Next Article in Journal
Maternal Passive Smoking, Vitamin D Deficiency and Risk of Spontaneous Abortion
Next Article in Special Issue
Traditional, Vegetarian, or Low FODMAP Diets and Their Relation to Symptoms of Eating Disorders: A Cross-Sectional Study among Young Women in Poland
Previous Article in Journal
Combination of Caloric Restriction and a Mixed Training Protocol as an Effective Strategy to Counteract the Deleterious Effects in Trabecular Bone Microarchitecture Caused by a Diet-Induced Obesity in Sprague Dawley Rats
Previous Article in Special Issue
Gender Differences in Treatment Outcomes for Eating Disorders: A Case-Matched, Retrospective Pre–Post Comparison
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Bacon, Brownie, or Broccoli? Beliefs about Stress-Relieving Foods and Their Relationship to Orthorexia Nervosa

Department for Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Frankfurt-Goethe University, 60528 Frankfurt, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2022, 14(18), 3673; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14183673
Submission received: 5 August 2022 / Revised: 2 September 2022 / Accepted: 2 September 2022 / Published: 6 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Eating Disorders, and Nutritional Beliefs, Trends or Practices)

Abstract

:
Background: Nutritional beliefs play an important role when it comes to food choice. However, little attention has been paid to which foods individuals believe to be comforting when experiencing stress. With increasing health awareness in the general public, this study aims to examine whether the nutritional belief exists that only healthy foods relieve stress. If so, we are interested in its relationship to Orthorexia Nervosa (ON) tendencies. Methods: 175 participants (mean age 28.5 ± 7.8 years, 124 females) completed questionnaires to assess beliefs about stress-relieving foods and ON tendencies. Principal component analysis was used to reduce foods to food groups. Subsequently, a latent profile analysis was performed to identify groups with distinct nutritional beliefs. Results: Among eight distinct groups, one group (8% of the sample) reported the belief that exclusively healthy foods relieve stress. Multinominal logistic regressions showed that higher ON tendencies were associated with that group. Conclusions: Our findings suggest that individuals with stronger ON tendencies believe that, in particular, healthy foods relieve stress. This indicates that nutritional beliefs in ON concern not only the somatic consequences of certain foods, but also psychological consequences, which might also drive orthorexic behaviour. This offers a new target for the diagnosis and treatment of ON.

1. Introduction

1.1. Nutritional Beliefs

Nutritional beliefs play an important role in everyday life as they influence which foods we buy and eat [1]. Nutritional beliefs include beliefs about ideal dietary patterns, such as when it is best to eat or whether three large meals or several small meals a day are better. Moreover, nutritional beliefs often concern the health impact of certain foods, i.e., which foods are healthy and which are unhealthy [2]. These beliefs are reinforced by a broad spectrum of research that focuses on the somatic impact of different dietary patterns, the mass media’s interaction with it and food industries’ interests [2].

1.2. Nutritional Beliefs about Comfort Foods

Nutritional beliefs do not only concern what and how much we eat, but also how foods influence us psychologically. Foods that are consumed with the intention to increase well-being or to dilute negative emotions are often referred to as comfort foods [3]. Although there is a broad range of research on comfort foods and beliefs about foods that can give comfort, there is no clear definition of comfort foods [4]. The belief about which foods are comforting varies across cultures and individuals. In Western society, the image of a hot soup on a couch on a rainy day appears very cosy and generates an image of comfort [4]. In the UK, black tea was rated as one of the top comfort foods [3]. On an individual level, age as well as gender influence what is considered comforting. Moreover, it could be shown that favourite dishes from childhood had comforting effects [3,4]. Thus, what someone believes to be comforting is influenced by associations of a time when parents or caregivers in general provided food [5].

1.3. Comfort Foods and Stress Relief

Comfort foods influence how we feel and are consumed to increase well-being. One major risk factor that impacts well-being in everyday life is the experience of stress. It has been shown that eating is used to comfort in stressed states [6,7]. Hence, some individuals use eating in order to relieve stress and increase well-being. Although the body of research on the relationship between stress and eating behaviour is large, it has not been examined which foods are believed to relieve stress and give comfort in stressed states. Instead, studies have mainly focused on what or how much is eaten. It could be shown that greater stress was accompanied by a greater drive to eat, even leading to binge-eating episodes [8]. Furthermore, stress is associated with an increase in high-sugar high-fat (HSHF) intake and a decrease in the consumption of low-energy high-nutrient foods (i.e., healthy foods), particularly fruits and vegetables [9,10]. Individuals with chronic stress are especially prone to consume more calories [11]. This is intriguing, as several studies reported that eating low-energy high-nutrient foods, such as fruits, vegetables, nuts or legumes, can increase overall psychological well-being [12,13,14]. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies confirm these results, showing that fruit and vegetable consumption is associated with feeling happy within the subsequent two hours, but also feeling happier and less sad in general [15,16].

1.4. The Role of Healthy Foods in Stress Relief

It seems counterintuitive that stress is mostly associated with unhealthy eating, while it appears that healthy foods increase well-being. An increase in health awareness has been consistently observed [17]. Beyond that, more attention is paid to the psychological effects of food. Finch et al. [18] showed experimentally that healthy comfort eating had the same stress-reducing effect as unhealthy comfort eating [18]. This and the previously mentioned findings build the foundation for our approach: The first aim of our study is to examine whether a group of individuals can be identified that is driven by the belief that healthy foods relieve stress and thus differs from the common assumption that HSHF intake provides comfort when experiencing stress.

1.5. Orthorexia Nervosa and Beliefs about Stress-Relieving Foods

In the context of the belief that healthy foods relieve stress, Orthorexia Nervosa (ON) seems particularly relevant, as it is the obsession with eating only foods recognised as healthy because of excessive health concerns [19]. The concept of ON is still rather new and subject to dispute. It is not yet defined as a mental disorder in the common disease classification systems (ICD-11 [20] and DSM-V [21]). However, it is discussed whether ON should be classified as a separate eating disorder [22]. So far, it has been reported that nutritional beliefs in individuals with ON concern mainly somatic consequences, such as that the intake of healthy foods prevents the development of severe somatic conditions [22]. So far, little is known about whether beliefs concerning psychological consequences of food choice also drive orthorexic behaviour. Consequently, nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods have not been investigated with regard to ON tendencies yet. We expect the nutritional belief that healthy foods relieve stress is more common in individuals with higher ON tendencies.
To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to investigate nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods and their association with higher ON tendencies. We assess whether a group characterised by the nutritional belief that only healthy foods can help to relieve stress can be identified. If so, this group will be—in accordance with the results of comfort food research—categorised demographically (average age, gender, education, diet and wish for body weight change). We hypothesize that this group is associated with higher levels of ON tendencies.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Data for this study were collected within the APPetite study [23], which is part of the European Union Horizon 2020 project Eat2beNICE. In total, 124 females and 51 males between the ages of 18 and 53 years (M = 28.5 years, SD = 7.8) were included in the study. The average BMI was 24.38 (SD = 3.95). Participants included healthy adults that were able to give informed consent. Lifetime diagnosis of bipolar I disorder, schizophrenia, organic mental disorder or substance dependence (other than nicotine dependence), current severe episode of other Axis 1 mental disorders, as well as severe somatic conditions were exclusion criteria.

2.2. Procedure

Participants who agreed to take part in the APPetite study were invited to complete digitalized questionnaires as part of a longer appointment on campus. Weight and height were measured to calculate Body Mass Index (BMI = weight (kg)/height (m)2).

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Nutritional Beliefs about Stress-Relieving Foods

Nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods were assessed through a 29-item questionnaire that was developed for the present study. Participants’ beliefs were captured by asking how much each of the 29 foods (see Table 1) helps them to feel better when feeling stressed. The items were rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). Due to the lack of a commonly used list of food groups suitable for the German population, the list of food items was developed for the present study. To do so, we (1) selected the most relevant food items from the food categories provided by the Competence Center for Nutrition of the Bavarian State Ministry of Food, Agriculture and Forestry [24]; (2) in some cases, split the groups to allow a more detailed assessment (e.g., rice and potatoes); and (3) added missing foods (e.g., plant-based milk/yoghurt) to account for recent developments in the food market, especially the trend towards plant-based foods.

2.3.2. Orthorexic Tendencies

The 10-item Düsseldorfer Orthorexie Scale [25] was used to capture ON tendencies. The DOS has been shown to have good psychometric properties, with Cronbach’s α = 0.84 and a high test–retest reliability of r = 0.79 [25]. It was recently reported that the DOS is a reliable self-report instrument [26]. Items are answered on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (does not represent myself) to 4 (this represents myself). Total scores of 30 and above indicate pathological ON tendencies.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Analyses were conducted using R (version 4.1, Vienna, Austria) and RStudio (version 1.4.1106, Boston, MA, USA). Significance was tested at 5% level.

2.4.1. Data Exclusion

All food groups that were rated 1 by at least 60% of participants were excluded, as this indicates that these foods are less relevant in the context of stress relief. The items “Sauces”, “Vegetarian spread”, “Flaxseeds/chia seeds”, “Meat substitute”, “Plant-based milk/Yoghurt” and “Vegetable oil” were excluded due to this criterion (see Supplementary Table S1).

2.4.2. Principal Component Analysis

Principal component analysis (PCA) was performed with the R-package psych [27] on the remaining 23 food items. In this study, PCA was used as a data reduction method. It allowed the grouping of items into factors and thus revealed latent constructs on the item level. To check the pattern of relationships as an initial data preparation, a correlation matrix including all correlations between all food items was examined (see Supplementary Table S2). Model adequacy was assessed via Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) [28] measure, Bartlett’s test [29] and minimum covariance determinant [30]. For the final PCA, 19 remaining food items were used. As recommended as the best method for factor extraction, parallel analysis was applied [31]. Orthogonal rotation was chosen to maximize the loading on only one factor for each variable. For further person-centred analysis, factor scores were calculated using the Anderson–Rubin method—a modification of the Bartlett’s method—adaptable for orthogonal factor solutions only (see Supplementary Table S3).

2.4.3. Latent Profile Analysis

Latent profile analysis (LPA) is a person-centred clustering approach that uses continuous variables as indicators. LPA was executed with the R-package tidyLPA [32]. The most complex model—usually the recommended model [33]—with regards to the constraint of class-specific (co)variance matrices of the indicator variables was configured [32]. Various fit indices are available to determine the number of latent profiles [34]. As recommended, estimation outputs were inspected initially for error messages, outliers and theoretical plausibility [35]. Subsequently, we compared the BLRT, SABIC, BIC and AIC. According to the literature, the BLRT (bootstrap likelihood ratio test) is seen as one of the most accurate fit indicators [36]. In particular, BLRT efficiently selected the correct number of latent profiles when outcome variables were nonnormal [37]. Thus, in model selection, we focused firstly on a significant BLRT. Another accurate fit indicator is the SABIC (sample-size adjusted BIC), which is corrected when compared to the BIC (Bayesian information criterion) [35]. The informative value of the AIC (Akaike information criterion) is, compared to the BIC and SABIC, considered to be weaker [38]. However, the performance of AIC is superior in selecting the correct number of profiles with sample sizes <500 [35]. The entropy quantifies separation, with values >0.8 being taken to indicate sufficient separation. In case of a disagreement of fit indices on the optimal number of profiles, the profile sizes of the different profile solutions were considered. The literature recommends that no profile should be <5% of the sample [38]. To further characterize the profiles, means and standard deviations were calculated for age and BMI. Moreover, sex distribution of the profiles was compared.

2.4.4. Multinominal Logistic Regression Analysis

Multinominal logistic regression analysis (MLRA) was applied to investigate the association between higher ON tendencies and nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods. The number of covariates used in MLRA depended on the group sizes of profiles formed through LPA. Beta values, their standard errors and odds ratios (OR) in combination with the corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CI) are given as estimates of effect sizes.

3. Results

3.1. Sociodemographic Characteristics

The sociodemographic characteristics of the sample are summarized in Table 2. In total, 124 females and 51 males between the ages of 18 and 53 (M = 28.5 years, SD = 7.8) were included in the study. Female participants were on average 28.1 (SD = 7.7) and male participants were on average 29.7 (SD = 8.1) years old. The average BMI of the total sample was 24.38 (SD = 3.95). The average BMI of female participants was 23.60 (SD = 3.68). Male participants had an average BMI of 26.31 (SD = 3.96). Within the sample, 40% had graduated from high school (Abitur: German equivalent of “A Levels”), 22% were bachelor’s graduates and 23% were master’s graduates. Moreover, 5% reported having accomplished their Ph.D., and another 5% had completed vocational training. Only 1% reported having an intermediate school-leaving certificate (mittlere Reife: German equivalent of “high school diploma”).

Diet

A pescetarian diet was followed by 5% of the sample, whereas 11% were vegetarians. Only 3% reported a vegan diet. The majority of the sample (81%) followed an omnivore diet. Regarding restricted diet to lose weight, 48% of the sample reported that they were trying to reduce their weight, whereas 3% reported that they aimed to gain weight. The remaining 49% reported that they did not regulate their weight at all.
The mean DOS score of the sample was 17.77 (SD = 5.18).

3.2. Principal Component Analysis

KMO measurement (KMO = 0.88) was “great” according to Kaiser [39]. This as well as the significant Bartlett’s test verified the sampling adequacy for the PCA. The minimum covariance determinant (MCD) was just above the cut-off value of 0.00001 (MCD = 0.0000341). There was no multicollinearity (bivariate correlation coefficients > 0.9) present (Supplementary Table S2). A first PCA was calculated (see Supplementary Table S4). All items that scored on more than one factor with similar factor loadings were excluded. This mainly affected food items rich in carbohydrates (“Bread”, “Rice”, “Potatoes”, “Pasta”). PCA was again calculated with 19 remaining variables (see Supplementary Table S5). Model adequacy parameters verified the sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.83 and Bartlett’s test was significant (p < 0.05)), and the determinant was sufficient (MCD = 0.00075). Parallel analysis estimated three components to be the precise number of components. The model explains 53% of the variance. A cut-off of 0.4 was considered for factor loadings. Items with factor loading higher than 0.4 were included. Inspection of the factors led to labelling of “animal-based products” (Factor one), “junk food” (Factor two) and “healthy vegetarian products” (Factor three).

3.3. Latent Profile Analysis

Profile enumeration was calculated from the most complex model, in which the variances and the covariances were freely estimated across profiles [32]. Fit indices for the three- to eight-profile solutions, listed in Table 3, disagreed on the optimal number of profiles. The BLRT revealed significant results in the three-profile- and eightprofile-solution, whereas the BIC was best in the three-profile solution. The SABIC and AIC were lowest in the six-profile solution, but second lowest in the eight-profile solution. Entropy showed sufficient profile separation in the three- and six- to eight-profile solutions. Thus, in order to determine the optimal profile solution, entropy was not useful, as all potential solutions had values >0.8.
As a significant BLRT outperforms other fit indices [34], we initially focused on this value. However, because AIC and SABIC were smallest in the six-profile solution, we decided to estimate profile sizes of the six- and eight-profile solutions to finally choose the optimal number (Table 4). Based on this procedure, the six-profile solution was rejected. Here, the smallest profile includes only five members. This violates the recommendation that profile size should be at least 5% of the sample [38]. Hence, the model with eight profiles was chosen to be the best model due to its significant BLRT, second best AIC and SABIC, and sufficiently large profile sizes, but also regarding its content.
Eight profiles emerged from the LPA, presented in Figure 1. Based on the mean factor scores, labels were chosen to describe the nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods of each profile. Profile 1 (n = 37) believed that junk as well as healthy foods relieve stress. Thus, we named it according to the participants described in this profile: “Junk and veggie believer”. Furthermore: Profile 2: “Low junk believer” (n = 39); Profile 3: “Non-believer” (n = 13); Profile 4: “Strong animal-product believer” (n = 20); Profile 5: “Junk and animal-product believer” (n = 10); Profile 6: “General believer” (n = 19); Profile 7: “Low animal-product believer” (n = 23); Profile 8: “Healthy believer” (n = 14). For instance, Profile 2 was named “Low junk believer” according to a negative mean value of the factors animal and healthy foods and a low positive mean value of the factor junk food. The profiles differed regarding age (see Table 5). Individuals that belonged to Profile 8 (“Healthy believer”) and Profile 6 (“General believer”) were on average 32 years old and, therefore, the oldest groups. Profile 5 (“Junk and animal-product believer”) (26.4 ± 6.43) represented the youngest group. The profiles showed no large differences in BMI, as all profiles showed normal average weight except for Profile 6 (“General believer”) (26.7 ± 5.5). Regarding sex distribution, LPA revealed that female sex predominated in most profiles (see Table 5). Exceptions were Profile 4 (“Strong animal-product believer”) and 6 (“General believer”), in which sex was almost equal, and Profile 7 (“Low animal-product believer”), in which male sex predominated.

3.4. Multivariate Logistic Regression

To test whether higher ON tendencies could predict membership to a certain profile, MLRA was performed. Profile 3—the profile of “Non-believer”—was chosen as a reference group. This profile was suitable as baseline because members did not believe any foods relieve stress. Table 6 shows that participants with higher ON tendencies were significantly more likely to be a member of the profile (Profile 8—Healthy believer) that only believed healthy foods relieved stress (OR = 1.28, CI = [1.07; 1.53], p < 0.01) compared to the reference group of “Non-believer”. Participants with higher ON tendencies were also significantly more likely to be a member of the “Junk and veggie believer” profile (OR = 1.24, CI = [1.05; 1.46], p < 0.05). Thus, higher levels of ON tendencies predict profile membership to the profiles with the nutritional belief that either only healthy or healthy and junk foods relieve stress.

4. Discussion

The aim of the present study was to examine whether (a) the nutritional belief exists that only healthy foods relieve stress, and (b) this belief is associated with higher ON tendencies.
First, we identified a group of individuals with the nutritional belief that healthy (i.e., whole-food-plant-based) foods, such as fruits, vegetables, salad, nuts and soups, help to relieve stress. Confirming our first hypothesis, higher ON tendencies were associated with this belief. Unexpectedly, the group with the nutritional belief that both healthy and junk foods, such as cakes, sweets, sweet spreads and salty nibbles, are stress-relieving was also associated with higher ON tendencies.
The nutritional belief about HSHF stress-relieving foods has been previously shown to be the most prevalent [40,41]. However, our results show that other nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods exist, namely, the nutritional belief about healthy stress-relieving foods. In comfort food research, 40% of what is reported as comforting could be categorized as homemade, natural or even as healthy [4]. Our results show that nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods are similarly heterogenic to nutritional beliefs about comfort food. Interestingly, we were able to identify a group of individuals who reported the nutritional belief that only healthy foods are stress-relieving.
Interestingly, the group of “Healthy believers” (i.e., the group with the nutritional belief that healthy foods are stress-relieving) showed demographical characteristics that were also reported in research on comfort foods. Thus, age and gender seem to influence the preferences for stress-relieving foods. For instance, it has been reported that the need for sweet comfort food decreases with age [42]. Simultaneously, older individuals report choosing healthier comfort food in general [4]. Our results on nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods are consistent with these observations. Compared to the other groups, participants of the “Healthy believers” group were older. A national survey in Germany reported that the interest in health information increases with age [17]. It can be suggested that this is related to a behaviour that is characterized by a higher health awareness and different nutritional beliefs regarding stress-relieving foods. Furthermore, women predominated in the group of “Healthy believers”. In comfort food research, it has been shown that gender has an impact on the choice of comfort foods [3]. Women have been reported to live healthier in general [43] and are often more aware of what is considered healthy [17].
Our hypothesis that individuals with higher ON tendencies believe that healthy foods can relieve stress could be confirmed. Previous research on ON has mainly focused on nutritional beliefs concerning the somatic consequences of nourishment [22]. Thus, our study extends the research on nutritional beliefs in individuals with higher ON tendencies. Nutritional beliefs play an important role in the development and maintenance of eating disorders, as they are the foundation for strict rules and the adherence to them [40]. Individuals profit from this pathological behaviour, as it is comforting and, in the case of ON, consuming healthy foods contributes to the feeling “to do everything right” [22]. Stress is often accompanied by negative feelings. Thus, in stressed states, it may be even more important to stick to orthorexic behaviour. Indeed, it could be shown that stress aggravates symptoms in patients with eating disorders. Bulimia Nervosa as well as Binge-Eating patients are triggered into binge-eating episodes when stressed [44], whereas Anorexia Nervosa patients decrease food consumption. ON is characterized by highly restrained eating patterns with inflexible dietary rules and compulsive behaviours [45]. Additionally, healthy foods are idealised in a way that they are attributed with exaggerated benefits. Consequently, it is not surprising that participants with higher ON tendencies believe that sticking to healthy eating behaviour can relieve stress. Further studies are required to prove these assumptions.
Notably, individuals with higher ON tendencies did not only belong to the group of Healthy believers. There was a small group of individuals with higher ON tendencies reporting that they find both healthy and junk food stress-relieving. Demographically, the group of “Junk and veggie believers” was on average younger than the group of ”Healthy believer”. This result confirms findings from earlier studies, as the need for sweets decreases with age [42]. However, given the association between ON and restrained eating [46], this finding also poses the question whether individuals with higher ON tendencies who believe only healthy products relieve stress have more inflexible dietary rules compared to those who believe both healthy and unhealthy foods relieve stress. ON is not listed as a psychiatric disorder in the existing diagnostic systems (ICD-11 [20] and DSM-V [21]). Particularly, the severity of symptoms associated with ON that would classify it as an eating disorder is still being discussed [45,47]. Thus, our results confirm the need to extend the research on ON. Further research is needed to understand who reported the nutritional belief that only healthy foods relieve stress and who reported the nutritional belief about junk and healthy stress-relieving foods.
Our study extends previous research in that a person-centred approach (i.e., LPA) was used to define different nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to explore nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods and their relation to ON.
However, our study has some limitations: (1) The sample of our study is not representative of the general population, as it entails an overrepresentation of female, younger and well-educated participants. University students are quite a homogenous group of people. They are easy to recruit, potentially having a higher economic motivation to participate in studies and also having sufficient time to take part in studies. (2) There are thousands of food products on the market. The countless number of foods makes the assessment of food-related constructs (such as beliefs about stress-relieving foods) difficult. Unfortunately, to the best of our knowledge, there is no commonly used list of food items suitable for the German population available. Therefore, food items had to be selected for the present study. On the one hand, including all available foods in the assessment was not feasible. On the other hand, only including very broad food categories could oversimply the matter. Therefore, we strived to strike a happy medium. However, the selection of the food items might have directly influenced the results of the present study. Therefore, the results should be interpreted with caution. (3) In person-centred approaches—such as LPA—sample size is crucial for statistical power. With a rather small sample size (N = 175), our study is at the lower bound of the recommendations [35]. For LPA, a power analysis is not necessarily expected [35], and previous studies revealed valid LPA results with smaller sample sizes [48]. However, in order to replicate the profile solution in future studies, we suggest performing a Monte Carlo simulation in advance to calculate the required sample size as recommended by experts in the field [49,50]. Furthermore, MLRA’s power is affected by small group sizes. Still, we complied with the recommendations regarding the rule of one predictor per ten observations. A further limitation is the lack of a validation analysis. Unfortunately, our sample size did not allow us to perform multiple-groups LPA for validation as recommended in the literature [35]. Due to capacity reasons, our study lacks the possibility to replicate the results across different samples, contexts and time points. As further research on nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods and their relationship with eating disorders is needed, we strongly recommend larger sample sizes for future studies. Furthermore, the plausibility of the profiles should be replicated by adding additional constructs, such as BMI and eating habits.

5. Conclusions

Our study provides novel evidence that helps advance our understanding of the underlying belief system of individuals with higher ON tendencies. Our findings suggest that nutritional beliefs in ON concern not only the somatic consequences of certain foods, but also psychological consequences. Hence, beliefs concerning psychological consequences of food choices seem to be a relevant driver of orthorexic behaviour. These findings offer a new target for the diagnosis as well as treatment of ON.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu14183673/s1 Table S1: excluded food items due to rating; Table S2: correlation matrix showing all correlations between the food items; Table S3: factor scores of all factors; Table S4: first PCA; Table S5: final PCA.

Author Contributions

A.R. (Alea Ruf), A.R. (Andreas Reif) and S.M. were involved in the conception and design of the study. A.S. was involved in data acquisition, the analysis and interpretation of data, manuscript writing and final approval of the version to be submitted. A.R. (Alea Ruf), K.F.A., A.R. (Andreas Reif) and S.M. were involved in the interpretation of data, revising and editing the article for important and intellectual content and final approval. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Program under grant agreement No 728018. The funding source had no involvement in the study design, data collection, interpretation of the findings or writing of this manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

All study procedures complied with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki; Rickham, 2013) and were approved by the local ethics committee of the faculty of medicine of the Goethe University Frankfurt (Ethikkommission des Fachbereichs Medizin der Goethe-Universität: reference number: 192/18). All subjects declared that they understood the study procedure and signed a written informed consent.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy and ethical concerns.

Acknowledgments

Parts of this study constitute the Bachelor Thesis of Maike Wisker and Emina Ricciardi, the Master Thesis (M.Sc.) of Maryam Golzarnia, Luise Wortmann, Simone Demmel and Krystyna Kotthaus and the Thesis for a Medical Doctorate of Julia Flemming, Söri Wenz, Hendrik Friedrichsen and Hiba El Jomaa.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Martins, Y.; Pliner, P. Human food choices: An examination of the factors underlying acceptance/rejection of novel and familiar animal and nonanimal foods. Appetite 2005, 45, 214–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Loss, J.; Nagel, E. Social Marketing-Verführung zum gesundheitsbewussten Verhalten? Gesundheitswesen 2010, 72, 54–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Spence, C. Comfort food: A review. Int. J. Gastron. Food Sci. 2017, 9, 105–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Wansink, B. Engineering Comfort Foods. Am. Demogr. 2014, 66–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Troisi, J.D.; Gabriel, S.; Derrick, J.L.; Geisler, A. Threatened belonging and preference for comfort food among the securely attached. Appetite 2015, 90, 58–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Gibson, E.L. The psychobiology of comfort eating: Implications for neuropharmacological interventions. Behav. Pharmacol. 2012, 23, 442–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Zellner, D.A.; Loaiza, S.; Gonzalez, Z.; Pita, J.; Morales, J.; Pecora, D.; Wolf, A. Food selection changes under stress. Physiol. Behav. 2006, 87, 789–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Groesz, L.M.; McCoy, S. What is eating you? Stress and the Drive to Eat. Appetite 2012, 58, 717–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Mikolajczyk, R.T.; El Ansari, W.; Maxwell, A.E. Food consumption frequency and perceived stress and depressive symptoms among students in three European countries. Nutr. J. 2009, 8, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Wallis, D.J.; Hetherington, M.M. Stress and eating: The effects of ego-threat and cognitive demand on food intake in restrained and emotional eaters. Appetite 2004, 43, 39–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Tomiyama, A.J.; Finch, L.E.; Cummings, J.R. Did that Brownie do its Job?: Stress, eating and the biobehavioral effects of Comfort food. Emerg. Trends Soc. Behav. Sci. 2015, 1, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Conner, T.S.; Brookie, K.L.; Richardson, A.C.; Polak, M.A. On carrots and curiosity: Eating fruit and vegetables is associated with greater flourishing in daily life. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2015, 20, 413–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Conner, T.S.; Brookie, K.L. Let them eat fruit! The effect of fruit and vegetable consumption on psychological well-being in young adults: A randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0171206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. White, B.A.; Horwath, C.C.; Conner, T.S. Many apples a day keep the blues away--daily experiences of negative and positive affect and food consumption in young adults. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2013, 18, 782–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Franja, S.; Wahl, D.R.; Elliston, K.G.; Ferguson, S.G. Comfort eating: An observational study of affect in the hours immediately before, and after, snacking. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2021, 26, 825–838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Liao, Y.; Schembre, S.M.; O’Connor, S.G.; Belcher, B.R.; Maher, J.P.; Dzubur, E.; Dunton, G.F. An Electronic Ecological Momentary Assessment Study to Examine the Consumption of High-Fat/High-Sugar Foods, Fruits/Vegetables, and Affective States Among Women. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2018, 50, 626–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Horch, K.; Wirz, J. Nutzung von Gesundheitsinformationen. Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundh. Gesundh. 2005, 48, 1250–1255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Finch, L.E.; Cummings, J.R.; Tomiyama, A.J. Cookie or clementine? Psychophysiological stress reactivity and recovery after eating healthy and unhealthy comfort foods. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2019, 107, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Bratman, S. Health Food Junkies: Overcoming the Obsession with Healthful Eating, 1st ed.; Doubleday: New York, NY, USA, 2000; ISBN 0767906306. [Google Scholar]
  20. International Classification of Diseases Eleventh Revision (ICD-11); World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2022.
  21. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; ISBN 0-89042-555-8. [Google Scholar]
  22. Strahler, J.; Stark, R. Orthorexia nervosa: A behavioral condition or a new mental disorder. Suchttherapie 2019, 20, 24–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ruf, A.; Koch, E.; Ebner-Priemer, U.; Knopf, M.; Reif, A.; Matura, S. Studying Microtemporal, Within-Person Processes of Diet, Physical Activity, and Related Factors Using the APPetite-Mobile-App: Feasibility, Usability, and Validation Study. J. Med. Internet. Res. 2021, 23, e25850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Bavarian State Ministry of Food, Agriculture and Forestry. Lebensmittelkategorien. Available online: https://www.kern.bayern.de/mam/cms03/wissenschaft/dateien/lebensmittelkategorien.pdf (accessed on 2 August 2018).
  25. Barthels, F.; Meyer, F.; Pietrowsky, R. Duesseldorf Orthorexia Scale–Construction and Evaluation of a Questionnaire Measuring Orthorexic Eating Behavior. Klin Psych. Psychother 2015, 44, 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Meule, A.; Holzapfel, C.; Brandl, B.; Greetfeld, M.; Hessler-Kaufmann, J.B.; Skurk, T.; Quadflieg, N.; Schlegl, S.; Hauner, H.; Voderholzer, U. Measuring orthorexia nervosa: A comparison of four self-report questionnaires. Appetite 2020, 146, 104512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Revelle, W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research; Northwestern University: Evanston, IL, USA, 2022; Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych (accessed on 20 August 2021).
  28. Kaiser, H.F. A second generation little jiffy. Psychometrika 1970, 35, 401–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bartlett, M.S. Properties of Sufficiency and Statistical Tests. R. Soc. Publ. 1937, 160, 268–282. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hubert, M.; Debruyne, M. Minimum covariance determinant. WIREs Comp. Stat. 2010, 2, 36–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Velicer, W.F.; Eaton, C.A.; Fava, J.L. Construct Explication through Factor or Component Analysis: A Review and Evaluation of Alternative Procedures for Determining the Number of Factors or Components. In Problems and Solutions in Human Assessment; Goffin, R.D., Helmes, E., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 41–71. ISBN 978-1-4613-6978-3. [Google Scholar]
  32. Rosenberg, J.M.; Beymer, P.N.; Anderson, D.J.; Van Lissa, C.J.; Schmidt, J.A. tidyLPA: An R Package to Easily Carry Out Latent Profile Analysis (LPA) Using Open-Source or Commercial Software. J. Open Source Softw. 2018, 3, 978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Preacher, K.J.; Merkle, E.C. The problem of model selection uncertainty in structural equation modeling. Psychol. Methods 2012, 17, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Tein, J.-Y.; Coxe, S.; Cham, H. Statistical Power to Detect the Correct Number of Classes in Latent Profile Analysis. Struct. Equ. Modeling 2013, 20, 640–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Spurk, D.; Hirschi, A.; Wang, M.; Valero, D.; Kauffeld, S. Latent profile analysis: A review and “how to” guide of its application within vocational behavior research. J. Vocat. Behav. 2020, 120, 103445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Nylund, K.L.; Asparouhov, T.; Muthén, B.O. Deciding on the Number of Classes in Latent Class Analysis and Growth Mixture Modeling: A Monte Carlo Simulation Study. Struct. Equ. Modeling A Multidiscip. J. 2007, 14, 535–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jung, T.; Wickrama, K. An Introduction to Latent Class Growth Analysis and Growth Mixture Modeling. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2008, 2, 302–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Wickrama, K.A.S.; Lee, T.K.; O’Neal, C.W.; Lorenz, F.O. Higher-Order Growth Curves and Mixture Modeling with Mplus: A Practical Guide; Routledge Taylor & Francis Group: New York, NY, USA, 2016; ISBN 9781138925151. [Google Scholar]
  39. Kaiser, H.F. An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika 1974, 39, 31–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Newman, E.; O’Connor, D.B.; Conner, M. Daily hassles and eating behaviour: The role of cortisol reactivity status. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2007, 32, 125–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Roberts, C.J.; Campbell, I.C.; Troop, N. Increases in weight during chronic stress are partially associated with a switch in food choice towards increased carbohydrate and saturated fat intake. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2014, 22, 77–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Dubé, L.; LeBel, J.L.; Lu, J. Affect asymmetry and comfort food consumption. Physiol. Behav. 2005, 86, 559–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mensink, G.B.M.; Truthmann, J.; Rabenberg, M.; Heidemann, C.; Haftenberger, M.; Schienkiewitz, A.; Richter, A. Obst- und Gemüsekonsum in Deutschland: Ergebnisse der Studie zur Gesundheit Erwachsener in Deutschland (DEGS1). Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundh. Gesundh. 2013, 56, 779–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Goldschmidt, A.B.; Wonderlich, S.A.; Crosby, R.D.; Engel, S.G.; Lavender, J.M.; Peterson, C.B.; Crow, S.J.; Cao, L.; Mitchell, J.E. Ecological momentary assessment of stressful events and negative affect in bulimia nervosa. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2014, 82, 30–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Barthels, F.; Barrada, J.R.; Roncero, M. Orthorexia nervosa and healthy orthorexia as new eating styles. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0219609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Barrada, J.R.; Roncero, M. Bidimensional structure of the orthorexia: Development and initial validation of a new instrument. An. Psicol 2018, 34, 283–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Depa, J.; Barrada, J.R.; Roncero, M. Are the Motives for Food Choices Different in Orthorexia Nervosa and Healthy Orthorexia? Nutrients 2019, 11, 697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Haynos, A.F.; Wang, S.B.; LeMay-Russell, S. An empirical taxonomy of reward response patterns in a transdiagnostic eating disorder sample. Eat. Behav. 2021, 42, 101531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Muthén, L.K.; Muthén, B.O. How to use a Monte Carlo study to decide on sample size and determine power. Struct. Equ. Modeling 2002, 9, 599–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Weller, B.E.; Bowen, N.K.; Faubert, S.J. Latent Class Analysis: A Guide to Best Practice. J. Black Psychol. 2020, 46, 287–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Profiles that were identified in the total sample through latent profile analysis. Error bars represent one standard error.
Figure 1. Profiles that were identified in the total sample through latent profile analysis. Error bars represent one standard error.
Nutrients 14 03673 g001
Table 1. Food items listed in the questionnaire to assess nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods.
Table 1. Food items listed in the questionnaire to assess nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods.
Food Items Listed in the 29-Item Questionnaire
BreadButter/margarineCakeCereals
CheeseCouscous/bulgur/pseudocerealsEggsFish
FruitsFlax- or Chia seedsLegumesMeat
Meat substituteMilk productsNutsPasta
PotatoesPlant-based milk/yoghurtReady mealsReady sauces
RiceSaladSalty nibblesSoups/stews
SweetSweet spreadVegetablesVegetable oil
Vegetarian spread
Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics of participants.
Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics of participants.
VariablesN = 175%
GenderMale5129
Female12471
EducationMittlere Reife21
Abitur7140
Completed vocational training95
Degree of university of Applied Sciences53
Bachelor’s graduates3922
Master’s graduates4023
Ph.D.95
DietOmnivore14281
Pescetarian95
Vegetarian1911
Vegan53
Body weight changeYes, I am trying to reduce my body weight8448
No8549
Yes, I am trying to gain body weight63
AgeMaleM = 29.7 (SD = 8.1)
FemaleM = 28.1 (SD = 7.7)
BMIMaleM = 26.31 (SD = 3.96)
FemaleM = 23.60 (SD = 3.68)
Table 3. Fit indices for the profile enumeration.
Table 3. Fit indices for the profile enumeration.
ProfilesLogLikAICBICSABICEntropyBLRT_p
3−692.2841442.5691534.3481442.5140.8654020.039604
4−680.861439.7211563.1471439.6460.7796990.158416
5−674.2531446.5061601.581446.4120.7939190.90099
6−649.7971417.5941604.3161417.4820.8684840.079208
7−659.241456.4811674.8511456.3490.8801460.742574
8−630.6261419.2531669.2711419.1020.9181130.009901
AIC = Akaike information criteria; BIC = Bayesian information criteria; SABIC = sample size-adjusted Bayesian information criteria; BLRT = bootstrap likelihood ratio test. Note: Bold font is used to highlight values corresponding to the best-fitting model.
Table 4. Profile sizes of the six- and eight-profile solutions.
Table 4. Profile sizes of the six- and eight-profile solutions.
Profiles12345678
Six-profile solution28312833550
Eight-profile solution3334131020192314
Table 5. Mean scores of indicators used in LPA, group name and size, as well as age, BMI, sex distribution and DOS of the 8 profiles showing 8 different nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods.
Table 5. Mean scores of indicators used in LPA, group name and size, as well as age, BMI, sex distribution and DOS of the 8 profiles showing 8 different nutritional beliefs about stress-relieving foods.
Size (n)Sex (m/f)% of Total (m/f)AgeBMIDOS
“Junk and veggie believer”375/329.8/25.827.84 ± 7.4123.3 ± 3.5819.41 ± 5.68
“Low junk believer”399/3017.6/24.228.59 ± 7.1224.2 ± 3.3617.51 ± 4.70
“Non-believer”133/105.9/829.07 ± 8.124.4 ± 3.6215.08 ± 3.04
“Strong animal-product believer”208/1215.6/9.727.25 ± 7.7625.0 ± 3.717.40 ± 4.32
“Junk and animal-product believer”101/91.9/7.326.4 ± 6.4323.9 ± 3.5218.30 ± 6.11
“General believer”198/1115.7/8.932.16 ± 9.5626.7 ± 5.517.68 ± 4.04
“Low animal-product believer”2314/927.5/7.326.43 ± 6.5623.6 ± 3.1215.48 ± 6.11
“Healthy believer”143/115.9/8.932.07 ± 9.724.9 ± 5.2320.64 ± 4.91
Table 6. Odds ratios dependent on DOS total score for being classified into a specific profile with the “NON-believer” profile as reference group.
Table 6. Odds ratios dependent on DOS total score for being classified into a specific profile with the “NON-believer” profile as reference group.
DOS Total Score
B (SE)OR95% CI
Non-believer vs.
Junk and veggie believer0.21 (0.09) *1.241.05; 1.46
Low junk believer0.14 (0.85)1.150.97; 1.36
Strong animal-product believer0.14 (0.09)1.150.96; 1.37
Junk and animal-product believer0.17 (0.1)1.190.98; 1.45
General believer0.15 (0.09)1.160.97; 1.39
Low animal-product believer0.03 (0.09)1.030.86; 1.24
Healthy believer0.25 (0.09) *1.281.07; 1.53
Asterisks indicate level of statistical significance: * p < 0.05; Log-Likelihood: −337.07, McFadden R2: 0.025971, Likelihood ratio test: χ2 = 17.975 (p value = 0.012083), bold numbers show significant results.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Steinmann, A.; Ruf, A.; Ahrens, K.F.; Reif, A.; Matura, S. Bacon, Brownie, or Broccoli? Beliefs about Stress-Relieving Foods and Their Relationship to Orthorexia Nervosa. Nutrients 2022, 14, 3673. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14183673

AMA Style

Steinmann A, Ruf A, Ahrens KF, Reif A, Matura S. Bacon, Brownie, or Broccoli? Beliefs about Stress-Relieving Foods and Their Relationship to Orthorexia Nervosa. Nutrients. 2022; 14(18):3673. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14183673

Chicago/Turabian Style

Steinmann, Annebirth, Alea Ruf, Kira F. Ahrens, Andreas Reif, and Silke Matura. 2022. "Bacon, Brownie, or Broccoli? Beliefs about Stress-Relieving Foods and Their Relationship to Orthorexia Nervosa" Nutrients 14, no. 18: 3673. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14183673

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop