Food-Specific Inhibition Training for Food Devaluation: A Meta-Analysis
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Food-Specific Inhibition Training
1.2. Food-Specific Inhibition Training and Food Devaluation
1.3. Potential Moderators of Training Effects
1.4. The Meta-Analysis
2. Method
2.1. Study Selection and Inclusion Criteria
2.2. Coding of Variables
2.3. Statistical Analysis
2.4. Quality Assessment
3. Results
3.1. Preliminary Analysis
3.1.1. Study Characteristics
3.1.2. Assessment of Publication Bias
3.1.3. Power Analysis
3.2. Overall Training Effect and Moderator Analyses
4. Discussion
4.1. Discussion of Moderators
4.2. Limitations and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- De Graaf, C.; Blom, W.A.; Smeets, P.A.; Stafleu, A.; Hendriks, H.F. Biomarkers of satiation and satiety. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2004, 79, 946–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Yang, Y.; Wu, Q.; Morys, F. Brain Responses to High-Calorie Visual Food Cues in Individuals with Normal-Weight or Obesity: An Activation Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oliveira, J.; Cordas, T.A. The body asks and the mind judges: Food cravings in eating disorders. Encephale 2020, 46, 269–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Berridge, K.C.; Ho, C.Y.; Richard, J.M.; DiFeliceantonio, A.G. The tempted brain eats: Pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating disorders. Brain Res. 2010, 1350, 43–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stice, E.; Burger, K. Neural vulnerability factors for obesity. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 68, 38–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stice, E.; Yokum, S. Neural vulnerability factors that increase risk for future weight gain. Psychol. Bull. 2016, 142, 447–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stice, E.; Lawrence, N.S.; Kemps, E.; Veling, H. Training motor responses to food: A novel treatment for obesity targeting implicit processes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016, 49, 16–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Veling, H.; Lawrence, N.S.; Chen, Z.; van Koningsbruggen, G.M.; Holland, R.W. What Is Trained During Food Go/No-Go Training? A Review Focusing on Mechanisms and a Research Agenda. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2017, 4, 35–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Logan, G.D.; Cowan, W.B.; Davis, K.A. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: A model and a method. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1984, 10, 276–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Shields, G.S.; Moons, W.G.; Slavich, G.M. Inflammation, Self-Regulation, and Health: An Immunologic Model of Self-Regulatory Failure. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 12, 588–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, Y.; Shields, G.S.; Wu, Q.; Liu, Y.; Chen, H.; Guo, C. Cognitive training on eating behaviour and weight loss: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Obes. Rev. 2019, 20, 1628–1641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Forcano, L.; Mata, F.; de la Torre, R.; Verdejo-Garcia, A. Cognitive and neuromodulation strategies for unhealthy eating and obesity: Systematic review and discussion of neurocognitive mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 87, 161–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yang, Y.; Morys, F.; Wu, Q.; Li, J.; Chen, H. Pilot Study of Food-Specific Go/No-Go Training for Overweight Individuals: Brain Imaging Data Suggest Inhibition Shapes Food Evaluation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2021, nsab137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lawrence, N.S.; Verbruggen, F.; Morrison, S.; Adams, R.C.; Chambers, C.D. Stopping to food can reduce intake. Effects of stimulus-specificity and individual differences in dietary restraint. Appetite 2015, 85, 91–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Guerrieri, R.; Nederkoorn, C.; Jansen, A. Disinhibition is easier learned than inhibition. The effects of (dis)inhibition training on food intake. Appetite 2012, 59, 96–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Houben, K.; Jansen, A. Chocolate equals stop. Chocolate-specific inhibition training reduces chocolate intake and go associations with chocolate. Appetite 2015, 87, 318–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Z.; Holland, R.W.; Quandt, J.; Dijksterhuis, A.; Veling, H. When mere action versus inaction leads to robust preference change. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2019, 117, 721–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Porter, L.; Bailey-Jones, C.; Priudokaite, G.; Allen, S.; Wood, K.; Stiles, K.; Parvin, O.; Javaid, M.; Verbruggen, F.; Lawrence, N.S. From cookies to carrots; the effect of inhibitory control training on children’s snack selections. Appetite 2018, 124, 111–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veling, H.; Verpaalen, I.A.M.; Liu, H.; Mosannenzadeh, F.; Becker, D.; Holland, R.W. How can food choice best be trained? Approach-avoidance versus go/no-go training. Appetite 2021, 163, 105226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolz, I.; Nannt, J.; Svaldi, J. Laboratory-based interventions targeting food craving: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes. Rev. 2020, 21, e12996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Allom, V.; Mullan, B.; Hagger, M. Does inhibitory control training improve health behaviour? A meta-analysis. Health Psychol. Rev. 2016, 10, 168–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Veling, H.; Chen, Z.; Liu, H.; Quandt, J.; Holland, R.W. Updating the p-curve analysis of Carbine and Larson with results from preregistered experiments. Health Psychol. Rev. 2020, 14, 215–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carbine, K.A.; Larson, M.J. Quantifying the presence of evidential value and selective reporting in food-related inhibitory control training: A p-curve analysis. Health Psychol. Rev. 2019, 13, 318–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Navas, J.F.; Verdejo-Garcia, A.; Vadillo, M.A. The evidential value of research on cognitive training to change food-related biases and unhealthy eating behavior: A systematic review and p-curve analysis. Obes. Rev. 2021, 22, e13338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chen, Z.; Veling, H.; Dijksterhuis, A.; Holland, R.W. How does not responding to appetitive stimuli cause devaluation: Evaluative conditioning or response inhibition? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2016, 145, 1687–1701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Johannes, N.; Buijzen, M.; Veling, H. Beyond inhibitory control training: Inactions and actions influence smartphone app use through changes in explicit liking. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2021, 150, 431–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veling, H.; Holland, R.W.; van Knippenberg, A. When approach motivation and behavioral inhibition collide: Behavior regulation through stimulus devaluation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 1013–1019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dreisbach, G.; Fischer, R. Conflicts as Aversive Signals for Control Adaptation. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 24, 255–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Quandt, J.; Holland, R.W.; Chen, Z.; Veling, H. The role of attention in explaining the no-go devaluation effect: Effects on appetitive food items. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2019, 45, 1119–1133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guitart-Masip, M.; Duzel, E.; Dolan, R.; Dayan, P. Action versus valence in decision making. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 194–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Guitart-Masip, M.; Huys, Q.J.; Fuentemilla, L.; Dayan, P.; Duzel, E.; Dolan, R.J. Go and no-go learning in reward and punishment: Interactions between affect and effect. Neuroimage 2012, 62, 154–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Verbruggen, F.; Best, M.; Bowditch, W.A.; Stevens, T.; McLaren, I.P. The inhibitory control reflex. Neuropsychologia 2014, 65, 263–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Adams, R.C.; Button, K.S.; Hickey, L.; Morrison, S.; Smith, A.; Bolus, W.; Coombs, E.; Randolph, S.; Hunt, R.; Kim, D.; et al. Food-related inhibitory control training reduces food liking but not snacking frequency or weight in a large healthy adult sample. Appetite 2021, 167, 105601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Z.; Veling, H.; de Vries, S.P.; Bijvank, B.O.; Janssen, I.M.C.; Dijksterhuis, A.; Holland, R.W. Go/no-go training changes food evaluation in both morbidly obese and normal-weight individuals. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2018, 86, 980–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wessel, J.R.; Aron, A.R. On the Globality of Motor Suppression: Unexpected Events and Their Influence on Behavior and Cognition. Neuron 2017, 93, 259–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wessel, J.R.; O’Doherty, J.P.; Berkebile, M.M.; Linderman, D.; Aron, A.R. Stimulus devaluation induced by stopping action. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2014, 143, 2316–2329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chiu, Y.C.; Egner, T. Inhibition-Induced Forgetting Results from Resource Competition between Response Inhibition and Memory Encoding Processes. J. Neurosci. 2015, 35, 11936–11945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chiu, Y.C.; Egner, T. Inhibition-induced forgetting: When more control leads to less memory. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 26, 27–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chen, Z.; Veling, H.; Dijksterhuis, A.; Holland, R.W. Do impulsive individuals benefit more from food go/no-go training? Testing the role of inhibition capacity in the no-go devaluation effect. Appetite 2018, 124, 99–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kakoschke, N.; Kemps, E.; Tiggemann, M. The effect of combined avoidance and control training on implicit food evaluation and choice. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2017, 55, 99–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Adams, R. Training Response Inhibition to Reduce Food Consumption. Doctoral Thesis, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Yang, Y.; Shields, G.S.; Guo, C.; Liu, Y. Executive function performance in obesity and overweight individuals: A meta-analysis and review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 84, 225–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stoeckel, L.E.; Weller, R.E.; Cook, E.W., 3rd; Twieg, D.B.; Knowlton, R.C.; Cox, J.E. Widespread reward-system activation in obese women in response to pictures of high-calorie foods. Neuroimage 2008, 41, 636–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schonberg, T.; Bakkour, A.; Hover, A.M.; Mumford, J.A.; Nagar, L.; Perez, J.; Poldrack, R.A. Changing value through cued approach: An automatic mechanism of behavior change. Nat. Neurosci. 2014, 17, 625–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.; Rothstein, H.R. Introduction to Meta-Analysis; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Morris, S.B. Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organ. Res. Methods 2008, 11, 364–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Camp, B.; Lawrence, N.S. Giving pork the chop: Response inhibition training to reduce meat intake. Appetite 2019, 141, 104315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chami, R.; Cardi, V.; Lawrence, N.; MacDonald, P.; Rowlands, K.; Hodsoll, J.; Treasure, J. Targeting binge eating in bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder using inhibitory control training and implementation intentions: A feasibility trial. Psychol. Med. 2020, 150, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keeler, J.L.; Chami, R.; Cardi, V.; Hodsoll, J.; Bonin, E.; MacDonald, P.; Treasure, J.; Lawrence, N. App-based food-specific inhibitory control training as an adjunct to treatment as usual in binge-type eating disorders: A feasibility trial. Appetite 2022, 168, 105788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lawrence, N.S.; O’Sullivan, J.; Parslow, D.; Javaid, M.; Adams, R.C.; Chambers, C.D.; Kos, K.; Verbruggen, F. Training response inhibition to food is associated with weight loss and reduced energy intake. Appetite 2015, 95, 17–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masterton, S.; Hardman, C.A.; Halford, J.C.G.; Jones, A. Examining cognitive bias modification interventions for reducing food value and choice: Two pre-registered, online studies. Appetite 2021, 159, 105063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Najberg, H.; Rigamonti, M.; Mouthon, M.; Spierer, L. Modifying food items valuation and weight with gamified executive control training. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2021, 8, 191288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tzavella, L.; Lawrence, N.S.; Button, K.S.; Hart, E.A.; Holmes, N.M.; Houghton, K.; Badkar, N.; Macey, E.; Braggins, A.J.; Murray, F.C.; et al. Effects of go/no-go training on food-related action tendencies, liking and choice. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2021, 8, 210666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morris, S.B.; DeShon, R.P. Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychol. Methods 2002, 7, 105–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scammacca, N.; Roberts, G.; Stuebing, K.K. Meta-Analysis With Complex Research Designs: Dealing With Dependence From Multiple Measures and Multiple Group Comparisons. Rev. Educ. Res. 2014, 84, 328–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hedges, L.V.; Tipton, E.; Johnson, M.C. Robust variance estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Res. Synth. Methods 2010, 1, 39–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tanner-Smith, E.E.; Tipton, E. Robust variance estimation with dependent effect sizes: Practical considerations including a software tutorial in Stata and spss. Res. Synth. Methods 2014, 5, 13–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tipton, E. Small sample adjustments for robust variance estimation with meta-regression. Psychol. Methods 2015, 20, 375–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Borenstein, M.; Higgins, J.P.; Hedges, L.V.; Rothstein, H.R. Basics of meta-analysis: I2 is not an absolute measure of heterogeneity. Res. Synth. Methods 2017, 8, 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Viechtbauer, W.; Cheung, M.W. Outlier and influence diagnostics for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 2010, 1, 112–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, T.; Oram, C.; Correll, C.U.; Tsermentseli, S.; Stubbs, B. Analgesic Effects of Alcohol: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Controlled Experimental Studies in Healthy Participants. J. Pain 2017, 18, 499–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jansen, E.T. The Effect of Go/No-Go Training Dosage on Weight Loss, Food Evaluation, and Disinhibition in Primarily Overweight and Obese Individuals: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Master’s Thesis, University of Minnesota Duluth, Duluth, MN, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, Y.; Chen, H.; Li, S.; Luo, N. Reducing unsuccessful restrained eaters’ unhealthy food choice: An internet-based inhibition control training. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2017, 49, 219–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Porter, L.; Gillison, F.B.; Wright, K.A.; Verbruggen, F.; Lawrence, N.S. Exploring Strategies to Optimise the Impact of Food-Specific Inhibition Training on Children’s Food Choices. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 653610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Serfas, B.G.; Florack, A.; Büttner, O.B.; Voegeding, T. What does it take for sour grapes to remain sour? Persistent effects of behavioral inhibition in go/no-go tasks on the evaluation of appetitive stimuli. Motiv. Sci. 2017, 3, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stice, E.; Yokum, S.; Veling, H.; Kemps, E.; Lawrence, N.S. Pilot test of a novel food response and attention training treatment for obesity: Brain imaging data suggest actions shape valuation. Behav. Res. Ther. 2017, 94, 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stice, E.; Rohde, P.; Gau, J.M.; Butryn, M.L.; Shaw, H.; Cloud, K.; D’Adamo, L. Enhancing efficacy of a dissonance-based obesity and eating disorder prevention program: Experimental therapeutics. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2021, 89, 793–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tzavella, L. Behavioural Measures and Training Interventions for Food-Related Cognition, Motivation and Affect. Ph.D. Thesis, Cardiff University (United Kingdom), Ann Arbor, MI, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Veling, H.; Aarts, H.; Stroebe, W. Using stop signals to reduce impulsive choices for palatable unhealthy foods. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2013, 18, 354–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shields, G.S.; Bonner, J.C.; Moons, W.G. Does cortisol influence core executive functions? A meta-analysis of acute cortisol administration effects on working memory, inhibition, and set-shifting. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2015, 58, 91–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Egger, M.; Davey Smith, G.; Schneider, M.; Minder, C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997, 315, 629–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Swinburn, B.A.; Sacks, G.; Hall, K.D.; McPherson, K.; Finegood, D.T.; Moodie, M.L.; Gortmaker, S.L. The global obesity pandemic: Shaped by global drivers and local environments. Lancet 2011, 378, 804–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veling, H.; Aarts, H.; Stroebe, W. Stop signals decrease choices for palatable foods through decreased food evaluation. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jones, A.; Di Lemma, L.C.; Robinson, E.; Christiansen, P.; Nolan, S.; Tudur-Smith, C.; Field, M. Inhibitory control training for appetitive behaviour change: A meta-analytic investigation of mechanisms of action and moderators of effectiveness. Appetite 2016, 97, 16–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jones, A.; Hardman, C.A.; Lawrence, N.; Field, M. Cognitive training as a potential treatment for overweight and obesity: A critical review of the evidence. Appetite 2018, 124, 50–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Study | Participants | Training Condition | Control Condition | Session(s); Critical Trials | Study Design | Unhealthy Stimulus | Evaluation Type |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Adams et al., 2021 [34] | N: 166/167 in training group; 146/141 in control group; Mean age: 23.69; Percent female: 77%; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: aged below 18 years or body mass index (BMI) < 18.5. | Inhibit 100 % of energy-dense food images | Filler images: 50% inhibit, 50% go | 4/7; 216/378 | Pre-test–post-test–control | Energy-dense food | Explicit: Liking taste |
Adams, 2014 Study 2 [42] | N: 67 in training group; 65 in control group; Mean age: 23.12; Percent female: 93%; Inclusion criteria: chocolate cravers or restrained eaters; Exclusion criteria: currently dieting or any history of eating disorders. | Inhibit 87.5% of chocolate images | Filler images: 87.5% go, 12.5% inhibit | 1; 70 | Post-test only with control | Chocolate | Implicit: Implicit association test |
Adams, 2014 study 4 sample 1 [42] | N: 13/38 in training group; 41/31 in control group; Mean age: 20.77/21.16; Percent female: 69%/95%; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 100% of unhealthy snack foods | Filler images: 50% inhibit, 50% go | 1; 36 | Post-test only with control | Unhealthy snack foods | Implicit: Implicit association test |
Adams, 2014 study 4 sample 2 [42] | N: 30/39 in training group; 28/31 in control group; Mean age: 24.47/21.41; Percent female: 67%/90%; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 100% of unhealthy snack foods | Filler images: 50% inhibit, 50% go | 1; 36 | Post-test only with control | Unhealthy snack foods | Explicit: Attractiveness; Tastiness; Desire to Eat |
Camp and Lawrence, 2019 [48] | N: 37 in training group; 30 in control group; Mean age: 24.1; Percent female: 85%; Inclusion criteria: 18–65 years, ate meat, and had some desire to reduce meat intake; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 100 % of meat | Filler images: 50% inhibit, 50% go | 4; 192 | Pre-test–post-test–control | Meat | Explicit: Liking |
Chami et al., 2020 [49] | N: 28 in training group; 26 in control group; Mean age: 33.38; Percent female: 90%; Inclusion criteria: bulimia nervosa or binge-eating disorder, BMI > 18.5; Exclusion criteria: currently pregnant, had a visual impairment, a neurological impairment, alcohol or drug dependence, or psychosis. | Inhibit 100% of high-energy dense foods food | Filler images: 50% inhibit, 50% go | 13.81; 756 | Pre-test–post-test–control | High-energy dense foods food | Explicit: Liking |
Chen et al., 2016 [26] | N: 41/38/43/27 in training group; Percent female: 83%/79%/89%; Mean age: 21.7/22.6/23.8/23.3; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: participants whose accuracy on go or no-go trials was 3 SD below sample mean and below 90%. | Inhibit 100% of palatable foods | Untrained | 1; 50;100;60 | Single-group Pre-test–post-test | Palatable foods | Explicit: Attractiveness |
Chen et al., 2018a [35] | N: 59/58 in training group; Percent female: 76%/74%; Mean age: 46.1/23.2; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 100% of palatable foods | Untrained | 1; 108 | Single-Group pre-test–post-test | Appetitive food | Explicit: Attractiveness |
Chen et al., 2018b [40] | N: 71/106 in training group; Percent female: 89%/72%; Mean age: 20.7/23.2; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 100% of palatable foods | Untrained | 1; 30 | Single-Group pre-test–post-test | Palatable foods | Explicit: Attractiveness |
Houben and Jansen, 2015 [17] | N: 21 in training group; 20 in control group; Mean age: 20.1; Percent female: 100%; Inclusion criteria: liked to eat chocolate on a regular basis; Exclusion criteria: had severe to moderate underweight (BMI < 18.5), disliked the chocolate that was presented during the taste test (mean rating < 5), or were outliers. | Inhibit 100 % of chocolate snacks | Inhibit 0% chocolate snacks | 1; 80 | Post-test only with control | Chocolate snacks | Explicit: Craving |
Jansen, 2022 [63] | N: 19/22 in training group; 23 in control group; Mean age: 44.8; Percent female: 83%; Inclusion criteria: aged 18 or older, a BMI ≥ 25, having a desire to lose weight, and consuming at least one of the no-go training foods used in the training at least two times per week; Exclusion criteria: medical condition limiting dietary intake or affecting weight, use of weight loss medication, history of bariatric surgery, current smoker, having quit smoking within the past year, or enrollment in a formal weight loss program in the past 6 months. | Inhibit 100 % of unhealthy foods | Inhibit 0% unhealthy foods | 16/4; 864/216 | Pre-test–post-test–control | Unhealthy foods | Explicit: Tastiness |
Kakoschke et al., 2017 [41] | N: 60 in training group; 60 in control group; Mean age: 20.6; Percent female: 100%; Inclusion criteria: liked most foods, and did not have any food allergies, intolerances, or special dietary requirements; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 90% of unhealthy food | Inhibit 0% unhealthy food | 1; 144 | Post-test only with control | Unhealthy food | Implicit: Implicit association test |
Keeler et al., 2022 [50] | N: 40 in training group; 40 in control group; Mean age: 30; Percent female: 98%; Inclusion criteria: bulimia nervosa or binge-eating disorder, receiving a form of treatment for their eating disorder (one or more of: psychotherapies, nutritional support, and/or psychiatric medications such as anti-depressants), had a BMI of at least 18.5 kg/m2, were between the ages of 18 and 60; Exclusion criteria: currently pregnant, had a visual impairment that could not be repaired with eyewear, a neurological impairment, alcohol or drug dependence, or psychosis. | Inhibit 100% of high energy-dense food and treatment-as-usual | Treatment-as-usual | 21; 168 | Pre-test–post-test–control | High energy-dense food | Explicit: Attractiveness |
Lawrence et al., 2015a [51] | N: 42 in training group; 42 in control group; Mean age: 50; Percent female: 76%; Inclusion criteria: aged 18–65, had a BMI based on self-reported height and weight of at least 18.5, consumed some of the “no-go” snack foods (see below) at least three times per week, and reported some disinhibition over eating; Exclusion criteria: allergies to the foods given during the taste test, smoking/recent smoking cessation, enrolment in a formal weight loss program, use of weight loss medication, metabolic disorders, or other health conditions affecting weight. | Inhibit 100% of energy-dense food | Filter images: 50% inhibit, 50% go | 4; 216 | Pre-test–post-test–control | Energy-dense food | Explicit: Attractiveness; Liking |
Liu et al., 2017 [64] | N: 33 in training group; 33 in control group; Mean age: 50; Percent female: 76%; Inclusion criteria: BMI between 18.5–23.9, restrained eater; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 87.5% of high-energy density foods | Filter images: 87.5% inhibit, 12.5% go | 7; 588 | Pre-test–post-test–control | High-energy density foods | Explicit: Attractiveness; Liking; Implicit: Implicit association test |
Masterton et al., 2021 [52] | N: 47/44 in training group; 35/44 in control group; Mean age: 28.5/28.0; Percent female: 57%/50%; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 100%/75% of unhealthy food | Inhibit 25%/50% of unhealthy food images | 1; 100/75 | Pre-test–post-test–control | Unhealthy food | Explicit: Appealing |
Najberg et al., 2021 [53] | N: 46 in training group; 44 in control group; Mean age: 25.2; Percent female: 59%; Inclusion criteria: healthy individuals, BMI > 20, liking of unhealthy food; Exclusion criteria: consumption of any prescribed medication, diagnosis of eating disorders, restrictive diet, history of weight gain/loss of more than 10% body weight in the last six months, no plan of actively losing weight with a restrictive diet in the next four months. | Inhibit 100% unhealthy food | Inhibit 50% of unhealthy food images | 20; n.a | Pre-test–post-test–control | Unhealthy food | Explicit: Palatability |
Porter et al., 2021 [65] | N: 67/69 in training group; 64 in control group; Mean age: 7/6.6; Percent female: 53%/44%; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 100% of energy-dense food | Filter images: 50% inhibit, 50% go | 1; 96/80 | Pre-test–post-test–control | Energy-dense food | Explicit: Yummy |
Quandt et al., 2019 [30] | N: 41/79 in training group; Mean age: 22.6/22.4; Percent female: 78%; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: correct at least 90% of the time during training. | Inhibit 100% of palatable food | Untrained | 1; 100 | Single-group pre-test–post-test | Palatable food | Explicit: Appealing |
Serfas et al., 2017 [66] | N: 51 in training group; Mean age: 26.7; Percent female: 47%; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 100% of attractive food | Untrained | 1; 40/50 | Single-group pre-test–post-test | Attractive food | Explicit: Attractiveness |
Stice et al., 2017 [67] | N: 21 in training group; 26 in the control group; Mean age: 19.2; Percent female: 95%; Inclusion criteria: weight concerns and a BMI of 25 or greater Exclusion criteria: current DSM-IV anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, or binge-eating disorder. | Inhibit 100% of high-calorie foods | Inhibit 0% of high-calorie foods | 4; 1120 | Pre-test–post-test–control | High-calorie foods | Explicit: Palatability and monetary value |
Stice et al., 2021 [68] | N: 21 in training group; 26 in the control group; Mean age: 19.2; Percent female: 95%; Inclusion criteria: between 17 and 20 years of age, had a BMI greater than 20 and less than 30, and reported concern about their weight; Exclusion criteria: a current diagnosis of anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, or binge-eating disorder. | Inhibit 100% of high-calorie foods | Inhibit 0% of high-calorie foods | 6; 840 | Pre-test–post-test–control | High-calorie foods | Explicit: Palatability and monetary value |
Tzavella et al., 2021 [54] | N: 163 in training group; Mean age: 22.4; Percent female: 81%; Inclusion criteria: at least 18 years of age, fluent in spoken and written English, and normal or corrected-to-normal vision; Exclusion criteria: dieting at the time of the study, with a weight goal and timeframe in mind, current and/or past diagnosis of any eating disorder(s), or a BMI lower than 18.5 kg/m2. | Inhibit 100% of energy-dense foods | Untrained | 1; 72 | Single-group pretest–post-test | Energy-dense foods | Explicit: Liking |
Tzavella et al., 2020 [69] | N: 96/117/113 in training group; Mean age: 21.6/26.9; Percent female: 57%; Inclusion criteria: at least 18 years of age, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision; Exclusion criteria: not able to understand written and spoken English well, reported having a food allergy and/or intolerance to any of the major food allergens, or had a self-reported past or current diagnosis of an eating disorder, with the exception of binge-eating disorder. | Inhibit 100% of energy-dense foods | Untrained | 1; 64/128 | Post-test only with control/Single-group pre-test–post-test | Energy-dense foods | Explicit: Liking; craving Implicit: Affective priming paradigm |
Veling et al., 2013a study 2 [70] | N: 22 in training group; 22 in the control group; Mean age: 21.5; Percent female: 61%; Inclusion criteria: N.A; Exclusion criteria: N.A. | Inhibit 100% of snack foods | Snack foods: 0% inhibit | 1; 32 | Post-test only with control | Snack foods | Explicit: Palatability |
Yang et al., 2021a [14] | N: 21 in training group; 26 in the control group; Mean age: 19.2; Percent female: 95%; Inclusion criteria: had weight concerns, were willing to participate in the current weight control trials, and had a BMI of 23 or greater; Exclusion criteria: self-reported current eating disorders, mental disorders, or head injuries. | Inhibit 100% of energy-dense foods | Filter image: 50% go, 50 inhibit | 5; 500 | Pre-test–post-test–control | Energy-dense foods | Explicit: Attractiveness |
Moderator | β | t/F (df) | k | g+ | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Participant age | 0.001 | 0.22 (4.5) | 0.834 | ||
Percentage of female participants | 0.089 | 0.30 (10.1) | 0.771 | ||
Type of evaluation | 3.23 (5.57) | 0.020 | |||
Explicit evaluation | 30 | 0.285 | <0.001 | ||
Implicit evaluation | 6 | −0.100 | 0.425 | ||
Training paradigm | 0.16 (2.24) | 0.728 | |||
Go/no-go task | 30 | 0.247 | <0.001 | ||
Stop-signal task | 3 | 0.112 | 0.556 | ||
Mixed | 3 | 0.341 | 0.296 | ||
Food novelty | 4.33 (8.07) | 0.071 | |||
Trained food | 19 | 0.291 | <0.001 | ||
Generalized food | 8 | 0.130 | 0.108 | ||
Mixed | 9 | 0.150 | 0.271 | ||
Weight status | 1.24 (8.35) | 0.316 | |||
Normal weight | 29 | 0.225 | <0.001 | ||
Overweight/obesity | 7 | 0.328 | 0.007 | ||
Length of follow-up | −0.57 (2.02) | 0.626 | |||
Immediate | 32 | 0.246 | <0.001 | ||
Post | 4 | 0.193 | 0.189 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Yang, Y.; Qi, L.; Morys, F.; Wu, Q.; Chen, H. Food-Specific Inhibition Training for Food Devaluation: A Meta-Analysis. Nutrients 2022, 14, 1363. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14071363
Yang Y, Qi L, Morys F, Wu Q, Chen H. Food-Specific Inhibition Training for Food Devaluation: A Meta-Analysis. Nutrients. 2022; 14(7):1363. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14071363
Chicago/Turabian StyleYang, Yingkai, Le Qi, Filip Morys, Qian Wu, and Hong Chen. 2022. "Food-Specific Inhibition Training for Food Devaluation: A Meta-Analysis" Nutrients 14, no. 7: 1363. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14071363
APA StyleYang, Y., Qi, L., Morys, F., Wu, Q., & Chen, H. (2022). Food-Specific Inhibition Training for Food Devaluation: A Meta-Analysis. Nutrients, 14(7), 1363. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14071363