Next Article in Journal
The Minimum Methionine Requirement for Adults Aged ≥60 Years Is the Same in Males and Females
Previous Article in Journal
Association of Vegetarian and Vegan Diets with Cardiovascular Health: An Umbrella Review of Meta-Analysis of Observational Studies and Randomized Trials
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Influence of Vitamin D Status on Cognitive Ability in Patients with Bipolar Disorder and Healthy Controls

by
Bernadette Leser
1,
Nina Dalkner
2,*,
Adelina Tmava-Berisha
2,
Frederike T. Fellendorf
2,
Human-Friedrich Unterrainer
3,
Tatjana Stross
2,
Alexander Maget
2,
Martina Platzer
2,
Susanne A. Bengesser
2,
Alfred Häussl
2,
Ina Zwigl
2,
Armin Birner
2,
Robert Queissner
2,
Katharina Stix
2,
Linda Wels
2,
Elena M. D. Schönthaler
2,
Melanie Lenger
2,
Andreas R. Schwerdtfeger
1,
Sieglinde Zelzer
4,
Markus Herrmann
4 and
Eva Z. Reininghaus
2
add Show full author list remove Hide full author list
1
Department of Psychology, University of Graz, 8010 Graz, Austria
2
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapeutic Medicine, Medical University Graz, 8036 Graz, Austria
3
Faculty of Psychotherapy Science, Sigmund Freud University, 1020 Vienna, Austria
4
Clinical Institute of Medical and Chemical Laboratory Diagnostics, Medical University of Graz, 8036 Graz, Austria
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2023, 15(19), 4111; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15194111
Submission received: 18 August 2023 / Revised: 13 September 2023 / Accepted: 21 September 2023 / Published: 22 September 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Micronutrients and Human Health)

Abstract

:
Recent evidence on the association between vitamin D and cognition in mentally healthy individuals is inconsistent. Furthermore, the link between vitamin D and cognitive ability in individuals with bipolar disorder has not been studied yet. Thus, we aimed to investigate the association between 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D), 24,25 dihydroxyvitamin D (24,25(OH)2D, the vitamin D metabolite ratio (VMR) and cognition in a cohort of euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Vitamin D metabolites were measured simultaneously by liquid-chromatography tandem mass-spectrometry in serum samples from 86 outpatients with bipolar disorder and 93 healthy controls. Neither the inactive precursor 25(OH)D, nor the primary vitamin D catabolite 24,25(OH)2D, or the vitamin D metabolite ratio were significantly associated with the domains “attention”, “memory”, or “executive function” in individuals with bipolar disorder and healthy controls. Further, no vitamin D deficiency effect or interaction group × vitamin D deficiency was found in the cognitive domain scores. In summary, the present study does not support vitamin D metabolism as a modulating factor of cognitive function in euthymic BD patients. Considering the current study’s cross-sectional design, future research should expand these results in a longitudinal setting and include additional aspects of mental health, such as manic or depressive symptoms, long-term illness course and psychopharmacological treatment.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Bipolar disorder (BD) is a serious mental illness marked by pathological mood changes. Depressive and (hypo)manic, as well as euthymic phases occur at an unpredictable sequence. Lifetime prevalence ranges from 0.3–1.5% for bipolar type I disorder and up to 5.5% for bipolar type II disorder with a high heritability [1]. Individuals who suffer from BD are often affected by impaired psychosocial functioning, cardiovascular and metabolic comorbidities, and earlier mortality. Importantly, many patients also exhibit cognitive deficits, which are associated with low everyday functioning [1].
In euthymic patients with BD, the largest effects on cognitive impairment are found in the domains of “memory”, “attention”, and “executive function” [2]. Different sociodemographic, genetic, and clinical factors including psychotic symptoms [3], episode frequency [4,5], somatic conditions such as obesity [6] and metabolic syndrome [7], are assumed to contribute to cognitive deficits. In addition, lifestyle and nutrition can influence the illness course [8]. Therefore, the potential role of vitamin D on cognition in mental disorders has gained significant interest in the recent past [9,10].
Vitamin D, which exists as skin-derived vitamin D3 (cholecalciferol) and food-derived vitamin D2 (ergocalciferol), is converted in the liver to 25-hydroxycholecalciferol (25(OH)D3) and 25-hydroxyergocalciferol (25(OH)D2), respectively. If not stated otherwise, laboratory tests for 25(OH)D capture both forms and thus report total 25(OH)D. Despite being the most abundant vitamin D metabolite in blood, 25(OH)D is still inactive and represents the body’s vitamin D reservoir. Biologically active 1,25-dihydroxycholecalciferol (1,25(OH)2D) is produced on demand via a second hydroxylation in position 1 that is catalyzed by 1α-hydroxylase (CYP27B1). This primarily takes place within the kidneys, with fewer occurrences observed in various other tissues, such as the gut, breast, skeletal muscle, immune cells, and brain. Similar to the expression of 1α-hydroxylase, the vitamin D receptor (VDR) is expressed in a broad range of tissues alluding to pleiotropic effects of vitamin D. In the brain, VDR has been detected in regions that are involved in cognitive processes [10]. Excess amounts of 25(OH)D and 1,25(OH)2D are eliminated by an additional hydroxylation in position 24. The resulting 24-hydroxylated metabolites are further processed and ultimately eliminated from the body. Overall, 24,25(OH)2D is the most abundant vitamin D catabolite that can be measured in blood [11].
Individuals with mental illnesses show a significant incidence of vitamin D deficiency [12,13]. For example, Rihal et al. [14] illuminate the associations between vitamin D and schizophrenia, autism, depression, and ADHD. As a result, a deficiency in vitamin D could potentially emerge as a contributing factor to the onset of these neuropsychiatric disorders. Today, it is well accepted that the metabolic actions of vitamin D go far beyond calcium-phosphate homeostasis and involve the brain as well as many other tissues [15]. In their extensive review, Cui et al. [16] provide a description of vitamin D in the central nervous system: Vitamin D supports myelination as well as functional recovery, and alterations in vitamin D levels within brain cells have been linked to a decrease in the density of nerve fibers, delayed and diminished development of dopamine neurons, and impaired release of gamma-aminobutyric acid and glutamate. Additionally, vitamin D is tied to the nerve growth factor, the restoration of dopamine levels, and stress responsiveness. Consequently, vitamin D can be regarded as a significant steroid in the physiology of brain neurons. Therefore, vitamin D contributes to mental health disorders and cognitive functions.
However, existing studies, which investigated potential associations between 25(OH)D and cognitive performance, show inconsistent results. Some studies indicate reduced cognitive performance in individuals with serum 25(OH)D concentrations below the recommended range [17,18], while others did not show any associations between those variables [19,20]. One possible explanation for the inconsistent results is the variable analytical performance of widely used immunoassays for 25(OH)D, which lack sensitivity and specificity [21]. Isotope dilution liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) is the preferred technique for the measurement of 25(OH)D and other related metabolites, which offers high sensitivity and specificity. Furthermore, LC-MS/MS enables the concurrent measurement of various vitamin D metabolites, offering dynamic insights into vitamin D metabolism [22]. A recent study has shown that the parallel measurement of 25(OH)D and 24,25(OH)2D, and calculation of the vitamin D metabolite ratio (VMR), is more specific to establish functional vitamin D deficiency than the isolated measurement of the inactive prohormone 25(OH)D [23]. Mayne et al. [9] provide an overview of the important roles that vitamin D plays in the body and brain, which are also related to cognitive functions. Among others, vitamin D is associated with a mechanism of synaptic plasticity [24]. Synaptic plasticity describes the ability to form new synapses, thus representing a fundamental aspect in learning and memory [25].

Aim of the Study

The primary objective of this study was to explore the relationship between 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D and VMR with cognitive function in patients with BD and a control group of healthy controls. It was hypothesized that patients with BD and healthy controls with a lower vitamin D status show lower performance in “attention”, “memory”, and “executive function” than those with a higher vitamin D status.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Study Design

Individuals included in this study were participants of the longitudinal BIPLONG (“bipolar disorder in the long-term”) cohort at the Division of Psychiatry and Psychotherapeutic Medicine of the Medical University of Graz (Austria). BIPLONG aims to investigate the relationship between lifetime psychiatric history and different variables, such as biological parameters, brain function, and lifestyle in the long term. For a detailed description of the study design and other results, we refer to previous publications [7,26].
BD was diagnosed by a psychiatrist or psychologist using the DSM-IV Structured Clinical Interview [27]. BD I and BD II patients were included, but this distinction was not considered in further analyses. Exclusion criteria were neurodegenerative disorders, a premorbid IQ < 70 and the intake of vitamin D supplements. The present study only included data from euthymic patients with BD and mentally healthy controls without a history of a psychiatric disorder life-time and among first-degree relatives. Euthymia was verified by a Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD; [28]) score of ≤9 and Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS; [29]) score of ≤8, which were rated by a psychiatrist or psychologist. Only euthymic patients were included to exclude possible effects caused by a manic or depressive episode, since Vrabie et al. [5] showed that patients in a manic episode have higher deficits in certain cognitive domains than depressive or euthymic patients. The study received approval from the ethics committee of the Medical University of Graz (EC-number: 25-335 ex 12/13).
From 635 participants of the BIPLONG study, 349 patients were diagnosed with BD. In total, 330 participants (180 patients with BD) were excluded due to missing vitamin D results. Further, information on current mood status was not available for 14 patients with BD, and 50 patients with BD were excluded because they had a YMRS score > 8 and/or a HAMD score > 9 and were therefore not considered euthymic. Cognitive data were missing for 57 participants (15 patients with BD). Four BD patients used vitamin D supplements. One healthy control participant was excluded due to impossible test values. Finally, 86 patients with BD (47 men, 39 women) and 93 healthy controls (29 men, 64 women) were included in the present study.

2.2. Determination of Vitamin D Metabolites

Fasting blood samples were collected for vitamin D testing and the measurement of numerous other blood biomarkers that are not subject of this study. Vitamin D status was assessed by measuring 25(OH)D and 24,25(OH)2D3 in serum. The results were used to calculate the VMR. Both vitamin D metabolites were measured simultaneously with a fully validated LC-MS/MS method that has performed satisfactorily for several years in the Vitamin D External Quality Assurance Scheme (DEQAS) and is regularly controlled by stringent internal quality control procedures. With the method used here, serum 25(OH)D represents the sum of 25(OH)D3 and 25(OH)D2, which are captured separately. Although both forms of 25(OH)D are catabolized by 24-hydroxylation, 25(OH)D2 normally accounts for 1–3% of the total 25(OH)D resulting in 24,25(OH)D2 concentrations below the limit of quantitation. Therefore, vitamin D catabolism is assessed by 24,25(OH)2D3 as the only catabolite that is measurable with the method used here.
According to current recommendations from the Institute of Medicine (IOM), a 25(OH)D concentration < 50 nmol/L is considered deficient [30]. Furthermore, functional vitamin D deficiency was diagnosed using a recently published approach from Herrmann [31]. This approach requires a 24,25(OH)2D3 concentration < 3 nmol/L in combination with a VMR < 4%. Individuals that fulfilled only one of these criteria were classified as having a non-optimal vitamin D metabolism. For more details, see Zelzer et al. [22].

2.3. Assessment Scales

Cognitive performance of the participants was assessed using an extensive cognitive test battery that included the Trail Making Test Part A/B [32], the Color-Word Interference Test by J. R. Stroop [33], and the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; [34]). All tests were provided in the German language. The raw scores were summed up to three cognitive domain scores: “attention”, “memory”, and “executive function”. For this purpose, the TMT-A [32] and the Color and Word part of the Stroop test [33] were combined into the domain score “attention”. The subscales of the CVLT [34] provided information about the domain score “memory”. The interference part of the Stroop test [33] as well as part B of the TMT [32] were used to measure “executive function”. Additionally, the premorbid IQ was measured with the Multiple-Choice Vocabulary Intelligence Test (MWT-B; [35]).

2.4. Statistics

Z-transformations were performed in order to sum up the cognitive raw scores to domain scores. Reaction times were inverted so that higher scores reflect higher performance in all domains. Since normal distribution was not given for the domain scores, a Rankit-transformation of these scores was performed.
Multiple hierarchical regression analyses were calculated to investigate whether 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D3, or VMR were associated with the cognition domain scores in BD patients and healthy controls. The independent variables were 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D3, and VMR. Premorbid IQ and age were included as control variables. The following calculations were made to verify the prerequisites for conducting a multiple hierarchical regression analysis: Correlations and chi-square tests for linearity between independent and dependent variables, Durbin–Watson coefficients for autocorrelations, variance of inflation factors (VIF), tolerance values for multicollinearity, scatterplots for homoscedasticity of residuals, and the Shapiro–Wilk test for testing the normal distribution of the residuals. These prerequisites were met in all regression analyses.
To investigate if patients with BD versus healthy controls with or without functional vitamin D deficiency show a difference in the cognitive domain scores, a two-way multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was calculated. For this purpose, the vitamin D catabolite 24,25(OH)2D3 and the VMR were considered together to classify the functional vitamin D deficiency. Premorbid IQ and age were included as covariables in the model. Skewness and kurtosis for normal distribution, Levene’s test for homogeneity, and graphs for linearity were calculated to verify the prerequisites of the MANCOVA. All prerequisites were met.

3. Results

3.1. Demographics and Sample Characteristics

The mean age of individuals with BD was 45.17 ± 13.15 years (min = 18 years, max = 72 years). The mean age of the healthy controls was 37.75 ± 15.32 years (min = 19 years, max = 76 years). Age, premorbid IQ, YMRS score, HAMD score, 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D3, VMR, the cognitive domain scores, and the differences (as calculated by Mann–Whitney U-tests and chi-square tests) between patients with BD and the healthy controls can be seen in Table 1.

3.2. Multiple Hierarchical Regressions

Three multiple hierarchical regression analyses were calculated to investigate whether there is an association between 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D as well as VMR with the three domain scores “attention”, “memory”, and “executive function” in individuals with BD and healthy controls. In the first model, age was included; in the second, age and premorbid IQ; in the third model, age, premorbid IQ, and 25(OH)D; in the fourth model, age, premorbid IQ, 25(OH)D, and 24,25(OH)2D3; and in the fifth model, all variables (age, premorbid IQ, 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D3, and VMR) were included. The order of entering the predictors was based on considerations of causal priority and served the purpose of controlling for potential effects.

3.3. Patients with Bipolar Disorder

3.3.1. Attention

Multiple hierarchical regression analysis revealed no significant association between the individual level of 25(OH)D 24,25(OH)2D3 or VMR and attention (Model 1: F(1, 84) = 7.31, p = 0.008; Model 2: F(2, 83) = 4.82, p = 0.010; Model 3: F(3, 82) = 3.73, p = 0.014; Model 4: F(4, 81) = 3.28, p = 0.015; Model 5: F(5, 80) = 2.68, p = 0.027). In all steps, only age showed a significant effect on attention. Table 2 shows the regression coefficients.

3.3.2. Memory

The results of the multiple hierarchical regression analysis indicated no significant association between 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D3 or VMR and “memory”. In all steps (Model 1: F(1, 84) = 8.03, p = 0.006; Model 2: F(2, 83) = 9.83, p < 0.001; Model 3: F(3, 82) = 6.82, p < 0.001; Model 4: F (4, 81) = 5.06, p = 0.001, Model 5: F(5, 80) = 4.20, p = 0.002), age showed a significant effect on “memory”, as well as premorbid IQ once it was taken into the model (see Table 2).

3.3.3. Executive Function

In all steps of the multiple hierarchical regression analysis, age showed a significant effect on “executive function” (Model 1: F(1, 84) = 11.48, p = 0.001). Moreover, premorbid IQ showed a significant effect on “executive function” once it was taken into the model (Model 2: F(2, 83) = 10.53, p < 0.001; Model 3: F (3, 82) = 7.01, p < 0.001; Model 4: F(4, 81) = 5.78, p < 0.001; Model 5: F(5, 80) = 4.83, p < 0.001). No significant associations between 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D3, or VMR and “executive function” were found (see Table 2).

3.3.4. Healthy Controls

The results from hierarchical regression analyses in the healthy controls did not differ from patients in BD and results can be seen in the Supplemental Table S1.

3.3.5. Multivariate Analysis of Covariance

A MANCOVA was calculated to test for differences between patients with BD and the healthy controls (factor group) with functional vitamin D deficiency versus those without (factor vitamin D deficiency) in the cognitive domain scores “attention”, “memory”, and “executive function”. The covariates were the participants’ age and their premorbid IQ, which was calculated with the MWT-B [35]. The descriptive results for vitamin D deficiency in both groups are listed in Table 3, showing that 43% of patients and 33.3% of controls were in the non-optimal group. MANCOVA showed a significant effect of group (F(3171) = 2.67, p = 0.049, ηp2 = 0.05). The main effect for vitamin D deficiency (F(3171) = 0.47, p = 0.703, ηp2= 0.01) and the interaction effect for group × vitamin D deficiency (F(3171) = 0.99, p = 0.542, ηp2 = 0.01) were not significant. Age (F(3171) = 24.23, p < 0.001, ηp2= 0.30) and premorbid IQ (F(3171) = 10.71, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.16) were significant covariates. Univariate post hoc analyses of variance (ANOVA) showed that BD patients exhibited lower performance in “memory” (F(1173) = 4.63, p = 0.033, ηp2 = 0.03) and “executive function” (F(1173) = 5.53, p = 0.020, ηp2 = 0.03) than the healthy controls. No difference between the two groups was found for the domain score attention (F(1173) = 3.67, p = 0.057, ηp2 = 0.21).

4. Discussion

The present study investigated whether the vitamin D status (25(OH)D), the main vitamin D catabolite (24,25(OH)2D3), or the vitamin D metabolite ratio (VMR) have an effect on cognition (divided into “attention”, “memory”, and “executive function”) in patients with BD. The findings do not substantiate a significant relationship between vitamin D metabolism and cognitive function. The different indices of vitamin D metabolism were not associated with “attention”, “memory”, or “executive function”, neither in BD patients nor in healthy controls. Our results are contrary to previous data, which showed reduced memory function in older individuals with low 24,25(OH)2D3 concentrations [36]. Besides vitamin D, other factors that influence cognition may also be of interest. For example, associations between cognitive deficits and elevated triacylglycerol and glucose [37] or higher cytokine IL6 [38] have already been found. Other factors, such as the metabolic syndrome [7] anthropometric parameters also showed an association with cognition in patients with BD. Recent research emphasizes the importance of utilizing vitamin D metabolites to determine the vitamin D status, especially to detect deficiency [11,22].
In addition, the occurrence of functional vitamin D deficiency was comparable in BD patients and healthy controls. Therefore, 24,25(OH)2D3 and VMR were used to determine functional vitamin D deficiency. However, no functional vitamin D deficiency effect could be found in our analyses, which tested differences in cognitive domain scores between patients with BD and the healthy controls. In concordance with the current literature, we found that individuals with BD showed poorer memory and executive function performance than the healthy controls. Our findings align with prior research, in which patients with BD also showed cognitive deficits in memory [2], executive function and processing speed [3].
The inconsistent results could be, at least partially, explained by the substantially different age of the participants. In BIPLONG, the participants were rather young, which limits the chance for significant effects induced by vitamin D deficiency. The absence of an association between vitamin D and the domain scores “attention”, “memory”, and “executive function” argues against vitamin D deficiency as a promotor of cognitive dysfunction in BD. This is an important finding since BD patients are often severely impaired by cognitive and somatic deficits. Further possible reasons for inconsistent results regarding an association between BD and poorer cognitive performance could be the use of different cognitive tests or different assignments of these tests to the cognitive domain scores.
A particular strength of our study is the measurement of 25(OH), 24,25(OH)2D3 and VMR, which provides a more comprehensive insight into vitamin D metabolism [11,22]. Determining 24,25(OH)2D3 and VMR offers important metabolic information beyond the sole measurement of 25(OH)D. Functional vitamin D deficiency was equally prevalent in BD patients and in the healthy controls, which argues against a significant role of vitamin D for cognition. However, mean 25(OH)D concentrations of 56.37 nmol/L and 57.29 nmol/L were found in BD patients and the controls, respectively, suggesting a relatively adequate vitamin D supply, which limits the margin for significant effects. In contrast to the present study, previous investigations mainly included hospitalized patients with severe symptoms, in whom 25(OH)D concentrations are typically lower [4]. Existing evidence [4] suggests that BD patients with more frequent manic episodes perform worse on the CVLT than those with less frequent manic episodes. This is an important aspect as manic patients are more often affected by worse memory and executive function compared to depressed and euthymic patients. Therefore, it remains plausible that there is a connection between vitamin D and cognition in symptomatic BD patients. Furthermore, it could be theorized that vitamin D supplementation might have positive effects on mood in BD patients that are mediated via the serotonergic pathway [39]. This hypothesis finds support in data from healthy individuals, which demonstrate a beneficial impact of vitamin D supplementation on mood [39]. However, in a recent literature review, Huiberts et al. [40] concluded that the existing data are inconsistent.

5. Limitations of the Study

Our study possesses various strengths and limitations that warrant attention when interpreting the findings. The simultaneous determination of 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D3 and VMR provides important metabolic information that extends beyond the evaluation of vitamin D stores by only measuring 25(OH)D. These parameters offered the possibility to establish functional vitamin D deficiency, which has been linked to adverse clinical outcomes [31]. Furthermore, the use of a validated state of the art LC-MS/MS method ensured accurate results that are not affected by the analytical limitations of commonly used 25(OH)D immunoassays [21]. Another advantage of this study is a rather large cohort of euthymic BD patients in whom the diagnosis of BD was thoroughly established and cognition was assessed by a well-elaborated cognitive test battery. However, when considering the potential impacts of vitamin D on sleep and mood, significant associations between vitamin D and cognition in manic or depressive BD patients cannot be excluded. Also, many participants were treated with psychopharmacological medication so that potential drug effects on vitamin D metabolism and cognitive function cannot be ruled out. Notably, patients with BD were taking different medications, making it difficult to identify the different effects of each medication. Previous studies reported heterogeneous cognitive effects of commonly used medications in BD patients [41,42]. For example, Paterson et al. [41] found no effect of lithium on attention, but a positive effect on psychomotor speed in patients with BD, whereas Holmes et al. [42] reported a negative effect on attention and affective processing. Factors such as medications, organic diseases, diet, and sun exposure are potential confounders of vitamin D metabolite concentrations, and the absence of this information weakens the statistical power of the present study. However, to reduce confounding factors, we excluded subjects with vitamin D supplementation. In addition, the known seasonal variations of 25(OH)D were accounted for by distributing the study dates relatively evenly throughout the year. Furthermore, the sex-specific differences in the concentration of vitamin D in patients with severe mental illness could be relevant to explore the potentially sex-specific involvement of vitamin D in the pathogenesis of mental health disorders. Previous research suggests a female-specific involvement of vitamin D in the pathogenesis of depression, relating vitamin D to the production/release of gonadal hormones [43,44]. This view has been supported by previous research providing evidence that the gender difference in the prevalence of depression is less evident during menopause, when gonadial hormonal flux stabilizes [45].
Another aspect to consider is the unknown duration of vitamin D deficiency that may vary between the study participants. When taking into account that vitamin D deficiency has potential adverse effects on the signaling pathways in the brain, a longer duration of vitamin D deficiency could potentially be more relevant for cognition than short-lasting deficiencies. As vitamin D3 production is also subject to pronounced seasonal variation, the time of blood collection may also be a factor [46]. Finally, this study included only euthymic patients so that potential effects of manic or depressive symptoms could not be assessed. Ultimately, this study is a cross-sectional study, which means that causal relationships cannot be assessed.
Further studies, ideally of a prospective nature, are needed to consolidate the present results. Such studies should include symptomatic and asymptomatic patients to reveal possible effects released by a manic or depressive episode. In addition, it cannot be excluded that vitamin D influences cognition indirectly through the modulatory effects of other metabolic pathways and inflammatory processes.

6. Conclusions

In summary, the present study does not support vitamin D metabolism as a modulating factor of cognitive function in euthymic BD patients. Moreover, the incidence of low vitamin D stores and functional vitamin D deficiency did not significantly differ between BD patients and healthy controls. As this is the first study that addressed the impact of vitamin D on cognitive function in BD patients, more research is needed to expand the present findings. Specifically, longitudinal studies that include euthymic and symptomatic BD patients are of particular interest.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu15194111/s1, Table S1: Association of age, premorbid IQ, 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D3, VMR with “attention”, “memory”, and “executive function” in healthy controls.

Author Contributions

B.L. was responsible for the conception of the study, the analysis, and interpretation of the data, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. N.D. supervised the process of data collection, the process of writing, and was responsible for publication of the data. A.T.-B. was responsible for patient recruitment and diagnostics, and vitamin D analyses. F.T.F. was responsible for patient recruitment and diagnostics. T.S. was responsible for patient recruitment and diagnostics. A.M. was responsible for patient recruitment and diagnostics. M.P. was responsible for patient recruitment and diagnostics. S.A.B. and A.B. was responsible for patient recruitment and diagnostics. A.H. was responsible for blood collection. I.Z. was responsible for blood collection. A.M. was responsible for patient recruitment and diagnostics. R.Q. was responsible for patient recruitment and diagnostics. K.S. was responsible for the process of data collection. L.W. was responsible for the process of data collection. M.L. was responsible for patient recruitment and diagnostics E.M.D.S. was responsible for the process of data collection and diagnostic. S.Z. was responsible for vitamin D analyses. M.H. was responsible for vitamin D analyses. H.-F.U. and A.R.S. supervised the process of writing. E.Z.R. supervised the whole BIPLONG study procedure. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board Ethics Committee of the Medical University of Graz (EC-number: 25−335 ex 12/13) The participants of the study gave their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Rothenhäusler, H.B.; Täschner, K.L. Kompendium Praktische Psychiatrie, 2nd ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Elias, L.R.; Miskowiak, K.W.; Vale, A.M.; Köhler, C.A.; Kjærstad, H.L.; Stubbs, B.; Kessing, L.V.; Vieta, E.; Maes, M.; Goldstein, B.I.; et al. Cognitive impairment in euthymic pediatric bipolar disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2017, 56, 286–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Bora, E. Neurocognitive features in clinical subgroups of bipolar disorder: A meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 2018, 229, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Martínez-Arán, A.; Vieta, E.; Reinares, M.; Colom, F.; Torrent, C.; Sánchez-Moreno, J.; Benabarre, A.; Goikolea, J.M.; Comes, M.; Salamero, M. Cognitive function across manic or hypomanic, depressed, and euthymic states in bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2004, 161, 262–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Vrabie, M.; Marinescu, V.; Talaşman, A.; Tăutu, O.; Drima, E.; Micluţia, I. Cognitive impairment in manic bipolar patients: Important, understated, significant aspects. Ann. Gen. Psychiatry 2015, 14, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Bora, E.; McIntyre, R.S.; Ozerdem, A. Neurocognitive and neuroimaging correlates of obesity and components of metabolic syndrome in bipolar disorder: A systematic review. Psychol. Med. 2019, 49, 738–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Dalkner, N.; Bengesser, S.A.; Birner, A.; Fellendorf, F.T.; Fleischmann, E.; Großschädl, K.; Lenger, M.; Maget, A.; Platzer, M.; Queissner, R.; et al. Metabolic syndrome impairs executive function in bipolar disorder. Front. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 717824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Simjanoski, M.; Patel, S.; De Boni, R.; Balanzá-Martínez, V.; Frey, B.N.; Minuzzi, L.; Kapczinski, F.; de Azevedo Cardoso, T. Lifestyle interventions for bipolar disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2023, 152, 105257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Mayne, P.E.; Burne, T.H. Vitamin D in synaptic plasticity, cognitive function, and neuropsychiatric illness. Trends Neurosci. 2019, 42, 293–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bivona, G.; Agnello, L.; Bellia, C.; Iacolino, G.; Scazzone, C.; Lo Sasso, B.; Ciaccio, M. Non-skeletal activities of vitamin D: From physiology to brain pathology. Medicina 2019, 55, 341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Bosworth, C.R.; Levin, G.; Robinson-Cohen, C.; Hoofnagle, A.N.; Ruzinski, J.; Young, B.; Schwatrz, S.M.; Himmelfarb, J.; Kestenbaum, B.; de Boer, I.H. The serum 24,25-dihydroxyvitamin D concentration, a marker of vitamin D catabolism, is reduced in chronic kidney disease. Kidney Int. 2012, 82, 693–700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Cereda, G.; Enrico, P.; Cippolino, V.; Devecchio, G.; Brambilla, P. The role of vitamin D in bipolar disorder: Epidemiology and influence on disease activity. J. Affect. Disord. 2021, 278, 209–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Cuomo, A.; Maina, G.; Bolognesi, S.; Rosso, G.; Beccarini Crescenzi, B.; Zanobini, F.; Goracci, A.; Facchi, E.; Favaretto, E.; Baldini, I.; et al. Prevalence and correlates of vitamin D deficiency in a sample of 290 inpatients with mental illness. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Rihal, V.; Kaur, A.; Singh, T.G.; Abdel-Daim, M.M. Therapeutic and mechanistic intervention of vitamin D in neuropsychiatric disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2022, 317, 114782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Christakos, S.; Dhawan, P.; Verstuyf, A.; Verlinden, L.; Carmeliet, G. Vitamin D: Metabolism, molecular mechanism of action, and pleiotropic effects. Physiol. Rev. 2016, 96, 365–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Cui, X.; Eyles, D.W. Vitamin D and the central nervous system: Causative and preventative mechanisms in brain disorders. Nutrients 2022, 14, 4353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Goodwill, A.M.; Szoeke, C. A systemic review and meta-analysis of the effect of low vitamin D on cognition. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2017, 65, 2161–2168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Pavlovic, A.; Abel, K.; Barlow, C.E.; Farrell, S.W.; Weiner, M.; DeFina, L.F. The association between serum vitamin d level and cognitive function in older adults: Cooper Center Longitudinal Study. Prev. Med. 2018, 113, 57–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Schneider, A.L.C.; Lutsey, P.L.; Alonso, A.; Gottesman, R.F.; Sharrett, A.R.; Carson, K.A.; Gross, M.; Post, W.S.; Knopman, D.S.; Mosley, T.H.; et al. Vitamin D and cognitive function and dementia risk in a biracial cohort: The ARIC Brain MRI Study. Eur. J. Neurol. 2014, 21, 1211–1218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Maddock, J.; Zhou, A.; Cavadino, A.; Kuzma, E.; Bao, Y.; Smart, M.C.; Saum, K.-U.; Schöttker, B.; Engmann, J.; Kjaergaard, M.; et al. Vitamin D and cognitive function: A Mendelian randomisation study. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 13230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Farrell, C.; Soldo, J.; Williams, P.; Herrmann, M. 25-Hydroxyvitamin D testing: Challenging the performance of current automated immunoassays. Clin. Chem. Lab. Med. 2012, 50, 1953–1963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Zelzer, S.; Meinitzer, A.; Enko, D.; Simstich, S.; Le Goff, C.; Cavalier, E.; Herrmann, M.; Goessler, W. Simultaneous determination of 24, 25-and 25, 26-dihydroxyvitamin D3 in serum samples with liquid-chromatography mass spectrometry—A useful tool for the assessment of vitamin D metabolism. J. Chromatogr. B 2020, 1158, 122394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Alonso, N.; Zelzer, S.; Eibinger, G.; Herrmann, M. Vitamin D metabolites: Analytical challenges and clinical relevance. Calcif. Tissue Int. 2023, 112, 158–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Taghizadeh, M.; Talaei, S.A.; Djazayeri, A.; Salami, M. Vitamin D supplementation restores suppressed synaptic plasticity in Alzheimer’s disease. Nutr. Neurosci. 2014, 17, 172–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Glasgow, S.D.; Ruthazer, E.S.; Kennedy, T.E. Guiding synaptic plasticity: Novel roles for netrin-1 in synaptic plasticity and memory formation in the adult brain. J. Physiol. 2021, 599, 493–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Reininghaus, E.Z.; McIntyre, R.S.; Reininghaus, B.; Geisler, S.; Bengesser, S.A.; Lackner, N.; Hecht, K.; Birner, A.; Kattnig, F.; Unterweger, R.; et al. Tryptophan breakdown is increased in euthymic overweight individuals with bipolar disorder: A preliminary report. Bipolar Disord. 2014, 16, 432–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Wittchen, H.U.; Wunderlich, U.; Gruschwitz, S.; Zaudig, M. SCID: Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (German Version); Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  28. Hamilton, M. Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 1967, 6, 278–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Young, R.C.; Biggs, J.T.; Ziegler, V.E.; Meyer, D.A. A rating scale for mania: Reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br. J. Psychiatry 1978, 133, 429–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ross, A.C.; Taylor, C.L.; Yaktine, A.L.; Del Valle, H.B. (Eds.) Dietary Reference Intakes for Calcium and Vitamin D; The National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Herrmann, M.; Zelzer, S.; Cavalier, E.; Kleber, M.; Drexler-Herlmber, C.; Schlenke, P.; Curcic, P.; Keppel, M.H.; Enko, D.; Scharnagl, H.; et al. Functional assessment of vitamin D status by a novel metabolic approach: The low vitamin D profile concept. Clin. Chem. 2023; revision under review. [Google Scholar]
  32. Reitan, R.M. Trail Making Test: Manual for Administration, Scoring and Interpretation; Indiana University Medical Center: Indianapolis, IN, USA, 1958. [Google Scholar]
  33. Bäumler, G. Farbe-Wort-Interferenztest (FWIT) Nach J. R. Stroop; Handanweisung; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  34. Niemann, H.; Sturm, W.; Thöne-Otto, A.I.T.; Willmes, K. CVLT California Verbal Learning Test. German Adaptation. Manual; Pearson Assessment: Frankfurt, Germany, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  35. Lehrl, S. Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest MWT-B; Spitta: Balingen, Germany, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  36. Zelzer, S.; Hofer, E.; Meinitzer, A.; Fritz-Petrin, E.; Simstich, S.; Goessler, W.; Schmidt, R.; Herrmann, M. Association of vitamin D metabolites with cognitive function and brain atrophy in elderly individuals—The austrian stroke prevention study. Aging 2021, 13, 9455–9467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Qiu, Y.; Li, S.; Teng, Z.; Tan, Y.; Xu, X.; Yang, M.; Zhao, Z.; Liu, J.; Tang, H.; Xiang, H.; et al. Association between abnormal glycolipid level and cognitive dysfunction in drug-naïve patients with bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2022, 297, 477–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Barbosa, I.G.; de Almeida Ferreira, R.; Rocha, N.P.; Mol, G.C.; Leite, F.D.M.C.; Bauer, I.E.; Teixeira, A.L. Predictors of cognitive performance in bipolar disorder: The role of educational degree and inflammatory markers. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2018, 106, 31–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Jorde, R.; Sneve, M.; Figenschau, Y.; Svartberg, J.; Waterloo, K. Effects of vitamin D supplementation on symptoms of depression in overweight and obese subjects: Randomized double blind trial. J. Intern. Med. 2008, 264, 599–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Huiberts, L.M.; Smolders, K.C. Effects of vitamin D on mood and sleep in the healthy population: Interpretations from the serotonergic pathway. Sleep Med. Rev. 2021, 55, 101379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Paterson, A.; Parker, G. Lithium and cognition in those with bipolar disorder. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2017, 32, 57–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Holmes, M.K.; Erickson, K.; Luckenbaugh, D.A.; Drevets, W.C.; Bain, E.E.; Cannon, D.M.; Snow, J.; Sahakian, B.J.; Husseini, K.M.; Zarate, C.A. A comparison of cognitive functioning in medicated and unmedicated subjects with bipolar depression. Bipolar Disord. 2008, 10, 806–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Kinuta, K.; Tanaka, H.; Moriwake, T.; Aya, K.; Kato, S.; Seino, Y. Vitamin D Is an Important Factor in Estrogen Biosynthesis of Both Female and Male Gonads. Endocrinology 2000, 141, 1317–1324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Lorenzen, M.; Boisen, I.M.; Mortensen, L.J.; Lanske, B.; Juul, A.; Blomberg Jensen, M. Reproductive Endocrinology of Vitamin D. Mol. Cell. Endocrinol. 2017, 453, 103–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Bebbington, P.E.; Dunn, G.; Jenkins, R.; Lewis, G.; Brugha, T.; Farrell, M.; Meltzer, H. The Influence of Age and Sex on the Prevalence of Depressive Conditions: Report from the National Survey of Psychiatric Morbidity. Psychol. Med. 1998, 28, 9–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Giuliani, S.; Barbieri, V.; Di Pierro, A.M.; Rossi, F.; Widmann, T.; Lucchiari, M.; Pusceddu, I.; Pilz, S.; Obermayer-Pietsch, B.; Herrmann, M. LC–MS/MS based 25(OH)D status in a large Southern European outpatient cohort: Gender- and age-specific differences. Eur. J. Nutr. 2019, 58, 2511–2520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Sample characteristics and differences between patients with BD and healthy controls.
Table 1. Sample characteristics and differences between patients with BD and healthy controls.
Patients with BD (n = 86)Healthy Controls (n = 93)Differences
Age (Years), M (±SD)45.17 (13.15)37.75 (15.33)U = 2732.00 **, Z = −3.66
Sex (n, %) χ2 = 10.07 *, φ = 0.24
Male
Female
47 (54.7%)
39 (45.3%)
29 (31.2%)
64 (68.8%)
Premorbid IQ, M (±SD)109.51 (15.23)112.18 (14.71)U = 3579.50, Z = −1.22, p = 0.224
YMRS, M (±SD)0.93 (1.88)0.12 (0.55)U = 3082.50 **, Z = −4.13
HAMD, M (±SD)4.63 (3.45)0.27 (0.95)U = 902.50 **, Z = −9.78
Vitamin D variables, (n, %)
25(OH)D, M (±SD)56.37 (23.64) nmol/L57.29 (23.95) nmol/LU = 3813.50, Z = −0.54
24,25(OH)2D3, M (±SD)3.59 (2.11) nmol/L4.02 (2.51) nmol/LU = 3608.50, Z = −1.13
VMR, M (±SD)6.31 (2.08) %6.91 (2.51) %U = 3465.00, Z = −1.54
25(OH)D χ2 = 0.32, φ = 0.04, p = 0.570
<50 nmol/L
>50 nmol/L
35 (40.7%)
51 (59.3%)
34 (36.6%)
59 (63.4%)
24,25(OH)2D3 χ2 = 1.39, φ = 0.09, p = 0.239
<3 nmol/L
>3 nmol/L
36 (41.9%)
50 (58.1%)
31 (33.3%)
62 (66.7%)
VMR (n, %) χ2 = 0.05, φ = 0.02, p = 0.829
<4%
>4%
13 (15.1%)
73 (84.9%)
13 (14.0%)
80 (86.0%)
Cognitive variables, M (±SD)
TMT-A (s)35.21 (11.98)26.04 (9.74)U = 2082.50 **, Z = −5.53
TMT-B (s)90.99 (51.14)57.12 (20.96)U = 2036.00 **, Z = −5.67
Stroop color word reading (s)33.15 (7.06)29.06 (4.63)U = 2465.00 **, Z = −4.43
Stroop color naming (s)51.70 (9.57)44.11 (6.92)U = 2051.00 **, Z = −5.62
Stroop interference (s)85.16 (24.33)67.67 (14.73)U = 2054.00 **, Z = −5.62
CVLT trial 1–549.74 (13.26)60.48 (11.80)U = 2141.00 **, Z = −5.37
CVLT short delay free recall10.02 (3.51)12.82 (2.96)U = 2090.00 **, Z = −5.54
CVLT short delay cued recall11.03 (3.31)13.27 (2.66)U = 2363.00 **, Z = −4.76
CVLT long delay free recall10.58 (3.57)13.24 (3.09)U = 2182.50 **, Z = −5.28
CVLT long delay cued recall11.34 (3.29)13.46 (2.91)U = 2390.00 **, Z = −4.68
Note: Results from Mann–Whitney U-tests or chi-square tests. IQ = Intelligence quotient, YMRS = Young Mania Rating Scale, HAMD = Hamilton Depression Scale, CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test, TMT = Trail Making Test; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05.
Table 2. Association of age, premorbid IQ, 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D3, VMR with “attention”, “memory”, and “executive function” in patients with BD.
Table 2. Association of age, premorbid IQ, 25(OH)D, 24,25(OH)2D3, VMR with “attention”, “memory”, and “executive function” in patients with BD.
AttentionMemoryExecutive Function
B95%CIβtpB95%CIβtpB95%CIβtp
Model 1Age−0.02[−0.04, −0.01]−0.28−2.700.008−0.02[−0.04, −0.01]−0.30−2.830.006−0.03[−0.04, −0.01]−0.35−3.390.001
Model 2Age−0.02[−0.04, −0.01]−0.32−3.010.003−0.03[−0.04, −0.01]−0.38−3.71<0.001−0.03[−0.05, −0.02]−0.42−4.15<0.001
Premorbid IQ0.01[−0.00, 0.02]0.161.490.1400.02[0.01, 0.04]0.333.270.0020.02[0.01, 0.03]0.302.930.004
Model 3Age−0.2[−0.04, −0.01]−0.31−2.860.005−0.03[−0.04, −0.01]−0.37−3.58<0.001−0.03[−0.05, −0.02]−0.42−4.06<0.001
Premorbid IQ0.01[−0.01, 0.02]0.141.340.1850.02[0.01, 0.03]0.323.130.0020.02[0.01, 0.03]0.292.840.006
25(OH)D−0.26[−0.68, 0.16]−0.13−1.220.226−0.19[−0.59, 0.22]−0.09−0.920.359−0.09[−0.49, 0.32]−0.04−0.420.674
Model 4Age−0.02[−0.04, −0.01]−0.31−2.910.005−0.03[−0.04, −0.01]−0.37−3.56<0.001−0.03[−0.05, −0.02]−0.42−4.11<0.001
Premorbid IQ0.01[−0.01, 0.02]0.131.150.2520.02[0.01, 0.04]0.323.090.0030.02[0.00, 0.03]0.272.650.010
25(OH)D−0.05[−0.57, 0.47]−0.03−0.200.841−0.19[−0.69, 0.31]−0.09−0.750.4530.11[−0.38, 0.61]0.060.460.649
24,25(OH)2D3−0.35[−0.87, 0.17]−0.17−1.350.1810.01[−0.50, 0.50]0.010.0200.985−0.34[−0.83, 0.15]−0.17−1.370.176
Model 5Age−0.02[−0.04, −0.01]−0.31−2.890.005−0.03[−0.04, −0.01]−0.37−3.54<0.001−0.03[−0.05, −0.02]−0.42−4.10<0.001
Premorbid IQ0.01[−0.01, 0.02]0.131.180.2400.02[0.01, 0.04]0.333.130.0020.02[0.01, 0.03]0.282.700.008
25(OH)D−0.03[−0.56, 0.49]−0.02−0.130.899−0.16[−0.67, 0.34]−0.08−0.640.5230.14[−0.35, 0.64]0.070.570.569
24,25(OH)2D3−0.29[−0.85, 0.27]−0.14−1.020.3130.10[−0.44, 0.64]0.050.360.722−0.24[−0.77, 0.30]−0.12−0.890.375
VMR−0.21[−0.87, 0.46]−0.08−0.630.534−0.29[−0.93, 0.35]−0.10−0.910.364−0.32[−0.95, 0.31]−0.12−1.010.315
Note: Attention: Model 1: R2 = 0.08, R2corr = 0.07, Model 2: R2 = 0.10, R2corr = 0.08, Model 3: R2 = 0.12, R2corr = 0.09, Model 4: R2 = 0.14, R2corr = 0.10, Model 5: R2 = 0.14, R2corr = 0.09; Memory: Model 1: R2 = 0.09, R2corr = 0.08, Model 2: R2 = 0.19, R2corr = 0.17, Model 3: R2 = 0.20, R2corr = 0.17, Model 4: R2 = 0.20, R2corr = 0.16, Model 5: R2 = 0.21, R2corr = 0.16; Executive Function: Model 1: R2 = 0.12, R2corr = 0.11, Model 2: R2 = 0.20, R2corr = 0.18, Model 3: R2 = 0.20, R2corr = 0.18, Model 4: R2 = 0.22, R2corr = 0.18, Model 5: R2 = 0.23, R2corr = 0.18. Bold printed p-values are significant.
Table 3. Percentage of optimal and non-optimal functional vitamin D status in individuals with BD and healthy controls.
Table 3. Percentage of optimal and non-optimal functional vitamin D status in individuals with BD and healthy controls.
Patients with BD (n = 86)Healthy Controls (n = 93)
Optimal49 (57.0%)62 (66.7%)χ2 = 1.78, φ = 0.10, p = 0.182
Non-optimal37 (43.0%)31 (33.3%)
Note: BD = Bipolar disorder; Optimal = 24,25(OH)2D3 > 3 and VMR > 4; Non-optimal = 24,25(OH)2D3 < 3 and/or VMR < 4. Results from chi-square tests.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Leser, B.; Dalkner, N.; Tmava-Berisha, A.; Fellendorf, F.T.; Unterrainer, H.-F.; Stross, T.; Maget, A.; Platzer, M.; Bengesser, S.A.; Häussl, A.; et al. The Influence of Vitamin D Status on Cognitive Ability in Patients with Bipolar Disorder and Healthy Controls. Nutrients 2023, 15, 4111. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15194111

AMA Style

Leser B, Dalkner N, Tmava-Berisha A, Fellendorf FT, Unterrainer H-F, Stross T, Maget A, Platzer M, Bengesser SA, Häussl A, et al. The Influence of Vitamin D Status on Cognitive Ability in Patients with Bipolar Disorder and Healthy Controls. Nutrients. 2023; 15(19):4111. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15194111

Chicago/Turabian Style

Leser, Bernadette, Nina Dalkner, Adelina Tmava-Berisha, Frederike T. Fellendorf, Human-Friedrich Unterrainer, Tatjana Stross, Alexander Maget, Martina Platzer, Susanne A. Bengesser, Alfred Häussl, and et al. 2023. "The Influence of Vitamin D Status on Cognitive Ability in Patients with Bipolar Disorder and Healthy Controls" Nutrients 15, no. 19: 4111. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15194111

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop