Social Distance Matters in Dictator Games: Evidence from 11 Mexican Villages
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Experimental Design
2.1. Participants
2.2. Study Implementation
2.3. Experimental Design: Comparative Dictator Game
3. Hypotheses
4. Results
5. Discussion
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Experimental Forms
Decision Forms a,c | |||||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stranger | Community | Main Decision Form I d | Main Decision Form II d | ||||||||||||||||
Decision b | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | |||
Recipient Identifying Information Given | |||||||||||||||||||
Picture Name | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | |||
Type of Recipient | |||||||||||||||||||
Stranger Same village | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | * | ||||||
Family e | * | * | * | ||||||||||||||||
Spouse Mother Father | * 51 * 17 * 8 | * 20 * 150 * 101 | * 358 * 178 * 148 | ||||||||||||||||
Child | * 23 | * 335 | * 84 | ||||||||||||||||
Other in household | * 20 | * 230 | * 68 | ||||||||||||||||
Subjects per Decision Form | 107 | 61 | 269 | 837 |
Appendix B. Development of the Comparative Dictator Game
Appendix C. Impact of Donor’s and Recipient’s Sociodemographic Characteristics on Giving
Dependent Variable: % Sent | (1) | (2) | (3) |
---|---|---|---|
Type of Recipient | Family Member | Community Member | Stranger |
Donor’s Sociodemographic Characteristics: | |||
Female | −0.019 | −0.058 *** | −0.032 * |
(0.019) | (0.018) | (0.016) | |
Head of household | 0.062 ** | 0.0005 | 0.015 |
(0.024) | (0.020) | (0.020) | |
Married | −0.0003 | 0.004 | −0.027 + |
(0.007) | (0.016) | (0.016) | |
Age | −0.002 ** | −0.001 + | 0.0004 |
(0.0008) | (0.0006) | (0.0006) | |
Having at least primary school | 0.049 ** | 0.007 | 0.012 |
(0.017) | (0.017) | (0.015) | |
Among poorest households | −0.025 | −0.012 | 0.009 |
(0.016) | (0.015) | (0.014) | |
Recipient’s Sociodemographic Characteristics: | |||
Female | 0.049 ** | 0.015 | −0.030 * |
(0.016) | (0.016) | (0.014) | |
Head of household | −0.016 | 0.024 | |
(0.019) | (0.020) | ||
Married | 0.010 | 0.007 | |
(0.020) | (0.016) | ||
Age | 0.001 + | 0.0001 | |
(0.0007) | (0.0006) | ||
Having at least primary school | −0.019 | −0.018 | |
(0.013) | (0.015) | ||
Constant | 0.405 *** | 0.315 *** | 0.264 *** |
(0.044) | (0.045) | (0.037) | |
Family Controls | Yes | No | No |
Community Controls | Yes | Yes | Yes |
R2—Within | 0.105 | ||
Between | 0.092 | ||
Overall | 0.092 | 0.053 | 0.041 |
No. of Observations | 1595 | 1046 | 1043 |
No. of Subjects | 1001 |
References
- Fafchamps, M.; Lund, S. Risk-sharing networks in rural Philippines. J. Dev. Econ. 2003, 71, 261–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miguel, E.; Gugerty, M.K. Ethnic diversity, social sanctions, and public goods in Kenya. J. Public Econ. 2005, 89, 2325–2368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ashraf, N. Spousal control and intra-household decision making: An experimental study in the Philippines. Am. Econ. Rev. 2009, 99, 1245–1277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Oliveira, A.C.M.; Croson, R.T.A.; Eckel, C. The Giving Type: Identifying Donors. J. Public Econ. 2011, 95, 428–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Oliveira, A.C.M.; Eckel, C.; Croson, R. Solidarity among the Poor. Econ. Lett. 2014, 123, 144–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bohnet, I.; Frey, B.S. The sound of silence in prisoner’s dilemma and dictator games. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 1999, 38, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charness, G.; Gneezy, U. What’s in a name? Anonymity and social distance in dictator and ultimatum games. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2008, 68, 29–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ligon, E.; Schechter, L. Motives for sharing in social networks. J. Dev. Econ. 2012, 99, 13–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Candelo, N.; Croson, R.; Eckel, C. Transmission of Information through Social Networks: Implications for Immigration. Forthcom. Exp. Econ. 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Granovetter, M.S. The strength of weak ties. Am. J. Sociol. 1973, 78, 1360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Granovetter, M.S. Getting a Job: A Study of Contacts and Careers, 2nd ed.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Abel, A.B. Asset prices under habit formation and catching up with the joneses. Am. Econ. Rev. 1990, 80, 38–42. [Google Scholar]
- Bertrand, M.; Luttmer, E.F.P.; Mullainathan, S. Network effects and welfare cultures. Q. J. Econ. 2000, 115, 1019–1055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Munshi, K. Networks in the modern economy: Mexican migrants in the U.S. labor market. Q. J. Econ. 2003, 118, 549–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jackson, M.O. Networks in the Understanding of Economic Behaviors. J. Econ. Perspect. 2014, 28, 3–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, C.A.; Gilles, R.P. Spatial Social Networks. Rev. Econ. Des. 2000, 5, 273–299. [Google Scholar]
- Candelo, N.; De Oliveira, A.; Eckel, C. Worthiness versus self-interest in charitable giving: Evidence from a low-income, minority neighborhood. Manuscript submitted for publication. 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Leider, S.; Möbius, M.M.; Rosenblat, T.; Do, Q. Directed altruism and enforced reciprocity in social networks. Q. J. Econ. 2009, 124, 1815–1851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Candelo, N.; Eckel, C.; Johnson, C. The Proposer’s Behavior in the Ultimatum Game in 11 Mexican Villages; Working Paper; Department of Economics, Texas A&M: College Station, TX, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Goeree, J.K.; McConnell, M.A.; Mitchell, T.; Tromp, T.; Yariv, L. The 1/d law of giving. Am. Econ. J. Microecon. 2010, 1, 183–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Binzel, C.; Fehr, D. Giving and sorting among friends: Evidence from a lab-in-the-field experiment. Econ. Lett. 2013, 121, 214–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Binzel, C.; Fehr, D. Social distance and trust: Experimental evidence from a slum in Cairo. J. Dev. Econ. 2013, 103, 99–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Batista, C.; Silverman, D.; Yang, D. Directed giving: Evidence from an inter-household transfer experiment. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2015, 118, 2–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tajfel, H.; Turner, J. The Social Identity Theory of Intergroup Behavior. In The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations; Worchel, S., Austin, W., Eds.; Nelson- Hall: Chicago, IL, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Chen, Y.; Li, S.X. Group Identity and Social Preference. Am. Econ. Rev. 2009, 99, 431–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dreber, A.; Ellingsen, T.; Johannesson, M.; Rand, D.G. Do people care about social context? Framing effects in dictator games. Exp. Econ. 2013, 16, 349–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engel, C. Dictator games: A meta study. Experimental Economics 2011, 14, 583–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
1 | The field work for this study, from its conception, was carefully orchestrated to collect data on several different research questions. This is a fairly common practice in lab-in-the-field experiments because of the large fixed cost of going into the field. Along with our comparative dictator game design, we decided to implement other games in the field that were testing orthogonal questions with respect to the main question in this paper. For example, the trust game, the lottery game and the time preferences task address immigration issues which are not related to the current paper (e.g., [19] uses the ultimatum data from our field work). None of the other games addresses social distance. |
2 | Family members are shown to be closer than friends using a different sample of Mexicans (See [9]). |
3 | This result also indicates the absence of order effects. |
4 | We run an adjustment of p-values for all the above pairwise comparisons using the Holm–Bonferroni method for completeness. We find that 3 out of 9 significant comparisons become insignificant using this benchmark. Not surprisingly, two of the insignificant comparisons were already marginally significant with unadjusted p-values. |
5 | To explore how individual characteristics impact giving, sociodemographic characteristics of donors and recipients are included in a regression analysis in Appendix C. |
% Amount Sent | |||
---|---|---|---|
Information a | Mean | Std. Dev. | Obs. b |
Overall c | 33.9 | 25.7 | 5096 |
Main Decision Form | 35.2 | 26.1 | 4424 |
Picture | 26.3 | 21.2 | 1106 |
Name of community member | 26.2 | 22.5 | 1426 |
Name of family member | 47.3 | 26.3 | 1892 |
Spouse | 47.8 | 27.1 | 529 |
Mother | 54.5 | 25.1 | 346 |
Father | 51.7 | 25.8 | 257 |
Child | 43.6 | 26.0 | 442 |
Other family member | 40.1 | 24.3 | 318 |
Robustness Checks | |||
Stranger Decision Form | 26.5 | 20.5 | 428 |
Picture of a stranger | 27.6 | 19.2 | 107 |
Name of a stranger | 26.1 | 20.9 | 321 |
Community Decision Form | 23.5 | 22.3 | 244 |
Picture of a stranger | 24.0 | 19.3 | 61 |
Name of community member | 23.3 | 23.3 | 183 |
Dependent Variable: % Sent | (1) | (2) |
---|---|---|
Recipient’s Type of Social Contact: | ||
Mother, β1 | 0.272 *** | 0.281 *** |
(0.011) | (0.022) | |
Father, β2 | 0.236 *** | 0.246 *** |
(0.013) | (0.024) | |
Spouse, β3 | 0.216 *** | 0.233 *** |
(0.009) | (0.024) | |
Child, β4 | 0.181 *** | 0.197 *** |
(0.010) | (0.020) | |
Other family member, β5 | 0.143 *** | 0.153 *** |
(0.012) | (0.021) | |
Community member, β6 | −0.0003 | 0.005 |
(0.007) | (0.011) | |
Constant, α | 0.263 *** | 0.309 *** |
(0.007) | (0.028) | |
Community Controls | No | Yes |
Subject’s Sociodemographic Controls | No | Yes |
R2—Within | 0.302 | 0.302 |
Between | 0.044 | 0.099 |
Overall | 0.173 | 0.201 |
No. of Observations | 4424 | 4424 |
No. of Subjects | 1106 | 1106 |
© 2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Candelo, N.; Eckel, C.; Johnson, C. Social Distance Matters in Dictator Games: Evidence from 11 Mexican Villages. Games 2018, 9, 77. https://doi.org/10.3390/g9040077
Candelo N, Eckel C, Johnson C. Social Distance Matters in Dictator Games: Evidence from 11 Mexican Villages. Games. 2018; 9(4):77. https://doi.org/10.3390/g9040077
Chicago/Turabian StyleCandelo, Natalia, Catherine Eckel, and Cathleen Johnson. 2018. "Social Distance Matters in Dictator Games: Evidence from 11 Mexican Villages" Games 9, no. 4: 77. https://doi.org/10.3390/g9040077
APA StyleCandelo, N., Eckel, C., & Johnson, C. (2018). Social Distance Matters in Dictator Games: Evidence from 11 Mexican Villages. Games, 9(4), 77. https://doi.org/10.3390/g9040077