1. Introduction
High-level sprint performance is a necessary fundamental skill in many sports, in not only adults, but also in youth. Therefore, there has been high interest in improving fundamental skills, as successful athletes and the physical fitness of general children require the acquisition of a higher sprint performance at a younger age [
1,
2]. Many considerations are required to establish effective and efficient training methods for improving sprint performance, because youth athletes must attain high-level performance in many physical aspects of their sport (e.g., agility, power and sport-specific techniques).
As children mature, increases in body height, body mass, lower limb strength and neuromuscular function allow for concurrent improvements in sprint performance. Sprint velocity, which is a major variable in sprint performance, is calculated as the product of step frequency (SF) and step length (SL), also known as the spatiotemporal variables. It is necessary to increase one factor as long as the other factor is maintained or increased, since the relationship between the two factors is negatively correlated at maximum effort [
3]. Along with other aspects of physical maturation, previous studies found that development of sprint velocity is associated with an increase in SL [
4,
5]. Although SL increases do continue into adulthood, there is a plateau of development in sprint performance associated with a decreased SF during 1.5–2.5 years before peak height velocity (PHV) in boys [
4,
5,
6].
Nagahara et al. [
5] reported that there is a stage of temporal slower development of sprint performance with a decrease in SF in Japanese boys aged 8.8–12.1 years. Therefore, research is needed to investigate the training methods to maintain or improve this SF in young boys aged before achieving PHV. However, training adaptations among spatiotemporal variables by any physical training have not been clarified in children.
Plyometric training is performed by exerting maximal muscular power in a short duration, and includes activities such as jumping, hopping, skipping and bounding. This training is a popular exercise for improving the performance of various athletic activities, including those using sprinting [
7,
8,
9,
10,
11,
12]. One meta-analysis revealed a moderate effect of plyometric training on sprint performance in healthy adults [
11]. Moreover, sprint time was also significantly reduced after plyometric training in youth players who belong to a sports club, including soccer and tennis [
7,
8,
12] and nonathletic children aged before achieving PHV [
9,
10]. Lloyd et al. [
10] reported that a 6-week plyometric training program reduced the 10-m sprint time in non-athletic boys aged 1.5 ± 0.4 years before achieving PHV (age: 12.7 ± 0.7 years). Those findings suggested that plyometric training reduces sprint time regardless of whether it was performed in athletic or nonathletic children aged before achieving PHV. However, as we mentioned, adaptive mechanisms leading to the decrease in sprint time (e.g., increase in SF or SL) after plyometric training have not been clarified in children, although the definitive positive efficacy of this training on jumping and sprinting performance is evident. In male adult sprinters, two weeks of plyometric training decreased a 20-m sprint time through an increased SF, which was the result of a reduction in ground contact time during the support phase [
13]. In contrast, Lockie et al. [
14] reported that plyometric training decreased the 10-m sprint time through an increase in SL. To the best of our knowledge, no study has examined the mechanisms by which plyometric training reduces sprint time by changes in sprint spatiotemporal variables in children.
Thus, we aimed to investigate the effect of plyometric training on sprint performance including spatiotemporal variables in boys aged before achieving PHV. We hypothesized that plyometric training has the positive effect on sprint velocity derived from enhancements in both SF and SL in this age group.
3. Results
There were no significant differences in variables among demographic, anthropometric, sprint performance and jumping performance between groups at the baseline. The CG did not show any significant changes in measurement variables after intervention. Significant group-by-time interactions were observed for 50-m sprint time (
F = 12.673,
p = 0.002, η
p2 = 0.413), maximal velocity (
F = 7.797,
p = 0.012, η
p2 = 0.302) and 50-m SL (
F = 5.401,
p = 0.032, η
p2 = 0.231) (
Table 3). In the PG, post hoc analysis revealed a significant decrease in 50-m sprint time (
p = 0.005, d = −0.442) and increase in maximal velocity (
p = 0.031, d = 0.489). An increase in 50-m SL tended to be significant (
p = 0.064, d = 0.235).
Regarding 10-m interval characteristics,
Table 4,
Table 5 and
Table 6 show the changes in sprint velocity, SL and SF, respectively. Significant group-by-time interactions were observed for sprint velocity for 10–20 m, 20–30 m, 30–40 m, and 40–50 m intervals (
F = 4.548–10.005,
p < 0.05 for all, η
p2 = 0.202–0.357). In PG, post hoc analysis showed significant increases in sprint velocity during 20–30 m (
p = 0.033, d = 0.424), 30–40 m (
p = 0.002, d = 0.474) and 40–50 m intervals (
p = 0.026, d = 0.398). In addition, a significant interaction for SL for 0–10 m and 20–30 m intervals was observed (
F = 8.620 and 5.820,
p = 0.009 and 0.027, η
p2 = 0.324 and 0.244, respectively). In PG, post hoc analysis showed a significant increase in SL during the 0–10 m (
p < 0.001, d = 0.258), 20–30 m (
p = 0.037, d = 0.281) and 30–40 m intervals (
p = 0.038, d = 0.389). There were no significant interactions observed for SF.
Statistical analysis showed significant interactions for SLJ (
F = 4.530,
p = 0.047, η
p2 = 0.201) and the RJ-index (
F = 8.189,
p = 0.010, η
p2 = 0.313). An interaction for RJCT tended to be significant (
F = 3.682,
p = 0.071, η
p2 = 0.170) (
Table 7). The in PG, post hoc analysis revealed significant increases in SLJ (
p = 0.005, d = 0.364) and the RJ-index (
p = 0.019, d = 0.542), and a decrease in RJCT (
p = 0.026, d = −0.571). There were no main effects and interactions in SJ, CMJ and RJ height.
4. Discussion
The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects of plyometric training on sprint performance including SF and SL in boys aged 9–12 years. Our study results showed that plyometric training has positive effects on sprint velocity and SL during the maximum velocity phase in young boys aged before achieving their PHV. In addition, small to medium increases in RJ performance and SLJ were observed. To our knowledge, this is the first study to clarify that the adaptive mechanism of improvement in sprint time was driven by an increase in SL in pre-adolescent boys.
Our primary finding was the significant positive effect of plyometric training on SL, not SF. This result was in line with the previous findings conducted by Lockie et al. [
14], which showed that 6-week plyometric training improved SL in male field sports players. The reduction of ground contact time is related to an increase in SF during sprint running [
3]. Although the program of this present study significantly reduced RJCT, SF did not increase. The present study also revealed an increasing trend in horizontal lower muscular strength (i.e., distance in SLJ). A previous study reported that an increase in horizonal power is related to the increase in step length [
14]. Our study also found a significant improvement of step length in 0–10 m. In addition, the distance during SLJ is related to flight time; horizontal jumps are negatively related to SF [
25]. Therefore, the present training program may maintain SF. In contrast, Mackala et al. [
13] showed that two weeks of plyometric training, including various types of jumping, significantly increased vertical and horizontal jumping. Furthermore, the increase in vertical jumps was greater than in horizonal jump in male sprinters [
13]. That study also reported that the program increased only SF, but not SL [
13]. Aforementioned, there is a slower temporal development of sprint performance with a decrease in SF in boys aged before achieving PHV [
5]. Therefore, our findings suggest that plyometric training might maintain SF in boys aged before achieving PHV. Future studies are needed to examine the effects on spatiotemporal variables in children of different age groups and the relationships between various types of training, jumping performance and spatiotemporal variables.
There were significant improvements of sprint velocity during 20–30 m, 30–40 m and 40–50 m intervals, which are categorized as the secondary acceleration and maximal velocity phases. This supports a previous study that reported that sprint time during the 10–20 m and 20–30 m, but not the 0–10 m intervals, were improved after plyometric training [
9]. In addition, Diallo et al. [
26] and Sohnlein et al. [
12] showed a greater decrease in 20-m sprint time after plyometric training than in that of the control group without exercise intervention in prepubescent soccer players; however, changes in 5-m and 10-m sprint time were not observed. The plyometric training program performed in the present study mainly consisted of repeated jumps requiring the shortest ground contact time possible (i.e., jumping over barriers, tuck jumps, power skips and bounding). Therefore, the RJ-index significantly increased after training intervention. Bret et al. [
27] reported that the hopping test, like the RJ test, is related to sprint velocity after 30 m, and the height of the CMJ is related to sprint velocity before 30 m in national level male sprinters. Therefore, in the present study, the sprint velocity during the maximal velocity phase was improved as reflected by an increase in the RJ-index. However, another previous study in male field sport athletes reported that plyometric training decreases 5-m sprint time, but not 10-m sprint time [
14]. These contrary results could be explained by differences in plyometric training modes and participant characteristics. In the present study, SJ and CMJ, which are related to sprint velocity during the acceleration phase by previous studies [
21,
27], did not exhibit improvements. We suggest that changes in sprint velocity during the initial acceleration phase were not observed in the present study owing to the lack of changes in SJ and CMJ performance.
Our study results have practical relevance for the methods of plyometric training used in pre-adolescent boys. Strength and conditioning coaches and physical education teachers should note that plyometric training, such as that used in this study, improves RJ performance and SL, particularly during the maximal running phase. In addition, if coaches wish to improve SF, other training modalities (e.g., assisted sprint training, as described by Macadam et al. [
28]) should be added to the present program. However, the optimal method to improve SF in pre-adolescent children needs to be clarified.
Furthermore, the present study observed a reduced 50-m sprint time as performed once per week for eight weeks (−2.78%, d = −0.442). A previous study showed significant decreases in 20-m sprint time (−2.94%, d = −0.45) after twice-weekly plyometric training for six weeks in prepubertal children [
10]. According to these results, increased training volume and frequency may not afford a larger effect of training. In adults, low-to-moderate training frequency (once per week or twice per week) produces a similar effect on jumping and sprint performance as that with higher training frequency (three times per week) [
29]. Regarding training volume within a single session, Chaabene et al. [
7] reported similar improvements on sprint time and jump performance between low (60–120 jumps per session) and high (110–220 jumps per session) volume training in pre-adolescent children. A meta-analysis that summarized the effects of plyometric training on jumping and sprint performance in healthy adults revealed no significant association between the total changes in sprint performance and the total number of ground contacts [
11]. Those findings suggest that plyometric training has equally efficacious methods to improve sprint performance, even with a lower training volume. The program used in the present study may be effective and time-efficient for improving sprint performance in both active and nonactive children.
The present study has some limitations. First, we did not assess ground contact and aerial times during sprint running. Second, we measured the mean values of spatiotemporal variables at 10-m intervals; thus, changes in variables for each step were not determined. Recently, there are different strategies of ascertaining changes in spatiotemporal variables for each step, and these variables are affected by age [
5] and athletic level [
30]. To the best of our knowledge, no study has revealed the effect of training on spatiotemporal variables and kinetic values for each step during 50-m or 100-m events. Thus, future studies are needed to determine the effect of training on variables during each step. Third, either the exercise intensity or volume in CG was not matched with PG. It is difficult to match the exercise intensity or volume of other exercise modalities with the plyometric training because the training volume of plyometric training depends only on the number of ground contacts, and the exercise intensity of plyometric training depends on the height of the jumps. Accordingly, exercise volume or intensity on plyometric training cannot be directly translated into other exercise modality with difference movements. However, the length of a training session was equated for approximately 60 min for both training groups.