of Personality, Psychological Well-Being, and Motivational Factors. Sports 2019, 7, 89. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports7040089

AMA Style

In de Braek D, Deckers K, Kleinhesselink T, Banning L, Ponds R. Working Memory Training in Professional Football Players: A Small-Scale Descriptive Feasibility Study—The Importance of Personality, Psychological Well-Being, and Motivational Factors. Sports. 2019; 7(4):89. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports7040089

Chicago/Turabian Style

In de Braek, Dymphie, Kay Deckers, Timo Kleinhesselink, Leonie Banning, and Rudolf Ponds. 2019. "Working Memory Training in Professional Football Players: A Small-Scale Descriptive Feasibility Study—The Importance of Personality, Psychological Well-Being, and Motivational Factors" Sports 7, no. 4: 89. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports7040089

APA Style

In de Braek, D., Deckers, K., Kleinhesselink, T., Banning, L., & Ponds, R. (2019). Working Memory Training in Professional Football Players: A Small-Scale Descriptive Feasibility Study—The Importance of Personality, Psychological Well-Being, and Motivational Factors. Sports, 7(4), 89. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports7040089

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop