Next Article in Journal
Comparing Two Days of Dietary Intake in What We Eat in America (WWEIA), NHANES, 2013–2016
Previous Article in Journal
Effects of Cranberry Juice Supplementation on Cardiovascular Disease Risk Factors in Adults with Elevated Blood Pressure: A Randomized Controlled Trial
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

High BMI Predicts Attention to Less Healthy Product Sets: Can a Prompt Lead to Consideration of Healthier Sets of Products?

by
Christopher R. Gustafson
1,*,
Kristina Arslain
2 and
Devin J. Rose
2,3
1
Department of Agricultural Economics, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68583, USA
2
Department of Food Science and Technology, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA
3
Department of Agronomy and Horticulture, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2021, 13(8), 2620; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13082620
Submission received: 31 May 2021 / Revised: 19 July 2021 / Accepted: 27 July 2021 / Published: 29 July 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Nutrition and Public Health)

Abstract

:
While the food environment has been implicated in diet-related health disparities, individuals’ ability to shape the food environment by limiting attention to a subset of products has not been studied. We examine the relationship between BMI category and consideration set—the products the individual considers before making a final choice—in an online hypothetical shopping experiment. Specifically, we focus on the healthiness of the consideration set the individual selected. Secondly, we examined the interaction of a health prompt (versus a no-prompt control) with BMI category on the healthiness of the consideration set. We used linear probability models to document the relationship between weight status and consideration set, between prompt and consideration set, and the effect of the interaction between prompt and weight status on consideration set. We found that (1) obese individuals are 10% less likely to shop from a consideration set that includes the healthy options, (2) viewing the prompt increased the probability of choosing a healthy consideration set by 9%, and (3) exposure to the prompt affected individuals in different BMI categories equally. While obese individuals are more likely to ignore healthier product options, a health-focused prompt increases consideration of healthy options across all BMI categories.

1. Introduction

The prevalence of overweight and obesity in the adult US population is currently over 70%, which has led to a crippling disease burden related to high body mass index (BMI) [1]. High BMI has been identified as a causative factor for the decrease in life expectancy in the US over the past few years [2,3]. Being overweight or obese is linked to poorer health outcomes, including a higher probability of developing non-communicable diseases, such as type 2 diabetes, cancer, and heart disease [4]. Overweight and obesity has become a leading cause of death in the US [5], and has contributed to millions of deaths globally [1]. Additionally, it is estimated to cost the US USD 150 billion per year in direct costs (in 2008 dollars), and USD 3–6 billion annually in indirect costs [6,7,8]. While genetics, physical activity, and other factors contribute to high BMI, diet is consistently recognized as a key behavioral element contributing to the high rates of overweight and obesity in the US [9]. According to recent research on energy expenditure, diet, and weight across hunter-gatherer and sedentary populations of the same ethnic groups, diet, rather than physical activity, appears to be the decisive factor in promoting higher BMI [10,11,12]. A diet composed of highly processed, calorically dense foods and low in fruits and vegetables leads to weight gain [13].
One area of research on diet and obesity has focused on the impact of food environment on the nutritional quality of individuals’ diets. A significant amount of this research has examined food deserts—areas that are typically in highly urban or rural settings in which residents lack ready access to healthy foods [14]. Links identified between food deserts and higher rates of overweight and obesity [15] have inspired public and private investment to eliminate food deserts [16]. However, while eliminating food deserts causes people to feel they have greater access to healthy foods, the quality of their diets does not improve [17,18,19]. This suggests that food deserts may actually reflect average local consumer demand for healthier versus less healthy foods, and that higher average BMIs in those areas are not caused by the environment but, like the food environment, reflect individuals’ food preferences [20].
The food environment as experienced by shoppers can vary, even within a single retail outlet, because people direct their attention to products differently. Shoppers face a vast array of products and product categories in food retail outlets; a typical well-stocked supermarket has tens of thousands of products, with many individual product categories containing hundreds of unique products [21,22]. With such a large number of products to select from, consumers cannot consider all available products. Instead, consumers form a “consideration set”—a small set of alternatives that the individual considers and ultimately chooses from [23,24,25]. In a recent eye-tracking study of supermarket shoppers, 67% of purchased products were chosen without the shopper considering any other products in that category [26]. The formation of consideration sets reflects people’s preferences [27], but is also influenced by what people expect the benefit of expanding their consideration set will be [28]. For example, in the context of food and health, there is significant evidence that people associate healthier foods with higher prices [29,30,31]. This assumption may deter them from considering healthier options when, in fact, the relationship between health and price is not definitive [32].
Research suggests that attention to nutrition information and health messaging is important in promoting healthy choices [33,34,35,36,37]. Intuitively, consideration of healthy products when making a purchase decision is also important, and is a necessary precondition to the purchase of healthy foods. However, there is little evidence that documents how individuals attend to a large array of product options when they have the ability to purposefully direct their attention. They may choose to consider all available products, or may restrict themselves to a small subset of options.
While individuals’ decisions about which products to consider are overlooked in most studies examining the healthiness of food choices, they shape the products and product information that people encounter when making choices. In this study, we examine the sets of products that individuals in an online supermarket pay attention to when making a food choice as a function of BMI. We focus, in particular, on how the weight status of individuals predicts their consideration of products. We then examine the effects of exposure to a fiber-based point-of-decision prompt (PDP) on consideration set, in order to evaluate whether prompts affect behavior differently for individuals of differing weight status.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Survey Design

2.1.1. Limited Product Consideration and Attention to Product Information

The design of our experiment aimed to (1) document choice process variables—such as participants’ considerations sets and the information that they used—that contributed to their ultimate product choice, and to (2) examine the effects of a fiber-based prompt message on the choice process and product variables. We were interested in examining how these relationships differed between groups of individuals with different body weight status in order to establish whether differences in attention to products and information may reinforce body weight status. We developed an online food choice experiment that was structured to replicate features common to online supermarket shopping interfaces.
There were two primary stages to the data collection process: (1) a shopping task, and (2) a survey. In the first stage, research participants faced three product categories: cereals, breads, and crackers. Participants first made a decision about the set of products to view in each category. Participants could examine all product options (N = 33 for each food category), or they could select to view a subset of products (N = 11 per subset). This breakdown reflects design features in many physical stores and online shopping environments, which permit consumers to quickly narrow the total set of products to a preferred subset. While ultimately structured to reflect real-world retail design, the subsets additionally separated products into less healthy, moderately healthy, and healthy options based on the Guiding Stars nutritional rating system rubric (https://www.guidingstars.com, (accessed on 12 March 2020).
In the Guiding Stars system, products are graded based on nutrient content on a 0–11-point scale. Products receive points if they meet or exceed criteria for vitamins, minerals, fiber, whole grains, and omega-3 fatty acids, while they lose points if they exceed benchmark levels of saturated fat, trans fat, added sodium, added sugar, and artificial colors (per standardized 100-calorie portion). Points are converted into stars—which constitute the consumer-facing information—in the following way: products with 0 points from the rubric receive zero stars; 1–2 points receive one star; 3–4 points receive two stars; and 5–11 points receive three stars. We created balanced product subsets, in which products received zero (11 products), one (11 products), and two or three stars (11 products) in each product category. We combined two- and three-star-rated products into one category because it was difficult to find enough products receiving three stars to create a separate category.
The subsets were described to participants according to examples of the products they contained, in order to avoid priming participants to explicitly think about the products in terms of health (again, following the design of real-world retail sites). In our experiment, the cereal sets were labeled as “Cereals such as Frosted Flakes, Froot Loops, Reese’s Puffs”, “Cereals such as Corn Flakes, Crispix, Special K”, “Cereals such as Cheerios, Wheat Chex, Grape Nuts”, and “All options”. Bread and cracker subsets were presented in the same manner.
The participants’ choices of product set determined the products viewed by the participants. After viewing the available products in a category, the participant then selected a product to “purchase”. The participant could also indicate that they would not purchase any of the available items (3% of participants indicated that they would not purchase a product in at least one of the three product categories). This option—indicating that they would not purchase any of the items—was always listed as the last option, while the presentation of the other available products was randomized. The product options were presented in a three-column format, with a photograph and the name of each product presented prominently. Underneath each product, the nutrient contents per serving for calories, fiber, fat, sodium, and sugar, as well as the price, were listed. After making choices in all three product categories, participants answered survey questions about their choices, typical shopping practices, and demographics.
The products included in the experiment were real brands that are widely available at regional and national supermarket chains in the US. These products were selected to represent a range of taste and nutrient profiles. Store brands were excluded to avoid differences in regional familiarity with products. The specific products included in the three categories are presented in Table A1, Table A2 and Table A3 in Appendix A. Each product also had a price associated with it, which was based on retail prices at the time at which the survey was conducted. We included a message in the introductory materials for the shopping task experiment encouraging participants to imagine they were making real choices with real money, which has been found to reduce hypothetical biases in economic choices [38].
For this paper, the important questions included in the post-experiment survey included questions about self-reported height and weight, and demographic variables, such as gender, age, income, and education. We used participants’ self-reported height and weight data to calculate each individual’s BMI, and then created a category variable based on BMI. Individuals with a BMI < 25 were categorized as normal weight; individuals were categorized as overweight if 25 ≤ BMI < 30; individuals with a BMI > 30 were categorized as obese. The dependent variable in the analyses was created by indicating whether the participant chose a consideration set that included the healthiest items (those that received a rating of 2 or 3 Guiding Stars). The healthy subset and the set that included all available options for each product type both qualified based on this definition. The “healthy consideration set” was coded as 1 for respondents that chose to view the healthy subset or the “all options” product set for a particular product type, and a 0 otherwise. We averaged the number of healthy consideration sets each participant chose to view across the three product categories to create the dependent variable used in the analyses.
The experiment and survey vehicle was programmed in Qualtrics XM (2021, SAP, Provo, UT, USA). The survey was distributed to adults 19 years of age or older in the United States through Amazon Mechanical Turk from 15 April to 20 April 2020. No additional inclusion or exclusion criteria were used. The University of Nebraska-Lincoln IRB approved the research (IRB protocol #20201020721EX). All participants provided informed consent before participating in the research.

2.1.2. Effects of Exposure to a Fiber Information Prompt on Individuals of Differing Weight Status

To examine how individuals of differing weight status reacted to a fiber information prompt, we randomized participants into control and one of two prompt conditions. The prompt messages varied only in their inclusion/exclusion of second-person pronouns, and did not result in differences in choice behavior [36], so we aggregated them into one condition. The prompt presented information about the health benefits of fiber, comprising weight management, reduction of disease risk, lowering of cholesterol, regulation of the digestive system, blood sugar control, and effects on the gut microbiome. Participants in the PDP condition viewed the PDP just before beginning the shopping task, while control group participants immediately began the shopping task.

2.2. Survey Analysis

We analyzed data using the open-source statistical analysis software, R [39]. We report summary statistics and use multivariate linear regression to analyze the data (and report ordinal regression results in Appendix B, Table A4). The dependent variable in the analysis was the proportion of times a participant chose a consideration set that included the healthiest options (either the healthiest subset itself, or the set that contained all available products). This variable ranged from 0 to 1, calculated by taking the total number of times the individual chose a healthy consideration set divided by the three product categories for which individuals made consideration set decisions. We report the results of a multivariate linear probability model because this model provides estimated coefficients that are directly interpretable as probabilities [40]. In the linear probability model, interaction terms capture differential responses to the prompt by individuals of differing weight status. We report the results of multivariate ordinal logistic regression models in Appendix B, which mirror the linear results in significance and direction.
We report results from six regression models. We first examined the relationship between BMI category and consideration set in order to evaluate whether there were significant relationships between participants’ BMI status and the sets of products they choose to pay attention to. Next, we added an indicator variable capturing whether a participant was exposed to the prompt (Prompt) to examine the impact of the prompt on the probability of examining a consideration set with the healthiest products. Finally, we examined the interaction of BMI category variables with the prompt, to study whether the people in different BMI categories responded differently to the prompt. We report each of these models with and without common demographic variables (gender, age, income, and education) to check the robustness of results to the inclusion of these variables. The inclusion of demographic variables did not affect the estimates of the target independent variables but did require more participants to be dropped from the dataset because of “prefer not to answer” responses, so the number of observations varied slightly between regressions that did and did not include demographic variables. We considered p < 0.05 to be statistically significant.

3. Results

Summary statistics for demographic and weight status variables are presented in Table 1. There were no significant differences between demographic and weight variables in the control and prompt conditions (using a chi-squared test to test for differences in the distribution of females across conditions, and t-tests to examine differences in age, household income, education, and BMI). Approximately 36% of participants were female. The average age of participants was 37 years, and their mean household income was approximately USD 60,000. Participants had received around 16 years of education (approximately equivalent to a bachelor’s degree). The average BMI of participants was 25.5, which is in the “overweight” category.
As we were interested in the relationship between individuals’ weight status and the sets of food items they chose to consider, we examined the distribution of respondents across BMI categories (i.e., normal weight, overweight, and obese) for the control and prompt conditions (Table 2). The distribution of participants among BMI categories was not significantly different between the control and prompt conditions (Chi-squared = 0.387; df = 2; p = 0.82). In both conditions, slightly over 40% of participants were in the normal weight category, slightly over 20% were overweight, and around 35% were obese.
The results of the six regressions examining the relationship between BMI category and consideration set are presented in Table 3. In Regression 1, we found that individuals with BMIs in the obese category were 10% less likely (p < 0.001) to select a consideration set that contained the healthier product options than normal-weight individuals (the omitted weight category in the regression). This result did not change when we included demographic control variables (Regression 2). Individuals who fell into the overweight category did not behave significantly differently from normal-weight individuals.
In Regressions 3 and 4, we included a variable that captured the effects of participants being exposed to the prompt message (along with controlling for demographic variables in Regression 4). Again, we found that obese individuals were 10% less likely to choose a consideration set that contained the healthiest items (p < 0.001) in both regressions. We also found a statistically significant, positive effect of the prompt on the probability of selecting a healthy consideration set. In both regressions, we found that exposure to the prompt increased the probability that an individual would select a consideration set that contained the healthier items by 9% (p < 0.001).
In Regressions 5 and 6, we introduced interaction terms between exposure to the prompt and weight status, in order to examine whether individuals of varying weight status responded differently to the prompt message. We continued to find that obese individuals were around 10% less likely to select a healthy consideration set (p < 0.05), corroborating the results of Regressions 1–4. We found an effect of the prompt that was consistent with the findings in Regressions 3 and 4: exposure to the prompt increased the probability that participants selected healthier consideration sets by 9% (p < 0.05). Furthermore, we did not find statistically significant interactions between weight status and exposure to the prompt. The point estimates of the interaction terms between overweight weight status and the prompt and obese weight status and the prompt were both small—between 1 and 3% in absolute value—and not statistically significant.

4. Discussion

Recent work attempting to untangle the relationship between the food environment and higher average BMIs has demonstrated that eliminating a food desert does not consistently lead to healthier food purchases [17,18,41,42]. While moving from a high-obesity area to a low-obesity area does improve the nutritional quality of a household’s food purchases over time, the effect is relatively small [43], suggesting that demand-side factors play an important role in explaining the lack of healthy food options [20]. In a more controlled setting, laboratory research has documented attentional biases of individuals with high BMI to more indulgent foods and has shown that overweight/obese individuals are willing to pay more for these foods [44,45,46,47,48]. However, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first work that documents systematic differences in choices of which elements of the product environment people want to consider, differentiated by BMI category.
Our research may also have implications for creating study designs that ensure external validity of research findings. Many studies examining nutritional labeling start out in laboratory settings, with simple product choice environments. Frequently, participants will make choices between—or value—two products at a time. However, attention to product attributes may decrease as the number of alternatives to be considered increases [49], meaning that choices made in a simple choice set may yield attention to product attributes that would not be evaluated in a complex choice setting. Disparities found between field and laboratory experiments in the impact of front-of-package labels may be a result of varying the number of items that individuals consider [50,51,52]. Our results have implications for this observation in two ways: First, initial research on interventions meant to promote healthier diets may need to occur in richer choice environments, such as the environment we examine in this research, and in two other recent articles [36,53]. These experiments allow participants to interact with the array of products in a more realistic manner, which may mean choosing not to view some products that are available, including labels/information that may be available on those products. Second, we find that prompts may help redirect the attention of individuals who are more likely to choose to view less healthy products to a healthier set of products—a finding that could not be examined in a simple experimental choice setting.
The results related to the prompt show promise in terms of promoting the consideration of healthier alternatives. Health prompts have been shown to encourage healthier choices in laboratory and field settings [35,36,54,55], and may work in part by recruiting parts of the brain that are important in self-control and accelerating the consideration of health attributes in food choices [54,55]. While we studied a prompt delivered in an online environment, other prompts have been shown to be effective in physical supermarket settings [34,35]. Technological advances—either adopted personally by individuals, or by retailers—may also provide a means to deliver prompts in physical or online retail settings. Some of these capabilities are being developed in the context of mobile health (mHealth) applications [56].
Interestingly, participants across BMI categories responded similarly to the prompt in our study—a finding that differs from the conclusions of a study on health primes by Papies et al. [34]. There are a few differences in the studies that may explain the variation in results. In their study, the authors examined a subtle health prime on a recipe card provided to shoppers (i.e., the recipe was surrounded by words such as “healthy” and “good for your figure”, p. 599. [34]. The findings that overweight and obese individuals reduced unhealthy snack purchases (more than normal-weight individuals) applied only to those individuals who initially paid attention to the prime, whereas our study purposefully drew attention to the prompt message. Differences in the subtlety of the message may have led to different responses. For instance, the individuals in their study who paid attention to the health prime may have been more health-motivated, which other studies have shown to predict both attention to nutrition information and the healthiness of food choices and exercise behaviors [57,58,59,60].
This study does have some potential limitations. While hypothetical choices do not provide the same strength of evidence that real choices do, food choice is so frequent and deeply ingrained that it may be less subject to hypothetical biases than other product types [61]. In fact, recent research shows that food choices made in identical hypothetical and non-hypothetical settings exhibit similar patterns of choice, including the influence of hunger on choices [62]. The BMI characteristics of our sample indicate that we have fewer overweight/obese participants than the average in the US population. This may reflect two things: First, our sample is younger on average than the US adult population, which reduces the prevalence of high BMI [63]. Second, the use of self-reported measures of height and weight has been found in previous studies to lead to underestimates of BMI [64]. In addition, there is evidence that incorporating individuals’ perceived weight status—whether they believe that they are normal weight, overweight, or obese—in addition to BMI-based weight categorizations can shed additional light on food choice behavior [65,66,67,68,69]. Finally, samples drawn from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) are less representative of the US population than consumer panels maintained by survey companies (though more representative than in-person convenience samples) [70]. MTurk samples tend to be younger on average and have a different composition of race/ethnicity than consumer panels [71]. However, both consumer panels and MTurk samples over-represent urban populations [71].
The data for this study were collected during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic, which may have influenced multiple elements of the study: First, the sample in the study featured a higher percentage of males than females (only 36% of respondents were female). This may reflect the childcare burden faced by many women during the pandemic [72]. The pandemic also led many people to try shopping online for groceries for the first time. In our sample, 31% of participants reported shopping online for groceries for the first time during the previous month (only 25% had never shopped for groceries online). Finally, there is also evidence that the pandemic changed people’s eating behavior, with average nutritional quality decreasing with the onset of the pandemic [73].
Our findings suggest that even though BMI status predicts attention to product subsets that differ in nutritional quality, prompts hold promise in improving the quality of the sets that people consider, regardless of weight status. A study design that is specifically powered to examine relationships between BMI, attention to products and information and, ultimately, product choice, would shed important light on links between body weight status and directed attention towards health-ranked consideration sets, as well as the potential for prompt messages to shift attention and behavior towards healthier alternatives.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.R.G.; methodology, K.A., C.R.G. and D.J.R.; formal analysis, C.R.G.; investigation, C.R.G.; resources, C.R.G. and D.J.R.; data curation, K.A. and C.R.G.; writing—original draft preparation, C.R.G.; writing—review and editing, K.A., C.R.G. and D.J.R.; project administration, K.A., C.R.G. and D.J.R.; funding acquisition, C.R.G. and D.J.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project is based on research that was supported by the University of Nebraska-Lincoln Agricultural Research Division Wheat Innovation Fund, and was partially supported by the Nebraska Agricultural Experiment Station, with funding from the Hatch Act (Accession Number 1011290) through the USDA National Institute of Food and Agriculture. The funder had no role in the research or the decision to publish.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. The University of Nebraska-Lincoln IRB approved the research (IRB protocol #20201020721EX).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available from the authors upon request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study, in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data, in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Table A1. Bread products, nutritional and price information, and subsets in the experiment.
Table A1. Bread products, nutritional and price information, and subsets in the experiment.
Bread ProductsCal.FatSodiumFiberSugarPriceSubsetGuiding Stars
Dave’s Killer Bread Good Seed1203160355.99High2
Dave’s Killer Bread Powerseed1002.5135415.99High3
Dave’s Killer Bread Thin Sliced Good Seed701.5115325.99High2
Fiber Up 100% Whole Wheat1101.5220854.49High2
Fiber Up Multigrain1101.5190844.49High2
Oroweat Sandwich Thins 100% Whole Wheat70215021.53.99High2
Pepperidge Farm 100% Whole Wheat1201120344.29High2
Pepperidge Farm Whole Grain 15 Grain1302.5130334.29High2
Thomas’ Light Multi-Grain English Muffin5018540.53.49High2
Thomas’ 100% Whole Wheat English Muffin6011151.50.53.49High3
Udi’s Omega Flax & Fiber75315030.54.79High2
Pepperidge Farm Butter Bread1201210133.99Low0
Pepperidge Farm Hearty White1301230133.99Low0
Sara Lee Artesano Brioche1101.51900.533.69Low0
Sara Lee Artesano Golden Wheat1001.5180133.69Low0
Thomas’ Bagels Blueberry140119514.54.69Low0
Thomas’ Bagels Cinnamon Swirl14011951.55.54.69Low0
Thomas’ Bagels Plain1351225134.69Low0
Thomas’ English Muffin Cinnamon Raisin1500.5180244.49Low0
Thomas’ English Muffin Original750.51200.524.49Low0
Udi’s Gluten-Free Plain Bagel16052951.504.98Low0
Udi’s Gluten-Free White7021350.51.54.98Low0
Dave’s Killer Bread White1102180225.99Medium1
Oroweat Whole Grains 12 Grain1002160323.99Medium1
Oroweat Whole Grains Oatnut1102135233.99Medium1
Sara Lee 100% Whole Wheat601120213.99Medium1
Sara Lee Butter Bread700.5110013.99Medium1
Sara Lee Delightful 45 Calories 100% Whole Wheat450.51001.513.99Medium1
Sara Lee Delightful 45 Calories Multi-Grain450.5851.513.99Medium1
Sara Lee Honey Wheat7011200.513.99Medium1
Thomas’ Bagel 100% Whole Wheat1250.51253.53.54.69Medium1
Thomas’ Bagel Thins Plain550.5105213.99Medium1
Udi’s Gluten-Free Millet-Chia7521502.50.54.79Medium1
Note: Nutritional information was provided on a standardized per-serving basis. Note that while the table presents the subset in which each product was included for participants who chose to see a subset, participants could also choose to view all available products in a particular category. We have also categorized product subsets by relative nutritional quality in this table rather than presenting the text used in the experiment, given the length of the descriptors. The text used in the experiment was (1) “Breads such as Dave’s Killer Powerseed, Fiber Up 100% Whole Wheat, Pepperidge Farm 15 Grain” (=High in this table); (2) “Breads such as Sara Lee 100% Whole Wheat, Thomas’ Bagel Thins Plain, Oroweat Oatnut” (=Medium in this table); and (3) “Breads such as Sara Lee Artesano Golden Wheat, Pepperidge Farm Hearty White, Thomas’ Plain Bagels” (=Low in this table).
Table A2. Cereal products, nutritional and price information, and subsets in the experiment.
Table A2. Cereal products, nutritional and price information, and subsets in the experiment.
CerealsCal.FatSodiumFiberSugarPriceSubsetGuiding Stars
All-Bran Buds1202951294.49High2
Cheerios1402.5190423.49High2
Fiber One Original901.51401404.29High3
Frosted Mini-Wheats Original140110462.88High2
Grape-Nuts1381193533.12High3
Great Grains Raisins Dates Pecans20011505133.18High2
Kashi Berry Fruitful12510463.97High2
Multi-Grain Cheerios1502150483.49High2
Shredded Wheat14010502.88High3
Wheat Chex1421231543.79High2
Wheaties1440.5267464.29High2
Apple Jacks1501.52102133.68Low0
Cap’n Crunch’s Crunch Berries15022700.5162.79Low0
Cookie Crisp15531702133.49Low0
Corn Pops1500.51400123.68Low0
Froot Loops1521.52104143.29Low0
Frosted Flakes14002000.5143.29Low0
Fruity Pebbles15522100132.99Low0
Honey Comb16011901133.19Low0
Lucky Charms15522552133.4Low0
Reese’s Puffs1704.52102122.99Low0
Trix16021801123.46Low0
Crispix1500260053.68Medium1
Corn Flakes1500300143.78Medium1
Golden Grahams16013002123.49Medium1
Oatmeal Squares1502136464.48Medium1
Special K Banana1602.5230393.19Medium1
Special K Blueberry with Lemon Clusters15012603123.19Medium1
Special K Cinnamon Brown Sugar Crunch Protein16012304123.19Medium1
Special K Cinnamon Pecan1602.52803103.19Medium1
Special K Original Protein1421176353.19Medium1
Special K Raspberry1500.52303123.19Medium1
Special K Red Berries1400.52503113.19Medium1
Note: Nutritional information was provided on a standardized per-serving basis. Note that while the table presents the subset in which each product was included for participants who chose to see a subset, participants could also choose to view all available products in a particular category. We have also categorized product subsets by relative nutritional quality in this table rather than presenting the text used in the experiment, given the length of the descriptors. The text used in the experiment was (1) “Cereals such as Cheerios, Wheat Chex, Grape Nuts” (=High in this table); (2) “Cereals such as Corn Flakes, Crispix, Special K” (=Medium in this table); and (3) “Cereals such as Frosted Flakes, Froot Loops, Reese’s Puffs” (=Low in this table).
Table A3. Cracker products, nutritional and price information, and subsets in the experiment.
Table A3. Cracker products, nutritional and price information, and subsets in the experiment.
CrackersCal.FatSodiumFiberSugarPriceSubsetGuiding Stars
Blue Diamond Artisan Nut Thins Flax Seeds1303.5135203.99High2
Farmhouse Cheddar Almond Flour150827010.55.69High2
Farmhouse Sprouted Seed Original1408210305.69High2
Pepperidge Farm Goldfish Baked with Whole Grain1405240202.49High2
Triscuit Balsamic Vinegar & Basil130413030.53.38High2
Triscuit Cracked Pepper and Olive Oil1304.5150303.38High2
Triscuit Original1304.5170403.38High2
Triscuit Reduced Fat Crackers1202.5160403.38High2
Wasa Light Rye670117703.49High3
Wasa Multi-Grain750139603.49High2
Wasa Whole Grain690115703.49High2
Cheez-It Hot & Spicy1508220103.69Low0
Cheez-It Original1508230103.69Low0
Cheez-It Pepper Jack150727010.53.69Low0
Cheez-It White Cheddar1507210103.69Low0
Keebler Cheese & Peanut Butter14572400.533.59Low0
Keebler Club & Cheddar14572400.543.59Low0
Keebler Original Club1506.5268022.99Low0
Keebler Town House Flipside Pretzel Original1407380024.49Low0
Keebler Town House Original1509.5280024.49Low0
Keebler Town House Sea Salt Pita Crackers140527000.54.49Low0
Nabisco Ritz Original Classic1508.5244023.38Low0
Crunchmaster Multi-Grain Sea Salt1203140313.99Medium1
Crunchmaster Multi-Seed Original1405110203.99Medium1
Crunchmaster Multi-Seed Roasted Garlic1405.5135203.99Medium1
Crunchmaster Multi-Seed Rosemary & Olive Oil140590203.99Medium1
Good Thins: The Beet One—Balsamic Vinegar & Sea Salt1304160234.38Medium1
Good Thins: The Cheese One—White Cheddar1304180224.38Medium1
Good Thins: The Potato One—Spinach & Garlic1304190313.38Medium1
Good Thins: The Rice One—Simply Salt1301.585003.38Medium1
Good Thins: The Rice One—Veggie Blend1201.590123.38Medium1
Nabisco Wheat Thins Multigrain1304190233.38Medium1
Pepperidge Farm Goldfish Cheddar14052500.502.49Medium1
Note: Nutritional information was provided on a standardized per-serving basis. Note that while the table presents the subset in which each product was included for participants who chose to see a subset, participants could also choose to view all available products in a particular category. We have also categorized product subsets by relative nutritional quality in this table rather than presenting the text used in the experiment, given the length of the descriptors. The text used in the experiment was (1) “Crackers such as Wasa, Triscuit, Simple Mill Crackers” (=High in this table); (2) “Crackers such as Wheat Thins, Good Thins, Crunchmaster” (=Medium in this table); and (3) “Crackers such as Cheez-It, Ritz, Club Original” (=Low in this table).

Appendix B

Table A4. Ordinal logistic regression model of choosing a consideration set that contains healthier options. Results represent odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals.
Table A4. Ordinal logistic regression model of choosing a consideration set that contains healthier options. Results represent odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals.
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)
Overweight1.13
(0.80, 1.59)
1.05
(0.74, 1.59)
1.11
(0.79, 1.56)
1.03
(0.73, 1.47)
1.24
(0.68, 2.28)
1.11
(0.60, 2.05)
Obese0.59
(0.44, 0.79)
0.59
(0.43, 0.79)
0.59
(0.44, 0.79)
0.59
(0.44, 0.80)
0.58
(0.35, 0.95)
0.55
(0.33, 0.91)
Prompt 1.60
(1.22, 2.11)
1.65
(1.25, 2.18)
1.64
(1.08, 2.50)
1.62
(1.06, 2.48)
Overweight × Prompt 0.85
(0.41, 1.77)
0.91
(0.43, 1.89)
Obese × Prompt 1.03
(0.56, 1.91)
1.11
(0.60, 2.08)
Demographic Controls?NoYesNoYesNoYes
N749739749739749739
AIC2039.12004.22029.71993.52033.41997.2

References

  1. GBD 2015 Obesity Collaborators; Afshin, A.; Forouzanfar, M.H.; Reitsma, M.B.; Sur, P.; Estep, K.; Lee, A.; Marczak, L.; Mokdad, A.H.; Moradi-Lakeh, M.; et al. Health effects of overweight and obesity in 195 countries over 25 years. N. Engl. J. Med. 2017, 377, 13–27. [Google Scholar]
  2. Harper, S.; Riddell, C.A.; King, N.B. Declining life expectancy in the United States: Missing the trees for the forest. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2020, 42, 381–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Mehta, N.K.; Abrams, L.R.; Myrskylä, M. US life expectancy stalls due to cardiovascular disease, not drug deaths. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 6998–7000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Preston, S.H.; Vierboom, Y.; Stokes, A. The role of obesity in exceptionally slow US mortality improvement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 957–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Adult Obesity Facts. Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/obesity/data/adult.html (accessed on 17 November 2020).
  6. Cawley, J. The impact of obesity on wages. J. Hum. Resour. 2004, 39, 451–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Finkelstein, E.; Fiebelkorn, L.C.; Wang, G. The costs of obesity among full-time employees. Am. J. Health Promot. 2005, 20, 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Finkelstein, E.A.; Trogdon, J.G.; Cohen, J.W.; Dietz, W. Annual medical spending attributable to obesity: Payer and service-specific estimates. Health Aff. 2009, 28, w822–w831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Hales, C.M.; Fryar, C.D.; Carroll, M.D.; Freedman, D.S.; Ogden, C.L. Trends in obesity and severe obesity prevalence in US youth and adults by sex and age, 2007–2008 to 2015–2016. JAMA 2018, 319, 1723–1725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Pontzer, H.; Raichlen, D.A.; Wood, B.M.; Mabulla, A.Z.P.; Racette, S.; Marlowe, F.W. Hunter-gatherer energetics and human obesity. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e40503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Urlacher, S.S.; Snodgrass, J.J.; Dugas, L.R.; Sugiyama, L.S.; Liebert, M.A.; Joyce, C.J.; Pontzer, H. Constraint and trade-offs regulate energy expenditure during childhood. Sci. Adv. 2019, 5, eaax1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Urlacher, S.S.; Snodgrass, J.J.; Dugas, L.R.; Madimenos, F.C.; Sugiyama, L.S.; Liebert, M.A.; Joyce, C.J.; Terán, E.; Pontzer, H. Childhood daily energy expenditure does not decrease with market integration and is not related to adiposity in Amazonia. J. Nutr. 2021, 151, 695–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Hall, K.D.; Ayuketah, A.; Brychta, R.; Cai, H.; Cassimatis, T.; Chen, K.; Chung, S.T.; Costa, E.; Courville, A.; Darcey, V.; et al. Ultra-processed diets cause excess calorie intake and weight gain: An inpatient randomized controlled trial of ad libitum food intake. Cell Metab. 2019, 30, 67–77.e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Cummins, S. Food deserts. In The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia of Health, Illness, Behavior, and Society; Cockerham, W.C., Dingwall, R., Quah, S., Eds.; John Wiley and Sons: Chichester, UK, 2014; Volume 6, pp. 562–564. ISBN 9781118410868. [Google Scholar]
  15. Chen, D.; Jaenicke, E.C.; Volpe, R.J. Food environments and obesity: Household diet expenditure versus food deserts. Am. J. Public Health 2016, 106, 881–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Ver Ploeg, M.; Breneman, V.; Farrigan, T.; Hamrick, K.; Hopkins, D.; Kaufman, P.; Lin, B.-H.; Nord, M.; Smith, T.A.; Williams, R.; et al. Access to Affordable and Nutritious Food-Measuring and Understanding Food Deserts and Their Consequences: Report to Congress. Available online: https://www.ers.usda.gov/publications/pub-details/?pubid=42729 (accessed on 4 March 2021).
  17. Cummins, S.; Flint, E.; Matthews, S. New neighborhood grocery store increased awareness of food access but did not alter dietary habits or obesity. Health Aff. 2014, 33, 283–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Dubowitz, T.; Ghosh-Dastidar, M.; Cohen, D.A.; Beckman, R.; Steiner, E.D.; Hunter, G.P.; Flórez, K.; Huang, C.; Vaughan, C.A.; Sloan, J.C.; et al. Diet and perceptions change with supermarket introduction in a food desert, but not because of supermarket use. Health Aff. 2015, 34, 1858–1868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Ghosh-Dastidar, M.; Hunter, G.; Collins, R.L.; Zenk, S.N.; Cummins, S.; Beckman, R.; Nugroho, A.K.; Sloan, J.C.; Wagner, L.; Dubowitz, T. Does opening a supermarket in a food desert change the food environment? Health Place 2017, 46, 249–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Allcott, H.; Diamond, R.; Dubé, J.-P.; Handbury, J.; Rahkovsky, I.; Schnell, M. Food deserts and the causes of nutritional inequality. Q. J. Econ. 2019, 134, 1793–1844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Botti, S.; Iyengar, S.S. The dark side of choice: When choice impairs social welfare. J. Public Policy Mark. 2006, 25, 24–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Zizzo, D.J.; Parravano, M.; Nakamura, R.; Forwood, S.; Suhrcke, M. The impact of taxation and signposting on diet: An online field study with breakfast cereals and soft drinks. Exp. Econ. 2021, 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Hauser, J.R.; Wernerfelt, B. An evaluation cost model of consideration sets. J. Consum. Res. 1990, 16, 393–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Roberts, J.H.; Lattin, J.M. Development and testing of a model of consideration set composition. J. Mark. Res. 1991, 28, 429–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Honka, E.; Hortaçsu, A.; Wildenbeest, M. Chapter 4—Empirical search and consideration sets. In Handbook of the Economics of Marketing; Dubé, J.-P., Rossi, P.E., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2019; Volume 1, pp. 193–257. [Google Scholar]
  26. Machín, L.; Curutchet, M.R.; Gugliucci, V.; Vitola, A.; Otterbring, T.; de Alcantara, M.; Ares, G. The habitual nature of food purchases at the supermarket: Implications for policy making. Appetite 2020, 155, 104844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Caplin, A.; Dean, M.; Leahy, J. Rational inattention, optimal consideration sets, and stochastic choice. Rev. Econ. Stud. 2019, 86, 1061–1094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Masatlioglu, Y.; Nakajima, D.; Ozbay, E.Y. Revealed attention. Am. Econ. Rev. 2012, 102, 2183–2205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Haws, K.L.; Reczek, R.W.; Sample, K.L. Healthy diets make empty wallets: The healthy = expensive intuition. J. Consum. Res. 2016, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Jo, J.; Lusk, J.L. If it’s healthy, it’s tasty and expensive: Effects of nutritional labels on price and taste expectations. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 68, 332–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Lusk, J.L. Consumer beliefs about healthy foods and diets. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0223098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Nansel, T.R.; Lipsky, L.M.; Eisenberg, M.H.; Liu, A.; Mehta, S.N.; Laffel, L.M. Can families eat better without spending more? Improving diet quality does not increase diet cost in a randomized clinical trial among youth with type 1 diabetes and their parents. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2016, 116, 1751–1759.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Elbel, B.; Kersh, R.; Brescoll, V.L.; Dixon, L.B. Calorie labeling and food choices: A first look at the effects on low-income people in New York City: Calorie information on menus appears to increase awareness of calorie content, but not necessarily the number of calories people purchase. Health Aff. 2009, 28, w1110–w1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Papies, E.K.; Potjes, I.; Keesman, M.; Schwinghammer, S.; van Koningsbruggen, G. Using health primes to reduce unhealthy snack purchases among overweight consumers in a grocery store. Int. J. Obes. 2013, 38, 597–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Gustafson, C.R.; Kent, R.; Prate, M.R., Jr. Retail-based healthy food point-of-decision prompts (PDPs) increase healthy food choices in a rural, low-income, minority community. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0207792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Arslain, K.; Gustafson, C.R.; Rose, D.J. Point-of-decision prompts increase dietary fiber content of consumers’ food choices in an online grocery shopping simulation. Nutrients 2020, 12, 3487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Zhao, A.W.; McGowan, C.C.; Zenk, S.N.; Kershaw, K.N. Associations of the consumer food environment with eating behaviours and BMI. Public Health Nutr. 2020, 23, 3197–3203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Bello, M.; Abdulai, A. Impact of ex-ante hypothetical bias mitigation methods on attribute non-attendance in choice experiments. Am. J. Agric. Econ. 2016, 98, 1486–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Bunn, A.; Korpela, M. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  40. McKenna, R.M.; Langellier, B.A.; Alcalá, H.E.; Roby, D.H.; Grande, D.T.; Ortega, A.N. The affordable care act attenuates financial strain according to poverty level. Inq. J. Health Care Organ. Provis. Financ. 2018, 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Dubowitz, T.; Ncube, C.; Leuschner, K.; Tharp-Gilliam, S. A natural experiment opportunity in two low-income urban food desert communities: Research design, community engagement methods, and baseline results. Health Educ. Behav. 2015, 42, 87S–96S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Vaughan, C.A.; Cohen, D.A.; Ghosh-Dastidar, M.; Hunter, G.P.; Dubowitz, T. Where do food desert residents buy most of their junk food? Supermarkets. Public Health Nutr. 2016, 20, 2608–2616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Hut, S. Determinants of dietary choice in the US: Evidence from consumer migration. J. Health Econ. 2020, 72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Aviram-Friedman, R.; Astbury, N.; Ochner, C.N.; Contento, I.; Geliebter, A. Neurobiological evidence for attention bias to food, emotional dysregulation, disinhibition and deficient somatosensory awareness in obesity with binge eating disorder. Physiol. Behav. 2018, 184, 122–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Nijs, I.M.T.; Franken, I.H.A. Attentional processing of food cues in overweight and obese individuals. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2012, 1, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Werthmann, J.; Roefs, A.; Nederkoorn, C.; Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P.; Jansen, A. Can(not) take my eyes off it: Attention bias for food in overweight participants. Health Psychol. 2011, 30, 561–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Just, D.R.; Wansink, B. One man’s tall is another man’s small: How the framing of portion size influences food choice. Health Econ. 2013, 23, 776–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Werthmann, J.; Jansen, A.; Roefs, A. Worry or craving? A selective review of evidence for food-related attention biases in obese individuals, eating-disorder patients, restrained eaters and healthy samples. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2014, 74, 99–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Meißner, M.; Oppewal, H.; Huber, J. Surprising adaptivity to set size changes in multi-attribute repeated choice tasks. J. Bus. Res. 2020, 111, 163–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Dubois, P.; Albuquerque, P.; Allais, O.; Bonnet, C.; Bertail, P.; Combris, P.; Lahlou, S.; Rigal, N.; Ruffieux, B.; Chandon, P. Effects of front-of-pack labels on the nutritional quality of supermarket food purchases: Evidence from a large-scale randomized controlled trial. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2020, 49, 119–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Gustafson, C.R.; Zeballos, E. The effect of ingredient-specific calorie information on calories ordered. Prev. Med. Rep. 2018, 12, 186–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Streletskaya, N.A.; Amatyakul, W.; Rusmevichientong, P.; Kaiser, H.M.; Liaukonyte, J. Menu-labeling formats and their impact on dietary quality. Agribusiness 2015, 32, 175–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Arslain, K.; Gustafson, C.R.; Rose, D.J. The effect of health prompts on product consideration, attention to information, and choice in large, online product assortments: The case of fiber. Food Qual. Prefer. 2021, 104329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hare, T.A.; Malmaud, J.; Rangel, A. Focusing attention on the health aspects of foods changes value signals in vmPFC and improves dietary choice. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 11077–11087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Lim, S.-L.; Penrod, M.T.; Ha, O.-R.; Bruce, J.M.; Bruce, A.S. Calorie labeling promotes dietary self-control by shifting the temporal dynamics of health- and taste-attribute integration in overweight individuals. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 29, 447–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Silva, B.M.; Rodrigues, J.J.; de la Torre Díez, I.; López-Coronado, M.; Saleem, K. Mobile-health: A review of current state in 2015. J. Biomed. Inform. 2015, 56, 265–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Moorman, C. A quasi experiment to assess the consumer and informational determinants of nutrition information processing activities: The case of the nutrition labeling and education act. J. Public Policy Mark. 1996, 15, 28–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Visschers, V.; Hess, R.; Siegrist, M. Health motivation and product design determine consumers’ visual attention to nutrition information on food products. Public Health Nutr. 2010, 13, 1099–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Turner, M.M.; Skubisz, C.; Pandya, S.P.; Silverman, M.; Austin, L. Predicting visual attention to nutrition information on food products: The influence of motivation and ability. J. Health Commun. 2014, 19, 1017–1029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Naughton, P.; McCarthy, S.N.; McCarthy, M.B. The creation of a healthy eating motivation score and its association with food choice and physical activity in a cross-sectional sample of Irish adults. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2015, 12, 74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Rangel, A. Regulation of dietary choice by the decision-making circuitry. Nat. Neurosci. 2013, 16, 1717–1724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. De-Magistris, T.; López-Galán, B.; Ballco, P. Do virtual reality experiments replicate projection bias phenomena? Examining the external validity of a virtual supermarket. J. Agric. Econ. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Fryar, C.D.; Carroll, M.D.; Afful, J. Prevalence of overweight, obesity, and extreme obesity among adults aged 20 and over: United States, 1960–1962 through 2017–2018; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC): Atlanta, GA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  64. Gorber, S.C.; Tremblay, M.; Moher, D.; Gorber, B. A comparison of direct vs. self-report measures for assessing height, weight and body mass index: A systematic review. Obes. Rev. 2007, 8, 307–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Mbarushimana, J.-C.; Gustafson, C.; Gitungwa, H.; Zeballos, E. The relationship between bodyweight status and weight perception explains differences in calories ordered in a food choice exercise. Nutrients 2021, 13, 1794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Wharton, C.M.; Adams, T.; Hampl, J. Weight loss practices and body weight perceptions among US college students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2008, 56, 579–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Lemon, S.C.; Rosal, M.C.; Zapka, J.; Borg, A.; Andersen, V. Contributions of weight perceptions to weight loss attempts: Differences by body mass index and gender. Body Image 2009, 6, 90–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Chen, H.-Y.; Lemon, S.C.; Pagoto, S.L.; Barton, B.A.; Lapane, K.L.; Goldberg, R.J. Personal and parental weight misperception and self-reported attempted weight loss in US children and adolescents, national health and nutrition examination survey, 2007–2008 and 2009–2010. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2014, 11, E132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. Yaemsiri, S.; Slining, M.M.; Agarwal, S.K. Perceived weight status, overweight diagnosis, and weight control among US adults: The NHANES 2003–2008 study. Int. J. Obes. 2010, 35, 1063–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Berinsky, A.J.; Huber, G.A.; Lenz, G.S. Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s mechanical Turk. Political Anal. 2012, 20, 351–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Huff, C.; Tingley, D. “Who are these people?” Evaluating the demographic characteristics and political preferences of MTurk survey respondents. Res. Politics 2015, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Sevilla, A.; Smith, S. Baby steps: The gender division of childcare during the COVID-19 pandemic. Oxf. Rev. Econ. Policy 2020, 36, S169–S186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Marty, L.; de Lauzon-Guillain, B.; Labesse, M.; Nicklaus, S. Food choice motives and the nutritional quality of diet during the COVID-19 lockdown in France. Appetite 2021, 157, 105005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample population a.
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample population a.
ControlPrompt
Female (%)36%35%
Age (Years)37.2 (10.5)36.6 (10.4)
Household Income (USD 10,000 s)61.9 (28.9)59.6 (28.5)
Education (Years)15.9 (2.1)15.8 (2.0)
BMI25.5 (5.9)25.5 (6.9)
N253500
a Mean (standard error).
Table 2. Percentage of participants in each BMI category by condition.
Table 2. Percentage of participants in each BMI category by condition.
CategoryNormal WeightOverweightObese
Control42.8%20.4%36.8%
Prompt43.0%22.1%34.9%
Notes: We omit individuals who did not submit height and/or weight data, preventing us from calculating BMI (n = 4: three in Control; one in Prompt).
Table 3. Linear probability model of choosing a consideration set that contains healthier options.
Table 3. Linear probability model of choosing a consideration set that contains healthier options.
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)
Intercept0.53 ***
(0.02)
0.69 ***
(0.03)
0.47 ***
(0.03)
0.62 ***
(0.11)
0.47 ***
(0.03)
0.62 ***
(0.11)
Overweight0.02
(0.03)
0.01
(0.03)
0.02
(0.03)
0.01
(0.03)
0.04
(0.06)
0.02
(0.06)
Obese−0.10 ***
(0.03)
−0.10 ***
(0.03)
−0.10 ***
(0.03)
−0.10 ***
(0.03)
−0.10 *
(0.05)
−0.11 *
(0.05)
Prompt 0.09 ***
(0.03)
0.09 ***
(0.03)
0.09 *
(0.04)
0.09 *
(0.04)
Overweight × Prompt −0.03
(0.07)
−0.02
(0.07)
Obese × Prompt 0.01
(0.06)
0.02
(0.06)
Demographic ControlsNoYesNoYesNoYes
N749739749739749739
Adj. R20.0210.0360.0340.0500.0320.048
Notes: Estimate (standard error); * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001. The models are linear probability models regressing the choice of a consideration set containing the healthier options (1 if yes, 0 if no) on the independent variables listed. Demographic control variables are female (1 if yes), age (numeric, in years), income (numeric in USD 1000s), and education (numeric, in years).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gustafson, C.R.; Arslain, K.; Rose, D.J. High BMI Predicts Attention to Less Healthy Product Sets: Can a Prompt Lead to Consideration of Healthier Sets of Products? Nutrients 2021, 13, 2620. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13082620

AMA Style

Gustafson CR, Arslain K, Rose DJ. High BMI Predicts Attention to Less Healthy Product Sets: Can a Prompt Lead to Consideration of Healthier Sets of Products? Nutrients. 2021; 13(8):2620. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13082620

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gustafson, Christopher R., Kristina Arslain, and Devin J. Rose. 2021. "High BMI Predicts Attention to Less Healthy Product Sets: Can a Prompt Lead to Consideration of Healthier Sets of Products?" Nutrients 13, no. 8: 2620. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13082620

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop