Next Article in Journal
Risk Assessment Method for Spontaneous Combustion of Pyrophoric Iron Sulfides
Previous Article in Journal
Research on Learning Concentration Recognition with Multi-Modal Features in Virtual Reality Environments
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Demographic Considerations in Incenting Reuse of Corrugated Cardboard Boxes

by
Harshwardhan Ketkale
and
Steven Simske
*
Systems Engineering Department, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO 80523, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2023, 15(15), 11600; https://doi.org/10.3390/su151511600
Submission received: 28 May 2023 / Revised: 20 July 2023 / Accepted: 25 July 2023 / Published: 27 July 2023

Abstract

:
Climate change is heavily impacted by greenhouse gases. Many sustainability efforts directly or indirectly affect greenhouse gas (GHG) emissions into the environment. In order to address climate change, sustainability efforts are promoted all around the world. The need to motivate the general population was identified by authors in their previous research. This paper proposes to use a positive reinforcement ethos as a psychological incentive to motivate the general population. This paper further examines the findings of the previous paper to better construct the structure of motivating the general population with the use of this positive reinforcement ethos. This paper attempts to segment the general population based on demographic information including age, gender, awareness of climate change, and current recycling efforts to examine its relevance with persuasion and operant conditions. Further, this paper also tests the hypothesis of using entropy as a tool to identify confusing/leading questions on the survey. Two different sustainability effort options are explored: returning and reusing Corrugated Cardboard Boxes (CCBs). An online survey is conducted, and its data are analyzed to test these hypotheses. The results indicate that reusing CCBs is statistically significantly preferred over returning them. Also, ethos and aesthetics are statistically significantly preferred over logos and pathos. Segmenting the general population based on demographic does not yield any significant effect on motivating the general population. The results of this study can be applied to motivate the general population for different sustainability efforts such as promoting green energy, waste management, and other initiatives.

1. Introduction

The United States Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) defines sustainability as “everything that we need for our survival and well-being depends, either directly or indirectly, on our natural environment. To pursue sustainability is to create and maintain the conditions under which humans and nature can exist in productive harmony to support present and future generations” [1]. Thus, promoting sustainability efforts is important, at a minimum, since humans are directly or indirectly dependent on the environment. According to the United Nations Climate Action (UNCA) [2], the largest contributor to global climate change is the use of fossil fuels and the carbon emissions from it. The seven causes identified by the United Nations Climate Action are generating power, manufacturing goods, cutting down forests, using transportation, producing food, powering buildings, and overconsuming. The recycling process, as seen in the recent literature reviews, is one of the options to reduce greenhouse gas emissions [3,4,5,6,7,8,9]. The Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) states that “Recycling reduces GHG emissions through lower energy demand for production (avoided fossil fuel) and by substitution of recycled feedstocks for virgin materials” [10] (p. 602). Although recycling would help in reducing GHG emissions, the motivation for recycling is lacking in the general population, as observed by Abila [11], Gilli et al. [12], Kattoua et al. [13], Seacat and Boileau [14], and Li et al. [15]. The authors, in a previous work [16], proposed ways to encourage the general population to reuse Corrugated Cardboard Boxes (CCBs) instead of landfilling them with the use of incentive methods combining operant conditioning and persuasion preferences. The authors, moreover, showed that a lifecycle assessment and economic cost analysis of reusing CCBs is possible [17]. The current research tries to reduce carbon emissions from five of the seven causes (apart from producing food and overconsumption) identified by UNCA in the case of CCBs. Promoting sustainable efforts is important, which is the reason behind focusing on studying the incentive techniques and recommendations from the authors’ previous papers in depth [16,17]. The authors [16] concluded that in terms of motivating the general population for sustainable efforts, segmenting the general population into groups and incenting each group according to their preference is ineffective. A more general incentivization approach for the general population was recommended. In order to effectively motivate the general population for sustainable efforts, it is important to evaluate this claim of segmentation using additional segmenting options. While conducting surveys, it is a common practice among researchers to collect demographic data and analyze the overall data based on subcategories. This paper explores additional segmenting options based on demographic data including age, gender, awareness of environment/climate change, and current recycling efforts.
One of the causes mentioned by the UN for climate change is transportation. It is important in terms of the lifecycle of CCBs to evaluate the transportation option for the proposed reuse phase. Thus, it is worth exploring the options in the collection of CCBs for the reuse phase. One approach is to have the general population assign the used CCBs to a specific bin called the “reuse” bin. These CCBs are then collected by a truck and transported to a specific location for further processing. The other option is that individuals gather their used CCBs and personally drive to the nearest specific location (collection site) for drop-off. These two explored options are very different and require different levels of motivation and carbon emissions. The hypothesis here is that more effort is required for individuals to drive to the collection site. Thus, they would need to be more motivated compared to the other option of assigning CCBs to the reuse bins. The carbon emissions vary for both options, as the option where individuals would need to drive to the collection site would have more carbon emissions as more vehicles are used. Thus, the survey attempts to elicit which method of collecting CCBs for recycling (a reuse bin or dumping at a reuse site) is more appealing to the general population, with respect to operant conditions and persuasion techniques.
Many research papers discuss the methodology for developing a questionnaire that avoids the use of leading/confusing questions [18,19,20,21,22,23]. The authors in [16] also proposed a new tool for using entropy calculations to evaluate the questions asked on the survey to identify if any particular question is biased/confusing/double-barreled. This research further investigates if entropy can be used to identify problems with the questions asked on surveys.
Similar studies where the general population was incentivized to reuse instead of the recycling process were not found in the literature review. Research papers [11,12,15,24] tried to promote sustainable efforts in the general population by using incentives. Based on waste management service charges, the authors of [25,26,27] tried to incentivize the general population to reduce waste generation. The indirect incentive used by [25,26,27] was to charge the individual household based on the weight of the waste they wanted to dispose. Gibovic and Bikfalvi [28] studied the use of virtual currency as a means of financial incentive to increase the plastic recycling rate in the general population. Thus, the literature review indicated a need to motivate the general population toward sustainable efforts. Also, a unique method of incentivization like using operant conditioning and persuasion techniques was not found.
Overall, this paper tests the hypothesis that segmenting the general population based on age, gender, recycling efforts, and awareness about environment/climate change has a significant impact on people’s preference over incentives. By testing this hypothesis, the findings may add a new way of motivating the general population to the body of knowledge. This research may also prove the use of entropy to analyze the survey data and examine the survey questions.

2. Materials and Methods

Reference [16] concludes that motivating the general population by segmenting them into different groups did not add significant value (measured in terms of overall cost of incenting). In order to further examine this methodology for incentivizing sustainable efforts from the general population, an additional survey was carried out. Survey questions and methodology were reviewed and approved by Colorado State University’s Institutional Review Board (IRB). The methodology used in this paper is to conduct the survey and analyze data to test the hypothesis. The research methodology used in this paper is consistent with the previous research [16,17] on which this research paper is based. This research method includes carefully wording the questionnaire to test the required hypothesis as well as making sure that the questions or options are not leading/confusing. In order to avoid these errors, entropy is calculated for all the questions. The survey in [16] also uses entropy calculations to determine the quality/clarity of questions with respect to participants’ responses. Entropy is a measure of randomness [16], with random data having higher entropy, and vice versa. It is important to test the hypothesis behind the use of entropy as a unit of measure to evaluate the clarity of questions. The authors identified a few questions from [16] under survey #1, which can be categorized as confusing or double-barreled questions. These questions could be confusing to the participants (indicated by exaggerated entropy, a measure of randomness). Thus, rephrasing the questions for clarity and evaluating entropy change would test the hypothesis.

Survey

There were 58 questions in total on this survey. The objective of this survey was to further evaluate and test the results and conclusions from [16] about incentivizing the general population without the necessity of performing market segmentation. This survey evaluates the preferences of the general population with respect to two different ways of collecting processes for the reuse phase (assigning and returning). This survey also evaluates the “entropy” tool by rephrasing the question with high entropy from survey #1 in [16]. Below are the types of questions that were included in this survey.
  • Six questions to note the demographics of the participants participating in this survey.
  • Questions to evaluate the collection process by assigning CCBs to reuse bins.
    • Multiple-choice questions (12 questions)
  • Questions to evaluate the collection process by returning CCBs to a specific location.
    • Multiple-choice questions (12 questions)
  • Questions to assess persuasion preferences.
    • Likert-type questions (20 questions)
  • Questions to evaluate entropy change by rephrasing.
    • Likert-type questions (5 questions)
This survey evaluates the possibility of adding value in motivation by segmenting the general population with respect to demographics. Additionally, it identifies the general population’s preferences over the collection process of CCBs for reusing.

3. Results

This survey was published online on the social media platform LinkedIn. The survey was also sent to participants from the survey conducted in [16,17]. Additionally, this survey was distributed to the students, faculty, and staff of Colorado State University. The survey was created, and the responses were collected online using the Qualtrics tool. The survey was active for 50 days and received 151 responses. Responses for the survey were provided by participants from seven countries on four continents. Qualtrics metadata show that the survey received responses from seven countries. The median time to complete this survey was 9.18 min. Once the responses were collected by the Qualtrics tool, the data were then exported and analyzed in Excel and by the IBM SPSS tool.

3.1. Results and Analysis for Assigning Method

3.1.1. Results for Multiple-Choice Questions

Multiple-choice questions were asked with two options representing two persuasion techniques or two operant conditions each for the assigning approach. Thus, the four persuasion techniques (Ethos, Pathos, Logos, and Aesthetics) and four operant conditions (Positive reinforcement, Negative reinforcement, Positive punishment, and Negative punishment) were compared to each other within their respective category. Table 1 gives the results for the multiple-choice questions for assigning CCBs.

3.1.2. Analysis of Multiple-Choice Questions

To analyze the answers for the general population’s preferences, a chi-square test was conducted to evaluate if one of the two options was significantly preferred by the participants. A chi-square test is used to statistically evaluate the goodness of fit between the expected values and measured values. The total number of participants was 151; thus, the expected value here is the midpoint between 0 and 151, or 75.5. Table 2 and Table 3 give the analysis results for assigning CCBs.

3.2. Results and Analysis for Returning Method

3.2.1. Results for Multiple-Choice Questions

Multiple-choice questions were asked with two options representing two persuasion techniques or two operant conditions each for the returning approach. Thus, the four persuasion techniques and four operant conditions were compared to each other within their respective categories. Table 4 gives the results for the multiple-choice questions for returning CCBs.

3.2.2. Analysis for Multiple-Choice Questions

A chi-square test was again conducted to evaluate if one of the two options is significantly preferred by the participants. The expected value here is considered to be 75.5, as mentioned earlier. Table 5 and Table 6 give the analysis results for returning CCBs.

3.3. Results and Analysis for Likert Scale Questions

3.3.1. Results for Likert Scale Questions

Likert scale questions were asked to evaluate the general population’s preferences for persuasion techniques. Likert scale questions include five options as follows: strongly agree, somewhat agree, neither agree nor disagree, somewhat disagree, and strongly disagree. To evaluate the results based on the responses, a linear scoring scale was considered with strongly disagree as 1 and strongly agree as 5. Table 7 gives the results for the Likert scale questions.

3.3.2. Analysis of Likert Scale Questions

To analyze the data from Likert scale questions, an independent t-test was calculated to compare each pair of persuasion technique scores. Table 8 gives the results of the independent t-tests on the Likert scale questions.

3.4. Results and Analysis of Data Based on Demographics

3.4.1. Results Based on Demographics

In total, six demographic questions were asked. These questions help to identify a participant’s age, gender, awareness of climate change, and current recycling efforts. Figure 1 shows the results of the distribution of participants based on the respective demographic information.

3.4.2. Analysis of Data Based on Demographics

The data are partitioned by demographics and analyzed based on the question types. The detailed results of the analyzed data are given in Appendix A. The sections below give a brief summary of those results.

Summary of Analyzed Data from Multiple-Choice Questions

Table 9 gives a summary of the results for the multiple-choice questions. Additionally, a chi-square test was conducted to analyze the data.

Summary of Analyzed Data from Likert Scale Questions

Table 10 below shows the results of the Likert scale questions for persuasion techniques based on the demographics. To analyze the following data, t-tests were conducted by comparing the persuasion techniques to each other.

3.5. Results and Analysis for Entropy Calculations

3.5.1. Results for Entropy Calculation Questions

In total, five questions were asked on the survey to examine the entropy change. These questions (originally from [16]) were reworded for clarity. Table 11 shows the answers to five Likert scale questions from the survey (originally reworded from [16]).

3.5.2. Analysis for Entropy Calculations

As explained in [16], entropy is a measure of randomness. Entropy increases as randomness in data increases, and vice versa. In the case of Likert scale questions, high entropy may indicate confusion in questions, as it is primarily expected that the population of response would be around two main options (strongly disagree or strongly agree). Entropy is calculated by using Equation (1).
e = i = 1 N p i l n ( p i )
The entropy values from [16] and this survey are compared, and the differences between the two values are calculated as shown in Table 12.

4. Discussion

In total, 24 multiple-choice questions were asked on the survey, each of which compared two options among the four choices (for motivation or the operant condition). A chi-square test was carried out to evaluate the participant’s preference among the six total comparisons (1 vs. 2, 1 vs. 3, 1 vs. 4, 2 vs. 3, 2 vs. 4, and 3 vs. 4). Table 1 and Table 4 give the preference results for assigning CCBs to the reuse bin and returning CCBs to specific locations, respectively. For assigning CCBs with respect to persuasion techniques, it can be observed from Table 2 that the general population statistically significantly more willingly responds toward ethos and aesthetics over logos and pathos. It can be observed that the difference between ethos and aesthetics, as well as the difference between pathos and logos, is not statistically significant. For assigning CCBs with respect to operant conditions, it can be observed from Table 3 that positive reinforcement is statistically significantly preferred over negative punishment, and negative reinforcement is statistically significantly preferred over negative punishment. The other four comparisons of operant conditions to each other are not statistically significantly different. For returning CCBs with respect to persuasion techniques, it can be observed from Table 5 that the general population statistically significantly prefers ethos over pathos and aesthetics over pathos. The difference between logos and both aesthetics and pathos is not statistically significant, whereas the difference between ethos and both logos and aesthetics is not statistically significant. For returning CCBs with respect to operant conditions, it can be observed from Table 6 that positive reinforcement is statistically significantly preferred over both negative punishment and positive punishment. Also, negative reinforcement is statistically significantly preferred over negative punishment. The difference between positive punishment and both negative punishment and negative reinforcement is not statistically significant. Additionally, the difference between positive punishment and negative reinforcement is not statistically significant. Table 7 shows the results for Likert scale questions that evaluate the persuasion preferences of participants. The questions are scored on a scale of 1 to 5, where 1 is strongly disagree (not preferred) and 5 is strongly agree (preferred). Aesthetics scored highest, followed by ethos with a small, statistically insignificant, margin (0.07), pathos, and logos. The t-test shows that at p ≤ 0.05, ethos and aesthetics are statistically significantly different from pathos and logos. It also shows that the difference between ethos and aesthetics as well as logos and pathos is not statistically significant.
This paper mainly evaluates if segmenting the general population based on their demographic information is an effective approach for motivating the general population to adopt desired sustainable efforts. Table 9 gives the mean and standard deviation of the scores that a multiple-choice question scored with respect to segmenting the general population based on demographics. Table A1, Table A2, Table A3 and Table A4 from Appendix A show the chi-square scores as well as the p-values for these multiple-choice questions based on age, gender, awareness, and recycling efforts, respectively. In total, 12 questions (from Q7 to Q18) compare four persuasion techniques to each other. Another 12 questions (from Q19 to Q30) compare four operant conditions to each other. Thus, every pair of persuasion techniques and operant conditions are evaluated twice. Table 10 gives the Likert scale for each persuasion technique with respect to demographics. These scores are calculated by taking the mean of the five questions asked for each persuasion technique. In order to analyze these scores, a t-test is conducted by comparing each persuasion technique to the others. Table A5, Table A6, Table A7, Table A8, Table A9, Table A10, Table A11 and Table A12 from Appendix A show the t-value as well as the p-value for these Likert scale questions based on age, gender, awareness, and recycling efforts, respectively. In order to better interpret the tabulated results in Appendix A, p-values below 0.01 are highlighted in green. Table 11 shows the results for entropy calculations as well as the reference questions from [16] that were reworded. It can be observed from Table 12 that entropy decreased for Q55, Q56, Q58, and Q59. Only Q57 had an increase in entropy by 3.94%. Overall, for five questions, the entropy decreased by 10.56%. The proposed incentivization tool can be used globally, as the overall recycling rate is low compared to other end-of-lifecycle processes. An example of this trend can be observed with the global end-of-lifecycle process of plastic waste. The Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD) [29,30] shows that as of 2015, 14–18% of global plastic waste is collected for recycling, and 24% of the global plastic waste is thermally treated. The remaining 58–62% of plastic waste ends up in a controlled or uncontrolled landfill. Plastic recycling percentages based on countries [29] include the USA (9%), Australia (12%), Japan (23%), and the EU (30%). As observed in the above data, different countries have different recycling rates for plastics. The plastic recycling example gives a rough idea about the infrastructure in place as well as the difference in the level of motivation for recycling.
From the results and analysis of the multiple-choice questions, it can be observed that for assigning CCBs to the reuse bin, the general population statistically significantly preferred aesthetics and ethos over pathos and logos. This indicates that both ethos and aesthetics persuasion techniques are preferred by the general population for assigning CCBs. In the case of assigning CCBs with respect to operant conditions, no statistically significant preference was found. In the case of returning CCBs with the help of persuasion techniques and operant conditions, no statistically significant preference for a single persuasion technique over another or a single operant condition over another was found. In the case of the Likert scale questions, the results are similar to those of assigning CCBs to reuse bins, with the general population statistically significantly preferring both ethos and aesthetics over logos and pathos. This implies that both aesthetics and ethos are recommended to use to motivate the general population for sustainable efforts. This survey segments the general population based on gender, age, awareness of environment/climate change, and current recycling efforts. The authors conducted t-test and chi-square tests on the results and evaluated each sub-category for assigning/returning CCBs with respect to persuasion techniques and operant conditions. It can be concluded that no statistically significant trend in the preferences was observed, implying that the same motivation techniques are broadly applicable across demographics. This paper also examines the use of entropy to evaluate questions for confusion and/or for being double-barreled. The results for the five reworded questions from [16] show that the entropy decreased by 10.6% overall. As these five questions were identified by the authors to be confusing and double-barreled in [16], they were reworded to make them clearer and more direct.

5. Conclusions

The purpose of this paper was to further examine the results from [16] regarding segmenting the general population to effectively motivate it for sustainable efforts. It can be concluded that the segmentation of the general population based on demographics does not yield an effective way of incentivizing the general population for sustainable efforts. Also, to motivate the general population to conduct sustainable efforts, ethos and aesthetics are preferred among the four types of motivation that were evaluated. This supports the claim from [16] about not segmenting the general population for motivation as well as using ethos to motivate the general population. In terms of assigning CCBs to the reuse bin and returning CCBs to a specific location, it can be concluded that assigning CCBs to the reuse bin is preferred by the general population over returning them, which is considered in the life cycle analysis (LCA) for reusing CCBs in [17]. It can be also concluded that entropy may be used in some cases to evaluate the clarity/quality of the survey questions.
Overall, the proposed model from [16,17] and this paper can be tailored to different products and their unique lifecycles. The life cycle analysis (LCA) conducted in [17] would have a different set of data and different processes with respect to the different countries but can still follow the same process. Thus, the overall research provides a repurposable model that can be adjusted for any other products or processes to promote sustainable efforts among the general population and estimate the carbon emissions savings from the LCA. One of the outlooks of this research is the potential application of this new incentive tool of operant condition and persuasion techniques being used to promote sustainable cars, renewable energy, healthcare applications like vaccinations, etc.
The future scope and prospects of this study include identifying a way to convey the incentive message as well as exploring different incentive delivery methods. Research in the area of the implementation of these incentives may play a vital role in further validating this new approach. As seen from the entropy calculations, it is important to frame a clear incentive message. The use of the entropy concept as a tool to evaluate questionnaires may help future researchers to evaluate their questions and improve them accordingly.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.K. and S.S.; writing—original draft preparation, H.K.; writing—review and editing, S.S.; visualization, H.K.; supervision, S.S.; funding acquisition, S.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Colorado State University SoGES (School of Global Environmental Sustainability), Global Challenges Research Team 4 Grant, and APC was funded by Colorado State University’s Systems Engineering Department.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Colorado State University (protocol code: 3265; date of approval: 11 March 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The (anonymized) data are available upon request and approval by the Colorado State University Institutional Review Board (CSU IRB).

Acknowledgments

We wish to show our appreciation to Angie Chromiak for guiding us through the IRB application. We would also like to thank the following people for helping us with the distribution of surveys and administrative tasks: Ingrid, Bridge, Chrissy Charny, Debra Dandaneau, and Mary Gomez.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of the data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Table A1. Analysis of multiple-choice questions based on age. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Table A1. Analysis of multiple-choice questions based on age. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
18–3031–4546+Prefer Not to Mention
ScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-Value
Q7Ethos4628.69<0.0014624.89<0.0013320.63<0.00155.000.025
Pathos7950
Q8Ethos280.170.68365.260.021265.160.02330.200.654
Logos2519122
Q9Ethos203.190.074240.890.345200.110.74520.200.654
Aesthetics3331183
Q10Pathos176.810.009332.200.138231.680.19430.200.654
Logos3622152
Q11Pathos825.83<0.0011217.470.001910.530.00111.800.179
Aesthetics4543294
Q12Logos221.530.2161413.260.001617.790.00111.800.179
Aesthetics3141324
Q13Ethos4628.69<0.0014522.27<0.0013320.63<0.00155.000.025
Pathos71050
Q14Ethos221.530.216332.200.138231.680.19430.200.654
Logos3122152
Q15Ethos203.190.007280.020.892190.00120.200.654
Aesthetics3327193
Q16Pathos176.810.009250.460.500200.110.74530.200.654
Logos3630182
Q17Pathos825.83<0.001924.89<0.001108.530.00311.800.179
Aesthetics4546284
Q18Logos290.470.492231.470.224812.740.00111.800.179
Aesthetics2432304
Q19Positive Reinforcement230.930.336300.460.500288.530.00341.800.179
Positive Punishment3025101
Q20Positive Reinforcement300.930.336365.260.021231.680.19430.200.654
Negative Punishment2319152
Q21Positive Reinforcement240.470.492222.200.138180.110.74520.200.654
Negative Reinforcement2933203
Q22Positive Punishment4013.760.001365.260.021108.530.00311.800.179
Negative Punishment1319284
Q23Positive Punishment300.930.336250.460.500910.530.00105.000.025
Negative Reinforcement2330295
Q24Negative Punishment194.250.039178.020.004520.63<0.00111.800.179
Negative Reinforcement3438334
Q25Positive Reinforcement311.530.216290.160.6852910.530.00141.800.179
Positive Punishment222691
Q26Positive Reinforcement4115.870.001365.260.021242.630.10441.800.179
Negative Punishment1219141
Q27Positive Reinforcement322.280.130280.020.892170.420.51620.200.654
Negative Reinforcement2127213
Q28Positive Punishment3911.790.001321.470.224142.630.10411.800.179
Negative Punishment1423244
Q29Positive Punishment366.810.009260.160.685715.160.00105.000.025
Negative Reinforcement1729315
Q30Negative Punishment212.280.130169.620.001910.530.00105.000.025
Negative Reinforcement3239295
Table A2. Analysis of multiple-choice questions based on gender. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Table A2. Analysis of multiple-choice questions based on gender. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
MaleFemalePrefer Not to MentionNon-Binary
ScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-Value
Q7Ethos6037.70<0.00165369.48<0.00133.000.08322.000.157
Pathos91200
Q8Ethos423.260.070473.750.05220.330.56322.000.157
Logos273010
Q9Ethos320.360.547322.200.13810.330.56310.001
Aesthetics374521
Q10Pathos391.170.278341.050.30520.330.56310.001
Logos304311
Q11Pathos1229.35<0.0011821.83<0.00103.000.08302.000.157
Aesthetics575932
Q12Logos1619.84<0.001268.120.00410.330.56302.000.157
Aesthetics535122
Q13Ethos5934.80<0.00165369.48<0.00133.000.08322.000.157
Pathos101200
Q14Ethos412.450.117370.120.73220.330.56310.001
Logos284011
Q15Ethos340.010.904331.570.2110.330.56310.001
Aesthetics354421
Q16Pathos360.130.717268.120.00420.330.56310.001
Logos335111
Q17Pathos1326.80<0.0011528.69<0.00103.000.08302.000.157
Aesthetics566232
Q18Logos255.230.022341.050.30510.330.56310.001
Aesthetics444321
Q19Positive Reinforcement412.450.117410.330.56833.000.08302.000.157
Positive Punishment283602
Q20Positive Reinforcement391.170.278518.120.00420.330.56302.000.157
Negative Punishment302612
Q21Positive Reinforcement301.170.278322.200.13820.330.56322.000.157
Negative Reinforcement394510
Q22Positive Punishment330.130.7175310.920.00103.000.08310.001
Negative Punishment362431
Q23Positive Punishment273.260.070360.330.56803.000.08310.001
Negative Reinforcement424131
Q24Negative Punishment1815.780.0012214.140.00110.330.56310.001
Negative Reinforcement515521
Q25Positive Reinforcement456.390.011452.200.13833.000.08302.000.157
Positive Punishment243202
Q26Positive Reinforcement423.260.0705921.83<0.00133.000.08310.001
Negative Punishment271801
Q27Positive Reinforcement350.010.904410.330.56820.330.56310.001
Negative Reinforcement343611
Q28Positive Punishment340.010.904506.870.00803.000.08322.000.157
Negative Punishment352730
Q29Positive Punishment264.190.040410.330.56803.000.08322.000.157
Negative Reinforcement433630
Q30Negative Punishment229.060.0022312.480.00103.000.08310.001
Negative Reinforcement475431
Table A3. Analysis of multiple-choice questions based on awareness. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Table A3. Analysis of multiple-choice questions based on awareness. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
TremendousHighModerateLittleVery Little
ScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-Value
Q7Ethos2010.670.0016645.46<0.0014122.22<0.00131.000.3170--
Pathos48810
Q8Ethos120.001497.780.005291.650.19831.000.3170--
Logos12252010
Q9Ethos120.001311.950.163201.650.19831.000.3170--
Aesthetics12432910
Q10Pathos91.500.220400.490.485250.020.88620.0010--
Logos15342420
Q11Pathos58.170.0041331.14<0.0011017.160.00120.0010--
Aesthetics19613920
Q12Logos74.170.0412015.620.001157.370.00611.000.3170--
Aesthetics17543430
Q13Ethos2010.670.0016645.46<0.0014122.22<0.00120.0010--
Pathos48820
Q14Ethos120.001431.950.163230.180.66831.000.3170--
Logos12312610
Q15Ethos120.001370.001183.450.06320.0010--
Aesthetics12373120
Q16Pathos91.500.220330.870.352201.650.19831.000.3170--
Logos15412910
Q17Pathos58.170.0041428.60<0.001725.00<0.00120.0010--
Aesthetics19604220
Q18Logos82.670.102311.950.163201.650.19820.0010--
Aesthetics16432920
Q19Positive Reinforcement120.001442.650.103281.000.31711.000.3170--
Positive Punishment12302130
Q20Positive Reinforcement151.500.220431.950.163335.900.01511.000.3170--
Negative Punishment9311630
Q21Positive Reinforcement66.000.014370.001220.510.47511.000.3170--
Negative Reinforcement18372730
Q22Positive Punishment120.001400.490.485335.900.01520.0010--
Negative Punishment12341620
Q23Positive Punishment82.670.102330.870.352211.000.31720.0010--
Negative Reinforcement16412820
Q24Negative Punishment313.500.0012310.600.001157.370.00611.000.3170--
Negative Reinforcement21513430
Q25Positive Reinforcement151.500.220497.780.005270.510.47520.0010--
Positive Punishment9252220
Q26Positive Reinforcement151.500.2205110.600.0013610.800.00131.000.3170--
Negative Punishment9231310
Q27Positive Reinforcement100.670.414400.490.485281.000.31711.000.3170--
Negative Reinforcement14342130
Q28Positive Punishment130.170.683390.220.641313.450.06331.000.3170--
Negative Punishment11351810
Q29Positive Punishment91.500.220330.870.352250.020.88620.0010--
Negative Reinforcement15412420
Q30Negative Punishment313.500.0012310.600.001183.450.06320.0010--
Negative Reinforcement21513120
Table A4. Analysis of multiple-choice questions based on recycling efforts. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Table A4. Analysis of multiple-choice questions based on recycling efforts. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
TremendousHighModerateLittleVery Little
ScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-ValueScoreChi^2 Valuep-Value
Q7Ethos51.290.2566239.56<0.0015034.57<0.001127.140.00710.330.563
Pathos29622
Q8Ethos40.140.705455.090.024353.500.06170.00120.330.563
Logos3262171
Q9Ethos40.140.705283.170.075290.070.78934.570.03220.330.563
Aesthetics34327111
Q10Pathos30.140.705350.010.905310.640.42251.140.28520.330.563
Logos4362591
Q11Pathos21.290.2561328.52<0.0011120.64<0.00127.140.00720.330.563
Aesthetics55845121
Q12Logos30.140.705187.250.001178.640.00342.570.10810.330.563
Aesthetics45339102
Q13Ethos51.290.2566342.61<0.0014931.50<0.001114.570.03210.330.563
Pathos28732
Q14Ethos30.140.705350.010.905342.570.10870.00120.330.563
Logos4362271
Q15Ethos40.140.705320.690.406290.070.78927.140.00720.330.563
Aesthetics33927121
Q16Pathos30.140.705256.210.012300.290.59251.140.28520.330.563
Logos4462691
Q17Pathos21.290.2561231.11<0.0011023.14<0.00127.140.00720.330.563
Aesthetics55946121
Q18Logos40.140.705265.090.024231.790.18170.00110.330.563
Aesthetics3453372
Q19Positive Reinforcement63.570.587401.140.285300.290.59270.00120.330.563
Positive Punishment1312671
Q20Positive Reinforcement40.140.705422.380.122331.790.181114.570.03220.330.563
Negative Punishment3292331
Q21Positive Reinforcement40.140.705320.690.406213.500.06170.00120.330.563
Negative Reinforcement3393571
Q22Positive Punishment30.140.705390.690.406375.790.01670.00110.330.563
Negative Punishment4321972
Q23Positive Punishment21.290.256292.380.122270.070.78960.290.59203.000.083
Negative Reinforcement5422983
Q24Negative Punishment07.000.0081523.68<0.001195.790.01680.290.59203.000.083
Negative Reinforcement7563763
Q25Positive Reinforcement63.570.587444.070.043331.790.18170.00133.000.083
Positive Punishment1272370
Q26Positive Reinforcement40.140.7055011.850.001387.140.007102.570.10833.000.083
Negative Punishment3211840
Q27Positive Reinforcement40.140.705380.350.552260.290.59291.140.28520.330.563
Negative Reinforcement3333051
Q28Positive Punishment30.140.705390.690.406364.570.03260.290.59220.330.563
Negative Punishment4322081
Q29Positive Punishment13.570.587301.700.191300.290.59280.290.59203.000.083
Negative Reinforcement6412663
Q30Negative Punishment13.570.5871719.280.001195.790.01691.140.28503.000.083
Negative Reinforcement6543753
Table A5. Results of Likert scale questions based on age.
Table A5. Results of Likert scale questions based on age.
18–3031–4546+Prefer Not to
Answer
Aesthetics4.394.304.143.84
Ethos4.404.253.963.56
Logos3.633.4323.263.32
Pathos4.054.053.873.32
Table A6. Analysis of Likert scale questions based on age. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Table A6. Analysis of Likert scale questions based on age. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Comparison of Persuasion Techniques18–3031–4546+Prefer Not to Answer
t-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Value
Ethos with Pathos2.470.0191.690.0630.69570.25321.540.080
Ethos with Logos1.800.0542.420.0202.410.0210.550.297
Aesthetics with Ethos0.100.4600.820.2172.570.0161.190.133
Aesthetics with Pathos2.570.0162.040.0372.140.0321.990.040
Aesthetics with Logos1.800.0542.570.0163.070.0071.070.155
Logos with Pathos0.960.1810.750.0591.950.0420.000.500
Table A7. Results of Likert scale questions based on gender.
Table A7. Results of Likert scale questions based on gender.
MaleFemaleNon-BinaryPrefer Not to Mention
Aesthetics4.264.314.303.80
Ethos4.134.294.303.66
Logos3.463.472.903.13
Pathos3.934.043.903.46
Table A8. Analysis of Likert scale questions based on gender. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Table A8. Analysis of Likert scale questions based on gender. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Comparison of Persuasion TechniquesMaleFemaleNon-BinaryPrefer Not to Mention
t-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Value
Ethos with Pathos1.590.0751.940.0442.520.0171.500.086
Ethos with Logos2.500.0181.910.0452.450.0190.960.181
Aesthetics with Ethos2.150.0310.300.3850.000.5001.000.173
Aesthetics with Pathos2.500.0182.240.0272.520.0171.760.057
Aesthetics with Logos2.950.0091.970.0412.450.0191.170.137
Logos with Pathos1.600.0731.30600.11391.760.0570.580.287
Table A9. Results of Likert scale questions based on awareness.
Table A9. Results of Likert scale questions based on awareness.
TremendousHighModerateLittle
Aesthetics4.444.284.154.75
Ethos4.514.224.024.50
Logos3.273.503.463.55
Pathos4.084.053.824.00
Table A10. Analysis of Likert scale questions based on awareness. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Table A10. Analysis of Likert scale questions based on awareness. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Comparison of Persuasion TechniquesTremendousHighModerateLittle
t-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Value
Ethos with Pathos3.180.0061.640.0691.190.1321.580.076
Ethos with Logos3.080.0072.090.0341.510.0843.160.006
Aesthetics with Ethos1.130.1441.240.1231.770.0571.820.052
Aesthetics with Pathos2.680.0132.270.0262.100.0342.170.030
Aesthetics with Logos2.900.0092.280.0251.900.0463.630.003
Logos with Pathos1.920.0451.550.0790.920.1911.030.166
Table A11. Results of Likert scale questions based on recycling efforts.
Table A11. Results of Likert scale questions based on recycling efforts.
TremendousHighModerateLittleVery Little
Aesthetics4.004.444.173.974.73
Ethos3.684.424.023.975.00
Logos2.913.463.543.423.00
Pathos3.744.203.823.424.86
Table A12. Analysis of Likert scale questions based on recycling efforts. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Table A12. Analysis of Likert scale questions based on recycling efforts. (Green highlight indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05).
Comparison of Persuasion TechniquesTremendousHighModerateLittleVery Little
t-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Valuet-Valuep-Value
Ethos with Pathos0.480.3201.770.0561.370.1033.550.0031.000.173
Ethos with Logos1.530.0812.400.0211.500.0851.650.0687.170.001
Aesthetics with Ethos2.550.0160.230.4112.770.0120.000.5001.370.103
Aesthetics with Pathos2.440.0201.880.0482.400.0212.480.0180.560.293
Aesthetics with Logos2.170.0302.440.0201.970.0421.490.0875.090.001
Logos with Pathos1.660.0671.800.0540.810.2190.000.5006.030.001

Appendix B

Survey questionnaires
Q1
Definitions:
Recycling process—You place the cardboard box in the dedicated recycle bin or return it to the dedicated recycling yard, which is then recycled to make a new cardboard box.
Reusing process—You place the cardboard box in the dedicated reuse bin or return it to the dedicated reuse yard, where it is reused for shipping goods, and then the cardboard box is cleaned and prepared for another use.
  • I understood the difference between these two processes.
Q2
Please enter your email id—_______________________
Q3
What gender do you identify as?
  • Male
  • Female
  • Non-binary
  • Prefer not to answer
Q4
What is your age?
  • 0–17 years old
  • 18–30 years old
  • 31–45 years old
  • 46+
  • Prefer not to answer
Q5
What are your current recycling efforts?
  • Very Little
  • Little
  • Moderate
  • High
  • Tremendous
Q6
How much awareness do you have of the environment and climate change?
  • Very Little
  • Little
  • Moderate
  • High
  • Tremendous
Q7
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • A charitable organization committed to preventing environmental degradation gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reusing process.
  • A charitable organization committed to helping Florida panthers from going extinct gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reusing process.
Q8
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • A charitable organization committed to preventing environmental degradation gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reusing process.
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I assign to the reusing process.
Q9
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • A charitable organization committed to preventing environmental degradation gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reusing process.
  • A charitable organization committed to keeping my city clean gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reusing process.
Q10
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • A charitable organization committed to helping Florida panthers from going extinct gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reusing process.
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I assign to the reusing process.
Q11
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • A charitable organization committed to helping Florida panthers from going extinct gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reusing process.
  • A charitable organization committed to keeping my city clean gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reusing process.
Q12
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I assign to the reusing process.
  • A charitable organization committed to keeping my city clean gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reusing process.
Q13
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • A charitable organization committed to preventing environmental degradation gets a suitable donation for each box I return to the reuse yard.
  • A charitable organization committed to helping Florida panthers from going extinct gets a suitable donation for each box I return to the reuse yard.
Q14
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • A charitable organization committed to preventing environmental degradation gets a suitable donation for each box I return to the reuse yard.
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I return to the reuse yard.
Q15
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • A charitable organization committed to preventing environmental degradation gets a suitable donation for each box I return to the reuse yard.
  • A charitable organization committed to keeping my city clean gets a suitable donation for each box I return to the reuse yard.
Q16
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • A charitable organization committed to helping Florida panthers from going extinct gets a suitable donation for each box I return to the reuse yard.
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I return to the reuse yard.
Q17
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • A charitable organization committed to helping Florida panthers from going extinct gets a suitable donation for each box I return to the reuse yard.
  • A charitable organization committed to keeping my city clean gets a suitable donation for each box I return to the reuse yard.
Q18
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I return to the reuse yard.
  • A charitable organization committed to keeping my city clean gets a suitable donation for each box I return to the reuse yard.
Q19
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I assign to the reuse process.
  • I get penalized with a suitable cash penalty for not assigning the boxes to the reuse process.
Q20
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I assign to the reuse process.
  • My product discount is taken away from me which was offered to me for every cardboard box I assign to the reuse process.
Q21
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I assign to the reuse process.
  • My shipping charges are waived after I assign a suitable number of boxes to the reuse process.
Q22
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • I get penalized with a suitable cash penalty for not assigning the boxes to the reuse process.
  • My product discount is taken away from me which was offered to me for every cardboard box I assign to the reuse process.
Q23
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • I get penalized with a suitable cash penalty for not assigning the boxes for the reusing process.
  • My shipping charges are waived after I assign a suitable number of boxes to the reuse process.
Q24
Which one is more likely to influence you for assigning the cardboard box to the reuse bin rather than the recycling bin–
  • My product discount is taken away from me which was offered to me for every cardboard box I assign to the reuse process.
  • My shipping charges are waived after I assign a suitable number of boxes to the reuse process.
Q25
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I return to the reuse yard.
  • I get penalized with a suitable cash penalty for not returning the boxes to the reuse yard.
Q26
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I return to the reuse yard.
  • My product discount is taken away from me which was offered to me for every cardboard box I return to the reuse yard.
Q27
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • I get a suitable cash reward for each box I return to the reuse yard.
  • My shipping charges are waived after I return a suitable number of boxes to the reuse yard.
Q28
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • I get penalized with a suitable cash penalty for not returning the boxes to the reuse yard.
  • My product discount is taken away from me which was offered to me for every cardboard box I return to the reuse yard.
Q29
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • I get penalized with a suitable cash penalty for not returning the boxes to the reuse yard.
  • My shipping charges are waived after I return a suitable number of boxes to the reuse yard.
Q30
Which one is more likely to influence you for returning the cardboard box to the reuse yard rather than the recycling yard –
  • My product discount is taken away from me which was offered to me for every cardboard box I return to the reuse yard.
  • My shipping charges are waived after I return a suitable number of boxes to the reuse yard.
Q31
I am likely to assign a cardboard box to the reuse process rather than assigning it to the recycling process if—(Strongly disagree, Somewhat disagree, Neither agree nor disagree, Somewhat agree, and Strongly agree) (NO QUESTION)
Q32
A charitable organization committed to preventing environmental degradation gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q33
A charitable organization committed to helping Florida panthers from going extinct gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q34
I get a suitable cash reward for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q35
A charitable organization committed to keeping my city clean gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q36
A charitable organization trying to reduce global warming gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q37
I am likely to assign a cardboard box to the reuse process rather than assigning it to the recycling process if—(Strongly disagree, Somewhat disagree, Neither agree nor disagree, Somewhat agree, and Strongly agree) (NO QUESTION)
Q38
A charitable organization trying to repair the ozone layer gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q39
I save money off my shipping charges for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q40
A charitable organization committed to preventing the addition of trash into landfills gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q41
A charitable organization committed to reducing pollution gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q42
A charitable organization committed to helping polar bears from going extinct gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q43
I am likely to assign a cardboard box to the reuse process rather than assigning it to the recycling process if—(Strongly disagree, Somewhat disagree, Neither agree nor disagree, Somewhat agree, and Strongly agree) (NO QUESTION)
Q44
I get a suitable discount on my favorite shopping brands for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q45
A charitable organization committed to cleaning the trash in my city gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q46
A charitable organization trying to decrease the depletion of fossil fuel gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q47
A charitable organization committed to helping endangered species gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q48
I get public recognition after I assign a suitable number of boxes to the reuse process.
Q49
I am likely to assign a cardboard box to the reuse process rather than assigning it to the recycling process if—(Strongly disagree, Somewhat disagree, Neither agree nor disagree, Somewhat agree, and Strongly agree) (NO QUESTION)
Q50
A charitable organization committed to keeping our environment clean gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q51
A charitable organization committed to reducing climate change gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q52
A charitable organization committed to preserving the environment for future generations gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q53
I get a gift card for my favorite fast-food brand for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q54
A charitable organization committed to decreasing dirty landfills gets a suitable donation for each box I assign to the reuse process.
Q55
I prefer driving sustainable electric cars over gasoline-powered cars.
Q56
I prefer environment-friendly fabric bags over cheap plastic bags in grocery stores.
Q57
I routinely donate food/money to the less fortunate.
Q58
I work hard to receive praise from my boss.
Q59
I avoid losing important documents by organizing them in the first place.

References

  1. United States Environmental Protection Agency. Learn about Sustainability. Available online: https://www.epa.gov/sustainability/learn-about-sustainability (accessed on 18 July 2023).
  2. United Nations. Causes and Effects of Climate Change. Available online: https://www.un.org/en/climatechange/science/causes-effects-climate-change (accessed on 18 July 2023).
  3. Korhonen, M.R.; Dahlbo, H. Reducing Greenhouse Gas Emissions by Recycling Plastics and Textiles into Products; Finnish Environment Institute: Helsinki, Finland, 2007; Available online: http://hdl.handle.net/10138/38425 (accessed on 18 July 2023).
  4. King, M.F.; Gutberlet, J. Contribution of cooperative sector recycling to greenhouse gas emissions reduction: A case study of Ribeirão Pires, Brazil. J. Waste Manag. 2013, 33, 2771–2780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Turner, D.A.; Williams, I.D.; Kemp, S. Greenhouse gas emission factors for recycling of source-segregated waste materials. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2015, 105, 186–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Dunn, J.B.; Gaines, L.; Sullivan, J.; Wang, M.Q. Impact of recycling on cradle-to-gate energy consumption and greenhouse gas emissions of automotive lithium-ion batteries. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2012, 46, 12704–12710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Nguyen, T.K.L.; Ngo, H.H.; Guo, W.; Nguyen, T.L.H.; Chang, S.W.; Nguyen, D.D.; Varjani, S.; Lei, Z.; Deng, L. Environmental impacts and greenhouse gas emissions assessment for energy recovery and material recycle of the wastewater treatment plant. Sci. Total Environ. 2021, 784, 147135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Gu, B. Research on the impact of plastic recycling industry on greenhouse gas emissions. IOP Conf. Ser. Earth Environ. Sci. 2021, 781, 032030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Meys, R.; Kätelhön, A.; Bachmann, M.; Winter, B.; Zibunas, C.; Suh, S.; Bardow, A. Achieving net-zero greenhouse gas emission plastics by a circular carbon economy. Science 2021, 374, 71–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Contribution of Working Group III to the Fourth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change; Metz, B.; Davidson, O.R.; Bosch, P.R.; Dave, R.; Meyer, L.A. (Eds.) Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2007; Available online: https://archive.ipcc.ch/publications_and_data/ar4/wg3/en/contents.html (accessed on 18 July 2023).
  11. Abila, B. Households’ perception of financial incentives in endorsing sustainable waste recycling in Nigeria. Recycling 2018, 3, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Gilli, M.; Nicolli, F.; Farinelli, P. Behavioural attitudes towards waste prevention and recycling. Ecol. Econ. 2018, 154, 294–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Kattoua, M.G.; Al-Khatib, I.A.; Kontogianni, S. Barriers on the propagation of household solid waste recycling practices in developing countries: State of Palestine example. J. Mater. Cycles Waste 2019, 21, 774–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Seacat, J.D.; Boileau, N. Demographic and community-level predictors of recycling behavior: A statewide, assessment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 56, 12–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Li, C.; Wang, Y.; Li, Y.; Huang, Y.; Harder, M.K. The incentives may not be the incentive: A field experiment in recycling of residential food waste. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2021, 168, 105316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Ketkale, H.; Simske, S. Encouraging Reuse in the Corrugated Packaging Industry Using Persuasion and Operant Conditioning. Sustainability 2022, 14, 6454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Ketkale, H.; Simske, S. A LifeCycle Analysis and Economic Cost Analysis of Corrugated Cardboard Box Reuse and Recycling in the United States. Resources 2023, 12, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Krrosnick, J.A.; Presser, S. Question and Questionnaire Design. In Handbook of Survey Research, 2nd ed.; Wright, J., Marsden, P., Eds.; Elsevier: San Diego, CA, USA, 2009; pp. 263–313. [Google Scholar]
  19. Glasow, P.A. Fundamentals of Survey Research Methodology; MITRE: McLean, VA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  20. Martin, E. Survey questionnaire construction. Surv. Methodol. 2006, 13, 1–13. [Google Scholar]
  21. Olson, K. An Examination of Questionnaire Evaluation by Expert Reviewers. Field Methods 2010, 22, 295–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Hinkin, T.R. A Brief Tutorial on the Development of Measures for Use in Survey Questionnaires. Organ. Res. Methods 1998, 1, 104–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Schriesheim, C.A.; Eisenbach, R.J. An Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor-Analytic Investigation of Item Wording Effects on the Obtained Factor Structures of Survey Questionnaire Measures. J. Manag. 1995, 21, 1177–1193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Mak, T.M.; Iris, K.M.; Tsang, D.C.; Hsu, S.C.; Poon, C.S. Promoting food waste recycling in the commercial and industrial sector by extending the Theory of Planned Behaviour: A Hong Kong case study. J. Clean. Prod. 2018, 204, 1034–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hong, S.; Adams, R.M. Household responses to price incentives for recycling: Some further evidence. Land Econ. 1999, 75, 505–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Thøgersen, J. Monetary incentives and recycling: Behavioural and psychological reactions to a performance-dependent garbage fee. J. Consum. Policy 2003, 26, 197–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Abila, B.; Kantola, J. The perceived role of financial incentives in promoting waste recycling—Empirical evidence from Finland. Recycling 2019, 4, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Gibovic, D.; Bikfalvi, A. Incentives for Plastic Recycling: How to Engage Citizens in Active Collection. Empirical Evidence from Spain. Recycling 2021, 6, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. OECD. Improving Markets for Recycled Plastics: Trends, Prospects and Policy Responses; OECD Publishing: Paris, France, 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Geyer, R.; Jambeck, J.R.; Law, K.L. Production, use, and fate of all plastics ever made. Sci. Adv. 2017, 3, e1700782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Results for participant’s demographics based on (a) age, (b) gender, (c) awareness, and (d) recycling efforts.
Figure 1. Results for participant’s demographics based on (a) age, (b) gender, (c) awareness, and (d) recycling efforts.
Sustainability 15 11600 g001
Table 1. Results for multiple-choice questions for assigning method.
Table 1. Results for multiple-choice questions for assigning method.
Q7Ethos130Q19Positive Reinforcement85
Pathos21Positive Punishment66
Q8Ethos93Q20Positive Reinforcement92
Logos58Negative Punishment59
Q9Aesthetics85Q21Negative Reinforcement85
Ethos66Positive Reinforcement66
Q10Pathos76Q22Positive Punishment87
Logos75Negative Punishment64
Q11Aesthetics121Q23Negative Reinforcement87
Pathos30Positive Punishment64
Q12Aesthetics108Q24Negative Reinforcement109
Logos43Negative Punishment42
Table 2. Chi-square analysis results for persuasion techniques for assigning method.
Table 2. Chi-square analysis results for persuasion techniques for assigning method.
Question NumberPersuasion TechniqueObserved ScoreExpected ScoreChi-Square Scorep-Value
Q7Ethos13075.578.68<0.001 *
Pathos21
Q8Ethos9375.58.110.004 *
Logos58
Q9Aesthetics8575.52.390.122
Ethos66
Q10Pathos7675.50.010.935
Logos75
Q11Aesthetics12175.554.84<0.001 *
Pathos30
Q12Aesthetics10875.527.98<0.001 *
Logos43
An asterisk (*) indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.01.
Table 3. Chi-square analysis results for operant conditioning for assigning method.
Table 3. Chi-square analysis results for operant conditioning for assigning method.
Question NumberOperant ConditionObserved ScoreExpected ScoreChi-Square Scorep-Value
Q19Positive Reinforcement8575.52.390.122
Positive Punishment66
Q20Positive Reinforcement9275.57.210.007 *
Negative Punishment59
Q21Negative Reinforcement8575.52.390.122
Positive Reinforcement66
Q22Positive Punishment8775.53.500.061
Negative Punishment64
Q23Negative Reinforcement8775.53.500.061
Positive Punishment64
Q24Negative Reinforcement10975.529.72<0.001 *
Negative Punishment42
An asterisk (*) indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.01.
Table 4. Results for multiple-choice questions for returning method.
Table 4. Results for multiple-choice questions for returning method.
Q13Ethos129Q25Positive Reinforcement93
Pathos22Positive Punishment58
Q14Ethos81Q26Positive Reinforcement105
Logos70Negative Punishment46
Q15Aesthetics82Q27Positive Reinforcement 79
Ethos69Negative Reinforcement72
Q16Logos86Q28Positive Punishment86
Pathos65Negative Punishment65
Q17Aesthetics123Q29Negative Reinforcement82
Pathos28Positive Punishment69
Q18Aesthetics90Q30Negative Reinforcement105
Logos61Negative Punishment46
Table 5. Chi-square analysis results for persuasion techniques for returning method.
Table 5. Chi-square analysis results for persuasion techniques for returning method.
Question NumberPersuasion TechniqueObserved ScoreExpected ScoreChi-Square Scorep-Value
Q13Ethos12975.575.82<0.001 *
Pathos22
Q14Ethos8175.50.800.370
Logos70
Q15Aesthetics8275.51.110.290
Ethos69
Q16Logos 8675.52.920.087
Pathos65
Q17Aesthetics12375.559.76<0.001 *
Pathos28
Q18Aesthetics9075.55.570.018
Logos61
An asterisk (*) indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.01.
Table 6. Chi-square analysis results for operant conditioning for assigning approach.
Table 6. Chi-square analysis results for operant conditioning for assigning approach.
Question NumberOperant ConditionObserved ScoreExpected ScoreChi-Square Scorep-Value
Q25Positive Reinforcement9375.58.1130.004 *
Positive Punishment58
Q26Positive Reinforcement10575.523.053<0.001 *
Negative Punishment46
Q27Positive Reinforcement 7975.50.3250.568
Negative Reinforcement72
Q28Positive Punishment8675.52.9210.087
Negative Punishment65
Q29Negative Reinforcement8275.51.1190.290
Positive Punishment69
Q30Negative Reinforcement10575.523.053<0.001 *
Negative Punishment46
An asterisk (*) indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.01.
Table 7. Results for Likert scale questions.
Table 7. Results for Likert scale questions.
Persuasion TechniqueQuestion NumberScoreMean Score
AestheticsQ354.374.28
Q404.31
Q454.32
Q504.28
Q544.13
EthosQ324.194.21
Q364.25
Q414.28
Q464.07
Q514.26
LogosQ344.063.46
Q394.06
Q443.80
Q482.21
Q533.15
PathosQ333.563.98
Q384.04
Q423.99
Q474.05
Q524.28
Table 8. Independent t-test results of Likert scale questions.
Table 8. Independent t-test results of Likert scale questions.
Comparison of Persuasion Techniquest-Valuep-Value
Ethos (4.21) with Pathos (3.98)2.250.024 *
Ethos (4.21) with Logos (3.46)2.590.013 *
Aesthetics (4.28) with Ethos (4.21)1.570.073
Aesthetics (4.28) with Pathos (3.98)2.940.007 *
Aesthetics (4.28) with Logos (3.46)2.840.008 *
Logos (3.46) with Pathos (3.98)1.610.069
An asterisk (*) indicates that the results are statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05.
Table 9. Summary of results for chi-square test on multiple-choice questions.
Table 9. Summary of results for chi-square test on multiple-choice questions.
EthosPathosLogosAestheticsPositive
Reinforcement
Positive
Punishment
Negative
Punishment
Negative
Reinforcement
Based on Age18–30Mean30.310.729.836.030.232.817.026.0
Std. Dev.12.54.95.79.06.56.84.66.7
31–45Mean35.316.321.735.830.228.318.832.7
Std. Dev.8.910.25.28.25.34.62.44.9
46+Mean25.712.012.324.823.29.815.827.2
Std. Dev.6.27.74.65.85.02.38.85.4
Prefer Not to AnswerMean3.31.31.73.63.20.72.04.2
Std. Dev.1.41.40.50.51.00.51.71.0
Based on GenderMaleMean44.719.826.545.838.728.728.042.7
Std. Dev.12.113.85.810.35.44.07.16.0
FemaleMean46.519.537.350.644.841.323.344.5
Std. Dev.15.38.89.29.19.38.43.28.5
Prefer Not to MentionMean2.00.71.02.42.50.01.32.2
Std. Dev.0.91.00.00.50.50.01.41.0
Non-BinaryMean1.50.30.71.40.71.71.00.7
Std. Dev.0.50.50.50.50.80.50.60.5
Based on AwarenessTremendousMean14.76.011.515.612.210.57.817.5
Std. Dev.4.12.43.43.53.72.13.93.0
HighMean48.719.330.348.844.033.328.242.5
Std. Dev.14.713.77.311.05.35.65.87.1
ModerateMean28.713.022.334.029.025.516.027.5
Std. Dev.10.37.65.06.14.95.31.94.7
LittleMean2.72.01.31.81.52.31.72.5
Std. Dev.0.50.60.50.40.80.50.80.5
Based on Recycling EffortsTremendousMean4.22.33.73.84.71.82.55.0
Std. Dev.0.80.50.51.11.01.01.61.7
HighMean44.217.031.348.841.032.524.344.2
Std. Dev.15.310.79.99.16.05.27.69.0
ModerateMean37.715.822.335.630.229.819.732.3
Std. Dev.9.511.53.29.46.05.61.84.6
LittleMean7.03.27.210.88.56.86.56.2
Std. Dev.4.01.51.82.21.80.82.41.2
Very LittleMean1.72.01.01.22.30.70.72.3
Std. Dev.0.50.00.00.40.50.80.81.0
Table 10. Independent t-test results.
Table 10. Independent t-test results.
AestheticsEthosLogosPathos
Based on
Age
18–304.404.403.634.06
31–454.314.253.434.05
46+4.153.973.273.87
Prefer Not to Answer3.843.563.323.32
Based on
Gender
Male4.274.143.473.94
Female4.324.303.484.05
Non-Binary4.304.302.903.90
Prefer Not to Mention3.803.673.133.47
Based on
Awareness
Tremendous4.444.523.284.08
High4.294.223.504.05
Moderate4.164.023.473.83
Little4.754.503.554.00
Based on
Recycling Efforts
Tremendous4.003.692.913.74
High4.444.423.474.21
Moderate4.174.033.543.83
Little3.973.973.433.43
Very Little4.735.003.004.87
Table 11. Results for Likert scale question for entropy calculation.
Table 11. Results for Likert scale question for entropy calculation.
[16] Reference Question NumberQuestion Number (Current Survey)Score
Q13Q553.66
Q14Q564.54
Q17Q573.60
Q27Q583.46
Q32Q594.14
Table 12. Entropy calculations for reworded questions.
Table 12. Entropy calculations for reworded questions.
[16] Reference Question NumberEntropy Values from [16]Question Number (Current Survey)Entropy Values from this Survey Entropy DifferenceEntropy Difference (%)
Q132.11Q552.100.010.47%
Q141.92Q561.300.6232.29%
Q172.03Q572.11−0.08−3.94%
Q272.21Q582.110.104.52%
Q322.16Q591.740.4219.44%
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ketkale, H.; Simske, S. Demographic Considerations in Incenting Reuse of Corrugated Cardboard Boxes. Sustainability 2023, 15, 11600. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151511600

AMA Style

Ketkale H, Simske S. Demographic Considerations in Incenting Reuse of Corrugated Cardboard Boxes. Sustainability. 2023; 15(15):11600. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151511600

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ketkale, Harshwardhan, and Steven Simske. 2023. "Demographic Considerations in Incenting Reuse of Corrugated Cardboard Boxes" Sustainability 15, no. 15: 11600. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151511600

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop