Next Article in Journal
Early-Life Gut Health Indicators and Reported Prevalence of Infant Functional Constipation by Healthcare Professionals
Previous Article in Journal
The 26-Item Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26): Psychometric Properties and Factor Structure in Vegetarians and Vegans
Previous Article in Special Issue
Reply to Zarguan et al. Comment on “Budryn et al. Hydroxybenzoic Acids as Acetylcholinesterase Inhibitors: Calorimetric and Docking Simulation Studies. Nutrients 2022, 14, 2476”
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Exposure to the Dutch Famine in Early Gestation and Cognitive Function and Decline in Older Age

by
Aline Marileen Wiegersma
1,2,3,*,
Amber Boots
1,2,3,
Tessa J. Roseboom
1,2,3,4 and
Susanne R. de Rooij
1,2,3
1
Epidemiology and Data Science, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Meibergdreef 9, Meibergdreef 9, 1105 AZ Amsterdam, The Netherlands
2
Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Aging & Later Life, Health Behaviors & Chronic Diseases, 1007 MB Amsterdam, The Netherlands
3
Amsterdam Reproduction and Development, 1105 AZ Amsterdam, The Netherlands
4
Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Meibergdreef 9, 1105 AZ Amsterdam, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2023, 15(2), 293; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15020293
Submission received: 13 October 2022 / Revised: 30 November 2022 / Accepted: 3 January 2023 / Published: 6 January 2023

Abstract

:
People exposed to the 1944–1945 Dutch famine in early gestation performed worse on a selective attention task at age 58 and reported more cognitive problems at age 72. We here hypothesized that undernutrition in early gestation is associated with poorer cognitive functioning in older age and a higher rate of cognitive decline. We tested this hypothesis in the Dutch famine birth cohort in men and women combined and separately. We assessed cognitive function using a Stroop-like, trail-making and 15-word task (at ages 68 and 74) and the Montreal cognitive assessment as well as self-perceived cognitive problems (at age 74) in 73 men (n = 34) and women (n = 39). We compared cognitive function and decline (change in cognitive function between age 68 and 74) between those exposed in early gestation and those not exposed (born before or conceived after the famine). Although in both men and women cognitive function declined from age 68 to 74, cognitive task scores and the rate of decline did not differ between those exposed or unexposed to famine. At age 74, men exposed to famine in early gestation more often reported cognitive problems, although this was not statistically different from unexposed men (OR 3.1 [95%CI 0.7 to 13.0]). We did not find evidence of increased cognitive decline after prenatal undernutrition. Selective participation and mortality may have hampered our ability to detect potential true effects. The self-perceived cognitive problems among men who had been exposed to famine in early gestation might be an indication of future dementia risk.

1. Introduction

The majority of brain development occurs during the prenatal period, with the largest part of neurogenesis occurring in the first half of gestation [1]. During this critical developmental phase, the brain is sensitive to the environment which may influence brain development with potentially lasting consequences for cognitive function across the lifespan [2].
Indeed, adverse prenatal factors, including air pollution and maternal stress, have been associated with poorer cognitive function in childhood as well as adulthood [3,4,5]. In addition to poorer cognitive function in early and adult life, there is evidence that adverse prenatal factors may increase the rate of cognitive decline that occurs with aging. Rodent studies have shown that adverse prenatal circumstances may accelerate cognitive decline and increase the vulnerability to developing Alzheimer’s disease-related pathologies [6]. In humans, markers for adverse prenatal circumstances, such as lower birth weight or being born in a high stroke incidence or high infant mortality area, have been associated with increased cognitive decline and dementia risk [7,8,9].
We have previously shown that men and women exposed to undernutrition in early gestation performed worse on a Stroop-like task administered during a stress protocol around age 58 in the Dutch famine birth cohort (DFBC), suggesting that prenatal famine exposure has lasting consequences for cognitive function. Performing the Stroop-like task requires selective attention and inhibition—cognitive abilities which are among the first to decline with age [10]. Therefore, our findings on the Stroop-like task may have been an early sign of accelerated cognitive aging [11]. Our subsequent findings, that men exposed to famine in early gestation reported more self-perceived cognitive problems at age 72 and more often consulted a healthcare professional for these problems, is in line with this hypothesis, as self-perceived cognitive problems have been associated with (future) cognitive decline and increased dementia risk [12,13,14,15]. Apart from worse performance on the Stroop-like task and poorer self-perceived cognitive function, we found that men exposed to famine in early gestation had worse brain perfusion, smaller total brain volumes, and increased BrainAGE (an MRI-based measure estimating clinical brain aging). These brain outcomes are potentially related to neurodegeneration and associated cognitive decline [16,17,18]. In addition, we observed patterns of altered resting-state functional connectivity at age 68, which in men were similar to patterns of network desegregation observed in brain aging [19]. Together, these neuroimaging studies suggest accelerated aging of the brain among men exposed to famine in early gestation [16,17,18,19].
Prenatal undernutrition may thus have led to accelerated cognitive and brain aging in men and women conceived during the Dutch famine. Similarly, studies that investigated prenatal exposure to the 1959–1961 Great Chinese Famine showed more cognitive impairment and a higher dementia prevalence among those exposed prenatally to famine [20,21,22,23]. However, the observed differences in cognition and brain size and function in the DFBC may have been present from early life as a result of hampered brain development and thereby not the result of accelerated aging [11,16,17,18,19].
In the current study, we aimed to test the hypothesis that exposure to the Dutch famine in early gestation is associated with worse cognitive function in older age and a higher rate of cognitive decline by studying cognitive function over time. Given our previous findings regarding sex-specific effects of prenatal famine exposure, we tested these hypotheses in the total group as well as in men and women separately.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants were a sample of men and women from the DFBC, which consists of 2414 individuals born alive as term singleton babies in the Wilhelmina Gasthuis (a teaching hospital in Amsterdam, the Netherlands) between 1 November 1943 and 28 February 1947 [24]. In 2012, a random subsample of the total eligible cohort (alive, living in the Netherlands and with a known address; n = 1307) was invited to participate in a study consisting of a home visit and MRI scan. In total, 151 persons agreed to be visited at home. A more detailed description of the selection procedure has been published before [17,25]. In 2019, we aimed to include all available participants from the 2012 study for a hospital visit encompassing extensive cognitive testing and a brain MRI scan. In the current study, we focused on the 73 participants who completed cognitive testing in both 2012 and 2019 (mean age 68 and 74 years). Two participants with dementia had been excluded as their scores on the cognitive tests were outliers and they did not complete all cognitive tests.

2.2. Exposure

Towards the end of World War II, a cascade of events led to a severe famine in the western part of the Netherlands [11,26]. Daily food rations dropped steeply, reaching a level below 1000 kilocalories per person on 26 November 1944 and improved relatively quickly after the Netherlands was liberated in May 1945. In the DFBC, three 16-week periods of exposure to undernutrition have been defined: individuals mainly exposed in late gestation (born between 7 January and 28 April 1945), mid-gestation (born between 29 April and 18 August 1945), or early gestation (born between 19 August and 8 December 1945) [24]. Individuals born in these periods were prenatally exposed to at least 13 weeks of famine, during which their mothers’ daily food ration contained on average fewer than 1000 kilocalories according to the official daily food rations for the general population of 21 years and older [27]. In previous DFBC studies, we observed the most negative health consequences after famine exposure in early gestation [24]. Therefore, only those exposed during early gestation were included in the current study [11]. Individuals born before (born before 7 January 1945) and conceived after the famine (born after 8 December 1945) were considered unexposed and acted as a control group.

2.3. Study Parameters

Maternal, pregnancy, and birth characteristics (e.g., maternal age and birth weight) were collected from medical birth records. We included the occupation (manual yes/no) of the head of the household as a measure of parental SES. Adult characteristics were collected from interview questions at ages 68 and 74. Level of education was defined as the highest level of finished schooling measured on a 7-point scale. Socioeconomic status (SES) was defined according to the International Index of Occupational Status 92, which is based on the participant’s or their partner’s (former) occupation, whichever status was highest [28].

2.4. Cognitive Function

Cognitive function was assessed with the Stroop-like task, trail-making task, and 15-word task at age 68 (at home) and age 74 (in the hospital). The Stroop-like task measures executive function, specifically selective attention and response inhibition, with a color-word incongruence task. The trail-making task also measures executive functioning, mainly, visual attention, task switching, and mental flexibility. The 15-word task measures verbal memory.
In addition to the three cognitive tasks that were repeated over time, we performed the standardized Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA; multiple cognitive domains) and measured self-perceived cognitive problems at age 74 to obtain a more complete picture of the cognitive function of the participants at an older age [29,30]. Similar to these measurements performed at age 72, we assessed self-perceived cognitive problems with two questions, assessing if participants had problems with their memory, attention, and thinking and if they ever consulted a healthcare practitioner for these problems [12]. In addition, we used the Dutch version of the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) to measure self-perceived cognitive functioning in daily life [31]. For detailed information on cognitive testing and the rationale for including these tasks, see Supplementary Text S1.

2.5. Statistical Analyses

We performed linear and logistic regression analyses to compare cognitive outcomes (measured by cognitive tasks or self-reporting) between those exposed to famine in early gestation and the unexposed group at a mean age of 68 or 74. The Stroop-like task and trail-making task scores did not follow a normal distribution and commonly applied transformations (e.g., log transformations) did not normalize the scores. Therefore, we used ranked scores as the dependent variables in linear regression analyses to analyze these tasks. As the Stroop-like task, trail-making task, and 15-word task were measured at both ages 68 and 74, we additionally used generalized estimating equation models (GEE) using an exchangeable working correlation structure to examine changes over time, thereby accounting for the dependency of repeated measurements within persons. We included interaction terms for ‘exposure × time’ to evaluate if the rate of cognitive decline differed among the exposed and unexposed groups. In addition to the crude analyses, we adjusted the models for covariates (sex and parental SES). We additionally performed the analyses separately for men and women.
To evaluate participation bias, we compared baseline characteristics in the complete DFBC (n = 2414) between those included and not included in the current study. Furthermore, for those participating in 2002 (n = 717), we compared cognitive scores and SES collected around age 58 between those included and not included in the current study.
We used IBM SPSS statistics version 28.0 to perform the statistical analyses. We considered differences to be statistically significant if p-values were ≤0.05.

3. Results

Of the 73 participants (34 men and 39 women) included in the study, 25 were exposed to famine in early gestation. We did not detect statistically significant differences in maternal and birth characteristics according to prenatal famine exposure (exposed versus unexposed). When comparing those born before the famine with those conceived after the famine, we found that those born before the famine had a statistically significant smaller head circumference at birth (Table 1).
We did not detect differences in performance on any of the cognitive tasks at ages 68 or 74 between participants exposed to famine in early gestation and unexposed participants in the total group (Table 2) or men and women separately (Table 3). Adjusting for covariates did not alter the results.
On average, participants scored worse on the cognitive tasks around age 74 compared with age 68 years, which was statistically significant for the trail-making and 15-word summary tasks (p < 0.001; Table 4). However, in the GEE models combining men and women, the interaction terms for ‘exposure × time’ were not statistically significant (p > 0.05), indicating no altered rate of cognitive decline for the exposed compared with the unexposed groups. Sex-specific analyses showed that the rate of decline on the trail-making task was lower (i.e., a smaller increase in time needed to complete the tasks) for women exposed to famine in early gestation compared with unexposed women, this difference was statistically significant for task B (Task A: B −5.9 [95%CI −12.3 to 0.6]; Task B: B −40.1 [95%CI −65.5 to −14.8]; Table 5). In contrast, men exposed to famine in early gestation had a larger decline in the trail-making task from age 68 to 74, which was close to statistical significance in task A (Task A: B 10.5 [95%CI −0.5 to 21.6]; task B: B 10.9 [95%CI −15.2 to 37.0]).
The prevalence of self-perceived cognitive problems did not significantly differ between those exposed to famine in early gestation and those unexposed across all participants. Sex-specific analyses showed no difference for women (OR 0.9 [95%CI 0.2 to 3.7]. Exposed men reported more self-perceived cognitive problems compared with unexposed men, although this difference was not statistically significant (OR 3.1 [95% CI 0.7 to 13.0]; Table 3).
Regarding the evaluation of participation bias, birth weight did not differ among cohort members included or not included in the current study. Head circumference was smaller for those born before the famine included in the current study compared with those not included. For individuals who participated in 2002, men and women exposed to famine in early gestation included in the current study had higher scores on the Stroop-like task (n = 65; mean score 48.4 compared with 33.1, p = 0.12) and a higher SES (n = 65; mean 52.6 compared with 43.7, p = 0.01) at age 58 compared with those not included in the current study. Included men and women conceived after the famine also had higher scores on the Stroop-like task at age 58 compared with those not included in the current study (n = 65; mean 68.3 compared with 49.3, p = 0.01).

4. Discussion

In the current study, we did not find evidence to support our hypothesis that exposure to famine in early gestation leads to worse cognitive function in older age and faster cognitive decline. Although the cognitive performance of the participants as a group declined over time, demonstrating the deterioration of memory and executive functions, the rate of cognitive decline did not differ for those exposed to famine in early gestation compared with those not prenatally exposed to the famine. In line with our previous findings, self-perceived cognitive problems were highest in men exposed to famine in early gestation; however, the difference compared with unexposed men was not statistically significant here.
In the current study, we did not detect differences in the Stroop-like task comparing men and women exposed to famine in early gestation with those unexposed, while in a previous study at age 58, participants from the DFBC exposed to famine in early gestation performed worse on the Stroop-like task [11]. An explanation for this discrepancy may be that the Stroop-like task conducted around age 58 was part of a psychological stress protocol, while the Stroop-like task at ages 68 and 74 was not. Performing the Stroop-like task under pressure may have added another level of complexity, resulting in an even larger demand for attentional resources than simply provoked by the task itself [32,33]. This may have unmasked differences in performance on the task that would go undetected under normal circumstances. The fact that the cognitive tasks were not part of a stress protocol at ages 68 and 74 may have reduced the detectability of group differences in selective attention. In line with this explanation are findings from a study by de Groot et al., which showed no overall association between prenatal famine exposure and performance on a Stroop task not conducted during a stress protocol at age 59 [34]. Alternative or additional explanations for the absence of an association between prenatal famine exposure and performance on the Stroop-like task in the present study are selective participation and selective mortality, which are explained in detail below.
Similar to the Stroop-like task, we did not detect differences in the trail-making task, 15-word task, or MoCA between those exposed or unexposed to famine in early gestation. Furthermore, although cognitive performance declined over time, the rate of decline did not differ between exposed and unexposed participants. Together, these findings do not support our hypothesis of accelerated cognitive aging after famine exposure in early gestation. Accelerated cognitive aging in those exposed to famine in early gestation was expected based on our previous findings showing more self-perceived cognitive problems, which may be predictive of future cognitive decline [12,13,14]. Furthermore, findings suggesting more rapid aging of the brain, especially in men exposed to famine in early gestation, would also indicate accelerated cognitive decline [16,17,18,19]. The discrepancy between expectations based on previous observations and the current results could be explained by two scenarios.
First, as cognitive decline and the risk of dementia increase with age, we could have been too early to detect differences in cognitive function between those prenatally exposed or unexposed to the famine in the current study. Studies have shown that cognitive tasks often do not detect cognitive problems at the moment that early signs of neurodegeneration become apparent in the brain and participants self-report cognitive problems in daily life, suggesting that cognitive tasks may not adequately capture the first stages of cognitive decline [13,14,35].
Secondly, in addition to having been too early to detect differences in cognitive function, selective participation of more healthy and cognitively fit exposed men and women may have prevented us from detecting an association between prenatal famine exposure and worse cognitive functioning. We have previously shown that women exposed to famine in early gestation had higher overall adult mortality compared with unexposed women, which may have resulted in stronger women participating in the present study [36]. This may explain the lower decline in performance on the trail-making task among exposed women compared with unexposed women. Furthermore, we have previously shown that men and women exposed to famine in early gestation more often rated their health as poor, had a more atherogenic lipid profile, and had a higher risk of coronary heart disease and diabetes [37,38,39,40,41]. Therefore, a relatively larger proportion of participants exposed during early gestation may not have been fit enough to visit the clinic compared with unexposed participants. This may have resulted in a larger proportion of exposed participants with relatively better health remaining in the study; who may also have better cognitive function, given the relationship between these outcomes [42]. At age 58, exposed individuals participating in the current study had higher Stroop-like scores compared with exposed individuals who did not participate in the current study, indicating that those with lower performance indeed have more often decided not to participate in the current study. Taken together, these findings suggest that participation bias may have prevented us from detecting differences in cognitive function that would have been present if selective participation had not occurred.
As self-perceived cognitive problems usually occur before cognitive decline can be detected on cognitive tasks and are predictive for future cognitive decline and dementia [13,14,15], measuring self-perceived cognitive problems may be a more sensitive method to detect subtle differences in cognitive function that are typical for the early stages of cognitive decline. We indeed observed more self-perceived cognitive problems in men exposed to famine in early gestation compared with unexposed men in the current study. Although the difference was not statistically significant, the effect size was in line with what we have observed before in a larger sample in the DFBC [12]. Due to undernutrition, fewer children were conceived during the famine [24,43]; therefore, the number of eligible participants exposed to famine in early gestation was limited. Together with decreased participation, this led to a relatively small sample size in the current study. This small sample size may have prevented us from detecting a statistically significant effect, and our results might point to a process of cognitive decline in exposed individuals.

5. Conclusions

We did not identify differences in cognitive functioning or the rate of cognitive decline among those exposed or unexposed to famine in early gestation. Selective participation and mortality may have hampered our ability to detect potential true effects. Corresponding to previous findings, men exposed in early gestation more often reported cognitive problems, possibly indicative of future cognitive decline and dementia.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu15020293/s1, Text S1: Detailed information on the cognitive tests used in the current study.

Author Contributions

T.J.R. and S.R.d.R. designed the study. A.M.W. and A.B. conducted the research. A.M.W. analysed the data and drafted the manuscript. All authors critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by a Dutch Research Council (NWO) Aspasia grant (grant number 015014039) awarded to S.R. de Rooij, by the European Commission Horizon 2020, project LongITools (grant number 874739) and by the European Community FP7 HEALTH, project BrainAGE (grant number 279281) and project DynaHealth (grant number 633595). The funding organizations had no role in any part of this study.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Medical Ethics Review Committee of the Academic Medical Center.

Informed Consent Statement

All participants gave written informed consent.

Data Availability Statement

Data described in the manuscript, code book, and analytic code will be made available upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We thank the participants of the Dutch famine birth cohort for their willing cooperation in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

None of the authors had a conflict of interest.

References

  1. Dobbing, J.; Sands, J. Quantitative growth and development of human brain. Arch. Dis. Child 1973, 48, 757–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Dufford, A.J.; Spann, M.; Scheinost, D. How prenatal exposures shape the infant brain: Insights from infant neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021, 131, 47–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Guxens, M.; Lubczyńska, M.J.; Muetzel, R.L.; Dalmau-Bueno, A.; Jaddoe, V.W.V.; Hoek, G.; van der Lugt, A.; Verhulst, F.C.; White, T.; Brunekreef, B.; et al. Air Pollution Exposure During Fetal Life, Brain Morphology, and Cognitive Function in School-Age Children. Biol. Psychiatry 2018, 84, 295–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Lubczyńska, M.J.; Muetzel, R.L.; El Marroun, H.; Hoek, G.; Kooter, I.M.; Thomson, E.M.; Hillegers, M.; Vernooij, M.W.; White, T.; Tiemeier, H.; et al. Air pollution exposure during pregnancy and childhood and brain morphology in preadolescents. Environ. Res. 2021, 198, 110446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bergman, K.; Sarkar, P.; O’Connor, T.G.; Modi, N.; Glover, V. Maternal stress during pregnancy predicts cognitive ability and fearfulness in infancy. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2007, 46, 1454–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Lesuis, S.L.; Hoeijmakers, L.; Korosi, A.; de Rooij, S.R.; Swaab, D.F.; Kessels, H.W.; Lucassen, P.J.; Krugers, H.J. Vulnerability and resilience to Alzheimer’s disease: Early life conditions modulate neuropathology and determine cognitive reserve. Alzheimers. Res. Ther. 2018, 10, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Raikkonen, K.; Kajantie, E.; Pesonen, A.K.; Heinonen, K.; Alastalo, H.; Leskinen, J.T.; Nyman, K.; Henriksson, M.; Lahti, J.; Lahti, M.; et al. Early Life Origins Cognitive Decline: Findings in Elderly Men in the Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e54707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Gilsanz, P.; Mayeda, E.R.; Glymour, M.M.; Quesenberry, C.P.; Whitmer, R.A. Association Between Birth in a High Stroke Mortality State, Race, and Risk of Dementia. JAMA Neurol. 2017, 74, 1056–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Gilsanz, P.; Mayeda, E.R.; Glymour, M.M.; Quesenberry, C.P., Jr.; Mungas, D.; DeCarli, C.S.; Whitmer, R.A. Birth in High Infant Mortality States and Dementia Risk in a Cohort of Elderly African American and White Health Care Members. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2019, 33, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Healey, M.K.; Campbell, K.L.; Hasher, L. Cognitive aging and increased distractibility: Costs and potential benefits. Prog. Brain Res. 2008, 169, 353–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. de Rooij, S.R.; Wouters, H.; Yonker, J.E.; Painter, R.C.; Roseboom, T.J. Prenatal undernutrition and cognitive function in late adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 16881–16886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Wiegersma, A.M.; Boots, A.; Roseboom, T.J.; de Rooij, S.R. Prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine is associated with more self-perceived cognitive problems at 72 years of age. BMC Geriatr. 2022, 22, 176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Rabin, L.A.; Smart, C.M.; Amariglio, R.E. Subjective Cognitive Decline in Preclinical Alzheimer’s Disease. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2017, 13, 369–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Kielb, S.; Rogalski, E.; Weintraub, S.; Rademaker, A. Objective features of subjective cognitive decline in a United States national database. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2017, 13, 1337–1344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Koppara, A.; Wagner, M.; Lange, C.; Ernst, A.; Wiese, B.; König, H.H.; Brettschneider, C.; Riedel-Heller, S.; Luppa, M.; Weyerer, S.; et al. Cognitive performance before and after the onset of subjective cognitivedecline in old age. Alzheimer’s Dement. Diagnosis Assess. Dis. Monit. 2015, 1, 194–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. de Rooij, S.R.; Mutsaerts, H.J.M.M.; Petr, J.; Asllani, I.; Caan, M.W.A.; Groot, P.; Nederveen, A.J.; Schwab, M.; Roseboom, T.J. Late-life brain perfusion after prenatal famine exposure. Neurobiol. Aging 2019, 82, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. De Rooij, S.R.; Caan, M.W.A.; Swaab, D.F.; Nederveen, A.J.; Majoie, C.B.; Schwab, M.; Painter, R.C.; Roseboom, T.J. Prenatal famine exposure has sex-specific effects on brain size. Brain 2016, 139, 2136–2142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Franke, K.; Gaser, C.; Roseboom, T.J.; Schwab, M.; de Rooij, S.R. Premature brain aging in humans exposed to maternal nutrient restriction during early gestation. Neuroimage 2018, 173, 460–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Boots, A.; Thomason, M.E.; Espinoza-Heredia, C.; Pruitt, P.J.; Damoiseaux, J.S.; Roseboom, T.J.; de Rooij, S.R. Sex-specific effects of prenatal undernutrition on resting-state functional connectivity in the human brain at age 68. Neurobiol. Aging 2022, 112, 129–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Kang, Y.; Zhang, Y.; Feng, Z.; Liu, M.; Li, Y.; Yang, H.; Wang, D.; Zheng, L.; Lou, D.; Cheng, L.; et al. Nutritional Deficiency in Early Life Facilitates Aging-Associated Cognitive Decline. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2017, 14, 841–849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Wang, C.; An, Y.; Yu, H.; Feng, L.; Liu, Q.; Lu, Y.; Wang, H.; Xiao, R. Association between Exposure to the Chinese Famine in Different Stages of Early Life and Decline in Cognitive Functioning in Adulthood. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Rong, H.; Lai, X.; Mahmoudi, E.; Fang, H. Early-Life Exposure to the Chinese Famine and Risk of Cognitive Decline. J. Clin. Med. 2019, 8, 484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Li, J.; Na, L.; Ma, H.; Zhang, Z.; Li, T.; Lin, L.; Li, Q.; Sun, C.; Li, Y. Multigenerational effects of parental prenatal exposure to famine on adult offspring cognitive function. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 13792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Bleker, L.S.; De Rooij, S.R.; Painter, R.C.; Ravelli, A.C.J.; Roseboom, T.J. Cohort profile: The Dutch famine birth cohort (DFBC) -a prospective birth cohort study in the Netherlands. BMJ Open 2021, 11, e042078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. De Rooij, S.R.; Van Pelt, A.M.M.; Ozanne, S.E.; Korver, C.M.; Van Daalen, S.K.M.; Painter, R.C.; Schwab, M.; Viegas, M.H.; Roseboom, T.J. Prenatal undernutrition and leukocyte telomere length in late adulthood: The Dutch famine birth cohort study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2015, 102, 655–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Roseboom, T.J.; Painter, R.C.; van Abeelen, A.F.M.; Veenendaal, M.V.E.; de Rooij, S.R. Hungry in the womb: What are the consequences? Lessons from the Dutch famine. Maturitas 2011, 70, 141–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Trienekes, G.M.T. Tussen Ons Volk En De Honger: De Voedselvoorziening, 1940–1945; Matrijs: Utrecht, The Netherlands, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  28. Bakker, B.F.; Sieben, I.; Nieuwbeerta, P.; Ganzeboom, H.B. Maten voor prestige, sociaal-economische status en sociale klasse voor de Standaard Beroepen Classificatie 1992. Soc. Wetenschap. 1997, 40, 1–22. [Google Scholar]
  29. Nasreddine, Z.S.; Phillips, N.A.; Bédirian, V.; Charbonneau, S.; Whitehead, V.; Collin, I.; Cummings, J.L.; Chertkow, H. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005, 53, 695–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Larner, A.J. Screening utility of the montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA): In place of-or as well as-The MMSE? Int. Psychogeriatr. 2012, 24, 391–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Broadbent, D.E.; Cooper, P.F.; FitzGerald, P.; Parkes, K.R. The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1982, 21, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kahneman, D. Attention and Effort; Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
  33. Stawski, R.S.; Sliwinski, M.J.; Smyth, J.M. Stress-related cognitive interference predicts cognitive function in old age. Psychol. Aging 2006, 21, 535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. de Groot, R.H.M.; Stein, A.D.; Jolles, J.; van Boxtel, M.P.J.; Blauw, G.J.; van de Bor, M.; Lumey, L.H. Prenatal famine exposure and cognition at age 59 years. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2011, 40, 327–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Van Rooden, S.; Van Den Berg-Huysmans, A.A.; Croll, P.H.; Labadie, G.; Hayes, J.M.; Viviano, R.; Van Der Grond, J.; Rombouts, S.A.R.B.; Damoiseaux, J.S. Subjective Cognitive Decline Is Associated with Greater White Matter Hyperintensity Volume. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2018, 66, 1283–1294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Van Abeelen, A.F.M.; Veenendaal, M.V.E.; Painter, R.C.; De Rooij, S.R.; Dijkgraaf, M.G.W.; Bossuyt, P.M.M.; Elias, S.G.; Grobbee, D.E.; Uiterwaal, C.S.P.M.; Roseboom, T.J. Survival effects of prenatal famine exposure. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2012, 95, 179–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Roseboom, T.J.; Van der Meulen, J.H.P.; Ravelli, A.C.J.; Osmond, C.; Barker, D.J.P.; Bleker, O.P. Perceived health of adults after prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2003, 17, 391–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Roseboom, T.J.; Van der Meulen, J.H.P.; Osmond, C.; Barker, D.J.P.; Ravelli, A.C.J.; Bleker, O.P. Plasma lipid profiles in adults after prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2000, 72, 1101–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. De Rooij, S.R.; Painter, R.C.; Roseboom, T.J.; Phillips, D.I.W.; Osmond, C.; Barker, D.J.P.; Tanck, M.W.; Michels, R.P.J.; Bossuyt, P.M.M.; Bleker, O.P. Glucose tolerance at age 58 and the decline of glucose tolerance in comparison with age 50 in people prenatally exposed to the Dutch famine. Diabetologia 2006, 49, 637–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Ravelli, A.C.J.; Van Der Meulen, J.H.P.; Michels, R.P.J.; Osmond, C.; Barker, D.J.P.; Hales, C.N.; Bleker, O.P. Glucose tolerance in adults after prenatal exposure to famine. Lancet 1998, 351, 173–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Painter, R.C.; De Rooij, S.R.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Simmers, T.A.; Osmond, C.; Barker, D.J.; Bleker, O.P.; Roseboom, T.J. Early onset of coronary artery disease after prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2006, 84, 322–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Nash, D.T.; Fillit, H. Cardiovascular Disease Risk Factors and Cognitive Impairment. Am. J. Cardiol. 2006, 97, 1262–1265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Burger, G.C.E.; Sandstead, H.R.; Drummond, J.C. Malnutrition and Starvation in Western Netherlands, September 1944 to July 1945. Part I and II; General State Printing Office: The Hague, The Netherlands, 1948. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. General, maternal, pregnancy, birth and adult characteristics by timing of prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine.
Table 1. General, maternal, pregnancy, birth and adult characteristics by timing of prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine.
nBorn beforeExposure to Famine in Early GestationConceived afterTotal
General characteristics
N7323252573
% Women7360.944.056.053.4
Maternal and pregnancy characteristics
Pregnancy duration (days)62284 (10)287 (11)282 (14)285 (11)
Maternal age at birth (years)7328.3 (5.5)26.5 (6.7)27.6 (6.1)27.5 (6.1)
% Primiparous7321.744.036.034.2
% Manual occupation head of household5693.360.961.169.6
Birth characteristics
Birth weight (g)733337 (533)3492 (507)3357 (534)3396 (522)
Head circumference (cm)7232.0 (1.3) *32.9 (1.2)33.0 (1.7)32.6 (1.5)
Adult characteristics
Level of education a734.8 (2.6)4.9 (1.9)4.4 (2.1)4.7 (2.2)
SES7349.8 (12.8)50.2 (13.8)47.7 (16.1)49.2 (14.2)
Age 2012 (years)7368.6 (0.4) **67.3 (0.266.7 (0.4)67.5 (0.8)
Age 2019 (years)7375.2 (0.5) **74.0 (0.2)73.4 (0.5)74.2 (0.8)
Numbers represent frequencies (%) or means (SD). Asterisks represent p-values of linear and logistic regression analyses for those exposed to famine in early gestation compared with participants unexposed to famine in gestation, or, for individuals born before the famine, compared with individuals conceived after the famine. a Based on a 7-point scale; (1) Less than six years of primary school, (2) six years of primary school, (3) more than primary school, without an additional diploma, (4) craft school, (5) (pre-)secondary vocational education, (6) pre-university education, (7) higher professional education/university. * < 0.05, ** < 0.001.
Table 2. Associations between prenatal exposure to famine and cognitive outcomes compared with individuals born before or conceived after the famine around age 68 (2012) and 74 (2019).
Table 2. Associations between prenatal exposure to famine and cognitive outcomes compared with individuals born before or conceived after the famine around age 68 (2012) and 74 (2019).
OutcomenBorn before
(Reference)
Exposure to Famine in Early GestationConceived after
(Reference)
B (95%CI)p-Value
73232525
2012
Stroop-like task a
% Correct b7354.0 (28.1)61.1 (29.9)53.3 (34.5)c0.41
Response time (s)683.4 (0.2)3.3 (0.6)3.4 (0.7)−0.1 (−0.4–0.2)0.47
Trail-making task a
Trail A (sec) d7334.0 (9.0)32.0 (7.0)31.0 (22.0)c0.67
Trail B (sec) d7380.0 (30.0)70.0 (28.0)75.0 (35.0)c0.29
15-word task
Summary 5 trials7332.8 (8.0)32.7 (8.6)30.4 (8.4)1.2 (−2.9–5.3)0.57
Recall735.5 (3.4)6.5 (2.6)6.2 (2.2)0.6 (−0.7–2.0)0.38
2019
Stroop-like task a
% Correct b7350.4 (29.9)53.0 (30.0)48.5 (34.5)c0.51
Response time (s)713.9 (0.5)3.8 (0.5)3.7 (0.6)−0.0 (−0.3–0.2)0.76
Trail-making task a
Trail A (sec) d7345.0 (18.0)38.0 (16.0)40.0 (14.5)c0.62
Trail B (sec) d73104.0 (58.0)84.0 (37.0)87.0 (106.0)c0.26
15-word task
Summary 5 trials7329.7 (9.5)29.2 (6.3)26.3 (7.5)1.3 (−2.6–5.1)0.52
Recall736.0 (2.6)5.4 (2.0)5.1 (2.4)−0.1 (−1.3–1.0)0.81
MoCA
Summary score7322.9 (2.2)23.5 (2.7)23.4 (3.3)0.3 (−1.1–1.7)0.67
Self-perceived cognitive problems
Problems with memory, attention and thinking738 (34.8)12 (48.0)9 (36.0)1.7 e (0.6–4.5)0.30
Sought medical help73002 (8.0)f
CFQ score7326.5 (9.8)28.7 (8.7)26.9 (10.2)2.0 (−2.7–6.7)0.40
CFQ = Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (lower score means more failures). MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment. a Analysis was based on rank scores. b As the Stroop-like scores followed a bimodal distribution, including a flat center including empty areas, median scores did not accurately reflect the central tendency. We therefore used mean (SD) to describe the Stroop-like results even though the data was not normally distributed. c Estimates were not meaningful as they are based on ranked data. d Data given as median (IQR) instead of mean (SD). e Odds ratio based on logistic regression. f Too few cases to perform statistical analyses.
Table 3. Associations between prenatal exposure to famine and cognitive outcomes compared with individuals born before or conceived after the famine around age 68 (2012) and 74 (2019) by sex.
Table 3. Associations between prenatal exposure to famine and cognitive outcomes compared with individuals born before or conceived after the famine around age 68 (2012) and 74 (2019) by sex.
OutcomenBorn before
(Reference)
Exposure to Famine in Early GestationConceived after
(Reference)
B (95%CI)p-Value
2012
Men 91411
Stroop-like task a
% Correct b3450.2 (10.7)62.6 (8.0)66.2 (10.0)c0.99
Response time (s)323.2 (0.1)3.2 (0.1)3.3 (0.2)−0.1 (−0.5–0.3)0.65
Trail-making task a
Trail A (sec) d3433.0 (9.0)31.0 (8.0)31.0 (22.0)c0.54
Trail B (sec) d3481.0 (35.0)70.5 (32.0)59.0 (37.0)c0.97
15 word task
Summary 5 trials3428.3 (2.1)29.5 (2.1)27.0 (2.6)1.9 (−3.5–7.3)0.48
Recall344.2 (0.9)5.6 (0.6)5.1 (0.7)0.9 (−0.8–2.5)0.29
Women 141114
Stroop-like task a
% Correct b3956.5 (7.0)59.1 (9.4)43.2 (8.9)c0.42
Response time (s)363.5 (0.0)3.5 (0.2)3.5 (0.2)−0.0 (−0.4–0.4)0.87
Trail-making task a
Trail A (sec) d3935.0 (13.0)35.0 (7.0)31.0 (22.0)c0.98
Trail B (sec) d3978.0 (37.0)70.0 (25.0)77.0 (27.0)c0.24
15 word task
Summary 5 trials3935.7 (2.1)36.8 (2.4)33.0 (2.1)2.5 (−3.2–8.1)0.38
Recall396.4 (1.0)7.6 (0.9)7.1 (0.4)0.9 (−1.1–2.9)0.36
2019
Men 91411
Stroop-like task a
% Correct b 57.0 (9.6)54.7 (6.7)55.9 (11.2)c0.86
Response time (s) 3.8 (0.2)3.8 (0.1)3.7 (0.2)0.0 (−0.3–0.4)0.80
Trail-making task a
Trail A (sec) d 34.0 (15.5)38.5 (21.5)30.0 (14.0)c0.24
Trail B (sec) d 85.0 (38.0)85.0 (68.0)87.0 (117.0)c0.76
15 word task
Summary 5 trials 27.0 (2.3)28.2 (1.4)25.4 (2.5)2.1 (−2.6–6.8)0.37
Recall 4.7 (0.8)5.0 (0.5)4.5 (0.8)0.5 (−1.2–2.1)0.58
MoCA
Summary score3423.9 (0.5)23.1 (0.7)24.5 (0.8)−1.1 (−2.8–0.5)0.18
Self-perceived cognitive problems
Problems with memory, attention and thinking344 (44.4)8 (57.1)2 (18.2)3.1 e (0.7–13.0)0.12
Sought medical help34001 (9.1)f
CFQ score3428.3 (3.3)29.8 (2.2)23.6 (3.8)4.0 (−3.3–11.4)0.27
Women
Stroop-like task a
% Correct b 46.2 (8.2)50.8 (11.0)42.6 (8.7)c0.61
Response time (s) 3.9 (0.1)3.7 (0.2)3.8 (0.1)−0.1 (−0.5–0.3)0.54
Trail-making task a
Trail A (sec) d 46.5 (27.3)36.0 (16.0)41.0 (11.5)c0.09
Trail B (sec) d 123.0 (112.0)83.0 (13.0)97.5 (98.0)c0.05
15 word task
Summary 5 trials 31.4 (2.9)30.5 (2.3)27.1 (1.9)1.2 (−5.1–7.5)0.70
Recall 6.8 (0.7)5.8 (0.6)5.6 (0.6)−0.4 (−2.0–1.3)0.67
MoCA
Summary score3922.3 (0.6)24.0 (0.9)22.6 (0.9)1.5 (−0.6–3.7)0.16
Self-perceived cognitive problems
Problems with memory, attention and thinking394 (28.6)4 (36.4)7 (50.0)0.9 e (0.2–3.7)0.89
Sought medical help39001 (7.1)f
CFQ score3925.3 (2.6)27.3 (2.8)29.4 (1.9)−0.1 (−6.5–6.4)0.98
CFQ = Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (lower score means more failures). MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment. a Analysis was based on rank scores. b As the Stroop-like scores followed a bimodal distribution, including a flat center including empty areas, median scores did not accurately reflect the central tendency. We therefore used mean (SD) to describe the Stroop-like results even though the data was not normally distributed. c Estimates were not meaningful as they are based on ranked data. d Data given as median (IQR) instead of mean (SD). e Odds ratio based on logistic regression. f Too few cases to perform statistical analyses.
Table 4. Generalized estimation equation (GEE) models with ‘exposure × time’ interaction terms comparing cognitive outcomes between participants prenatally exposed to famine and unexposed participants.
Table 4. Generalized estimation equation (GEE) models with ‘exposure × time’ interaction terms comparing cognitive outcomes between participants prenatally exposed to famine and unexposed participants.
Mean/Median Difference a
2012
Mean/Median Difference a
2019
GEE Model
Estimate (B) (95%CI)p-Value
15-word task
Summary
Prenatal famine1.181.261.2 (-2.9-5.2)0.57
Time −3.6 (−5.4–1.8)<0.001
Interaction b 0.1 (−3.4–3.5)0.96
Recall
Prenatal famine0.61−0.140.6 (−0.7–1.9)0.36
Time −0.4 (−1.1–0.3)0.30
Interaction b −0.7 (−1.9–0.4)0.19
Trail-making task (higher score is worse)
Test A
Prenatal famine−1−2−2.9 (−7.3–1.4)0.19
Time 6.4 (3.1–9.7)<0.001
Interaction b 2.1 (−5.0–9.1)0.57
Test B
Prenatal famine−7.5−18−10.5 (−25.5–4.5)0.17
Time 36.8 (20.5–53.0)<0.001
Interaction b −15.6 (−35.4–4.23)0.12
Stroop-like task
Prenatal famine7.413.577.4 (−7.0–21.9)0.32
Time −4.3 (−11.8–3.3)0.27
Interaction b −3.8 (−19.5–11.8)0.63
Statistically significant results are depicted in bold. a Mean or median (for the trail-making task) difference between the scores for those exposed or unexposed prenatally to the famine at age 68 (T1) and age 74 (T2). b Interaction term for ‘exposure × time’ to evaluate if the rate of cognitive decline differed among the exposed and unexposed groups.
Table 5. Generalized estimation equation (GEE) models with ‘exposure × time’ interaction terms comparing cognitive outcomes between participants prenatally exposed to famine and unexposed participants by sex.
Table 5. Generalized estimation equation (GEE) models with ‘exposure × time’ interaction terms comparing cognitive outcomes between participants prenatally exposed to famine and unexposed participants by sex.
Mean/Median Difference
T1 a
Mean/Median Difference
T2 a
GEE Model
Estimate (B) (95%CI)p-Value
Men
15-word task
Summary
Prenatal famine1.902.111.9 (−3.2–7.0)0.47
Time −1.5 (−3.7–0.7)0.18
Interaction b 2.0 (−3.8–4.2)0.92
Recall
Prenatal famine0.870.450.9 (−0.6–2.4)0.26
Time −0.2 (−1.1–0.8)0.77
Interaction b −0.4 (−2.0–1.2)0.60
Trail-making task (higher score is worse)
Task A
Prenatal famine−1.005.50−2.7 (−9.2–3.9)0.42
Time 1.2 (−3.1–5.6)0.58
Interaction 10.5 (−0.5–21.6)0.06
Task B
Prenatal famine−8.50−10−6.7 (−33.0–19.7)0.62
Time 20.3 (0.1–40.5)0.05
Interaction 10.9 (−15.2–37.0)0.41
Stroop-like task
Prenatal famine3.64−1.683.6 (−17.0–24.3)0.73
Time −2.6 (−14.9–9.7)0.68
Interaction b −5.3 (−28.0–17.4)0.65
Women
15-word task
Summary
Prenatal famine2.461.212.5 (−2.9–7.8)0.37
Time −5.1 (−7.5–2.7)<0.001
Interaction b −1.3 (−6.6–4.1)0.64
Recall
Prenatal famine0.92−0.360.9 (−1.0–2.8)0.34
Time −0.5 (−1.5–0.4)0.28
Interaction b −1.3 (−2.7–0.2)0.08
Trail-making task (higher score is worse)
Task A
Prenatal famine0.50−7.50−2.8 (−8.5–2.9)0.34
Time 10.0 (5.8–14.3)<0.001
Interaction b −5.9 (−12.3–0.6)0.08
Task B
Prenatal famine−7.00−34.50−14.7 (−30.7–1.4)
Time 48.5 (25.6–71.4)<0.001
Interaction b −40.1 (−65.5–14.8)0.002
Stroop-like task
Prenatal famine9.236.359.2 (−11.4–29.9)0.38
Time −5.4 (−15.0–4.2)0.27
Interaction b −2.9 (−24.5–18.8)0.79
Statistically significant results are depicted in bold. a Mean or median (for the trail-making task) difference between the scores of those exposed or unexposed prenatally to the famine at age 68 (T1) and age 74 (T2). b Interaction term for ‘exposure × time’ to evaluate if the rate of cognitive decline differed among the exposed and unexposed groups.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wiegersma, A.M.; Boots, A.; Roseboom, T.J.; de Rooij, S.R. Exposure to the Dutch Famine in Early Gestation and Cognitive Function and Decline in Older Age. Nutrients 2023, 15, 293. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15020293

AMA Style

Wiegersma AM, Boots A, Roseboom TJ, de Rooij SR. Exposure to the Dutch Famine in Early Gestation and Cognitive Function and Decline in Older Age. Nutrients. 2023; 15(2):293. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15020293

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wiegersma, Aline Marileen, Amber Boots, Tessa J. Roseboom, and Susanne R. de Rooij. 2023. "Exposure to the Dutch Famine in Early Gestation and Cognitive Function and Decline in Older Age" Nutrients 15, no. 2: 293. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15020293

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop