Next Article in Journal
Modulatory Effects of Co-Fermented Pu-erh Tea with Aqueous Corn Silk Extract on Gut Microbes and Fecal Metabolites in Mice Fed High-Fat Diet
Next Article in Special Issue
BMI Increases in Individuals with COVID-19-Associated Olfactory Dysfunction
Previous Article in Journal
Nutritional Support for Liver Diseases
Previous Article in Special Issue
Relation of Overweight/Obesity to Reward Region Response to Food Reward and the Moderating Effects of Parental History of Eating Pathology in Adolescent Females
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Factors Associated with Poor Olfaction and Olfactory Decline in Older Adults in the ARIC Neurocognitive Study

1
The Memory Impairment and Neurodegenerative Dementia (MIND) Center, University of Mississippi Medical Center, Jackson, MS 39216, USA
2
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA
3
Department of Epidemiology, Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA
4
Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
5
Department of Biostatistics, Epidemiology, and Informatics, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
6
Department of Neurology, University of North Carolina Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA
7
Stroke Branch, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Intramural Research Program, Bethesda, MD 20892, USA
8
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2023, 15(16), 3641; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15163641
Submission received: 17 July 2023 / Revised: 13 August 2023 / Accepted: 18 August 2023 / Published: 19 August 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Taste and Olfaction in Health and Disease)

Abstract

:
Olfactory function has significant implications for human health, but few risk factors for olfactory decline have been identified. We examined the factors associated with olfactory status and decline over five years in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Neurocognitive Study. A 12-item odor identification test was used to assess olfaction in 6053 participants in 2011–2013 (ARIC visit 5, mean age: 75.6, 41% male, 23% Black race) and in 3235 participants in 2016–2017 (visit 6). We used Poisson regression models to examine cross-sectional associations of a range of potential factors with the total odor identification errors (mean errors: 2.8 ± 2.4) in visit 5 participants. We used mixed-effect Poisson regression to examine associations with olfactory decline between visits 5 and 6. We also examined associations with visit 5 anosmia prevalence (847 cases, 14%) and incident anosmia between the two visits (510 cases, 16%) using Poisson models. Older age, male sex, lower education, Black race, APOE ε4 alleles, and diabetes were associated with higher odor identification errors and higher anosmia prevalence, and greater physical activity and hypertension with better olfaction. Age, male sex, lower education, Black race, APOE ε4 allele, and vitamin B12 levels were associated with incident anosmia over 5 years. Older age was associated with faster olfactory decline. Future studies with longer follow-ups are warranted.

1. Introduction

Olfactory impairment affects approximately one-fourth of older adults [1], with prevalence increasing with age up to 63% among individuals aged ≥ 80 years [2]. Although highly prevalent, it is often under-recognized as people tend to be unaware of their olfactory functioning [3]. Olfactory loss can occur from sudden or gradual damage to peripheral olfactory structures (e.g., the olfactory epithelium), the olfactory bulb, or central brain structures responsible for olfactory processing (e.g., the entorhinal cortex). Although generally underappreciated, olfaction has significant implications for human health. Poor olfaction affects multiple aspects of everyday life, including nutrition, safety, social relationships, and mental health, diminishing the overall quality of life [4,5,6]. Furthermore, central olfactory structures are one of the earliest brain structures to be affected in neurodegenerative diseases, including Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease, resulting in very early loss of olfactory ability years before the onset of clinical symptoms [7,8]. Olfactory impairment has also been shown to be associated with higher mortality, frailty, and physical function decline, suggesting broader health implications beyond its established associations with neurodegenerative diseases [9,10].
While acquired causes of acute olfactory loss, such as viral infections, traumatic brain injuries, rhinosinusitis, and olfactory structural abnormalities, have been well documented [11,12], few other risk factors of olfactory decline over several years have been identified. In older adults, the prevalence of olfactory impairment increases with age and is about two times as high in men as in women and similarly elevated in Black and White individuals [2,11,13,14]. Studies have also linked smoking [2,15], cardiovascular factors [16,17], diabetes [17], lower physical activity [18], and systemic inflammatory markers [19] with poor olfaction, but findings have been sparse and inconsistent [13,20,21,22,23]. Furthermore, the existing evidence on determinants of olfactory impairment mostly comes from cross-sectional studies [13,23,24,25,26], and only a few prospective studies have investigated the factors associated with longitudinal changes in olfactory function [17,21,22]. Furthermore, while olfactory impairment can affect future nutritional status by altering food perception and dietary behaviors, optimal nutritional status is also important for normal olfaction, as it can influence neuronal health through mechanisms such as neuroinflammation, oxidative stress, and other vascular- and chronic-disease-mediated effects [27,28]. Yet, very few studies have evaluated the associations between nutritional factors and olfaction [26,29,30,31].
Here, we sought to identify the factors associated with olfactory status as well as its change over time in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Neurocognitive Study. Previously, an investigation used ARIC study data to examine cross-sectional associations of a few sociodemographic, behavioral, and cardiovascular risk factors with anosmia prevalence [23]. Here, we extend the prior investigation by examining comprehensive factors, including additional cardiovascular, inflammatory, and nutritional factors that were not previously examined, and conduct new analyses for incident anosmia and 5-year olfactory change.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Population

The ARIC study is a prospective cohort study of 15,792 community-dwelling adults (aged 45–64 years at enrollment in 1987–1989) from four US communities: Washington County, Maryland; Forsyth County, North Carolina; Jackson, Mississippi; and suburbs of Minneapolis, Minnesota [32,33]. ARIC participants have undergone multiple comprehensive in-person examinations and are followed up through hospitalization and mortality surveillance. Olfaction was assessed at the fifth (visit 5, 2011–2013) and the sixth (visit 6, 2016–2017) in-person exams (as a part of the ARIC Neurocognitive Study [34]), which were attended by 6538 and 4003 participants, respectively. The current analysis includes participants who completed the visit 5 exam.
After excluding visit 5 participants without olfaction data (n = 445), those other than of Black or White race (n = 15, due to small sample size), and Black participants from the Maryland and Washington sites (n = 25, due to small sample size), we had 6053 participants eligible for our analysis. Of these, 3238 participants completed a second olfaction test at visit 6. The primary analyses included all eligible participants but, in a sensitivity analysis, we excluded prevalent dementia (n = 247) and Parkinson’s disease (n = 33). The study was approved by institutional review boards at all participating study centers, and all participants provided written informed consent.

2.2. Olfaction

We used the 12-item Sniffin’ Sticks odor identification test to assess participants’ olfaction [35,36]. This test is commonly used in clinical and epidemiological studies to screen for olfaction impairment. Participants were asked to smell 12 odorants, one at a time, presented in a felt-tip pen, and then pick the correct descriptor from four options. The target odorants were banana, cinnamon, cloves, coffee, fish, leather, lemon, licorice, orange, peppermint, pineapple, and rose. For each correct response, one point was given, with a total possible score of 12. For our primary analysis, we used the total number of odorants incorrectly identified as our continuous analytic outcome. Furthermore, we also examined associations with the categorical outcome anosmia (defined as any participant with a score ≤ 6) [36].

2.3. Potential Determinants of Olfaction

Based on the prior literature, we considered sociodemographic (age, sex, race, study site, and education), behavioral (smoking, alcohol consumption, and physical activity), vascular and/or metabolic (body mass index (BMI), serum total cholesterol, diabetes, hypertension status, coronary heart disease, and myocardial infarction), genetic (apolipoprotein E gene epsilon 4 allele (APOE ε4)), inflammatory (C-reactive protein), blood hemoglobin, and nutritional factor (serum vitamin B12) as the potential risk factors of olfaction [17,18,19,20,21,22]. With the exception of sex, race, study site, and education, which were obtained from the visit 1 exam, other risk factors data were obtained from the visit 5 exam.
Information on sex, race, study site, and education (<high school, high school or equivalent certificate/vocational training, and >high school) was self-reported. Study site and race (site–race) were jointly examined as Washington–White, Forsyth–Black, Forsyth–White, Jackson–Black, and Minneapolis–White due to site–race aliasing. Smoking (current, former, and never) and alcohol consumption status (current, former, and never) were collected using questionnaires. The Modified Baecke Physical Activity Questionnaire was used to obtain information on physical activities including their frequency and duration. The overall physical activity information was expressed as the total metabolic equivalent of task (MET) minutes per week (i.e., energy expenditure per week); details are described elsewhere [37].
BMI (kg/m2) was obtained from measured height and weight and examined as a continuous as well as a categorical variable (normal < 25, overweight 25.0–<30, and obese ≥ 30). Serum total cholesterol (mg/dL) was measured using enzymatic methods [38]. Diabetes was defined as fasting glucose ≥ 126 mg/dL or nonfasting glucose ≥ 200 mg/dL, self-report of physician’s diagnosis, or antidiabetic medication use. Hypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure ≥ 140 mm Hg, diastolic blood pressure ≥ 90 mm Hg, or antihypertensive medication use. We also performed a sensitivity analysis with hypertension categorized as normal, prehypertension (defined as systolic blood pressure ≥ 120 and <140 mmHg), and hypertension. History of coronary heart disease, stroke, and myocardial infarction was based on self-reports at baseline and then adjudicated through medical records during the course of the study. The APOE ε4 polymorphisms were genotyped using the TaqMan assay (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA).
High-sensitivity C-reactive protein, a nonspecific marker of systemic inflammation, was measured in visit 5 samples using an immunoturbidimetric assay on the Beckman Coulter Olympus AU400e analyzer [38]. Serum vitamin B12 levels were measured in visit 5 samples using a direct chemiluminescent competitive immunoassay method, and hemoglobin levels were measured in visit 5 whole blood using automated hematology analyzers [38]. Anemia was defined as hemoglobin levels lower than 13 g/dL for men and 12 g/dL for women [39].

2.4. Statistical Analysis

We used Poisson regression models to examine the cross-sectional associations of potential risk factors with the number of odor identification errors (i.e., olfaction status at visit 5) and estimated “rate” ratios (RRs) (i.e., the ratio of expected error counts interpreted as the percentage difference in the scores) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). We used mixed-effect Poisson regression models with random intercepts to examine the associations between these factors and the change in the number of odor identification errors from visit 5 to visit 6. We used an interaction term between the exposure of interest and time to estimate its association with olfactory decline, which we presented as the ratio of “rate” ratio (RRR). In sensitivity analyses, to account for the potential floor effect, we excluded individuals with anosmia at visit 5 and repeated the mixed-effect Poisson models. Covariates in the model varied depending on the exposure of interest under analysis. For example, for site–race as the exposure of interest, we adjusted for age, sex, education, cigarette smoking, and alcohol consumption, and for diabetes as the exposure, we adjusted for age, sex, site–race, education, cigarette smoking, alcohol consumption, BMI, physical activity, total cholesterol, and hypertension.
We also examined the associations of these factors with visit 5 anosmia prevalence using Poisson regression to estimate prevalence ratios (PRs) (n = 6053) although some of these factors have been analyzed in a previous pooled data publication [23]. Furthermore, among those who completed olfactory assessments at both visit 5 and visit 6 and did not have anosmia at visit 5 (n = 2977), we used Poisson regression to estimate the risk ratio (RR) for associations with “incident” anosmia at visit 6. We also repeated analyses for some factors by incorporating the inverse probability of attrition weights to correct for potential bias due to selective attrition from visit 5 to visit 6 as sensitivity analyses.
Furthermore, given the prior findings suggesting racial differences in olfaction [13,14], we also report the associations of these factors with olfaction for White and Black races separately. We used Stata/SE version 16.0 (StataCorp LLC, College Station, TX, USA) for statistical analyses; p-value (two-sided) ≤ 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

3. Results

Participants’ mean ± SD age at visit 5 was 75.6 ± 5.2 years. About 41% were male, 23% were Black race, and 44% had education higher than high school (Table 1). The participants correctly identified 9.2± 2.4 and 8.9 ± 2.4 odorants on average (the number of errors: 2.8 ± 2.4 and 3.1 ± 2.4) at visit 5 and visit 6, respectively (Table 1, Tables S1 and S2). About 14% (847 of 6053) had anosmia at visit 5 and 16% (510 of 3238) at visit 6. Of the 3238 who completed the olfactory tests at both visits, 261 (8.0%) had anosmia at visit 5. Of the 2977 individuals without anosmia at visit 5, 317 (10.7%) developed anosmia at visit 6. Those who did not attend the visit 6 exam tended to be older and diabetic and were less educated than those who attended the visit 6 exam (Table S2).
In the cross-sectional analysis examining the associations with the number of odor identification errors at visit 5, older age, male sex, lower education, Black race (from Forsyth or Jackson), current or former smoking, any APOE ε4 (≥1 allele) carrier, diabetes, and anemia were significantly associated with worse olfactory performance (Table 2). For example, 10-year higher (older) age and male sex were associated with 39% (RR: 1.39 (1.34, 1.43)) and 32% (1.32 (95% CI: 1.26, 1.38)) higher number of error in odor identification, respectively. Compared with White participants in the Washington center, Black participants from the Jackson and Forsyth centers had 64% higher (RR: 1.64 (95% CI: 1.54, 1.74)) and 31% higher (1.31 (95% CI: 1.08, 1.59)) number of error, respectively. For instance, this RR of 1.64% or 64% higher errors can be interpreted as follows: compared with Washington–White participants who made on average three errors in the olfaction test, Jackson–Black participants made five errors (two additional errors) on average. Current drinking, higher physical activity, and hypertension were each associated with lower number of error in odor identification. When hypertension was examined as a three-level categorical variable, only hypertension, but not prehypertension, was associated with olfaction (RR: 0.91 (95% CI: 0.85, 0.98)). Given the inverse association with hypertension, we also examined potential heterogeneity in the association with antihypertensive medication use, but we did not find any interaction (p-value for the interaction term: 0.53).
In race-specific analysis, for most of the factors, the associations were similar between White and Black participants (Table S3), except that current smoking was more strongly associated with olfaction only among Whites, and higher BMI was associated with better olfaction only among Blacks. Statistical interaction was significant for concurrent BMI (p-value for the interaction term = 0.001) but not significant for smoking (≥0.05).
In the analyses examining changes in continuous olfactory performance from visit 5 to visit 6, we observed that older age was associated with modestly faster olfactory decline, and male sex and Black participants from the Jackson center were associated with slower decline (Table 2). For example, the RRR for 10-year higher age was 1.01 (95% CI: 1.00, 1.02). The results were similar when we excluded individuals with Parkinson’s disease or dementia at visit 5 (Table S4), but only age (RRR: 1.02 (95% CI: 1.01, 1.03) per 10-year higher) was associated with faster olfactory decline after excluding individuals with prevalent anosmia at visit 5 (Table S5).
In the analyses examining anosmia, older age, male sex, and education < high school, Black participants from Forsyth and Jackson, and APOE ε4 carrier (≥1 allele) were significantly associated with higher anosmia prevalence at visit 5 (Table 3). BMI ≥ 30, physical activity, and hypertension were significantly associated with lower anosmia prevalence. Unlike odor identification errors as the outcome, alcohol consumption, smoking status, and anemia/hemoglobin were not significantly associated with anosmia prevalence. In the incident anosmia analysis, older age (RR: 2.12 (1.70, 2.65) per 10-year higher); male sex (RR: 1.41 (95% CI: 1.12, 1.76)); lower education (high school: 1.48 (95% CI:1.16, 1.89) and <high school: 1.53 (95% CI:1.10, 2.13) compared with >high school); Forsyth–Black (2.30 (95% CI: 1.12, 4.72)), Forsyth–White (1.46 (95% CI: 1.05, 2.02)), and Jackson–Black (2.51 (95% CI:1.82, 3.45)), each compared with Washington–White; APOE ε4 carrier (1.33 (95% CI: 1.05, 1.68)); and history of myocardial infarction (1.55 (95% CI: 1.00, 2.40)) were associated with elevated risk of anosmia at visit 6, and higher serum vitamin B12 (0.71 (95% CI: 0.51, 0.98) per 1 SD higher) was associated with lower risk. The incident anosmia analyses for some factors that incorporated the inverse probability of attrition weights were similar to the primary analyses (Table S6). There were no consistent racial differences in prevalent and incident anosmia analyses except that education was more strongly associated with prevalent and incident anosmia in Black participants than in White participants; the interaction term between race and education was statistically significant for prevalent (p-value for the interaction term ≤ 0.05) but not for incident anosmia (Table S7).

4. Discussion

In this sample of community-dwelling adults, we found that older age, male sex, lower education, Black race, and APOE ε4 allele were cross-sectionally associated with poor olfactory status, including anosmia. Furthermore, current cigarette smoking status, diabetes, and anemia were cross-sectionally associated with higher odor identification error, and current alcohol consumption, higher physical activity, and hypertension were associated with fewer errors, although only physical activity and hypertension showed associations with anosmia prevalence. Only older age was associated with faster subject-specific olfactory decline. On the other hand, older age, male sex, lower education, Black race, and APOE ε4 allele were associated with higher incident anosmia risk over 5 years.
Despite a high prevalence of poor olfaction in older adults and its potential broad implications on health [9], we know little about potential risk factors for age-related olfaction loss in older adults. Prospective studies on risk factors of olfaction loss are particularly rare; these were mainly based on the Epidemiology of Hearing Loss Study (EHLS) [18,22], the Beaver Dam Offspring Study (BOSS) [16,21], and the Swedish National Study on Aging and Care in Kungsholmen (SNAC-K) cohorts [15,17], all predominantly White populations. These primarily evaluated risk factors in relation to olfactory impairment incidence, and, to our knowledge, only the SNAC-K study has comprehensively examined the associations of risk factors with subject-specific longitudinal change in olfaction performance [17]. The overall study findings have been largely inconclusive, which could, in part, be attributed to the difference in the timing of the exposure and olfaction assessments (i.e., middle-aged vs. older groups); instruments used to assess olfaction (for example, an eight-item test used in the EHLS and BOSS vs. a 16-item test in the SNAC-K study); and follow-up durations in prospective investigations.
In our study, older age was the only factor consistently associated with olfaction, including longitudinal decline. Among all the factors, the association of age with poor olfactory performance is also the most consistent finding across studies [2,17,21,23,24]. Aging individuals may experience greater functional and pathological alterations in the olfactory regions due to wide-ranging factors, resulting in diminished olfactory ability [40,41]. For example, chronic inflammatory nasal conditions and long-term exposure to airborne environmental toxicants may affect peripheral olfaction, and aging-associated neurodegenerative changes within certain brain structures may affect central olfactory processing. We found that male sex and lower education were associated with poor olfaction in cross-sectional analyses and with incident anosmia, but not with olfactory decline. These findings also align with prior studies showing their associations with an elevated risk of olfactory impairment in cross-sectional [2,17,23,24] and prospective investigations [21,22], but not with decline [17]. The potential explanations for sex differences in olfactory ability include sex-hormone-mediated effects on the olfactory system, differences in underlying cognitive/semantic abilities between men and women, and greater hazardous occupational exposure in men [42]. Likewise, educational attainment influences cognition and other lifestyle and occupational exposures that are linked with olfaction.
Compared with White participants from Washington, Maryland, Black participants from Jackson, Mississippi, and Forsyth, North Carolina, had higher anosmia prevalence, consistent with prior cross-sectional reports suggesting worse odor identification ability in Blacks than in Whites [13,23,43,44]. While participants from Jackson and Forsyth overall also showed higher risks of anosmia at visit 6, we did not see faster rates of olfactory decline in these groups; these findings were comparable to one US-based study that found higher odds of poor performance in the future but not greater mean decline in Black participants than in White participants [14]. The higher burden of olfactory impairment in the Black vs. in White race is likely due to systemic socioeconomic and environmental inequities. Black individuals, compared with their White counterparts, tend to have lower education, live in poor housing conditions and areas with heavy pollution (for example, areas with poor air quality and near contaminated sites), and have more hazardous jobs [44,45,46], which are associated with olfactory impairment [47]. Furthermore, Black older adults experience a disproportionately higher burden of cognitive impairment, including dementia [48], for which olfactory impairment is an early symptom; this may also partly explain the racial differences in olfactory abilities. It has also been suggested that Blacks perform worse in odor identification tests due to less familiarity with the odorants used, although the prior ARIC investigation found that Black participants performed consistently worse than White participants in item-wise test performance comparisons, suggesting otherwise [23]. In the ARIC study, due to its study design, we cannot rule out that the observed site–race associations with olfaction are mainly due to racial disparities unique to ARIC sites. When we performed the analyses separately for Whites and Blacks, we did not find consistent race-specific associations, except that education was more strongly associated with prevalent and incident anosmia in Black participants compared with White participants. Future studies are warranted to shed additional light on the association between race and olfaction.
As with race, we observed that the APOE ε4 carrier status was associated with poor olfaction at visit 5 and elevated anosmia risk at visit 6, but not with longitudinal olfactory decline. The APOE ε4 allele has been linked with elevated tau and amyloid pathology and atrophy in the medial temporal lobe regions (the brain structures implicated in olfactory processing and early Alzheimer’s disease) [8,49,50], even in cognitively normal individuals [49]. Individuals carrying at least one ε4 allele have been shown to have poor odor identification ability and faster rate of olfactory decline in prior studies, although not always [15,17,23,51,52,53]. For example, in the SNAC-K study, the APOE ε4 carrier status was associated with a 12-year olfactory decline [17]. In the same cohort with 6-year follow-up data, the APOE ε4 carrier status was associated with elevated odds of future olfactory impairment [15]. In another study, APOE ε4 homozygosity was associated with a 10-year decline only in middle-aged participants (45–60 years) but not in older participants (aged ≥ 65 years), suggesting that APOE ε4-associated decline may start in midlife [54].
Current and former smokers at visit 5 had lower olfactory scores, but smoking status was not associated with anosmia prevalence or olfactory decline. Cigarette smoke, due to its irritant properties as well as neurotoxic constituents (e.g., cadmium and polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons), can affect peripheral olfaction through inflammation and direct neuronal damage. Furthermore, these neurotoxicants can directly enter the brain by bypassing the blood–brain barrier and affect central olfactory structures. Prior studies have linked current smokers with higher prevalence and incidence of anosmia, although not consistently [15,18,21,22,23,24,55], whereas former smokers have mostly shown no association [55], leading to the notion that smoking’s effect on olfaction may be reversible. In the ARIC study, there were only a few current smokers at visit 5, and most of the former smokers reported quitting prior to visit 1, which may explain the limited associations observed in the study.
Furthermore, while we found significant associations between physical activity and other cardiovascular risk factors and olfaction in cross-sectional examinations, the directions of the associations were not consistent across the factors. For example, lower physical activity and diabetes were associated with poor olfactory status, whereas BMI ≥ 30 and hypertension were associated with better olfaction. Other than myocardial infarction’s association with incident anosmia, no cardiovascular factors were associated with anosmia risk or longitudinal decline. Due to the well-established links between cardiovascular risk factors and brain health, we anticipated that these factors would be associated with olfactory impairment. Cardiovascular factors, including obesity, diabetes, and hypertension, have been linked with cerebrovascular and neurodegenerative changes [56,57,58] and can affect brain structures involved in olfactory processing, leading to olfactory impairment. Diabetes may disrupt olfactory function also through insulin resistance and other hormonal modulation [59]. Consistent with our findings, prior studies have reported cross-sectional associations of lower physical activity and diabetes with olfactory impairment, although prospective study findings have generally been inconsistent [16,17,18,21,22,59]. Prior findings on BMI and olfaction have also been mixed [15,16,17,18,21,22,60]. In this study, we examined BMI as a potential determinant of olfaction and olfactory decline; however, the association between BMI and olfaction is likely bidirectional, as poor olfaction can affect body mass by altering nutritional intake [61], which may explain the positive association between higher BMI and better olfaction in our cross-sectional analysis. Our finding with hypertension was unexpected and could be a chance finding, but it aligns with the SNAC-K study that found a link between hypertension and attenuation in olfactory decline among older participants (aged ≥ 78 years) [17]. In the EHLS, a population-based study in Beaver Dam, Wisconsin (median age: 66 years at baseline olfactory testing), hypertension was not associated with olfaction [18,22], but in the BOSS study involving adult children of the EHLS (mean age: 49 years at baseline olfactory testing), hypertension was associated with higher incidence of 5-year olfactory impairment and antihypertension medication use in women (but not hypertension) with 10-year olfactory impairment [16,21]. However, these factors were examined as covariates (rather than the exposure of interest) in the analyses that examined subclinical atherosclerosis markers and environmental risk factors as the primary exposures of interest. The disparate associations seen across the studies could also be due to differences in the timing of cardiovascular risk factors and olfaction measurements. Further prospective studies with long-term follow-ups are warranted to provide more insights into this aspect. In the ARIC study, olfaction was measured at a relatively older age (mean age: 75.6 ± 5.8). Although the study collected comprehensive data on cardiovascular factors from midlife through late life, due to a lack of midlife (baseline) olfaction data, we were limited in capturing olfactory change occurring during that period and thus in providing a complete picture of the associations.
Our findings with vitamin B12 and hemoglobin were not robust; for example, higher vitamin B12 levels were associated with incident anosmia, and higher blood hemoglobin was associated with lower rates of olfactory errors but not with other analytic olfactory outcomes. We know of only a handful of studies that have linked vitamin B12 and anemia with poor olfaction, all cross-sectional in nature and some based on small clinical samples [29,31]. So, due to a paucity of data, the current understanding of the association between nutrition and olfaction is very minimal. Still, the associations of these factors with adverse neurological outcomes, including reduced brain volumes (including of those regions involved in olfactory processing, for example, the hippocampus) [62,63,64], lend some indirect support to the notion that these factors might be associated with olfaction.
Although we found that some factors (e.g., male sex, Black race, and lower education) were associated with incident anosmia, there was no evidence for their association with rates of olfactory decline. In our study, olfaction was assessed at only two time points (3 to 7 years apart, median: 5 years) among older participants (mean age: 75.6). So, for within-person changes, it is likely that the 12-item odor identification test was not sensitive enough to detect the subtle declines in olfaction occurring within this relatively short period. Statistically, greater power is required to detect individual differences in change compared with differences in performance levels. Furthermore, if individuals with higher risk profiles (say, male sex, and black race) start with lower olfactory ability in early life, they may reach a threshold for olfactory impairment earlier even if they experience similar rates of decline (as individuals with lower risk profiles), potentially explaining our discrepant findings. Future studies in younger populations and long-term follow-ups may shed more light on the associations of these factors with olfactory decline.
There are also several other limitations to consider. Olfaction was measured at a somewhat older age when the potential risk factors examined (for example, cardiovascular diseases) were already likely at play, and thus this time window when olfaction was assessed may not have been most relevant. We cannot rule out bias due to selective attrition, as only half of the visit 5 participants attended visit 6, and those least likely to attend were also those mostly to have olfactory problems. We did not examine other olfactory domains (e.g., an odor threshold test) that may have provided a comprehensive understanding of the associations with olfaction. For example, an odor threshold test measures a person’s ability to detect odorants and has been attributed more to peripheral olfaction (due to its minimal cognitive dependency), as compared to the odor identification test that may involve central olfactory processing to a greater extent (although they both share common elements). Finally, we used a one-time measurement of nutritional markers which may not reflect long-term body burden due to day-to-day variation in nutrient intake. Nonetheless, our study is one of the largest studies so far to evaluate a wide range of factors in relation to olfactory decline in a sample of Black and White adults.
In conclusion, we found several determinants of poor olfaction in this cross-sectional analysis. While some of these factors were also associated with future anosmia risk, other than with age, we did not see any association between these factors and the longitudinal decline in olfaction. Future studies, especially in younger populations, with longer follow-ups and more detailed assessments of olfaction, are warranted.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu15163641/s1, Table S1: Visit 5 and visit 6 olfaction score (total number of errors, mean (SD)) by visit 5 characteristics; Table S2: Visit 5 characteristics of the participants by visit 6 completion status (n = 6053); Table S3: Associations of visit 5 characteristics with visit 5 olfaction status and change in olfaction from visit 5 to visit 6, stratified by race; Table S4: Associations of visit 5 characteristics with visit 5 olfaction status and change in olfaction from visit 5 to visit 6, excluding those with dementia and Parkinson’s disease at visit 5; Table S5: Associations of visit 5 characteristics with change in olfaction from visit 5 to visit 6, excluding individuals with anosmia at visit 5; Table S6: Associations of visit 5 characteristics with visit 6 anosmia using inverse probability weights; Table S7: Associations of visit 5 characteristics with visit 5 anosmia prevalence and visit 6 anosmia risk stratified by race.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.S., T.H.M., M.E.G. and H.C.; formal analysis, X.Z. and M.E.G.; methodology and investigation, S.S., X.Z., V.K., K.J.S., J.A.D., A.R.S., A.L.C.S., P.P., R.F.G., B.G.W., T.H.M., M.E.G. and H.C.; writing and revision, S.S., X.Z., V.K., K.J.S., J.A.D., A.R.S., A.L.C.S., P.P., R.F.G., B.G.W., T.H.M., M.E.G. and H.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study is carried out as a collaborative study supported by National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute contracts (75N92022D00001, 75N92022D00002, 75N92022D00003, 75N92022D00004, 75N92022D00005). The ARIC Neurocognitive Study is supported by U01HL096812, U01HL096814, U01HL096899, U01HL096902, and U01HL096917 from the NIH (NHLBI, NINDS, NIA, and NIDCD) and olfaction data collection was funded by the intramural research program of NIH, NIEHS (1ZIAES101986). Dr. Deal is supported by NIH/NIA grant K01AG054693. Dr. Schneider is supported by NIH NINDS grant K23 NS123340. Dr. Kamath is supported by grants from the NIH (R01AG064093 and R01NS108452). Dr. Gottesman is supported by the NINDS Intramural Research Program. Dr. Chen is supported by the NIH grant R01AG071517. Dr. Palta is in part supported by NIH/NIA grant R00AG052830.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of each participating ARIC center.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The ARIC study data used for this analysis are available to qualified investigators upon request. Details on data availability and study protocols can be accessed at the ARIC website (https://sites.cscc.unc.edu/aric/).

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the staff and participants of the ARIC study for their important contributions.

Conflicts of Interest

Dr. Deal reports honoraria from Velux Stiftung and the Medical Education Speakers Network. Dr. Schneider is an Associate Editor at Neurology. The other authors report no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Desiato, V.M.; Levy, D.A.; Byun, Y.J.; Nguyen, S.A.; Soler, Z.M.; Schlosser, R.J. The Prevalence of Olfactory Dysfunction in the General Population: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Am. J. Rhinol. Allergy 2021, 35, 195–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Murphy, C.; Schubert, C.R.; Cruickshanks, K.J.; Klein, B.E.; Klein, R.; Nondahl, D.M. Prevalence of olfactory impairment in older adults. JAMA 2002, 288, 2307–2312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Adams, D.R.; Wroblewski, K.E.; Kern, D.W.; Kozloski, M.J.; Dale, W.; McClintock, M.K.; Pinto, J.M. Factors Associated with Inaccurate Self-Reporting of Olfactory Dysfunction in Older US Adults. Chem. Senses 2016, 42, 223–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Mattes, R.D.; Cowart, B.J. Dietary assessment of patients with chemosensory disorders. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1994, 94, 50–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Santos, D.V.; Reiter, E.R.; DiNardo, L.J.; Costanzo, R.M. Hazardous events associated with impaired olfactory function. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2004, 130, 317–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Croy, I.; Nordin, S.; Hummel, T. Olfactory disorders and quality of life—An updated review. Chem. Senses 2014, 39, 185–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Fullard, M.E.; Morley, J.F.; Duda, J.E. Olfactory Dysfunction as an Early Biomarker in Parkinson’s Disease. Neurosci. Bull. 2017, 33, 515–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Murphy, C. Olfactory and other sensory impairments in Alzheimer disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2019, 15, 11–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Liu, B.; Luo, Z.; Pinto, J.M.; Shiroma, E.J.; Tranah, G.J.; Wirdefeldt, K.; Fang, F.; Harris, T.B.; Chen, H. Relationship between Poor Olfaction and Mortality among Community-Dwelling Older Adults: A Cohort Study. Ann. Intern. Med. 2019, 170, 673–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Yuan, Y.; Li, C.; Luo, Z.; Simonsick, E.M.; Shiroma, E.J.; Chen, H. Olfaction and Physical Functioning in Older Adults: A Longitudinal Study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2022, 77, 1612–1619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Doty, R.L. The olfactory system and its disorders. Semin. Neurol. 2009, 29, 74–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Schneider, A.L.C.; Gottesman, R.F.; Mosley, T.H.; Shrestha, S.; Rowan, N.R.; Sharrett, A.R.; Chen, H.; Kamath, V. Associations of Prior Head Injury with Olfaction in Older Adults: Results from the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study. JAMA Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2022, 148, 840–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Pinto, J.M.; Schumm, L.P.; Wroblewski, K.E.; Kern, D.W.; McClintock, M.K. Racial disparities in olfactory loss among older adults in the United States. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2014, 69, 323–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Pinto, J.M.; Wroblewski, K.E.; Kern, D.W.; Schumm, L.P.; McClintock, M.K. The Rate of Age-Related Olfactory Decline among the General Population of Older U.S. Adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2015, 70, 1435–1441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Palmquist, E.; Larsson, M.; Olofsson, J.K.; Seubert, J.; Backman, L.; Laukka, E.J. A Prospective Study on Risk Factors for Olfactory Dysfunction in Aging. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2020, 75, 603–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Schubert, C.R.; Cruickshanks, K.J.; Fischer, M.E.; Huang, G.H.; Klein, R.; Tsai, M.Y.; Pinto, A.A. Carotid Intima Media Thickness, Atherosclerosis, and 5-Year Decline in Odor Identification: The Beaver Dam Offspring Study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2015, 70, 879–884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Ekstrom, I.; Larsson, M.; Rizzuto, D.; Fastbom, J.; Backman, L.; Laukka, E.J. Predictors of Olfactory Decline in Aging: A Longitudinal Population-Based Study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2020, 75, 2441–2449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Schubert, C.R.; Cruickshanks, K.J.; Nondahl, D.M.; Klein, B.E.; Klein, R.; Fischer, M.E. Association of exercise with lower long-term risk of olfactory impairment in older adults. JAMA Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2013, 139, 1061–1066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Ekstrom, I.; Vetrano, D.L.; Papenberg, G.; Laukka, E.J. Serum C-Reactive Protein Is Negatively Associated with Olfactory Identification Ability in Older Adults. i-Perception 2021, 12, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Schubert, C.R.; Cruickshanks, K.J.; Fischer, M.E.; Klein, B.E.; Klein, R.; Pinto, A.A. Inflammatory and vascular markers and olfactory impairment in older adults. Age Ageing 2015, 44, 878–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Schubert, C.R.; Pinto, A.A.; Paulsen, A.J.; Cruickshanks, K.J. Exposure to Cadmium, Lead, and Tobacco Smoke and the 10-Year Cumulative Incidence of Olfactory Impairment: The Beaver Dam Offspring Study. JAMA Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2021, 147, 510–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Schubert, C.R.; Cruickshanks, K.J.; Klein, B.E.; Klein, R.; Nondahl, D.M. Olfactory impairment in older adults: Five-year incidence and risk factors. Laryngoscope 2011, 121, 873–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Dong, J.; Pinto, J.M.; Guo, X.; Alonso, A.; Tranah, G.; Cauley, J.A.; Garcia, M.; Satterfield, S.; Huang, X.; Harris, T.; et al. The prevalence of anosmia and associated factors among U.S. black and white older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2017, 72, 1080–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Bramerson, A.; Johansson, L.; Ek, L.; Nordin, S.; Bende, M. Prevalence of olfactory dysfunction: The skovde population-based study. Laryngoscope 2004, 114, 733–737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Rawal, S.; Hoffman, H.J.; Bainbridge, K.E.; Huedo-Medina, T.B.; Duffy, V.B. Prevalence and Risk Factors of Self-Reported Smell and Taste Alterations: Results from the 2011–2012 US National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES). Chem. Senses 2016, 41, 69–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Bigman, G. Age-related Smell and Taste Impairments and Vitamin D Associations in the U.S. Adults National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Nutrients 2020, 12, 984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Vauzour, D.; Camprubi-Robles, M.; Miquel-Kergoat, S.; Andres-Lacueva, C.; Banati, D.; Barberger-Gateau, P.; Bowman, G.L.; Caberlotto, L.; Clarke, R.; Hogervorst, E.; et al. Nutrition for the ageing brain: Towards evidence for an optimal diet. Ageing Res. Rev. 2017, 35, 222–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. McGrattan, A.M.; McGuinness, B.; McKinley, M.C.; Kee, F.; Passmore, P.; Woodside, J.V.; McEvoy, C.T. Diet and Inflammation in Cognitive Ageing and Alzheimer’s Disease. Curr. Nutr. Rep. 2019, 8, 53–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Derin, S.; Koseoglu, S.; Sahin, C.; Sahan, M. Effect of vitamin B12 deficiency on olfactory function. Int. Forum Allergy Rhinol. 2016, 6, 1051–1055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ang, W.W.; Goh, E.T.; Lai, K.; McKay-Davies, I. Vitamin D and Smell Impairment: A Systematic Literature Review. J. Laryngol. Otol. 2022, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Dinc, M.E.; Dalgic, A.; Ulusoy, S.; Dizdar, D.; Develioglu, O.; Topak, M. Does iron deficiency anemia affect olfactory function? Acta Otolaryngol. 2016, 136, 754–757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. ARIC Investigators. The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study: Design and objectives. The ARIC investigators. Am. J. Epidemiol. 1989, 129, 687–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Wright, J.D.; Folsom, A.R.; Coresh, J.; Sharrett, A.R.; Couper, D.; Wagenknecht, L.E.; Mosley, T.H., Jr.; Ballantyne, C.M.; Boerwinkle, E.A.; Rosamond, W.D.; et al. The ARIC (Atherosclerosis Risk In Communities) Study: JACC Focus Seminar 3/8. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 2021, 77, 2939–2959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Knopman, D.S.; Penman, A.D.; Catellier, D.J.; Coker, L.H.; Shibata, D.K.; Sharrett, A.R.; Mosley, T.H., Jr. Vascular risk factors and longitudinal changes on brain MRI: The ARIC study. Neurology 2011, 76, 1879–1885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hummel, T.; Konnerth, C.G.; Rosenheim, K.; Kobal, G. Screening of olfactory function with a four-minute odor identification test: Reliability, normative data, and investigations in patients with olfactory loss. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 2001, 110, 976–981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Palta, P.; Chen, H.; Deal, J.A.; Sharrett, A.R.; Gross, A.; Knopman, D.; Griswold, M.; Heiss, G.; Mosley, T.H. Olfactory function and neurocognitive outcomes in old age: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Neurocognitive Study. Alzheimers Dement. 2018, 14, 1015–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Palta, P.; Sharrett, A.R.; Gabriel, K.P.; Gottesman, R.F.; Folsom, A.R.; Power, M.C.; Evenson, K.R.; Jack, C.R., Jr.; Knopman, D.S.; Mosley, T.H.; et al. Prospective Analysis of Leisure-Time Physical Activity in Midlife and Beyond and Brain Damage on MRI in Older Adults. Neurology 2021, 96, e964–e974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. The ARIC Investigators. Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study Manual 8: Laboratory Methods Visit 5. Available online: https://sites.cscc.unc.edu/aric/sites/default/files/public/manuals/Manual%208%20Laboratory%20Determinations%20revised%2009242012.pdf (accessed on 20 February 2023).
  39. World Health Organization. Haemoglobin Concentrations for the Diagnosis of Anaemia and Assessment of Severity. Available online: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/85839/WHO_NMH_NHD_MNM_11.1_eng.pdf (accessed on 20 February 2023).
  40. Doty, R.L.; Kamath, V. The influences of age on olfaction: A review. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Papazian, E.J.; Pinto, J.M. Olfactory loss and aging: Connections with health and well-being. Chem. Senses 2021, 46, bjab045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Sorokowski, P.; Karwowski, M.; Misiak, M.; Marczak, M.K.; Dziekan, M.; Hummel, T.; Sorokowska, A. Sex Differences in Human Olfaction: A Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Liu, G.; Zong, G.; Doty, R.L.; Sun, Q. Prevalence and risk factors of taste and smell impairment in a nationwide representative sample of the US population: A cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e013246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Khan, S.; Gutierrez, J.A., 3rd; Chapurin, N.; Nguyen, S.A.; Soler, Z.M.; Schlosser, R.J. The impact of race on olfaction: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. Forum Allergy Rhinol. 2023, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Ash, M.; Boyce, J.K. Racial disparities in pollution exposure and employment at US industrial facilities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 10636–10641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Kodros, J.K.; Bell, M.L.; Dominici, F.; L’Orange, C.; Godri Pollitt, K.J.; Weichenthal, S.; Wu, X.; Volckens, J. Unequal airborne exposure to toxic metals associated with race, ethnicity, and segregation in the USA. Nat. Commun. 2022, 13, 6329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Doty, R.L. Neurotoxic exposure and impairment of the chemical senses of taste and smell. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2015, 131, 299–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Rajan, K.B.; Weuve, J.; Barnes, L.L.; Wilson, R.S.; Evans, D.A. Prevalence and incidence of clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease dementia from 1994 to 2012 in a population study. Alzheimers Dement. 2019, 15, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Donix, M.; Burggren, A.C.; Suthana, N.A.; Siddarth, P.; Ekstrom, A.D.; Krupa, A.K.; Jones, M.; Rao, A.; Martin-Harris, L.; Ercoli, L.M.; et al. Longitudinal changes in medial temporal cortical thickness in normal subjects with the APOE-4 polymorphism. Neuroimage 2010, 53, 37–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Therriault, J.; Benedet, A.L.; Pascoal, T.A.; Mathotaarachchi, S.; Chamoun, M.; Savard, M.; Thomas, E.; Kang, M.S.; Lussier, F.; Tissot, C.; et al. Association of Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 with Medial Temporal Tau Independent of Amyloid-beta. JAMA Neurol. 2020, 77, 470–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Murphy, C.; Bacon, A.W.; Bondi, M.W.; Salmon, D.P. Apolipoprotein E status is associated with odor identification deficits in nondemented older persons. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1998, 855, 744–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Calhoun-Haney, R.; Murphy, C. Apolipoprotein epsilon4 is associated with more rapid decline in odor identification than in odor threshold or Dementia Rating Scale scores. Brain Cogn. 2005, 58, 178–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Devanand, D.P.; Lee, S.; Manly, J.; Andrews, H.; Schupf, N.; Doty, R.L.; Stern, Y.; Zahodne, L.B.; Louis, E.D.; Mayeux, R. Olfactory deficits predict cognitive decline and Alzheimer dementia in an urban community. Neurology 2015, 84, 182–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Josefsson, M.; Larsson, M.; Nordin, S.; Adolfsson, R.; Olofsson, J. APOE-e4 effects on longitudinal decline in olfactory and non-olfactory cognitive abilities in middle-aged and old adults. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 1286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Ajmani, G.S.; Suh, H.H.; Wroblewski, K.E.; Pinto, J.M. Smoking and olfactory dysfunction: A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Laryngoscope 2017, 127, 1753–1761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Tanaka, H.; Gourley, D.D.; Dekhtyar, M.; Haley, A.P. Cognition, Brain Structure, and Brain Function in Individuals with Obesity and Related Disorders. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2020, 9, 544–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Biessels, G.J.; Reijmer, Y.D. Brain changes underlying cognitive dysfunction in diabetes: What can we learn from MRI? Diabetes 2014, 63, 2244–2252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Gottesman, R.F.; Seshadri, S. Risk Factors, Lifestyle Behaviors, and Vascular Brain Health. Stroke 2022, 53, 394–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Zaghloul, H.; Pallayova, M.; Al-Nuaimi, O.; Hovis, K.R.; Taheri, S. Association between diabetes mellitus and olfactory dysfunction: Current perspectives and future directions. Diabet. Med. 2018, 35, 41–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Seubert, J.; Laukka, E.J.; Rizzuto, D.; Hummel, T.; Fratiglioni, L.; Backman, L.; Larsson, M. Prevalence and Correlates of Olfactory Dysfunction in Old Age: A Population-Based Study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2017, 72, 1072–1079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Purdy, F.; Luo, Z.; Gardiner, J.C.; Pinto, J.M.; Shiroma, E.J.; Simonsick, E.M.; Harris, T.B.; Chen, H. Olfaction and Changes in Body Composition in a Large Cohort of Older U.S. Adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2020, 75, 2434–2440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Vogiatzoglou, A.; Refsum, H.; Johnston, C.; Smith, S.M.; Bradley, K.M.; de Jager, C.; Budge, M.M.; Smith, A.D. Vitamin B12 status and rate of brain volume loss in community-dwelling elderly. Neurology 2008, 71, 826–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Hooshmand, B.; Mangialasche, F.; Kalpouzos, G.; Solomon, A.; Kareholt, I.; Smith, A.D.; Refsum, H.; Wang, R.; Muhlmann, M.; Ertl-Wagner, B.; et al. Association of Vitamin B12, Folate, and Sulfur Amino Acids with Brain Magnetic Resonance Imaging Measures in Older Adults: A Longitudinal Population-Based Study. JAMA Psychiatry 2016, 73, 606–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Beydoun, M.A.; Hossain, S.; MacIver, P.H.; Srinivasan, D.; Beydoun, H.A.; Maldonado, A.I.; Katzel, L.I.; Davatzikos, C.; Gullapalli, R.P.; Seliger, S.L.; et al. Red Cell Distribution Width, Anemia, and Brain Volumetric Outcomes among Middle-Aged Adults. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2021, 81, 711–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Visit 5 characteristics of the participants relative to visit 5 anosmia status.
Table 1. Visit 5 characteristics of the participants relative to visit 5 anosmia status.
CharacteristicsOverall (n = 6053)No Anosmia (n = 5206)Anosmia (n = 847)
Olfaction (Total Number of Correct Scores, Mean, SD)9.19 (2.43)9.96 (1.50)4.47 (1.57)
Olfaction (Total Number of Errors, Mean, SD)2.81 (2.43)2.04 (1.50)7.53 (1.57)
Age (Years, Mean, SD)75.60 (5.18)75.28 (5.05)77.57 (5.55)
Male (n, %)2502 (41%)2061 (40%)441 (52%)
Black Race (n, %)1374 (23%)1028 (20%)346 (41%)
Education (n, %)
<High School878 (15%)669 (13%)209 (25%)
High School2516 (42%)2206 (42%)310 (37%)
>High School2648 (44%)2324 (45%)324 (38%)
Study Site (n, %)
Forsyth, North Carolina1360 (22%)1220 (23%)140 (17%)
Jackson, Mississippi1279 (21%)949 (18%)330 (39%)
Minneapolis, Minnesota1735 (29%)1568 (30%)167 (20%)
Washington, Maryland1679 (28%)1469 (28%)210 (25%)
Cigarette Smoking (n, %)
Never Smoker2261 (42%)1962 (42%)299 (42%)
Former Smoker2799 (52%)2419 (52%)380 (53%)
Current Smoker344 (6%)303 (6%)41 (6%)
Alcohol Consumption (n, %)
Never Drinker1236 (21%)1046 (21%)190 (24%)
Former Drinker1707 (30%)1414 (28%)293 (37%)
Current Drinker2828 (49%)2529 (51%)299 (38%)
APOE ε4 status (any ε4 allele, n, %)1642 (28%)1351 (27%)291 (36%)
BMI (kg/m2, Mean, SD)28.81 (5.74)28.85 (5.64)28.52 (6.37)
Physical Activity (Total MET-Min/week, Mean, SD)767.39 (837.95)791.31 (847.64)610.85 (753.52)
Serum Total Cholesterol (mg/dL, Mean, SD)181.48 (42.01)182.32 (41.78)176.21 (43.11)
Diabetes (n, %)1970 (34%)1650 (32%)320 (40%)
Hypertension (n, %)4464 (74%)3836 (74%)628 (76%)
Myocardial Infarction History (n, %)453 (8%)376 (8%)77 (10%)
CHD History (n, %)884 (15%)729 (14%)155 (19%)
Stroke History (n, %)229 (4%)177 (3%)52 (6%)
Serum C-Reactive Protein (mg/L, Mean, SD)4.22 (8.16)4.16 (8.19)4.56 (8.01)
Dementia Status (n, %)
Normal4529 (75%)4076 (78%)453 (54%)
Mild Cognitive Impairment1264 (21%)1028 (20%)236 (28%)
Dementia247 (4%)92 (2%)155 (18%)
Parkinson’s Disease (n, %)33 (1%)15 (1%)18 (4%)
Serum Vitamin B12 (pg/mL, Mean, SD)658.92 (532.09)657.15 (557.51)664.49 (442.82)
Blood Hemoglobin (g/dL, Mean, SD)13.30 (1.48)13.32 (1.47)13.15 (1.51)
Anemia (n, %)1255 (21%)1022 (20%)233 (29%)
Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; CHD, coronary heart disease; MET, metabolic equivalent of task; SD, standard deviation. Missing values: education, n = 11; smoking status, n = 649; alcohol consumption, n = 282; APOE ε4 status, n = 254; BMI, n = 179; physical activity, n = 311; total cholesterol, n = 74; diabetes, n = 174; hypertension, n = 54; myocardial infarction, n = 412; coronary heart disease, n = 104; stroke, n = 10; serum C-reactive protein, n = 76; dementia, n = 13.
Table 2. Associations of visit 5 characteristics with visit 5 olfaction status and change in olfaction from visit 5 to visit 6.
Table 2. Associations of visit 5 characteristics with visit 5 olfaction status and change in olfaction from visit 5 to visit 6.
Visit 5 OlfactionVisit 5 to Visit 6 Olfactory Change
CharacteristicsRR (95% CI) aRRR (95% CI) b
Age (per 10-year higher) c1.386 (1.343, 1.429)1.010 (1.000, 1.020)
Male c1.322 (1.264, 1.383)0.989 (0.980, 0.999)
Education c
>High SchoolReferentReferent
High School1.069 (1.017, 1.124)1.006 (0.996, 1.017)
<High School1.168 (1.097, 1.245)0.997 (0.983, 1.011)
Study Site–Race c
Washington–WhiteReferentReferent
Forsyth–Black1.308 (1.075, 1.592)1.006 (0.966, 1.047)
Forsyth–White1.016 (0.946, 1.092)1.009 (0.994, 1.023)
Jackson–Black1.637 (1.541, 1.739)0.985 (0.973, 0.998)
Minneapolis–White0.978 (0.915, 1.045)0.992 (0.979, 1.006)
Cigarette Smoking c
Never SmokerReferentReferent
Former Smoker1.051 (1.000, 1.103)0.999 (0.989, 1.009)
Current Smoker1.127 (1.035, 1.227)1.006 (0.988, 1.025)
Alcohol Consumption c
Never DrinkerReferentReferent
Former Drinker0.980 (0.922, 1.042)1.000 (0.987, 1.013)
Current Drinker0.920 (0.862, 0.981)0.996 (0.983, 1.008)
APOE ε4 (any ε4 allele presence) d1.123 (1.071, 1.178)0.997 (0.986, 1.008)
BMI (kg/m2, per 1SD) e0.981 (0.958, 1.005)1.003 (0.998, 1.008)
Obesity Status (BMI (kg/m2)) e
<25ReferentReferent
25–<300.941 (0.881, 1.005)1.012 (0.999, 1.025)
≥300.920 (0.831, 1.020)1.012 (0.998, 1.025)
Physical Activity (Total MET-Min/week, per 1SD) e0.956 (0.933, 0.981)1.000 (0.996, 1.005)
Serum Total Cholesterol (mg/dL, per 1SD) f0.977 (0.952, 1.002)0.999 (0.994, 1.004)
Diabetes f1.075 (1.023, 1.129)1.004 (0.994, 1.015)
Hypertension f0.931 (0.881, 0.983)1.005 (0.994, 1.015)
Myocardial Infarction History g0.982 (0.895, 1.077)1.003 (0.984, 1.023)
CHD History g1.046 (0.970, 1.129)0.996 (0.981, 1.011)
Stroke History g1.037 (0.928, 1.160)0.986 (0.957, 1.016)
C-Reactive Protein (mg/L, per 1SD) h1.008 (0.986, 1.030)0.997 (0.989, 1.005)
Serum Vitamin B12 (pg/mL, per 1SD) h1.003 (0.965, 1.042)0.995 (0.984, 1.005)
Serum Vitamin B12 Categories h
1st QuartileReferentReferent
2nd Quartile0.991 (0.898, 1.093)1.001 (0.975, 1.028)
3rd Quartile1.015 (0.917, 1.124)0.996 (0.971, 1.023)
4th Quartile1.037 (0.934, 1.153)0.989 (0.962, 1.016)
Blood Hemoglobin (g/dL, per 1SD) h0.974 (0.951, 0.996)0.999 (0.995, 1.003)
Anemia h1.074 (1.016, 1.135)0.994 (0.982, 1.007)
Abbreviations: CHD, coronary heart disease; BMI, body mass index; RR, rate ratio; RRR, ratio of (error) rate ratio; SD, standard deviation. One SD unit: BMI: 5.7 kg/m2; physical activity: 838 total MET-Min/week; serum total cholesterol: 42 mg/dL; C-reactive protein: 8.2 mg/L; serum vitamin B12: 532 pg/mL; blood hemoglobin: 1.5 g/dL. a: Obtained from Poisson regression models examining cross-sectional associations between visit 5 factors and the number of total errors in the olfactory test; the error rate ratio (RR) is an exponentiated beta estimate (regression parameter) associated with the exposure of interest. b: Obtained from mixed-effect Poisson regression models with the number of total errors in the olfactory test as the repeated measures; the ratio of error rate ratio (RRR) is an exponentiated beta estimate (regression parameter) associated with the interaction term of time with the exposure of interest. c: Obtained from Model 1: age, sex, site–race, education, cigarette smoking, and alcohol consumption. d: Model 2: adjusted for covariates in Model 1. e: Obtained from Model 3: Model 1 covariates, BMI, and physical activity. f: Obtained from Model 4: Model 3 covariates, total cholesterol, diabetes, and hypertension. g: Obtained from Model 5: Model 4 covariates, myocardial infarction history, CHD, and stroke. h: Model 6: adjusted for covariates in Model 4.
Table 3. Associations of visit 5 characteristics with visit 5 anosmia prevalence and visit 6 anosmia risk.
Table 3. Associations of visit 5 characteristics with visit 5 anosmia prevalence and visit 6 anosmia risk.
Visit 5 AnosmiaVisit 6 Anosmia
CharacteristicsPrevalence
Ratio (95% CI) a
Incidence
Risk Ratio (95% CI) b
Age (per 10-year higher) c1.965 (1.740, 2.219)2.124 (1.701, 2.651)
Male c1.689 (1.470, 1.941)1.405 (1.119, 1.764)
Education c
>High SchoolReferentReferent
High School1.014 (0.865, 1.189)1.479 (1.159, 1.888)
<High School1.302 (1.090, 1.555)1.531 (1.099, 2.134)
Study Site–Race c
Washington–WhiteReferentReferent
Forsyth–Black1.719 (1.007, 2.933)2.296 (1.117, 4.719)
Forsyth–White0.893 (0.706, 1.129)1.458 (1.050, 2.024)
Jackson–Black2.354 (1.978, 2.802)2.506 (1.824, 3.445)
Minneapolis–White0.903 (0.730, 1.117)1.023 (0.732, 1.430)
Cigarette Smoking c
Never SmokerReferentReferent
Former Smoker0.979 (0.844, 1.135)1.172 (0.919, 1.494)
Current Smoker0.895 (0.654, 1.225)1.448 (0.922, 2.275)
Alcohol Consumption c
Never DrinkerReferentReferent
Former Drinker1.013 (0.844, 1.216)1.167 (0.859, 1.585)
Current Drinker0.940 (0.769, 1.150)0.881 (0.646, 1.201)
APOE ε4 (any ε4 allele presence) d1.254 (1.088, 1.446)1.330 (1.053, 1.680)
BMI (kg/m2, per 1SD) e0.922 (0.848, 1.002)1.021 (0.908, 1.147)
Obesity Status (BMI (kg/m2)) c
<25ReferentReferent
25–<300.865 (0.733, 1.019)1.131 (0.840, 1.523)
≥300.752 (0.624, 0.905)1.215 (0.888, 1.663)
Physical Activity (Total MET-Min/week, per 1SD) e0.893 (0.821, 0.973)0.941 (0.838, 1.056)
Serum Total Cholesterol (mg/dL, per 1SD) f0.929 (0.855, 1.009)0.939 (0.831, 1.061)
Diabetes f1.163 (0.999, 1.355)1.037 (0.811, 1.327)
Hypertension f0.830 (0.701, 0.983)1.050 (0.800, 1.378)
Myocardial Infarction History g0.959 (0.739, 1.245)1.550 (1.000, 2.403)
CHD History g1.177 (0.956, 1.451)0.785 (0.531, 1.160)
Stroke History g1.019 (0.755, 1.375)0.477 (0.194, 1.174)
C-Reactive Protein (mg/L, per 1SD) h0.990 (0.915, 1.070)1.036 (0.878, 1.222)
Serum Vitamin B12 (pg/mL, per 1SD) h0.976 (0.886, 1.074)0.706 (0.510, 0.977)
Serum Vitamin B12 Categories h
1st QuartileReferentReferent
2nd Quartile0.845 (0.647, 1.104)1.054 (0.646, 1.721)
3rd Quartile1.027 (0.794, 1.328)1.049 (0.618, 1.781)
4th Quartile1.014 (0.779, 1.319)0.650 (0.326, 1.297)
Blood Hemoglobin (g/dL, per 1SD) h0.962 (0.896, 1.032)0.961 (0.873, 1.058)
Anemia h1.109 (0.940, 1.308)0.939 (0.704, 1.252)
Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; CHD, coronary heart disease; SD, standard deviation. One SD unit: BMI: 5.7 kg/m2; physical activity: 838 total MET-Min/week; serum total cholesterol: 42 mg/dL; C-reactive protein: 8.2 mg/L; serum vitamin B12: 532 pg/mL; blood hemoglobin: 1.5 g/dL. a: Obtained from regression models examining cross-sectional associations between visit 5 factors and anosmia prevalence among 6053 visit 5 participants. b: Obtained from regression models examining the associations between visit 5 factors and risk of anosmia among participants who attended both visits 5 and 6. c: Obtained from Model 1: age, sex, site–race, education, cigarette smoking, and alcohol consumption. d: Model 2: adjusted for covariates in Model 1. e: Obtained from Model 3: Model 1 covariates, BMI, and physical activity. f: Obtained from Model 4: Model 3 covariates, total cholesterol, diabetes, and hypertension. g: Obtained from Model 5: Model 4 covariates, myocardial infarction history, CHD, and stroke. h: Model 6: adjusted for covariates in Model 4. Note: we repeated Model 1 for incident anosmia analysis by incorporating the inverse probability of attrition weights to correct for potential bias due to selective attrition from visit 5 to visit 6. The results were similar to those without incorporating the inverse probability weights.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Shrestha, S.; Zhu, X.; Kamath, V.; Sullivan, K.J.; Deal, J.A.; Sharrett, A.R.; Schneider, A.L.C.; Palta, P.; Gottesman, R.F.; Windham, B.G.; et al. Factors Associated with Poor Olfaction and Olfactory Decline in Older Adults in the ARIC Neurocognitive Study. Nutrients 2023, 15, 3641. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15163641

AMA Style

Shrestha S, Zhu X, Kamath V, Sullivan KJ, Deal JA, Sharrett AR, Schneider ALC, Palta P, Gottesman RF, Windham BG, et al. Factors Associated with Poor Olfaction and Olfactory Decline in Older Adults in the ARIC Neurocognitive Study. Nutrients. 2023; 15(16):3641. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15163641

Chicago/Turabian Style

Shrestha, Srishti, Xiaoqian Zhu, Vidyulata Kamath, Kevin J. Sullivan, Jennifer A. Deal, A. Richey Sharrett, Andrea L. C. Schneider, Priya Palta, Rebecca F. Gottesman, B. Gwen Windham, and et al. 2023. "Factors Associated with Poor Olfaction and Olfactory Decline in Older Adults in the ARIC Neurocognitive Study" Nutrients 15, no. 16: 3641. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15163641

APA Style

Shrestha, S., Zhu, X., Kamath, V., Sullivan, K. J., Deal, J. A., Sharrett, A. R., Schneider, A. L. C., Palta, P., Gottesman, R. F., Windham, B. G., Mosley, T. H., Griswold, M. E., & Chen, H. (2023). Factors Associated with Poor Olfaction and Olfactory Decline in Older Adults in the ARIC Neurocognitive Study. Nutrients, 15(16), 3641. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15163641

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop