Next Article in Journal
Efficacy of Manual Therapy and Electrophysical Modalities for Treatment of Cubital Tunnel Syndrome: A Randomized Interventional Trial
Previous Article in Journal
Exposure to Per- and Polyfluoroalkyl Substances (PFASs) in Healthcare: Environmental and Clinical Insights
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Prenatal Valproic Acid Exposure Affects Song Learning in Zebra Finches: A Potential Model for Vocal Development in Autism

by
Estifanos Ghebrihiwet Tewelde
1,2,
Boglárka Morvai
1,
Gergely Zachar
3,* and
Ákos Pogány
1
1
Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, 1117 Budapest, Hungary
2
Department of Biology, Mai-Nefhi College of Science, Mai-Nefhi P.O. Box 12676, Eritrea
3
Department of Anatomy, Histology and Embryology, Semmelweis University, 1094 Budapest, Hungary
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Life 2025, 15(7), 1058; https://doi.org/10.3390/life15071058
Submission received: 16 May 2025 / Revised: 22 June 2025 / Accepted: 27 June 2025 / Published: 1 July 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Animal Science)

Abstract

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) encompasses a range of neurodevelopmental conditions characterized by impairments in social abilities and communication. Studying appropriate animal models can enhance our understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying these conditions, potentially leading to improved treatment and intervention strategies. Modeling impairments in linguistic development and vocal communication caused by autism had been a challenging goal for a long time. Prenatal valproic acid (VPA) treatment has been successfully used to induce ASD-like behavioral symptoms in several vertebrate species including birds. Applying VPA-treatment on songbirds, therefore, offers a promising research paradigm to model ASD. In this study, we investigated the effect of embryonic VPA treatment on song learning in the highly social zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Fertilized eggs were injected with either 0.45 µmol VPA or a saline solution on day 9 of incubation, and hatchlings were allowed to stay with their parents until day 35 post-hatching to facilitate song learning from the father. Once male offsprings reached adulthood, their songs were recorded and compared to those of their fathers. VPA-exposed males exhibited significantly greater similarity to their fathers’ songs compared to control males (74% vs. 31%, respectively), suggesting a reduced ability to modify and develop their own vocal patterns as subadults. Additionally, they showed higher entropy in their songs compared to controls (−1.4 vs. −1.7), indicating more disorganized vocalizations. These findings suggest that prenatal VPA exposure disrupts typical song learning and vocal development in zebra finches, likely by affecting neural mechanisms involved in vocal learning and crystallization of the songs. Our study suggests VPA treatment in songbirds provides a useful tool to model and investigate linguistic developmental disorders related to ASD in humans.

1. Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD), ranked as the third most common human developmental disorder, encompasses a range of neurodevelopmental conditions characterized by difficulties in social interaction, communication, and behavior [1]. Besides a lack of interest in others, further symptoms include difficulties in expressing emotions, resistance to physical contact, and repetitive behaviors [1,2]. Classic autism, at one end of the spectrum, is also associated with delayed or absent speech development and, in some cases, cognitive delays [3,4]. The causes of ASD remain unclear, though research suggests a complex interplay of genetic, biological, psychosocial, and environmental risk factors [1,5].
Valproic acid (VPA), chemically known as 2-propyl-pentanoic acid, is a branched short-chain fatty acid derived from valeric acid, a compound found in the Valerian plant (Valeriana officinalis). It exhibits significant pharmacological properties and has been widely prescribed to treat epilepsy, bipolar, and anxiety disorders [6,7,8,9,10]. At non-toxic therapeutic doses, it acts as a potent and selective inhibitor of histone deacetylase (HDAC) enzymes [6]. However, VPA also induces teratogenic effects, particularly in human embryos, leading to the ‘valproate syndrome’, which is characterized by reduced verbal intelligence and communication difficulties commonly associated with ASD [11].
VPA’s teratogenic effects have been documented in various species and taxa, including mammals, birds, and fish. In domestic chickens, exposure to VPA reduces hatching rates, and hatched chicks display paralysis, locomotor impairments, and altered social behaviors [12,13]. In mice, prenatal VPA exposure results in delayed physical growth, reduced play behavior, and abnormal social interactions in juveniles, which persists into adulthood [2,14]. Similarly, zebrafish embryos exposed to VPA exhibit developmental abnormalities, including skeletal deformities, abnormal swimming patterns, and pericardial fluid accumulation. These impairments affect locomotion, social interactions, and macrocephalic traits linked to ASD. The severity of these symptoms depends on VPA concentration and timing of exposure [15,16,17]. Embryonic VPA treatment is proven to be useful as a pharmacological model of autism in rodents [18,19], domestic chicks [20], and zebrafish [21]. Certain aspects of autism, however, are very difficult if not impossible to study in the aforementioned N taxa, including the impairment of linguistic development which occurs in autistic children [22].
The zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata), a highly social songbird, is an ideal species for studying developmental impairments of social behavior and vocal communication. These passerine exhibit sexual dimorphism and form monogamous pair bonds following mate choice based partially on courtship songs [23,24]. Parents cooperate to raise their offspring [25], and behavioral coordination during biparental care relies on vocalizations [26]. Zebra finches are well-suited for research due to their adaptability; they breed year-round and well in the laboratory with a generational cycle of approximately 90 days [23,27]. Among others, they are used as the primary avian model for studying song learning, a process that shares similarities with early communication development in various species, including humans [24]. Research by Chen et al. [28], for instance, found that zebra finch song tutors adjust their vocal patterns when teaching juveniles, much like how humans modify their speech when speaking to infants.
Due to their complex social interactions, vocal learning abilities, and cooperation during biparental care, we propose that zebra finches are excellent potential models for investigating social and communicative impairments, including those seen in autism. The similarities between zebra finch vocal learning, i.e., the ability to alter innate vocalizations, and human speech acquisition offer insights into the neurobiological basis of communication disorders [29,30]. Specifically, comparable expression patterns of FOXP1 and FOXP2, two genes associated with coordinated motor control and communication, have been described in relation to vocal learning in both humans and songbirds [31,32,33]. Based on the above, studying the effects of social and environmental factors on zebra finch behavior and neurodevelopment can deepen our understanding of social impairments in humans, potentially leading to better diagnostic tools and therapies for ASD.
Despite the unique opportunity to study a vocal learning system so similar to that of the human [30,34,35], no attempt has been made so far to adapt the VPA model to any of the songbird species. By adapting this well-established research paradigm to a new songbird animal model, the zebra finch, our aim is to offer new insights into the neurodevelopmental mechanisms underlying ASD, potentially contributing to better treatment and intervention strategies by increasing validity. Specifically, we investigated the impact of embryonic (in ovo) VPA treatment on song learning in zebra finches, by focusing on similarities between the songs of male offspring and those of their fathers and non-related other males in the population.

2. Results

The entropy of female-directed songs produced by VPA-treated males was significantly higher than that of control males (Linear Mixed Model (LMM) of entropy, likelihood ratio tests (LRT) of experimental group: χ21 = 8.824, p = 0.003; Ctrl → VPA: b ± SE = 0.29 ± 0.09, z = 3.13, p = 0.002; Figure 1, Table 1). None of the other investigated acoustic properties showed significant differences between the two groups, although pitch was somewhat (non-significantly) lower in the VPA group than in the control group (LMM of pitch, LRT of experimental group: χ21 = 3.131, p = 0.077; for all other acoustic properties p > 0.542, Table 1).
Male offspring in the VPA group exhibited significantly greater high-scale similarity to their fathers’ songs compared to males in the control group (LMM of high-scale similarity, LRT of experimental group: χ21 = 5.720, p = 0.017; Ctrl → VPA: b ± SE = 35.04 ± 13.88, z = 2.52, p = 0.012; Figure 2, Table 2). Low-scale similarity, sequential match and father–son differences in any of the other investigated acoustic properties were not significantly different between experimental groups (all p > 0.144, Table 2). Bold p values represent significant differences.

3. Discussion

Learning and producing songs are crucial developmental aspects in songbirds and can be paralleled in many ways with human language acquisition and speech. In this study, we demonstrated for the first time that prenatal exposure to valproic acid (VPA) affected male zebra finch offspring’s song learning process, leading to changes in the acoustic properties of the learned song and its resemblance to the father’s song.
A comparison between the songs of fathers and their male offspring revealed that VPA-treated males exhibited a higher syllable-level similarity to their fathers’ songs, indicating that a greater proportion of paternal syllables contributed to their songs. In contrast, control males displayed lower similarity, suggesting that they modified their songs to a higher degree after they were separated from their parents (from post-hatching day 35). These findings imply that VPA-treated males may have difficulties adapting and developing unique song patterns at later developmental stages, potentially due to biological constraints. One possibility is that they are only capable of recalling and reproducing songs heard during early development (up to day 35 post-hatching). Alternatively, they may lack the ability to refine or modify their songs by learning from other males. The song circuitry in zebra finches becomes fully functional by 35 days of age, and a sensitive learning phase occurs between days 25 and 65 post-hatching [36]. During this period, young birds may acquire songs not only from their fathers but also from unrelated males and brothers, after leaving the family [37,38]. In our study, while both VPA-treated and control males initially learned songs from their fathers, results suggest only the control group was able to modify and further develop their vocalizations.
Additionally, VPA-treated males produced songs with higher entropy, suggesting that their vocalizations were more disorganized or closer to random noise as those of control males. Higher entropy usually signifies lower stability and tonality of the song [39]. In general, VPA-treated males retained a more similar song to those of their fathers’ and developed a less organized song compared to controls. A possible explanation for such differences is that early song learning is less affected, whereas crystallization of songs is more impaired by VPA treatment, either by affecting social learning from cage mates or by impairing the self-correcting feedback mechanism underlying song learning.
Our results extend the empirical evidence for prenatal VPA treatment to alter acoustic communication in rodents (mouse and rats) and birds (domestic chicken) [13,40,41,42,43,44]. Although these studies reported prenatal VPA treatment to result in vocalizing at a lower volume and/or with reduced intensity, our results, focusing on the more elaborated vocalization of zebra finches, do not support such an effect. Besides accurately recalling and reproducing a significant portion of their fathers’ syllables, VPA-treated males modulated the amplitude of their songs similarly to control birds. Song complexity in male zebra finches is closely linked to cognitive abilities and plays a crucial role during mate selection. Female zebra finches are attracted to males with intricate and diverse vocalizations [45] and prefer songs with higher frequencies [46]. Additionally, they tend to choose social partners and extra-pair mates based on greater syllable variety in songs [46,47]. Consequently, developing complex, frequent, and powerful songs are under positive selection, as these traits enhance male reproductive success. In contrast, male zebra finches that are not able to refine their songs over time to increase their complexity, individuality, and audibility, develop inferior vocalizations that can reduce their survival and mating success [48].
In conclusion, our results suggest that VPA-treated males could not develop and produce typical, complex songs and we assume an impaired crystallization is behind these findings. Future research should investigate how these VPA-induced changes relate exactly to autism spectrum disorder, i.e., to what extent acoustic communication and/or social impairments (e.g., via impaired social learning) are responsible for such atypical song acquisition and production. To further validate zebra finches as models of vocal development during autism, more experiments are needed. It is imperative to dissect the effects of VPA on the vertical (paternal) and oblique (non-related other conspecifics) influence on the crystallized song. Our study, taken together, suggests VPA-treatment in songbirds, and especially in zebra finches is a promising model to investigate the mechanisms of linguistic impairments associated with ASD.

4. Materials and Methods

4.1. Subjects

The study was conducted at the Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary. We used captive zebra finches from the stock population of the Department of Ethology, which was established in 2013 and originated from the domesticated zebra finches maintained at Bielefeld University, Germany [49]. 52 pairs were formed from unrelated individuals, of which 23 pairs reproduced successfully, generating at least one brood; from the resulting 79 offspring 42 were males. Some of the males; therefore, contributed to our dataset with multiple broods (mean ± SD n of broods per male: 1.7 ± 0.9), and some of the broods included more than one male offspring (mean ± SD n of male siblings per brood: 1.6 ± 0.8), and we considered these sources of non-independence in our dataset statistically (see below).
The breeding pairs were housed in separate cages (dimensions: 100 cm × 30 cm × 35 cm), each equipped with a nest box (12 × 12 × 12 cm), attached to the outside of the cage. Coconut fibers were provided as nesting material. Numbered aluminum rings were used to identify individuals (Principle Kft., Újlengyel, Hungary). Room temperature was kept between 18.2 °C and 23.4 °C, and the humidity level between 41% and 66%. An artificial light-dark cycle of 14:10 h was maintained in the room. Birds were provided with ad libitum access to water and food, containing red and white millet (Panicum miliaceum), canary grass (Phalaris canariensis), and a hint of Niger seed (Guizotia abyssinica). To supplement their protein and vitamin intake, the birds received daily portions of egg food (egg food tropical finches, Orlux, Versele-Laga, Deinze, Belgium) and germinated seeds (produced on-site using the aforementioned seed blend). To reduce the stress caused by captivity the animals were kept in the social environment that is natural for their developmental stage (with their family, same sex colonies, or in mating pairs).

4.2. Prenatal VPA Treatment

In a separate study (Pogány et al. [50], submitted), we adapted VPA-treatment for zebra finches by optimizing dosage and timing based on offspring survival and development, and using the reported parameters for the domestic chicken as reference [12,51,52]. Briefly, we started from the applied 35 µmol/egg VPA in 200 µL volume per egg in chicken and considered the ca. 60-fold egg mass difference as well as different incubation times between the two species. We then used these derived parameters, and slightly increased/decreased values in a 3 × 3 factorial design (i.e., both dosage and timing had three levels). Based on our findings (Pogány et al. [50], submitted), here we applied 0.45 µmol valproic acid (VPA group) or 0.9% saline solution (control group) on day 9 of incubation; injections had a volume of 3.3 µL per egg.
The first brood of a given pair was assigned to either of two experimental groups randomly, so that all eggs in the brood were injected with either valproic acid solution (VPA) or saline solution (sham control). If there were subsequent broods of the same pair, they were assigned to experimental groups in a balanced design (e.g., if the first brood received VPA-treatment, the second brood received control treatment, and vice versa). Valproic acid solution was prepared by dissolving Sodium valproate (Depakine®, Sanofi-Aventis, Paris, France) in sterile, pyrogen-free distilled water. To prevent contamination, alcohol was used to sanitize the needle, the injector, and the egg surface. A sterile needle was used to perforate the eggshell, then the substance was administered into the egg’s air sac using a Hamilton constant rate syringe (CR700, 20 µL volume, Hamilton Central Europe S.R.L., Timișoara, Romania). Following injection, the hole was sealed using a drop of super glue. Eggs were monitored daily until they hatched.

4.3. Song Recording and Analysis

Juvenile birds were separated from the parents on day 35 post-hatching (Figure 3). At first, they were housed in (mixed-sex) juvenile cages, and later (around day 60 post-hatching), when their sex-specific traits became evident, subadults were moved into separate male and female aviaries in another room, in which adult males and females were also present [37].
We recorded female-directed songs from 16 fathers and 42 male offspring, including 23 VPA-treated and 19 control males at the age of 133.6 ± 9.2 (mean ± SD) days. One day before the recording, the recorded male and a female stimulus bird were placed into the opposite sides of a double cage within a song recording chamber, separated by a wire mesh barrier. Each father was tested with their current female partner as stimulus, whereas male offspring were tested with randomly selected females. On the following day, female-directed songs were audio and video recorded continuously between 4:00 am and 12:00 am. In case the subject failed to sing at least 10 clear syllables, recording was repeated on a different day; this involved five male offspring (all of these second recordings were successful). We used an Electret Tie-Clip ECM-2500 microphone (Alan Butcher Components Ltd., Blandford, Dorset, UK) and a Mobius Maxi 4K camera (Huizhou Tuopu Xunshi Technology Co., Huizhou, China) for recordings.
We selected the first 10 clearly audible syllables of each subject for song analysis [45] using the Audacity software (v. 3.7.0) [53]. To analyze the selected 10-syllable long songs, we used Sound Analysis Pro 2011 (SAP2011, v.11.104) [54]. The following acoustic properties were calculated and compared between VPA-treated and control male offspring: duration, amplitude, mean frequency, peak frequency, pitch, pitch goodness (an estimate of harmonic pitch periodicity), frequency modulation (FM), amplitude modulation (AM), and Wiener entropy (see Supplementary Materials). Song samples of the male offspring were also compared to those of their fathers based on five acoustic properties: pitch, pitch goodness, FM, AM, and entropy. The automated analysis broke down the samples into larger (50–70 ms) and smaller (5–10 ms) time interval units and provided in percentage how closely the father–son songs resemble each other (high- and low-scale similarities, respectively). Additionally, it calculated the similarity in terms of the sequential structure of the songs (sequential match). Furthermore, we calculated the difference between VPA-treated and control father–son pairs in the following acoustic properties of the songs: pitch, pitch goodness, FM, AM, and entropy.

4.4. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were carried out using the R statistical environment (v. 4.4.2) [55]. Each acoustic property was compared between the VPA (n = 23) and control (n = 19) male offspring. Additionally, we investigated differences between the groups in how closely offspring songs resembled those of their fathers and compared the differences in each acoustic property between sons and fathers. Acoustic properties (and father–son differences) were analyzed in separate Linear Mixed Models (LMMs) using Template Model Builder (R package ‘glmmTMB’) [56]. LMMs included the experimental group as a two-level fixed factor (VPA vs. control). In addition, LMMs of acoustic properties included father ID/brood ID as nested random terms, whereas LMMs of father–son acoustic differences included brood ID as a random term to control for non-independence due to multiple broods and siblings from the same father. The effect of experimental group was analyzed by likelihood ratio tests (LRTs), and we provide χ2 and p-values of LRTs of the models when including or excluding experimental group.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/life15071058/s1, Table S1: dataset.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, G.Z. and Á.P.; methodology, G.Z. and E.G.T.; formal analysis, E.G.T., B.M., and Á.P.; investigation, E.G.T.; data curation, B.M. and Á.P.; writing—original draft preparation, E.G.T. and Á.P.; writing—review and editing, all authors; funding acquisition, G.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Research Development and Innovation Office (NKFIH FK131966). Á.P. was supported by the Hungarian Ethology Foundation. G.Z. was supported by a Bolyai Scholarship from the Hungarian Academy of Sciences and by a UNKP Bolyai+ Scholarship from the Ministry for Innovation and Technology.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Eötvös Loránd University (certificate no. ELTE MÁB 02/2014) and the Food Chain Safety and Animal Health Directorate Government Office (Budapest, Hungary) under the certificate no. PE/EA/00486-2/2023 (approval date 30 March 2023).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study is available as Supplementary Materials of this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
ASDAutism spectrum disorder
VPAValproic acid
LMMLinear mixed model
LRTLikelihood ratio test

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Text Revision, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2022; ISBN 0890425760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Campolongo, M.; Kazlauskas, N.; Falasco, G.; Urrutia, L.; Salgueiro, N.; Höcht, C.; Depino, A.M. Sociability Deficits after Prenatal Exposure to Valproic Acid Are Rescued by Early Social Enrichment. Mol. Autism 2018, 9, 36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Simmons, K.L. The Official Autism 101 Manual. Everything You Need to Know About Autism from Experts Who Know and Care, 3rd ed.; Alderson, J., Ed.; Skyhorse Publishing: New York, NY, USA, 2018; ISBN 1510722556. [Google Scholar]
  4. Gagnon, D.; Zeribi, A.; Douard, É.; Courchesne, V.; Rodríguez-Herreros, B.; Huguet, G.; Jacquemont, S.; Loum, M.A.; Mottron, L. Bayonet-Shaped Language Development in Autism with Regression: A Retrospective Study. Mol. Autism 2021, 12, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Zeidan, J.; Fombonne, E.; Scorah, J.; Ibrahim, A.; Durkin, M.S.; Saxena, S.; Yusuf, A.; Shih, A.; Elsabbagh, M. Global Prevalence of Autism: A Systematic Review Update. Autism Res. 2022, 15, 778–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Činčárová, L.; Zdráhal, Z.; Fajkus, J. New Perspectives of Valproic Acid in Clinical Practice. Expert Opin. Investig. Drugs 2013, 22, 1535–1547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Safdar, A.; Ismail, F. A Comprehensive Review on Pharmacological Applications and Drug-Induced Toxicity of Valproic Acid. Saudi Pharm. J. 2023, 31, 265–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Perucca, E. Pharmacological and Therapeutic Properties of Valproate: A Summary after 35 Years of Clinical Experience. CNS Drugs 2002, 16, 695–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chateauvieux, S.; Morceau, F.; Dicato, M.; Diederich, M. Molecular and Therapeutic Potential and Toxicity of Valproic Acid. BioMed Res. Int. 2010, 2010, 479364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Pagliaro, L.A.; Pagliaro, A.M. Valproic Acid [Divalproex Sodium; Sodium Valproate; Valproate Sodium]. In PNDR: Psychologists’ Neuropsychotropic Drug Reference; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2020; pp. 1–9. ISBN 9781315825748. [Google Scholar]
  11. Ornoy, A. Valproic Acid in Pregnancy: How Much Are We Endangering the Embryo and Fetus? Reprod. Toxicol. 2009, 28, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Nishigori, H.; Kagami, K.; Takahashi, A.; Tezuka, Y.; Sanbe, A.; Nishigori, H. Impaired Social Behavior in Chicks Exposed to Sodium Valproate during the Last Week of Embryogenesis. Psychopharmacology 2013, 227, 393–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Zachar, G.; Tóth, A.S.; Gerecsei, L.I.; Zsebok, S.; Ádám, Á.; Csillag, A. Valproate Exposure in Ovo Attenuates the Acquisition of Social Preferences of Young Post-Hatch Domestic Chicks. Front. Physiol. 2019, 10, 881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Roullet, F.I.; Wollaston, L.; deCatanzaro, D.; Foster, J.A. Behavioral and Molecular Changes in the Mouse in Response to Prenatal Exposure to the Anti-Epileptic Drug Valproic Acid. Neuroscience 2010, 170, 514–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Chen, J.; Lei, L.; Tian, L.; Hou, F.; Roper, C.; Ge, X.; Zhao, Y.; Chen, Y.; Dong, Q.; Tanguay, R.L.; et al. Developmental and Behavioral Alterations in Zebrafish Embryonically Exposed to Valproic Acid (VPA): An Aquatic Model for Autism. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2018, 66, 8–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Liu, X.; Zhang, Y.; Lin, J.; Xia, Q.; Guo, N.; Li, Q. Social Preference Deficits in Juvenile Zebrafish Induced by Early Chronic Exposure to Sodium Valproate. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Aluru, N.; Deak, K.L.; Jenny, M.J.; Hahn, M.E. Developmental Exposure to Valproic Acid Alters the Expression of MicroRNAs Involved in Neurodevelopment in Zebrafish. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2013, 40, 46–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ornoy, A.; Echefu, B.; Becker, M. Animal Models of Autistic-like Behavior in Rodents: A Scoping Review and Call for a Comprehensive Scoring System. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2024, 25, 10469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Li, Z.; Zhu, Y.X.; Gu, L.J.; Cheng, Y. Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders with Animal Models: Applications, Insights, and Perspectives. Zool. Res. 2021, 42, 800–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Csillag, A.; Ádám, Á.; Zachar, G. Avian Models for Brain Mechanisms Underlying Altered Social Behavior in Autism. Front. Physiol. 2022, 13, 1032046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Camussi, D.; Marchese, M.; Nicoletti, F.; Santorelli, F.M.; Ogi, A. Valproate-Induced Model of Autism in Adult Zebrafish: A Systematic Review. Cells 2025, 14, 109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ornoy, A.; Echefu, B.; Becker, M. Valproic Acid in Pregnancy Revisited: Neurobehavioral, Biochemical and Molecular Changes Affecting the Embryo and Fetus in Humans and in Animals: A Narrative Review. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2023, 25, 390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Clayton, N.S. Assortative Mating in Zebra Finch Subspecies, Taeniopygia guttata guttata and T. g. castanotis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 1990, 330, 351–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Swaddle, J.P. Zebra Finches. In Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior; Choe, J.C., Ed.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2019; pp. 279–284. [Google Scholar]
  25. Pogány, Á.; Szurovecz, Z.; Vincze, E.; Barta, Z.; Székely, T. Mate Preference Does Not Influence Reproductive Motivation and Parental Cooperation in Female Zebra Finches. Behaviour 2014, 151, 1885–1901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Boucaud, I.C.A.; Mariette, M.M.; Villain, A.S.; Vignal, C. Vocal Negotiation over Parental Care? Acoustic Communication at the Nest Predicts Partners’ Incubation Share. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2016, 117, 322–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hauber, M.E.; Louder, M.I.; Griffith, S.C. The Natural History of Model Organisms Neurogenomic Insights into the Behavioral and Vocal Development of the Zebra Finch. eLife 2021, 10, e61849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Chen, Y.; Matheson, L.E.; Sakata, J.T. Mechanisms Underlying the Social Enhancement of Vocal Learning in Songbirds. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, 6641–6646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. White, S.A.; Fisher, S.E.; Geschwind, D.H.; Scharff, C.; Holy, T.E. Singing Mice, Songbirds, and More: Models for FOXP2 Function and Dysfunction in Human Speech and Language. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 10376–10379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Pfenning, A.R.; Hara, E.; Whitney, O.; Rivas, M.V.; Wang, R.; Roulhac, P.L.; Howard, J.T.; Wirthlin, M.; Lovell, P.V.; Ganapathy, G.; et al. Convergent Transcriptional Specializations in the Brains of Humans and Song-Learning Birds. Science 2014, 346, 1256846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Teramitsu, I.; Kudo, L.C.; London, S.E.; Geschwind, D.H.; White, S.A. Parallel FoxP1 and FoxP2 Expression in Songbird and Human Brain Predicts Functional Interaction. J. Neurosci. 2004, 24, 3152–3163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Garcia-Oscos, F.; Koch, T.M.I.; Pancholi, H.; Trusel, M.; Daliparthi, V.; Co, M.; Park, S.E.; Ayhan, F.; Alam, D.H.; Holdway, J.E.; et al. Autism-Linked Gene FoxP1 Selectively Regulates the Cultural Transmission of Learned Vocalizations. Sci. Adv. 2021, 7, eabd2827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Heim, F.; Scharff, C.; Fisher, S.E.; Riebel, K.; ten Cate, C. Auditory Discrimination Learning and Acoustic Cue Weighing in Female Zebra Finches with Localized FoxP1 Knockdowns. J. Neurophysiol. 2024, 131, 950–963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Brainard, M.S.; Doupe, A.J. Translating Birdsong: Songbirds as a Model for Basic and Applied Medical Research. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2013, 36, 489–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Mello, C.V. The Zebra Finch, Taeniopygia Guttata: An Avian Model for Investigating the Neurobiological Basis of Vocal Learning. Cold Spring Harb. Protoc. 2014, 2014, 1237–1242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Braaten, R.F. Song Recognition in Zebra Finches: Are There Sensitive Periods for Song Memorization? Learn. Motiv. 2010, 41, 202–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Roper, A.; Zann, R. The Onset of Song Learning and Song Tutor Selection in Fledgling Zebra Finches. Ethology 2006, 112, 458–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Derégnaucourt, S.; Gahr, M. Horizontal Transmission of the Father’s Song in the Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Biol. Lett. 2013, 9, 20130247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Tchernichovski, O.; Mitra, P.P.; Lints, T.; Nottebohm, F. Dynamics of the Vocal Imitation Process: How a Zebra Finch Learns Its Song. Science 2001, 291, 2564–2569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Tsuji, T.; Mizutani, R.; Minami, K.; Furuhara, K.; Fujisaku, T.; Pinyue, F.; Jing, Z.; Tsuji, C. Oxytocin Administration Modulates the Complex Type of Ultrasonic Vocalisation of Mice Pups Prenatally Exposed to Valproic Acid. Neurosci. Lett. 2021, 758, 135985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Tsuji, C.; Furuhara, K.; Mizutani, R.; Minami, K.; Fu, P.; Zhong, J.; Higashida, H.; Yokoyama, S.; Tsuji, T. Early-Onset of Social Communication and Locomotion Activity in F2 Pups of a Valproic Acid-Induced Mouse Model of Autism. Neurosci. Lett. 2022, 788, 136827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Cezar, L.C.; da Fonseca, C.C.N.; Klein, M.O.; Kirsten, T.B.; Felicio, L.F. Prenatal Valproic Acid Induces Autistic-Like Behaviors in Rats via Dopaminergic Modulation in Nigrostriatal and Mesocorticolimbic Pathways. J. Neurochem. 2025, 169, e16282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Gzielo, K.; Potasiewicz, A.; Hołuj, M.; Litwa, E.; Popik, P.; Nikiforuk, A. Valproic Acid Exposure Impairs Ultrasonic Communication in Infant, Adolescent and Adult Rats. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020, 41, 52–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Agarwalla, S.; Yuvarani, M.S.; Bandyopadhyay, S. Alterations in the Ultrasonic Vocalization Sequences in Pups of an Autism Spectrum Disorder Mouse Model: A Longitudinal Study over Age and Sex. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2025, 139, 111372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Boogert, N.J.; Giraldeau, L.A.; Lefebvre, L. Song Complexity Correlates with Learning Ability in Zebra Finch Males. Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 1735–1741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Houtman, A.M. Female Zebra Finches Choose Extra-Pair Copulations with Genetically Attractive Males. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 1992, 249, 3–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Riebel, K. Song and Female Mate Choice in Zebra Finches: A Review. Adv. Study Behav. 2009, 40, 197–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Woodgate, J.L.; Mariette, M.M.; Bennett, A.T.D.; Griffith, S.C.; Buchanan, K.L. Male Song Structure Predicts Reproductive Success in a Wild Zebra Finch Population. Anim. Behav. 2012, 83, 773–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Forstmeier, W.; Segelbacher, G.; Mueller, J.C.; Kempenaers, B. Genetic Variation and Differentiation in Captive and Wild Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Mol. Ecol. 2007, 16, 4039–4050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Pogány, Á.; Tewelde, E.G.; Morvai, B.; Zachar, G. Optimization of valproic acid treatment to study autism in a novel, ecologically valid animal model, the zebra finch. Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary. 2025; submitted for publication. [Google Scholar]
  51. Sgadò, P.; Rosa-Salva, O.; Versace, E.; Vallortigara, G. Embryonic Exposure to Valproic Acid Impairs Social Predispositions of Newly-Hatched Chicks. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 5919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lorenzi, E.; Pross, A.; Rosa-Salva, O.; Versace, E.; Sgadò, P.; Vallortigara, G. Embryonic Exposure to Valproic Acid Affects Social Predispositions for Dynamic Cues of Animate Motion in Newly-Hatched Chicks. Front. Physiol. 2019, 10, 501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Audicity Team Audacity(R); Version 3.7.0; Free Audio Editor and Recorder [Computer Program]; Muse Group: Limassol, Cyprus, 2024; Available online: http://audacity.sourceforge.net/ (accessed on 30 November 2024).
  54. Tchernichovski, O.; Nottebohm, F.; Ho, C.E.; Pesaran, B.; Mitra, P.P. A Procedure for an Automated Measurement of Song Similarity. Anim. Behav. 2000, 59, 1167–1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; The R Foundation: Vienna, Austria, 2024; Available online: http://www.r-project.org/ (accessed on 12 December 2024).
  56. Brooks, M.E.; Kristensen, K.; van Benthem, K.J.; Magnusson, A.; Berg, C.W.; Nielsen, A.; Skaug, H.J.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.M. GlmmTMB Balances Speed and Flexibility among Packages for Zero-Inflated Generalized Linear Mixed Modeling. R J. 2017, 9, 378–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Entropy of female-directed songs of valproic acid-treated (VPA) and control (Ctrl) male zebra finches. Boxplots indicate the median, 1st and 3rd quartiles (boxes), and whiskers represent data within 1.5 IQR from them.
Figure 1. Entropy of female-directed songs of valproic acid-treated (VPA) and control (Ctrl) male zebra finches. Boxplots indicate the median, 1st and 3rd quartiles (boxes), and whiskers represent data within 1.5 IQR from them.
Life 15 01058 g001
Figure 2. High-scale similarity between female-directed songs of valproic acid-treated (VPA) and control (Ctrl) male zebra finches and their fathers. Boxplots indicate the median, 1st and 3rd quartiles (boxes), and whiskers represent data within 1.5 interquartile range from them.
Figure 2. High-scale similarity between female-directed songs of valproic acid-treated (VPA) and control (Ctrl) male zebra finches and their fathers. Boxplots indicate the median, 1st and 3rd quartiles (boxes), and whiskers represent data within 1.5 interquartile range from them.
Life 15 01058 g002
Figure 3. Timeline of the experiment and the social environment of the animals.
Figure 3. Timeline of the experiment and the social environment of the animals.
Life 15 01058 g003
Table 1. Linear Mixed Models (LMM) with various acoustic properties of female-directed songs in valproic acid-treated (VPA; n = 23) and control (Ctrl; n = 19) male zebra finches. LMMs were fitted separately for each acoustic property (response), and we provide χ2 and p-values of likelihood ratio tests between models when including or excluding experimental group as a fixed factor (father ID and brood ID were included in LMMs as nested random terms).
Table 1. Linear Mixed Models (LMM) with various acoustic properties of female-directed songs in valproic acid-treated (VPA; n = 23) and control (Ctrl; n = 19) male zebra finches. LMMs were fitted separately for each acoustic property (response), and we provide χ2 and p-values of likelihood ratio tests between models when including or excluding experimental group as a fixed factor (father ID and brood ID were included in LMMs as nested random terms).
Acoustic PropertyExp. GroupMean ± SEχ21p
duration (ms)VPA90.88 ± 6.410.0020.966
Ctrl84.16 ± 8.97
amplitudeVPA45.32 ± 0.410.3720.542
Ctrl44.57 ± 0.22
mean frequency (Hz)VPA2744.93 ± 66.61<0.0010.993
Ctrl2743.96 ± 102.21
peak frequency (Hz)VPA2878.36 ± 84.750.0050.946
Ctrl2868.75 ± 122.90
pitch (Hz)VPA470.50 ± 35.313.1310.077
Ctrl594.27 ± 68.18
goodness of pitchVPA150.04 ± 3.360.1670.683
Ctrl146.41 ± 5.64
FM (kHz)VPA42.33 ± 1.060.3260.568
Ctrl41.23 ± 1.75
AMVPA0.00 ± 0.010.2380.626
Ctrl0.02 ± 0.02
Wiener entropyVPA−1.41 ± 0.068.8240.003
Ctrl−1.70 ± 0.07
Table 2. Linear Mixed Models (LMM) with father–son differences in various acoustic properties of female-directed songs in valproic acid-treated (VPA; n = 23) and control (Ctrl; n = 19) male zebra finches. LMMs were fitted separately for each acoustic property and difference (response), and we provide χ2 and p-values of likelihood ratio tests when including or excluding experimental group as a fixed factor (brood ID was included in LMMS as a random term). Bold p values represent significant differences.
Table 2. Linear Mixed Models (LMM) with father–son differences in various acoustic properties of female-directed songs in valproic acid-treated (VPA; n = 23) and control (Ctrl; n = 19) male zebra finches. LMMs were fitted separately for each acoustic property and difference (response), and we provide χ2 and p-values of likelihood ratio tests when including or excluding experimental group as a fixed factor (brood ID was included in LMMS as a random term). Bold p values represent significant differences.
Acoustic Property (Diff)Exp. GroupMean ± SEχ21p
high-scale similarity (%)VPA73.63 ± 6.925.7200.017
Ctrl30.89 ± 8.67
low-scale similarity (%)VPA62.12 ± 4.571.5010.221
Ctrl48.60 ± 6.08
sequential match (%)VPA73.20 ± 6.612.1350.144
Ctrl87.55 ± 6.78
pitch difference (Hz)VPA1.55 ± 0.180.4430.506
Ctrl1.88 ± 0.33
goodness of pitch differenceVPA2.91 ± 0.510.1900.663
Ctrl2.40 ± 0.71
FM difference (kHz)VPA1.04 ± 0.041.4330.231
Ctrl1.14 ± 0.06
AM differenceVPA1.10 ± 0.031.9400.164
Ctrl0.95 ± 0.06
Wiener entropy differenceVPA1.42 ± 0.370.3510.554
Ctrl2.03 ± 0.69
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tewelde, E.G.; Morvai, B.; Zachar, G.; Pogány, Á. Prenatal Valproic Acid Exposure Affects Song Learning in Zebra Finches: A Potential Model for Vocal Development in Autism. Life 2025, 15, 1058. https://doi.org/10.3390/life15071058

AMA Style

Tewelde EG, Morvai B, Zachar G, Pogány Á. Prenatal Valproic Acid Exposure Affects Song Learning in Zebra Finches: A Potential Model for Vocal Development in Autism. Life. 2025; 15(7):1058. https://doi.org/10.3390/life15071058

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tewelde, Estifanos Ghebrihiwet, Boglárka Morvai, Gergely Zachar, and Ákos Pogány. 2025. "Prenatal Valproic Acid Exposure Affects Song Learning in Zebra Finches: A Potential Model for Vocal Development in Autism" Life 15, no. 7: 1058. https://doi.org/10.3390/life15071058

APA Style

Tewelde, E. G., Morvai, B., Zachar, G., & Pogány, Á. (2025). Prenatal Valproic Acid Exposure Affects Song Learning in Zebra Finches: A Potential Model for Vocal Development in Autism. Life, 15(7), 1058. https://doi.org/10.3390/life15071058

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop