Impact of Beef and Beef Product Intake on Cognition in Children and Young Adults: A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Methods
2.1. Study Selection Criteria
2.2. Search Strategy
2.3. Data Extraction and Synthesis
2.4. Study Quality Assessment
3. Results
3.1. Study Selection
3.2. Basic Characteristics of the Included Studies
3.3. Study Quality Assessment
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
References
- Black, R.E.; Victora, C.G.; Walker, S.P.; Bhutta, Z.A.; Christian, P.; De Onis, M.; Ezzati, M.; Grantham-McGregor, S.; Katz, J.; Martorell, R.; et al. Maternal and child undernutrition and overweight in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet 2013, 382, 427–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramakrishnan, U. Prevalence of micronutrient malnutrition worldwide. Nutr. Rev. 2002, 60, S46–S52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kihlstrom, J.F.; Park, L. Cognitive psychology: Overview. In Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology; Elsevier: Oakland, CA, USA, 2018; pp. 480–482. [Google Scholar]
- Allen, L.H. Malnutrition and human function: A comparison of conclusions from the incap and nutrition crsp studies. J. Nutr. 1995, 125, 1119S–1126S. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Grantham-McGregor, S. A review of studies of the effect of severe malnutrition on mental development. J. Nutr. 1995, 125, 2233S–2238S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bailey, R.L.; West, K.P.; Black, R.E. The epidemiology of global micronutrient deficiencies. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2015, 66, 22–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beard, J.L.; Li, N.-Q.; Piñero, D.J.; Connor, J.R. Variations in dietary iron alter brain iron metabolism in developing rats. J. Nutr. 2000, 130, 254–263. [Google Scholar]
- Seshadri, S.; Gopaldas, T. Impact of iron supplementation on cognitive functions in preschool and school-aged children-the indian experience. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1989, 50, 675–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peirano, P.D.; Algarín, C.R.; Garrido, M.I.; Lozoff, B. Iron deficiency anemia in infancy is associated with altered temporal organization of sleep states in childhood. Pediatr. Res. 2007, 62, 715–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Black, M.M. Effects of B-12 and folate deficiency on brain development in children. Food Nutr. Bull. 2008, 29, S126–S131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Healton, E.B.; Savage, D.G.; Brust, J.C.M.; Garrett, T.J.; Lindenbaum, J. Neurologic aspects of cobalamin deficiency. Medicine 1991, 70, 229–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rasmussen, S.A.; Fernhoff, P.M.; Scanlon, K.S. Vitamin B12 deficiency in children and adolescents. J. Pediatr. 2001, 138, 10–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kar, B.R.; Rao, S.L.; Chandramouli, B.A.J.B.; Functions, B. Cognitive development in children with chronic protein energy malnutrition. Behav. Brain Funct. 2008, 4, 31–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Felt, B.; Lozoff, B.; Beard, J.; Connor, J.; Georgieff, M.; Schallert, T. Long-lasting neural and behavioral effects of iron deficiency in infancy. Nutr. Rev. 2006, 64, S34–S43. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmermann, M.B. The effects of iodine deficiency in pregnancy and infancy. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2012, 26, 108–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lukowski, A.F.; Koss, M.; Burden, M.J.; Jonides, J.; Nelson, C.A.; Kaciroti, N.; Jimenez, E.; Lozoff, B. Iron deficiency in infancy and neurocognitive functioning at 19 years: Evidence of long-term deficits in executive function and recognition memory. Nutr. Neurosci. 2010, 13, 54–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Black, M.M. Micronutrient deficiencies and cognitive functioning. J. Nutr. 2003, 133, 3927S–3931S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kolasa, K. Diet and nutrition in dementia and cognitive decline. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2015, 47, 402.e7–402.e8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grantham-McGregor, S.; Cheung, Y.B.; Cueto, S.; Glewwe, P.; Richter, L.; Strupp, B. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet 2007, 369, 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Eaton, J.C.; Rothpletz-Puglia, P.; Dreker, M.R.; Iannotti, L.; Lutter, C.; Kaganda, J.; Rayco-Solon, P. Effectiveness of provision of animal-source foods for supporting optimal growth and development in children 6 to 59 months of age. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2019, 2, 1–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walker, A.R.P.; Walker, B.F.; Glatthaar, I.I. Fiber, phytic acid, and mineral metabolism. Nutr. Rev. 1992, 50, 246–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bwibo, N.O.; Neumann, C.G. The need for animal source foods by Kenyan children. J. Nutr. 2003, 133, 3936S–3940S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, V.R.; Marchini, J.S.; Cortiella, J. Assessment of protein nutritional status. J. Nutr. 1990, 120, 1496–1502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neumann, C.; Bwibo, N.O.; Gewa, C.; Drorbaugh, N. Animal source foods as a food-based approach to improve diet and nutrition outcomes. In Improving Diets and Nutrition: Food-Based Approaches; Thompson, B., Amoroso, L., Eds.; The Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations and CABI: Rome, Italy, 2010; pp. 137–157. [Google Scholar]
- Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bethesda (MD): National Center for Biotechnology Information (US). Available online: www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK3827/ (accessed on 10 February 2019).
- Whaley, S.E.; Sigman, M.; Neumann, C.; Bwibo, N.; Guthrie, D.; Weiss, R.E.; Alber, S.; Murphy, S.P. The impact of dietary intervention on the cognitive development of Kenyan school children. J. Nutr. 2003, 133, 3965S–3971S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neumann, C.G.; Murphy, S.P.; Gewa, C.; Grillenberger, M.; Bwibo, N.O. Meat supplementation improves growth, cognitive, and behavioral outcomes in Kenyan children. J. Nutr. 2007, 137, 1119–1123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gewa, C.A.; Weiss, R.E.; Bwibo, N.O.; Whaley, S.; Sigman, M.; Murphy, S.P.; Harrison, G.; Neumann, C.G. Dietary micronutrients are associated with higher cognitive function gains among primary school children in rural Kenya. Br. J. Nutr. 2009, 101, 1378–1387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hulett, J.L.; Weiss, R.E.; Bwibo, N.O.; Galal, O.M.; Drorbaugh, N.; Neumann, C.G. Animal source foods have a positive impact on the primary school test scores of Kenyan schoolchildren in a cluster-randomised, controlled feeding intervention trial. Br. J. Nutr. 2014, 111, 875–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI). Quality Assessment of Controlled Intervention Studies. Available online: https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools (accessed on 12 February 2019).
- Coleman, M.S.; Bednar, C.M.; King, C.C.; Alford, B.B. Comparison of food intake of pureed beef products by institutionalized elderly subjects. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1995, 95, A60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Magnusson, A. Do beef-eaters have an increased risk of dementia and early death? J. Public Health Med. 1997, 19, 476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Linseisen, J.; Kesse, E.; Slimani, N.; Bueno-De-Mesquita, H.B.; Ocke, M.C.; Skeie, G.; Kumle, M.; Dorronsoro Iraeta, M.; Morote Gomez, P.; Janzon, L.; et al. Meat consumption in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC) cohorts: Results from 24-hour dietary recalls. Public Health Nutr. 2002, 5, 1243–1258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, S.P.; Allen, L.H. Nutritional importance of animal source foods. J. Nutr. 2003, 133, 3932S–3935S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, S.P.; Gewa, C.; Liang, L.J.; Grillenberger, M.; Bwibo, N.O.; Neumann, C.G. School snacks containing animal source foods improve dietary quality for children in rural Kenya. J. Nutr. 2003, 133, 3950S–3956S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siekmann, J.H.; Allen, L.H.; Bwibo, N.O.; Demment, M.W.; Murphy, S.P.; Neumann, C.G. Kenyan school children have multiple micronutrient deficiencies, but increased plasma vitamin B-12 is the only detectable micronutrient response to meat or milk supplementation. J. Nutr. 2003, 133, 3972S–3980S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gao, X.; Chen, H.; Fung, T.T.; Logroscino, G.; Schwarzschild, M.A.; Hu, F.B.; Ascherio, A. Prospective study of dietary pattern and risk of Parkinson disease. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007, 86, 1486–1494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Murphy, S.P.; Gewa, C.; Grillenberger, M.; Bwibo, N.O.; Neumann, C.G. Designing snacks to address micronutrient deficiencies in rural Kenyan schoolchildren. J. Nutr. 2007, 137, 1093–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pakseresht, M.; Lang, R.; Rittmueller, S.; Roache, C.; Sheehy, T.; Batal, M.; Corriveau, A.; Sharma, S. Food expenditure patterns in the Canadian Arctic show cause for concern for obesity and chronic disease. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2014, 11, 94–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steenbruggen, T.G.; Hoekstra, S.J.; van der Gaag, E.J. Could a change in diet revitalize children who suffer from unresolved fatigue? Nutrients 2015, 7, 1965–1977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hanson, J.A.; Lin, Y.-H.; Dretsch, M.N.; Strandjord, S.E.; Haub, M.D.; Hibbeln, J.R. Whole food, functional food, and supplement sources of omega-3 fatty acids and omega-3 HUFA scores among US soldiers. J. Funct. Foods. 2016, 23, 167–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mischley, L.K.; Lau, R.C.; Bennett, R.D. Role of diet and nutritional supplements in Parkinson's disease progression. Oxid. Med. Cell Longev. 2017, 9, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sauerbier, A.; Schrag, A.; Martinez-Martin, P.; Hall, L.J.; Parry, M.; Mischley, L.K.; Zis, P.; Chaudhuri, K.R. Dietary variations in a multiethnic Parkinson’s disease cohort and possible influences on nonmotor aspects: A cross-sectional multicentre study. Parkinsons Dis. 2018, 12, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rahman, A.; Sawyer Baker, P.; Allman, R.M.; Zamrini, E. Dietary factors and cognitive impairment in community-dwelling elderly. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2007, 11, 49–54. [Google Scholar]
- Logroscino, G.; Gao, X.; Chen, H.; Wing, A.; Ascherio, A. Dietary iron intake and risk of Parkinson’s disease. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2008, 168, 1381–1388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sánchez-Rodríguez, M.A.; Arronte-Rosales, A.; Mendoza-Núñez, V.M. Effect of a self-care program on oxidative stress and cognitive function in an older Mexican urban-dwelling population. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2009, 13, 791–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dror, D.K.; Allen, L.H. The importance of milk and other animal-source foods for children in low-income countries. Food Nutr. Bull. 2011, 32, 227–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Okubo, H.; Murakami, K.; Inagaki, H.; Gondo, Y.; Ikebe, K.; Kamide, K.; Masui, Y.; Arai, Y.; Ishizaki, T.; Sasaki, S.; et al. Hardness of the habitual diet and its relationship with cognitive function among 70-year-old Japanese elderly: Findings from the SONIC Study. J. Oral. Rehabil. 2019, 46, 151–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNeill, S.; Lofgren, P.; Van Elswyk, M. The role of lean beef in healthful dietary patterns. Nutr. Today 2013, 48, 181–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montgomery, S.C.; Streit, S.M.; Beebe, M.L.; Maxwell Iv, P.J. Micronutrient needs of the elderly. Nutr. Clin. Pract. 2014, 29, 435–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Agim, Z.S.; Cannon, J.R. Dietary factors in the etiology of Parkinson’s disease. Biomed. Res. Int. 2015, 2014, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bwibo, N.O.; Neumann, C.G. Animal Source Food Snacks Improve School Test Scores. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2009, 55, 635. [Google Scholar]
- Yokoi, K.; Suzuki, Y.; Konomi, A.; Esashi, T. A preliminary report: Effect of supplemental beef product on cognitive performance in premenopausal women. Faseb. J. 2005, 19, A457. [Google Scholar]
- Nct. The Consumption of Beef on Appetite and Cognitive Function. Available online: https://clinicaltrials.gov/show/nct02614729 (accessed on 12 February 2019).
- Krebs, N.F.; Westcott, J.E.; Butler, T.; Robinson, C.; Bell, M.; Hambidge, K.M. Meat as a first complementary food for breastfed infants: Feasibility and impact on zinc intake and status. J. Pediatr. Gastr. Nutr. 2006, 42, 207–214. [Google Scholar]
- Krebs, N.F.; Mazariegos, M.; Chomba, E.; Sami, N.; Pasha, O.; Tshefu, A.; Carlo, W.A.; Goldenberg, R.L.; Bose, C.L.; Wright, L.L.; et al. Randomized controlled trial of meat compared with multimicronutrient-fortified cereal in infants and toddlers with high stunting rates in diverse settings. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2012, 96, 840–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blanton, C. Improvements in iron status and cognitive function in young women consuming beef or non-beef lunches. Nutrients 2014, 6, 90–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loo, K.K.; Rizzo, S.; Qiaolin, C.; Weiss, R.E.; Sugar, C.A.; Ettyang, G.; Ernst, J.; Samari, G.; Neumann, C.G. Effects of biscuit-type feeding supplementation on the neurocognitive outcomes of HIV-affected school-age children: A randomized, double-blind, controlled intervention trial in Kenya. Fam. Med. Commun. Health 2017, 5, 245–258. [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, R.S. Content and bioavailability of trace elements in vegetarian diets. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1994, 59, 1223S–1232S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramakrishna, T. Vitamins and brain development. Physiol. Res. 1999, 48, 175–187. [Google Scholar]
- Shipton, M.J.; Thachil, J. Vitamin B12 deficiency—A 21st century perspective. Clin. Med. 2015, 15, 145–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van de Rest, O.; Van Hooijdonk, L.W.A.; Doets, E.; Schiepers, O.J.G.; Eilander, A.; De Groot, L.C.P.G.M. B Vitamins and n-3 fatty acids for brain development and function: Review of human studies. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2012, 60, 272–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wenger, M.J.; Murray-Kolb, L.E.; Haas, J.D. Variations in body iron status determine variations in body energy expenditure and brain dynamics as a function of perceptual and cognitive workload. FASEB J. 2013, 27, 814–840. [Google Scholar]
- Wenger, M.J.; Scott, S.P.; Murray-Kolb, L.E.; Ghugre, P.; Udipi, S.; Haas, J.D. Brain dynamics as a function of iron status: Relating electroencephalographic (EEG) patterns and body iron measures in Indian adolescents. FASEB J. 2013, 27, 845–846. [Google Scholar]
- Kececi, H.; Degirmenci, Y. Quantitative EEG and cognitive evoked potentials in anemia. Neurophysiol. Clin. 2008, 38, 137–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Khedr, E.; Hamed, S.A.; Elbeih, E.; El-Shereef, H.; Ahmad, Y.; Ahmed, S.J.E.A.O.P.; Neuroscience, C. Iron states and cognitive abilities in young adults: Neuropsychological and neurophysiological assessment. Eur. Arch. Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 2008, 258, 489–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snetselaar, L.; Stumbo, P.; Chenard, C.; Ahrens, L.; Smith, K.; Zimmerman, B. Adolescents eating diets rich in either lean beef or lean poultry and fish reduced fat and saturated fat intake and those eating beef maintained serum ferritin status. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2004, 104, 424–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Navas-Carretero, S.; Pérez-Granados, A.M.; Schoppen, S.; Sarria, B.; Carbajal, A.; Vaquero, M.P. Iron status biomarkers in iron deficient women consuming oily fish versus red meat diet. J. Physiol. Biochem. 2009, 65, 165–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lee, Y.-B.; Lee, H.J.; Sohn, H.S. Soy isoflavones and cognitive function. J. Nutr. Biochem. 2005, 16, 641–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sakai, T.; Kogiso, M. Soy isoflavones and immunity. J. Med. Invest. 2008, 55, 167–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Masilamani, M.; Wei, J.; Sampson, H.A.J.I.R. Regulation of the immune response by soybean isoflavones. Immunol. Res. 2012, 54, 95–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adams, S.M.; Aksenova, M.V.; Aksenov, M.Y.; Mactutus, C.F.; Booze, R.M. Soy isoflavones genistein and daidzein exert anti-apoptotic actions via a selective ER-mediated mechanism in neurons following HIV-1 Tat1–86 exposure. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e37540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Study ID | First Author (year) | Country | Sample Size 1 | Age at Baseline | Feeding Frequency | Intervention Duration (month) | Follow-Up (year) | No. of Repeated Measures | Statistical Model | Attrition Rate |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | Whaley (2003) [27] | Kenya | 555 a | 6–14 years | Every school day | 21 | 2.5 | 4 | Linear hierarchical regression | 8.6% |
2 | Krebs (2006) [56] | USA | 88 b | 5 months | Monthly supply of complementary food | 2 | 0.8 | 3 | Hierarchical regression | 18.0% |
3 | Neumann (2007) [28] | Kenya | Cohort I: 525 Cohort II: 375a | 6–14 years | Every school day | 21 | 2.3 | 4 | Linear hierarchical regression | 8.6% |
4 | Gewa (2009) [29] | Kenya | 554 a | 7 years | Every school day | 24 | 2.0 | 4 | Panel data model | 4.5% |
5 | Krebs (2012) [57] | Guatemala, Pakistan, DR Congo, Zambia | 1062 c | 6 months | Daily | 12 | 1.0 | 2 | Linear regression | 14.1% |
6 | Blanton (2014) [58] | USA | 43 d | 21 years | Three times a week | 4 | 0.3 | 2 | Mixed effects regression | 23.0% |
7 | Hulett (2014) [30] | Kenya | 360 e | 7 years | Every school day | 18 | 1.6 | 5 | Panel data model | 3.5% |
8 | Loo (2017) [59] | Kenya | 49 f | 4-8 years | Five days a week | 18 | 2.0 | 4 | Mixed effects regression | 22.4% |
Intervention Arms | Type/Quantity of Feeding | Feeding Specifics | Cognitive Domains | Cognitive Measures | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 4: control, vegetable snack, milk snack and beef snack | Control: no food; Vegetable snack: githeri plus corn, beans and vegetables; Milk snack: githeri plus 200 mL milk; Beef snack: githeri with 60g beef. | The snacks were equicaloric (~240 kcal/day). After 1 year, the energy content increased to ~315 kcal/d —vegetable snack to ~230 g, milk snack to 250 mL and beef snack to 85 g. | 1. Perceptual, reason and comparisons (fluid intelligence); 2. Vocabulary; 3. Basic knowledge of arithmetic. | 1.Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPM); 2. Verbal Meaning test (VMT); 3. Arithmetic skills test (AST). |
2 | 2: beef, iron-fortified cereal | Beef: beef and beef gravy with 25 mg Zn/g and 15 mg Fe/g; Cereal: iron-fortified infant rice cereal with 15 mg Zn/g and 740 mg Fe/g. | Any infant who was found to be reluctant to accept cereal feeding had the option of mixing the food with selected pureed fruits. | Mental, motor and behavior. | Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-II). |
3 | 4: control, vegetable snack, milk snack and beef snack | Control: no food; Vegetable snack: githeri plus corn, beans and vegetables; Milk snack: githeri plus 200 mL milk; Beef snack: githeri with 60 g beef. | The snacks were equicaloric (~240 kcal/day). After 1 year, the energy content increased to ~315 kcal/d —vegetable snack to ~230 g, milk snack to 250 mL and beef snack to 85 g. | 1. Perceptual, reason and comparisons (fluid intelligence); 2. Vocabulary; 3. Basic knowledge of arithmetic; 4. Concentration, attention and immediate memory; 5. Academic performance. | 1. Raven’ s Progressive Matrices (RPM); 2. Verbal Meaning Test (VMT); 3. Arithmetic skills test (AST); 4. Digit Span test (DST); 5. Zonal-wide multi-test (ZMT). |
4 | 4: control, vegetable snack, milk snack and beef snack | Control: no food; Vegetable snack: githeri plus corn, beans and vegetables; Milk snack: githeri plus 200 mL milk; Beef snack: githeri with 60 g beef. | The snacks were equicaloric (~240 kcal/day). After 1 year, the energy content increased to ~315 kcal/d —vegetable snack to ~230 g, milk snack to 250 mL and beef snack to 85 g. | 1. Perceptual, reason and comparisons (fluid intelligence); 2. Vocabulary; 3. Basic knowledge of arithmetic; 4. Concentration, attentionand immediate memory. | 1. Raven’ s Progressive Matrices (RPM); 2. Verbal Meaning Test (VMT); 3. Arithmetic skill Test (AST); 4. Digit Span test (DST). |
5 | 2: beef, micronutrient-fortified cereal | Beef: lyophilized beef provides 30 g/day from 6-11 m of age and 45 g/day from 12–18 month of age; Cereal: a micronutrient-fortified rice-soy, provides 70 and 105 kcal/day for the first and second 6- month periods. | Two feeding arms were equicaloric. | Psychomotor developmental and mental developmental. | The Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-II). |
6 | 2: beef and non-beef lunch | Beef lunch: consisted of 3 oz/85 g beef, 2 oz (56 g) starch and 8 oz (237 mL) bottled water; Non-beef lunch: 3 oz/85 g non-beef entrée, 2 oz (56 g) starch and 8 oz (237 mL) bottled water. | Lunches followed a 4-week cycle menu. within each lunch day, the starch food was the same for all women and the beef or non-beef entrée was the same within each lunch arm. | 1. Motor skill; 2. Immediate and delayed memory; 3. Spatial planning ability and working memory; 4. Retain spatial information, working memory and devise strategy for searching task; 5. Sustained attention with a minor working memory component. | 1. Motor Screening Test (MOT); 2. Verbal Recognition Memory (VRM); 3. One Touch Stockings of Cambridge (OTS); 4 Spatial Working Memory (SWM); 5. Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVP). |
7 | 4: control, vegetable snack, milk snack and beef snack | Control: no food; Vegetable snack: githeri plus corn, beans and vegetables, ~230 g; Milk snack: githeri plus 250 mL milk; Beef snack: githeri with 85 g beef. | The snacks were equicaloric (~315 kcal/day). | Academic performance. | School end-term test: Arithmetic, English, Kiembu, Kiswahili, Science, Geography, Arts. |
8 | 3: wheat biscuits, beef biscuits and soy biscuits | Wheat biscuits: wheat flour biscuits used as the control arm; Beef biscuits: dried beef powder was added to the basic recipe made of wheat flour; Soy biscuits: soy flour was added to the basic recipe made of wheat flour. | Isocaloric biscuits were made with wheat flour, 4.0 g protein per 100 kcal. | 1. Concentration, attention and immediate memory; 2. Perceptual, reason and comparisons (fluid intelligence); 3. Vocabulary; 4. Basic knowledge of arithmetic; 5. Cognitive style; 6. Integrate visual and motor abilities. | 1. Digit Span Test (DS); 2. Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPM); 3. Verbal Meaning Test (VMT); 4. Arithmetic skill test (AST); 5. Embedded figure test (EFT); 6. Beery test of visual–motor integration (VMI). |
Study ID | Results | Main Findings | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Intervention Effectiveness of Beef Consumption (vs. Control) | Intervention Effectiveness of Beef Consumption (vs. other Intervention Arms) | Intervention Effectiveness of Beef Consumption (vs. Control) | Intervention Effectiveness of Beef Consumption (vs. Other Intervention Arms) | |
1 | 1. Beef snack arm showed greater gains on RPM than control: ES = 0.34, SE = 0.2, p = 0.045; 2. Beef snack arm showed no significant difference on VMT than control: ES = 0.2, SE = 0.23; 3. Beef snack arm showed greater gains on AST than control: ES = 0.18, SE = 0.1, p = 0.033. | 1. Beef snack arm showed greater gains on RPM than vegetable (ES = 0.41, SE = 0.2, p = 0.02) and milk snack arms (ES = 0.68, SE = 0.2, p < 0.01); 2. Beef snack arm showed no significant difference on VMT compared with vegetable (ES = −0.09, SE = 0.22) and milk snack arms (ES = 0.14, SE = 0.22); 3. Beef snack arm showed no significant difference on AST compared with vegetable (ES = −0.08, SE = 0.09) and milk snack arms (ES = 0.15, SE = 0.09). | Children fed with beef snacks showed greater gains on RPM and AST compared with control arm but no significant difference on VMT. | Children fed with beef snacks showed greater gains on RPM than vegetable and milk arms but no significant difference on VMT and AST. |
2 | 1. The mental percentile sub-scores in the BSID-II for beef and cereal arms showed no significant difference (SMD = −0.11; 95% CI = −0.53, 0.31); 2. The motor percentile sub-scores in the BSID-II for beef and cereal arms showed no significant difference (SMD = 0.28; 95% CI = −0.14, 0.70); 3. The behavior percentile sub-scores in the BSID-II showed no significant difference (SMD = 0.41; 95% CI = −0.01, 0.84). | Motor, mental and behavior sub-scores in the BSID-II did not differ between arms. Introduction of meat as an early complementary food for exclusively breastfed infants is feasible and was associated with improved zinc intake and potential benefits. | ||
3 | 1. RPM: beef snack arm increased rate was steeper than control arm; 2. AST: beef snack arms performed better over time than control arm (p < 0.05); 3. VMT and DS: no significant differences; 4. Zone-wide school end-term: beef snack arm increased greater than control arm; 5. Arithmetic subtest: greater percentage increased in beef snack arm than control arm. | 1. RPM: beef snack arm increased rate was steeper than milk and vegetable snack arms; 2. AST: beef snack arm performed better over time than milk and vegetable snack arms; 3. VMT and DS: no significant differences; 4. Zone-wide school end-term scores: beef snack arm performed better than milk and vegetable snack arms; 5. Arithmetic subtest: greater percentage increased in beef snack arm than vegetable and milk snack arms. | Beef snack arm showed steeper rate of increasing on RPM, AST, zone-wide school end-term total scores and arithmetic subtest scores than control arm. | Beef snack arm showed steeper rate of increase on RPM, AST, zone-wide school end-term total and arithmetic subtest scores than milk and vegetable snack arm. |
5 | 1. Psychomotor developmental index: 99.1 (95% CI: 97.9, 100.3) and 99.7 (95% CI: 98.8, 100.7) (p = 0.54) for beef and cereal arms. 2. Mental developmental index: 95.2 (95% CI: 94.2, 96.2) and 95.3 (95% CI: 94.5, 96.2) (p = 0.82) for beef and cereal arms. | No significant different was found in the index of BSID-II in beef and cereal arms. | ||
6 | 1. VRM: lunch arm had significant main effects on free recall of correct targets, with more words recalled by women in beef arm than non-beef arm (p = 0.007); 2. SWM: latency to first response was different (p = 0.0003), speed was greater in non-beef arm than beef arm; token search time was affected by different arms (p = 0.003). SWM strategy showed a significant effect of arm (p = 0.018) with better strategy showed in non-beef than beef arm; 3. RVP: lunch arm had no significant effect on latency to respond but more total hits were achieved in beef arm than non-beef arm (p = 0.0038), total misses were lower in beef arm than non-beef arm (p = 0.006), correct rejections were higher in beef arm than non-beef arm (p = 0.009). | Lunch arm had no consistent main effects on test performance. Beef arm performance better on VRM and RVP. Overall, the current findings do not show that intake of beef improves cognitive performance in women with decreased iron status to a greater degree than non-beef protein foods. | ||
7 | Beef snack arm showed difference with control arm on scores of Arithmetic (ES = 5.41, SE = 2.66, p < 0.05), English (ES = 14.3, SE = 3.34, p < 0.05), Kiembu (ES = 7.71, SE = 3.24, p < 0.05), Kiswahili (ES = 8.29, SE = 3.63, p < 0.05), Geography (ES = 9.31, SE = 2.37, p < 0.05) , Arts (ES = 5.26, SE = 1.82, p < 0.05) and total scores (ES = 57.5, SE = 16.3, p < 0.05). | Beef snack arm showed difference with milk snack arm on score of English (ES = 6.58, SE = 2.87, p < 0.05); Beef snack arm showed difference with vegetable snack arm on the score of Arithmetic (ES = 6.18, SE = 2.28, p < 0.05), English (ES = 12.5, SE = 3.14, p < 0.05), Kiembu (ES = 6.03, SE = 3.05, p < 0.05), Kiswahili (ES = 7.11, SE = 3.41, p < 0.05), Geography (ES = 7.00, SE = 2.24, p < 0.05) , Arts (ES = 4.67, SE = 1.71, p < 0.05) and total scores (ES = 44.8 SE = 12.55, p < 0.05). | Children fed with beef snack showed improvements in scores in six of the seven subjects (Arithmetic, English, Kiembu, Kiswahili, Geography and Arts) and overall total test scores compared with control arm. | Children fed with beef snack showed improvements in scores in English compared with milk snack arm; Children fed with beef showed improvements in scores in six of the seven subjects (Arithmetic, English, Kiembu, Kiswahili, Geography and Arts) and overall total test scores compared with vegetable snack arm. |
8 | Soy biscuits arm showed no significant with beef arm on seven of the tests including DS-forward (ES = 0.13, 95% CI: −0.27,0.52), DS-backward (ES = 0.28, 95% CI: −0.09,0.64), DS-total (ES = 0.33, 95% CI: −0.27,0.94), VMT (ES = 1.10, 95%CI: −0.48,2.68), AST (ES = 0.568, 95% CI: −0.01,1.13), EFT (ES = 0.07, 95% CI: −051,0.64), VMI (ES = 0.15, 95% CI: −0.63,0.93), except for RPM (ES = 1.87, 95% CI: 0.56,3.18, p < 0.05). Beef biscuits arm showed no difference with wheat biscuits arm on all of tests including DS-forward (ES = 0.09, 95% CI: −0.33, 0.51), DS-backward (ES = 0.14, 95% CI: −0.25, 0.54), DS-total (ES = 0.18, 95% CI: −0.46, 0.83), RPM (ES = −0.14, 95% CI: −1.56, 1.28), VMT (ES = −0.50, 95% CI: −2.19,1.19), AST (ES = 0.02, 95% CI: −0.59, 0.63), EFT (ES = −0.21, 95% CI: −0.83, 0.40), VMI (ES = −0.52, 95% CI: −0.36, 4.48). | HIV-affected school-age children provided with beef biscuits showed no significant difference on all of the cognitive tests compared with wheat biscuits arm. | HIV-affected school-age children provided with soy biscuits showed greater improvement in nonverbal cognitive (fluid intelligence) performance compared with beef biscuits arm. |
Criteria | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Was the study described as randomized, a randomized trial, a randomized clinical trial or an RCT? | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
2. Was the method of randomization adequate (i.e., use of randomly generated assignment)? | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 |
3. Was the treatment allocation concealed (so that assignments could not be predicted)? | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 |
4. Were study participants and providers blinded to treatment group assignment? | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 |
5. Were the people assessing the outcomes blinded to the participants' group assignments? | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 |
6. Were the groups similar at baseline on important characteristics that could affect outcomes (e.g., demographics, risk factors, co-morbid conditions)? | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
7. Was the overall drop-out rate from the study at endpoint 20% or lower of the number allocated to treatment? | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 |
8. Was the differential drop-out rate (between treatment groups) at endpoint 15 percentage points or lower? | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 |
9. Was there high adherence to the intervention protocols for each treatment group? | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
10. Were other interventions avoided or similar in the groups (e.g., similar background treatments)? | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
11. Were outcomes assessed using valid and reliable measures, implemented consistently across all study participants? | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
12. Did the authors report that the sample size was sufficiently large to be able to detect a difference in the main outcome between groups with at least 80% power? | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
13. Were outcomes reported or subgroups analyzed pre-specified (i.e., identified before analyses were conducted)? | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
14. Were all randomized participants analyzed in the group to which they were originally assigned, that is, did they use an intention-to-treat analysis? | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
Total scores | 8 | 12 | 8 | 8 | 13 | 9 | 8 | 10 |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
An, R.; Nickols-Richardson, S.M.; Khan, N.; Liu, J.; Liu, R.; Clarke, C. Impact of Beef and Beef Product Intake on Cognition in Children and Young Adults: A Systematic Review. Nutrients 2019, 11, 1797. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11081797
An R, Nickols-Richardson SM, Khan N, Liu J, Liu R, Clarke C. Impact of Beef and Beef Product Intake on Cognition in Children and Young Adults: A Systematic Review. Nutrients. 2019; 11(8):1797. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11081797
Chicago/Turabian StyleAn, Ruopeng, Sharon M Nickols-Richardson, Naiman Khan, Jianxiu Liu, Ruidong Liu, and Caitlin Clarke. 2019. "Impact of Beef and Beef Product Intake on Cognition in Children and Young Adults: A Systematic Review" Nutrients 11, no. 8: 1797. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11081797