Next Article in Journal
The Design and Performance Prediction Model of an Integrated Scheme of a Membrane Bioreactor and Anaerobic Digester for the Treatment of Domestic Wastewater and Biowaste
Next Article in Special Issue
Workplace-Related Negative Career Shocks on Perceived Employability: The Role of Networking Behaviors and Perceived Career Opportunities
Previous Article in Journal
The Importance of Investing in the Energy Refurbishment of Hospitals: Results of a Case Study in a Mediterranean Climate
Previous Article in Special Issue
Analyzing Attitudes to Promote Sustainability: The Adaptation of the Environmental Concern Scale (ECs) to the Italian Context
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Positive or Negative Viewpoint Determines the Overall Scenic Beauty of a Scene: A Landscape Perception Evaluation Based on a Panoramic View

School of Architecture and Art, Central South University, Changsha 410083, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Sustainability 2023, 15(14), 11458; https://doi.org/10.3390/su151411458
Submission received: 4 July 2023 / Revised: 21 July 2023 / Accepted: 21 July 2023 / Published: 24 July 2023

Abstract

:
In the contemporary world, the swift advancement of urbanization, the pressing need for environmental conservation, and humanity’s unyielding quest for a better quality of life have jointly underscored the escalating importance of research on landscape aesthetics and perceptual experiences. Researchers have often evaluated the overall scene’s beauty based on photos taken from a single viewpoint. However, it has been observed that different viewpoints of the same scene can lead to varying degrees of beauty perception. Some positive viewpoints highlight landscape features that contribute to beauty preferences, while negative viewpoints emphasize aspects that may evoke discomfort and decrease perceived beauty. Therefore, a crucial question arises: which viewpoint, positive or negative, holds more influence over the overall beauty of the scene? This paper aimed to address this question by utilizing panoramic map technology to establish a landscape perception evaluation model. The model was based on empirical evidence from various spatial scenes along the Yaozijian Ancient Road in Anhua County, encompassing towns and villages. The study analyzed the functional relationship between landscape factors, positive and negative viewpoints, and the degree of scenic beauty. It was found that (1) it is difficult to reflect the overall scenic beauty of a scene (OSBS) of a single viewpoint photo, and both positive and negative viewpoints of scenic beauty have significant effects on the OSBS. In the empirical case study, it was found that the overall effect of a positive viewpoint of scenic beauty (PVSB) on OSBS was greater; (2) PVSB had a major effect on OSBS with a high visual hierarchy and cloud ratio and a low type of vegetation and proportion of man-made objects; (3) a negative viewpoint of scenic beauty (NVSB) had a major effect on OSBS with a low visual hierarchy of the landscape. The results of the study reveal the relationship between landscape factors of different viewpoints and the OSBS, which can be applied to landscape beauty evaluation and landscape planning and design processes.

1. Introduction

In today’s globalized world, landscape perception evaluation as an important research area has attracted widespread attention among different fields and stakeholders. In the contemporary world, the rapid expansion of urbanization has ushered in profound transformations in urban landscapes, triggering substantial shifts in people’s lifestyles and living environments. Against this backdrop, the evaluation of urban landscape perception goes beyond mere considerations of the quality of life and well-being of urban residents; it is intricately linked to the sustainable development and societal harmony of cities. A comprehensive investigation into urban landscape perception evaluation holds promise for uncovering innovative approaches to forging livable, enchanting urban environments and elevating the satisfaction and sense of belonging amongst city dwellers. Furthermore, environmental conservation has emerged as a pressing global concern, and landscape perception evaluation plays a paramount role in this narrative. Gaining insights into people’s perceptions and evaluations of natural landscapes is pivotal in underscoring the positive impact of natural environments on human health and well-being. Simultaneously, the preservation and restoration of natural landscapes necessitate due consideration of public perceptions and evaluations, necessitating amplified public engagement to drive the pursuit of sustainable environmental conservation. Moreover, landscape perception evaluation transcends the realm of individual environmental perceptions, delving into their influence on the broader aesthetics and emotional experiences. Embracing a comprehensive perspective, landscape enmeshes subjective evaluations and perceptual encounters concerning natural landscapes, urban settings, and architectural constructs. Unearthing people’s perceptions and evaluations of distinct landscape typologies can unveil the psychological and emotional ramifications of diverse environments for humanity, bestowing invaluable theoretical guidance to craft more captivating and comforting landscapes. In this vein, the assessment and enhancement of urban landscape aesthetics and perceptual experiences emerge as pivotal imperatives in the domains of urban planning and landscape design.
In recent years, important progress has been made in the study of landscape beauty and perceptual experience in the fields of urban planning [1,2,3], environmental design [4,5,6], tourism planning [7,8], and psychology [9,10,11,12]. In the current research landscape, the primary focus lies in investigating the impact of emotional experiences, environmental features, and socio-cultural aspects on the evaluation of landscape aesthetics. Concurrently, these studies grapple with intricate subjective factors, giving rise to noteworthy variations in perceptions and evaluations among individuals of diverse cultural backgrounds. Indeed, the subjective experiences of beauty and comfort in natural and architectural environments play a pivotal role in shaping how individuals assess and interact with their surroundings. In this pursuit, researchers employ a diverse array of methodologies, encompassing survey questionnaires, rating techniques, on-site observations, and cutting-edge advancements in neuroscience. This multifaceted approach enables a comprehensive understanding of landscape perception evaluation, delving into the intricate interplay of cognitive and emotional responses in shaping individuals’ aesthetic judgments. Furthermore, within the realm of landscape perception evaluation, the linkage between different landscape elements (ranging from natural landscape features, urbanization aspects, environmental quality, and conservation considerations to visual hierarchy and organizational components and even cultural diversity factors) remains a critical and stimulating research frontier. Through an in-depth exploration of the influence wielded by these diverse landscape elements on aesthetic perceptions and emotional experiences, researchers have unlocked valuable insights that resonate deeply with landscape designers and urban planners, offering indispensable guidance and inspiration. In conclusion, the ongoing quest to comprehend landscape aesthetics evaluation delves into multifaceted dimensions, from understanding human emotions and societal dynamics to employing diverse research methodologies. The findings from these studies are reshaping our understanding of landscape perception and propelling the creation of harmonious, appealing urban environments that foster a sense of connectedness and appreciation among inhabitants. However, in past studies, landscape perception evaluation usually relied on photographs or 2D images, ignoring the influence of different viewpoints. Different perspectives of the same scene can trigger different degrees of beauty, in fact, and this choice of perspective is often closely related to beauty preferences and emotional responses. Some perspectives reflect positive landscape factors that determine beauty preferences, such as open landscape views and the presence of natural elements, while other perspectives may highlight negative landscape factors that lead to beauty discomforts, such as visual clutter and environmental oppressiveness. Therefore, exploring the relative contribution of positive and negative perspectives (referred to as capturing the most beautiful aspect and the least appealing aspect within a panoramic shot) to the overall scenic beauty of a scene (OSBS) becomes an important and pending issue for further research.
To more accurately assess and predict the OSBS, emerging panoramic mapping technologies provide a more realistic and immersive perceptual experience. Traditional landscape studies typically rely on photographs or 2D images to depict scenes [5,6,12,13]. However, this approach falls short in recreating the genuine feelings and experiences of people within the actual environment. By capturing image information from multiple viewpoints, the panorama technique provides a broader and more lifelike view of the surroundings, creating a more authentic experience for observers. As a result, there has been considerable research interest in exploring the potential of this technique for landscape perception evaluation. However, current studies on the specific application of the panorama technique in landscape perception evaluation are mostly focused on a certain type of landscape empirical evidence, such as natural landscapes and architectural landscapes [5,6,12]. As a result, significant knowledge gaps persist regarding the relationship between different viewpoints and the varying influence of landscape factors on beauty perception. Some recent scholars have recently started to use panorama techniques to investigate the effects of different scenes on landscape perception evaluation. For example, Chen et al. (2022) found that the contribution of natural landscape elements and public space structures to rural aesthetics varied across scenes [2]. However, these studies still do not reveal the mechanisms of the influence of positive and negative perspectives on OSBS. Especially in the context of incorporating panoramic mapping techniques, there is a need to delve deeper into the functional relationship between different perspectives and scenic beauty degrees. Understanding the mechanisms through which landscape factors influence scenic beauty under various viewpoints is crucial.
Therefore, the objective of this study was to develop a landscape perception evaluation model based on panoramic map technology using the Yaozijian Ancient Road in Anhua County as an empirical object and to analyze the relationship between positive and negative viewpoints and the scenic beauty degree. Through systematic empirical research, this study explored the relative importance of different viewpoints in the OSBS and the degree of contribution of landscape factors to the beauty under different viewpoints. This will help deepen the understanding of landscape perception evaluation and provide a scientific basis to guide landscape planning and design.

2. Review

2.1. Advantages and Limitations of Traditional Photos and 2D Images in Visual Evaluation of Landscapes

The visual evaluation of landscapes holds significant importance for researchers in landscape planning and design. Traditional photographs and 2D images are widely used as evaluation tools in this field. They have been extensively applied in various studies, including visual evaluation [2,12,14,15], preference surveys [1,5,6,8], affective assessment [16,17,18], and simulated experiences [1,3,16,19]. For example, Tang et al. (2015) employed photographs and images, prompting participants to assess the quality of three different types of rural forest landscapes based on their personal feelings and subjective evaluations. These assessments enabled researchers to compare differences between landscapes [12]. Luckmann et al. (2013) presented participants with various landscape photographs or images, requesting them to rank, score, or select photos according to their individual preferences. This approach allowed researchers to collect data on individual landscape preferences [6]. Petrova et al. (2015) asked participants to view photos or images and assess them on emotional dimensions, yielding similarities and differences in visual and emotional assessments of landscapes based on ethno-cultural and regional differences [18]. Chen et al. (2022b) provided participants with real or virtual landscape images, allowing them to mentally experience specific landscape environments, and subsequently assessed the effects of landscape on individual psychological and emotional states [16].
These related studies reveal the advantages of the controllability, reproducibility, and generalizability of photographs or 2D images in landscape perception assessment. Scholars such as Qin et al. (2023) emphasize that utilizing photographs and geospatial data for landscape visual quality assessment offers controllability, enabling direct comparisons between scenes and delivering consistent quantitative and qualitative results [1]. Munoz-Pedreros (2004) noted that using photographs as an assessment tool can increase efficiency and reduce participant workload, thereby reducing the cost and complexity of surveys [13]. Svobodova et al. (2018) and others used photographs as stimulus material to compare and measure participants’ visual evaluations with natural values. By using photographs, researchers were able to ensure that each participant saw the same landscape stimuli, eliminating possible variability in field surveys [20]. Chen et al. explored the use of photographs and images to visualize and measure changes in urban green spaces. Employing photographs and images allows researchers to conduct assessments in a safe and controlled environment, especially for landscape types that may be potentially risky or inaccessible in the field [3,21,22]. Akten and Celik et al. (2013) assessed the visual quality of landscapes in urban parks by using photographs. Studies have shown that photographs can provide reliable visual material to compare and analyze the visual quality of landscapes in different geographical and cultural contexts [8]. Photographs or two-dimensional images substantially help existing studies, however, some researchers have raised concerns about the validity of photo evaluation techniques. It has been pointed out that individual photographs may fully capture the diversity and dynamics of the scene, leading to potentially inaccurate evaluation results. Since photographs from different viewpoints have different degrees of beauty, using a single photograph may not accurately reflect the observer’s beauty evaluation of the overall scene [1,20,22,23]. Moreover, traditional photographs or 2D image evaluation techniques may not accurately convey the realism and emotional experience of the landscape; photographs do not provide a 3D and immersive experience and may not fully capture the overall scenic beauty of the landscape [2,24,25,26]. In the realm of panoramic imagery, the direct observation of two-dimensional photographs differs significantly from the immersive experience afforded by alternative methods, such as utilizing the “computer screen + indoor large screen projection” setup or engaging with VR devices to perceive 720° VR panoramas. These distinctions give rise to variations in comfort, perceptual experiences, and their suitability for diverse contexts. Specifically, the adoption of the “computer screen + indoor large screen projection” approach to view and experience 720° VR panoramas may present heightened levels of comfort and convenience. Consequently, the choice of viewing method may exert distinct influences on the perception and experiential aspects of the showcased landscapes. Importantly, observing panoramas on a screen may resonate more closely with respondents’ inherent perceptual habits of the real world, primarily due to their preference for engaging with images and videos on curved displays. As the discourse surrounding the most optimal method for immersive panoramic experiences continues to evolve, an in-depth exploration of the interplay between perceptual habits and viewing technologies sheds invaluable light on the frontiers of panoramic landscape exploration. In addition, because photographs are selected and composed by the photographer, photo evaluation may be influenced by the subjective choices of the photographer and editing, and there may be inconsistencies in individual preferences and assessment results.
In conclusion, traditional photographs or 2D images offer certain advantages, such as controllability, cost-effectiveness, and safety, making them valuable tools in visual evaluation studies. However, they also come with inherent limitations, including a lack of foreground bias, limited field experience, static presentation, and subjective selectivity. Therefore, when utilizing these tools for evaluation, it is essential to carefully consider their drawbacks and complement them with other methods to achieve more comprehensive and accurate results.

2.2. Application of Panoramic Techniques in Landscape Perception Evaluation

The panorama technique is progressively gaining traction in landscape perception evaluation studies due to its comprehensive image presentation capabilities. Numerous studies have highlighted the evident advantages of using panoramas for landscape evaluation as they tend to stimulate observers’ attention and evoke more positive emotions, resulting in enhanced reliability in beauty assessment [2,27,28,29]. Exploring the effects of different environments on observers’ emotional experiences using panorama techniques has revealed that panoramas create a greater sense of relaxation, pleasure, and satisfaction among observers. Consequently, panorama techniques have proven to be a reliable means for emotional research [16,30,31]. In addition, panoramas can provide more details and visual information, enabling observers to perceive and evaluate the beauty of a scene more comprehensively. Chen et al. (2023) conducted a study using VR panoramas to present landscape scenes and discovered that panorama technology better portrayed the overall scenic beauty, garnering more interest and appreciation from observers [32,33,34,35,36]. Similarly, Sun et al. (2019) utilized VR panorama technology to present urban landscape scenes and extracted street vegetation coverage by using Sentinel-2 images and the greenscape index by using Baidu Street View panorama to assess the comprehensive evaluation index of street greening. The results of the study showed that the panorama technology can comprehensively measure the street greening distribution information and guide the planning and management of the urban greening landscape [37]. Several studies have also explored the application of panorama techniques in the evaluation of different types of landscapes [1,28,29,38,39]. For example, Zhang et al. used panoramic techniques to present natural landscape scenes to study participants’ beauty evaluations of different landscape features (e.g., water, vegetation). The results of the study showed that the panorama technique could provide more diverse scene information, enabling participants to more accurately evaluate the impact of different landscape features on beauty [28,29,38]. Additionally, Qin et al. delved into the panorama technique’s application in natural landscape perception. Utilizing panoramic images to showcase different natural landscapes, the researchers assessed participants’ beauty experiences through subjective evaluations and physiological indicators, such as heart rate and electrical skin response. The findings indicated that panorama technology could provide a realistic landscape perception experience and capture participants’ emotional and physiological responses [1,28,39].
Although the existing literature suggests that panoramas can reflect the OSBS, the beauty relationship between different viewpoints and the overall scene has not been adequately studied. Most studies have focused on assessing the OSBS of panoramas, while there is limited understanding of the effects of different viewpoints on the beauty experience and evaluation of the observer. Consequently, further research is necessary to explore the role of different viewpoints within panorama techniques and the observers’ beauty preferences concerning these various perspectives. Such a study would be instrumental in enhancing our understanding of how observers develop an overall sense of scenic beauty, and it could offer more precise guidance for design and planning.
In summary, the existing literature suggests that panoramic technology has a wide potential for application in landscape perception evaluation. It can provide a more realistic and immersive observation experience and can more accurately capture participants’ emotional responses and beauty evaluations. In addition, panoramic technology can help to study the influence of different environmental factors and perspectives on the OSBS, providing new research tools and perspectives. However, further in-depth research on the beauty relationship between different perspectives and the overall scene is needed to better understand and evaluate the overall beauty of the landscape.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Study Area

To analyze the mechanism of scenic beauty influence of different perspectives and landscape factors in different scenes and scenes of landscape tourism routes in urban and rural areas, historical walking trails through rural and urban areas were selected for empirical research. The study area is located in Huanghuaxi Village, Zhulinxi Village, Dongshi Village, Xitan Village, Sixian Village, Jiangnan Town, and Dongping Town in Anhua County, Yiyang City, Hunan Province, a total of two towns and five villages, which include the Yaozijian Ancient Road and the modern towns within the radiation of its tourism supporting services. Yaozijian Ancient Road is an important starting point of the Wanli tea road (Hunan section) inscription, which is about 30 km from the Yuanqi Bridge in Huanghuaxi Village, Jiangnan Town, Anhua County, to the Wufu Gong Pier on the bank of Zijang River in Jiangnan Town, with a summit elevation of about 512 m. The main nodes of the ancient road from south to north are Yuanqi Bridge—Dapingxiehuo Store—Yaozijian Stone Road (Ganlu Pavilion, Yizhong, ancient monuments, cliff carvings, Meng Gong Temple)—Juegong Bridge—Dongsi Old Street—Yongxi Bridge Yongxi Bridge)—Sixian Bridge—Zijiang River in Jiangnan Town (Liangzuo Tea House, Dehe Tea House, Liangjia Pier in Jiangnan, Wufugong Pier). As the most important section of Anhua’s ancient tea route, it witnessed the historical process of the prosperity and decline of the tea trade in the ancient Meishan area. At the same time, this section of the ancient tea road involves a variety of landscape types and there are great differences in visual beauty between different scenes, mainly natural landscape, rural landscape, or town landscape. Because the historical relics need to be kept intact and the modernization of the surrounding environment varies, there are also large visual beauty differences between different viewpoints within the same scene. The above two aspects of the difference in characteristics just met the needs of the research questions in this study. In addition, the eight scenes in Anhua County were not collected at the Yaozijian Ancient Road but were selected as modern townscapes within the radiation of tourism support services along the ancient tea horse road, forming a strong contrast in style with other scenes, which is more capable of supporting the issues explored in this paper. The remaining 53 scenes were all located on the Yaozijian Ancient Road (as shown in Figure 1).

3.2. Site Photos and VR Scenes

Sixty-one panoramic photographs were taken of selected representative landscape nodes within the study area. These photos were taken between 08:00 and 18:00 in clear weather in April 2019 and September 2021, respectively, and were able to reproduce the landscape elements within the scenes. To ensure that the images were captured under relatively uniform environmental conditions, a series of shooting guidelines were developed for this study: 1. Photographs were taken in sufficient natural light without the use of flash; 2. Photographs were taken using a panoramic camera to maximize the reflection of the landscape within the field of view; and 3. The photographer remained upright with the lens at the same height as both eyes to ensure a consistent angle.
In the next steps, we invited nine university professors majoring in landscape design and visual design as experts to evaluate these 61 photos, from which we selected 20 panorama photos of urban and rural scenes with the most distinctive characteristics of both kinds of differences as the samples for the empirical study. To ensure that the selected photos present as many positive and negative perspective differences as possible, this study sought to take this into full consideration in the selection of experts. At the same time, this study referred to a pre-drafted list of landscape perception evaluation indicators to ensure that the screened landscape samples could reflect the landscape patterns of each survey area as much as possible, and these samples were categorized and labeled. With the premise of minimizing the testers’ testing time, we made efforts to present the original appearance of the landscape space in the region comprehensively.
To provide experts with a comprehensive understanding of the site, this study also collected 192 general photographs and 28 video recordings from each site, which demonstrated the various evaluation elements of the site and provided them with additional information.
Through the above methods, a set of high-quality panoramic photographs and related supporting materials were obtained (as shown in Figure 2).

3.3. Respondents

Studies have shown that the aesthetic scales of evaluators from different cultural backgrounds do not differ statistically significantly and that experts in design and university students have better aesthetic perceptions than the general public [40,41]. In particular, college students, as an urban youth population, have a gradually increasing voice in the family, and their behaviors and preferences have an impact on family travel plans. In addition, young people have high energy and a strong ability to travel, and they are the main group of tourism consumers and the main participants in rural tourism [42,43,44]. Urban youth are more receptive to new things and master the operation of computers and VR experience software, which helped ensure the smooth conduct of this experiment and to obtain experimental data with complete and reliable references. Therefore, it is hoped that this study will provide strong support for the construction of rural and urban landscapes.
In consideration of shortening the subjects’ experimental operation time to ensure that their ratings truly reflect their feelings, this study chose to organize students’ participation in the experiment in a school computer room to obtain sample data for subjective evaluation (as shown in Table 1 and Figure 3). Meanwhile, the uniform scores of objective indicators were determined using expert evaluation.

3.4. Experimental Design

3.4.1. Extraction of Landscape Factors

After sorting out the existing studies in the field of landscape evaluation, a set of landscape perception evaluation indicator systems was initially constructed mainly based on the content of relevant studies published by Rogge E et al. [45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57], and was then adjusted by combining the characteristics of the sample and the effect of small-scale pre-experiments in this study to ensure that each indicator was reflected in each panorama. At the same time, the subjects conducted scoring experiments to ensure that there were no critical problems such as confusion. A scale of measurement for the village landscape perception evaluation indicators applicable to this study was finally determined (as shown in Table 2 below). The element layer was divided into four major categories: overall environment, sky, soft elements, and man-made elements, of which six indicators were evaluated by the public (C1, C2, C3, C4, C8, C15) and eleven indicators were evaluated by experts (C5, C6, C7, C9, C10, C11, C12, C13, C14, C16, C17), totaling seventeen indicators.

3.4.2. Hypothesized Model

In this study, SmartPLS 3.0 was used as the statistical modeling technique. Based on the preliminary theoretical foundation and experimental research, we constructed a structural model for analysis using the scenic beauty degree from different viewpoints as the mediating variable, the overall scenic beauty degree (OSBD) of the scene as the dependent variable, and the landscape factor as the independent variable (as shown in Figure 4). The following research hypotheses were proposed in this study:
Hypothesis 1.
There is an influential relationship between landscape indicators and the overall beauty of the scene (H1);
Hypothesis 2.
There is a direct influence of positive/negative viewpoint of scenic beauty on the OSBD (H2);
Hypothesis 3.
There is a significant mediating role of positive/negative viewpoint of scenic beauty in the relationship between landscape indicators and OSBS (H3).

3.4.3. Measurement of Variables

Compared with the traditional landscape visual evaluation experiments, the subjects in this experiment used the “computer screen + indoor large screen projection” method to view the perceptual 720° VR panorama, which can enable the subjects to perceive the elements in the scene more comprehensively and show them a more comprehensive and realistic scene situation as much as possible, which helps the subject’s score to be more similar to that of the scene. First, the 20 scene samples were disordered and renumbered. The organizer first explained the purpose, process, and requirements of the test to the judges about 30 min before the evaluation but did not involve standardized descriptions of the details of the judged subjects.
The first session of the experiment. In scoring the OSBD, the scenic beauty degree evaluation method (SBE) was used. The subjects viewed 20 panoramas by themselves through panorama projection software (DevalVR player) and rated the OSBS of the 20 scenes in order according to their overall impression. The response scale was based on a 5-point scale, with five levels of “dislike, dislike, average, like, and like very much”, and the corresponding scores were 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 in order.
The second part of the experiment. The group extracted the landscape elements reflected in the panorama map and constructed a landscape perception evaluation index system for Hunan Yiyang villages and towns. Based on this, the experimental subjects rated the content corresponding to the evaluation indexes in these 20 scenes in turn according to their feelings when viewing the 20 panoramic pictures. To ensure that the subjects’ scoring could reflect their subjective feelings, the process required the subjects to avoid mutual communication and reference.
The third session of the experiment. Based on the premise of panorama technology and the purpose of the experiment, the experiment added a session on the scenic beauty of positive and negative viewpoints. The expert group cut out 20 scenes of positive perspective (numbered 1–20) and negative perspective (numbered 21–40) photos in advance (as shown in Figure 5). The subjects were asked to set aside their overall impression of the scene and rate the beauty of these 40 photos in turn according to their feelings at the moment.
The fourth session of the experiment. The experts were called into an online meeting and the expert leader showed 20 scenes in turn through screen sharing and divided into 11 rounds to ask opinions on the objective indicator scores of the scenes, with each round scoring a single indicator 20 times, and finally filling in the scoring table with the scores unanimously approved by the nine experts, which was intended to ensure a more objective and fair score for each scene through horizontal comparison.

3.4.4. Sampling and Sample Size

One-hundred-and-fifty-two experimental subjects and nine experts in this field were recruited as evaluation subjects to conduct subjective evaluations of 20 scenes in two towns and five villages. The experiment obtained 157 samples of subjective evaluation data, eliminated 26 samples participating in the pre-experiment, and finally retained 131 samples, totaling 2620 data. Nine experts conducted an objective evaluation in the form of talks and discussions, and made a unified decision on the objective evaluation indicator scores, obtaining 220 data points.

3.4.5. Test Method

  • Reliability test: to ensure the reliability and consistency of the measurement instruments, this study conducted a reliability test on the sample data obtained from the experiment. In the reliability test, internal consistency indicators, such as Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, were calculated for each measurement item, and the correlation between the measurement items and the total score was assessed.
  • Validity test: To verify the validity and accuracy of the measurement instrument, a validity test was conducted in this study. In the validity test, the degree of correlation between the measurement items and the theoretical concepts was assessed using the construct validity and reflective validity methods.
  • Covariance test: To exclude a high correlation between independent variables, this study conducted a covariance test. The degree of covariance between the independent variables was assessed by calculating the variance inflation factor (VIF) and the number of conditions, and variables that could lead to multiple covariances were excluded.
  • Correlation test: To determine the correlation between the variables, this study conducted a correlation test. The correlation between the independent and dependent variables was assessed by calculating the Pearson correlation coefficient.
  • Mediating effects test: In SmartPLS 3.0, this study first constructed a structural equation model based on the research objectives and theoretical assumptions. This included identifying the independent, mediating, and dependent variables and defining the relationships among them; second, in evaluating the structural model, this study used path analysis to test the direct effects of the independent variables on the dependent variable. By analyzing the path coefficients, t-values, significance levels, and R-squared values, it was possible to determine the degree of influence and statistical significance of the independent variables; finally, to test the mediating effects, the Bootstrap method was used to calculate the confidence intervals and significance of the indirect effects.

4. Results

4.1. Reliability and Validity Tests

Based on the sample data obtained from this experiment, a reliability test was conducted and the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.884, indicating that there is internal consistency among multiple items of each variable and the reliability of the sample data is good. At the same time, the validity of the sample data was tested, and the KMO value was 0.911, and the p-value was 0.000 (significant), which indicated that the variables were suitable for factor analysis and the index system had good coverage and scientific validity.

4.2. Covariance, Correlation Test

First, this study examined the correlations among the variables, as well as assessed whether covariance among the predictor variables would cause problems in the structural model. In this study, after correlation analysis of the sample data, it was found that only the C12 indicator was not significantly correlated with the scenic beauty and was not necessary to exist in the subsequent model calculation, and the C12 indicator (percentage of exposed soil) was excluded (as shown in Figure 6). After covariance statistics, it was found that the VIF values of the C6 and C11 indicator scores were greater than 10 and, to ensure the best model construction, the C6 and C11 indicators (visual area depth, vegetation ratio). Finally, the remaining 14 indicators were tested for covariance again, in which the VIF values were all greater than 0.20 and less than 5.00, indicating that the model did not have multiple covariance problems, the model was well constructed, and the variables had significant correlations within a reasonable range, and the structural model construction and evaluation could be continued in the next step (as shown in Table 3).

4.3. Meditational Model Evaluation

The R2 test was conducted in this study and the R2 value was 0.45, indicating that the model possesses good predictive accuracy. In the mediation model, it was first tested whether the direct relationship between landscape indicators and scenic beauty was moderated by the mediation effect of positive/negative viewpoint scenic beauty. The structural equation model performs the mediation effect analysis in several steps: in the first stage, it assesses the direct effect, i.e., the direct effect of landscape indicators on scene beauty perception (OSBSP), and the direct effect of positive/negative viewpoint of scenic beauty perception on OSBSP, while excluding the mediation effect between both landscape indicators and OSBSP. As the data in Table 4 show, the path coefficients between the nine landscape indicators C1, C2, C3, C4, C7, C8, C9, C14, and C15 and OSBSP are significant, indicating that the indicator variables consisting of 14 indicators have an extremely strong direct effect on OSBSP, supporting research hypothesis H1. The path coefficients between positive and negative viewpoints of scenic beauty perception and OSBSP are significant, and the path coefficients of positive viewpoint scenic beauty perceptions (PVSBP) were greater than negative viewpoint scenic beauty perceptions (NVSBP), indicating that the direct influence of PVSBP on OSBSP was greater, thus supporting research hypothesis H2.
The next step was to test for mediating effects. This study utilized the variance as a percentage (VAF) calculation to determine the extent to which mediating variables absorb the direct relationship [58,59,60]. It determines the extent to which the variance in OSBSP is explained by the indirect effect of positive/negative viewpoint scenic beauty perception. As shown in Table 5, C5 (Visual hierarchy), C10 (Cloud ratio), C13 (Type of vegetation), and C16 (Proportion of man-made objects) significantly mediated the OSBS through the PVSB with VAF of 46.7%, 30.6%, 52.0%, and 76.2%, respectively. In addition, C5 (visual hierarchy of the landscape) had a significant mediating effect on scenic beauty through negative perception, with a VAF of 23.3%. The above results are all “partially mediated” effects and therefore support the research hypothesis H3.

5. Discussion

5.1. Landscape Factors that Have a Direct and Significant Impact on the OSBS

As shown in Figure 7, the results of this study indicate that C1 (visual harmony of the scenery), C2 (color Beauty), C3 (historic ambiance), C4 (sense of order and regularity), C7 (dirt and pollution level), C8 (skyline beauty), C9 (sky ratio), C14 (water ratio), and C15 (proportion of man-made objects) were all found to play an important role in the observer’s beauty experience.
Firstly, C1 positively affects the evaluation of the OSBS, as it imparts a sense of balance and harmony to the panorama, aligning with people’s beauty preferences. This result is consistent with previous studies and supports the importance of C1 on the OSBS [4,61,62,63]. Secondly, C2 also plays a key role in the evaluation of the OSBS. Brighter, moderately saturated, and color-coordinated scenes tend to be more popular. Therefore, the presence of C2 can enhance the observer’s beauty experience of the panorama [49,64,65,66,67]. Moreover, the presentation of C3 has a significant positive impact on the beauty evaluation. Scenes conveying historical, cultural, and traditional elements evoke emotional resonance in observers, thereby enhancing their beauty evaluation. This finding aligns with social research highlighting the influence of historical and cultural factors in aesthetic perception [68,69,70]. Furthermore, C4 exhibits a positive association with the evaluation of OSBS. Observers are more inclined to have a positive beauty experience of an ordered and neat scene. This may be related to human preferences for clarity, legibility, and simplicity [71]; On the other hand, C7 negatively influences the evaluation of OSBS. Observers tend to perceive clean and tidy scenes as beautiful and appealing [72,73,74,75]. Consequently, a higher level of dirtiness and pollution in the panorama can negatively impact observers’ beauty experiences. C8 has a positive effect on the OSBS evaluation. This shows that the shape, height, and curved features of the skyline have an impact on the viewer’s beauty experience. When the skyline in the panorama has a unique and beautiful shape, appropriate height, and smooth lines, observers are more likely to have a positive aesthetic experience. C9 has an important negative impact on scenic beauty. A disproportionately large sky area can result in a scene lacking focus and direction for the viewer’s eye. Landscape elements are often employed in design to guide the viewer’s gaze and eye flow, creating a compelling focal point. Nevertheless, an excessive sky area can dilute the overall focus, leading to a diminished beauty experience. Further investigation is needed to pinpoint the exact reasons for this effect. C14 plays an important positive role in the OSBS evaluation. The viewer is more likely to have a positive beauty experience when a moderate amount of water is present in the scene and the characteristics of the water body can be reasonably displayed. This result is consistent with previous studies that found that the presence of water bodies can increase the visual appeal and beauty experience of a scene [76,77]. The reflection and flow of water bodies can bring dynamism and variability, thus enhancing the vividness and fascination of the scene. Finally, C15 also shows a significant positive effect on the beauty evaluation. Viewers are more likely to perceive artifacts as attractive and aesthetically pleasing scenes when they exhibit characteristics that are unique, exquisite, and in harmony with the environment, which is consistent with findings in architectural beauty regarding the influence of architectural structures on beauty evaluation [78,79,80]. In conclusion, these findings are in line with existing research on visual perception, beauty preferences, and sociocultural factors, and providing valuable insight and understanding for the field of landscape aesthetics and design.

5.2. The Beauty of Viewpoint Has a Direct and Significant Impact on the OSBS

In this study, it was found that positive and negative viewpoints of scenic beauty perceptions have a direct and significant positive impact on the OSBS perceptions, with PVSBP having a greater degree of impact than negative viewpoints. This finding has important theoretical and practical implications in the study of landscape perception evaluation. First, the results indicate that viewers are more inclined to make positive beauty evaluations of positive viewpoints when perceiving scenes. This is consistent with relevant findings in affective psychology, where affective evaluations have an important influence on the experience of beauty. Viewers may be more likely to experience pleasurable and satisfying emotions from the positive viewpoint, which in turn enhances the beauty perception of the whole scene [1,81,82]. In addition, studies have revealed the role of visual guidance and focus creation in the beauty experience. A positive viewpoint is usually effective in guiding the viewer’s gaze and eye flow, creating a compelling focal point. This focused composition allows the viewer to immerse themselves more deeply, resulting in a more profound and comprehensive beauty experience [3,16,19]. In contrast, a negative viewpoint may not effectively direct the line of sight, resulting in a distraction of the viewer’s attention, thus reducing the focus of the beauty experience. In addition, the findings have been correlated with the association of individual preferences and cultural background. According to existing studies, viewers’ individual preferences and cultural backgrounds also have an impact on the beauty experience. Some studies have shown that people are more inclined to make positive comments about positive, beautiful views of the landscape. This is related to individual beauty preferences and cultural values [9,10,11,83]. Thus, the positive viewpoint of scenic beauty degree (PVSBD) may be supported to some extent by individual preferences and cultural contexts, thus having a greater impact on the OSBS.
In light of the foregoing, the diverse effects of positive and negative viewpoints on the overall aesthetic perception of a scene can be ascribed to an intricate interplay of emotional experiences, heightened aesthetic appreciation, subjective evaluations, and varying points of focus. Specifically, positive viewpoints tend to evoke positive emotional experiences, encompassing joy, satisfaction, and a sense of happiness. Consequently, individuals are more inclined to perceive the landscape as aesthetically pleasing, deeming it delightful and deserving of admiration. As viewpoints and emotions are inherently subjective, positive perspectives may engender favorable evaluations of the same landscape, while contrasting negative viewpoints may result in less favorable assessments. Moreover, positive and negative viewpoints can distinctly shape individuals’ focus during the process of perceiving a landscape. Positive viewpoints incline individuals to pay heightened attention to the landscape’s beauty and merits, while downplaying or diminishing their focus on any existing flaws. In contrast, negative viewpoints tend to drive individuals to concentrate more on the landscape’s problems and shortcomings, thereby overlooking or reducing their attention to its exquisite aspects. In essence, the dichotomy between positive and negative viewpoints introduces intriguing implications for the holistic aesthetic perception of landscapes, shedding light on the multifaceted factors that influence our subjective appreciation of the natural environment. These insights have significant ramifications for landscape design, planning, and management, paving the way for a more comprehensive understanding of the nuanced relationship between human perception and landscape aesthetics.

5.3. Landscape Factors That Indirectly Affect the OSBS through the Viewpoint Scenic Beauty

As shown in Figure 8, the results of this study show that C5 (visual hierarchy) plays an important mediating role in the influence of positive and negative viewpoints of scenic beauty on the OSBS, a finding that is supported by existing studies. First, PVSB focuses more on the positive features and beautiful elements of the scene. In this case, the role of visual hierarchy in enhancing the beauty experience may be more obvious. A higher visual hierarchy provides more visual variation and hierarchy, increasing the visual appeal and emotional evaluation of the viewer, which further enhances their evaluation of the OSBS [49]. In contrast, the NVSBP focuses more on the negative features and flaws of the scene. When viewers hold a negative viewpoint, they are more inclined to perceive and focus on the discord, incongruity, and unpleasantness of the scene. In this case, the effect of C5 on the beauty experience may be relatively small. Even though visual hierarchy is higher, it may not change the viewer’s negative affective evaluation of the scene as a whole, which is consistent with previous findings, such as a study that found that, in urban landscapes, low-level landscape organization may lead to a sense of chaos and dissonance, triggering negative affective evaluations and thus reducing OSBS perception [64,84]. In addition, individual psychological preferences and cognitive biases may also differ in their mediating influence on positive and negative viewpoint scenic beauty perceptions. Different viewers may differ in the importance of beauty elements and the way they perceive them, which may lead them to perceive and evaluate the visual hierarchy differently and thus influence the extent of its mediating effect [8,11,81,83].
Secondly, C10 (cloud ratio) had a significant positive impact on the OSBS through the PVSBD. This finding can be supported by existing studies. Previous research has demonstrated that the positive viewpoint offers an open and wide visual experience, which is positively correlated with the beauty rating of the scene. Positive viewpoint screenshots typically present expansive views, bright environments, and a positive and pleasant atmosphere, characteristics that are strongly associated with aesthetic perception and satisfaction [4,85,86]. In addition, C10, an important element in the landscape, has a significant impact on the beauty of the scene. A higher cloud ratio usually implies greater sky coverage, creating a light and serene feeling that aligns the sense of openness and positive emotions brought about by a positive viewpoint, thus positively influencing the OSBS [87].
Furthermore, this study also found that C13 (type of vegetation) had a significant negative effect on the OSBS through PVSBP. This may be attributed to the different perceptual mechanisms and viewing perspectives. The positive viewpoint usually emphasizes the openness and expansiveness of the scene, which may create a visual conflict with the large range of type of vegetation. An abundance of vegetation may make the scene appear too dense or complex, thus reducing the OSBD. Moreover, dense vegetation may also cause visual confusion, making it difficult to focus on the overall scene and consequently impacting the perception of beauty.
In addition, C16 (proportion of man-made objects) was also found to have a significantly negative effect on the OSBS through the PVSBD. One possible explanation is the balance between the naturalness and the artificiality of the landscape. In natural landscapes, man-made objects are often considered to be a disruptive factor in the harmony of nature. Viewers may prefer to look for natural elements in a natural landscape, and excessive or bulky man-made objects may upset this balance [6,79]. The negative impact on the PVSBP may be due to the fact that excessive or bulky man-made objects cause differences in viewers’ perceptions of the overall naturalness of the scene, thus reducing the beauty evaluation.

5.4. Bias

It is essential to acknowledge that the findings of this dissertation might be affected by sampling bias and constrained by the scope and characteristics of the study sample, which could limit the generalizability of the results. The determinants of scene aesthetics can vary significantly from person to person and are influenced by numerous factors, such as culture, social context, individual experiences, and subjective preferences. As a result, other studies may arrive at different conclusions. To enhance the reliability and generalizability of the findings, future studies should consider employing larger sample sizes and collecting data from diverse regions. This approach would allow for a more comprehensive understanding of the factors influencing scene aesthetics. Additionally, further investigations are necessary to explore the mechanisms through which positive and negative perspectives impact the OSBS in various contexts and among different individuals.

6. Conclusions

In this study, 20 different spatial scenes of Anhua County Yaozijian Ancient Road in town and countryside were selected as empirical objects, and 20 photos of positive/negative viewpoints were intercepted to establish a landscape perception evaluation model and analyze the functional relationship between landscape factors, positive and negative viewpoints, and the OSBD of the scenes. As a result, the following important conclusions were drawn:
  • Visual harmony of the scenery, color beauty, historic ambiance, sense of order and regularity, dirt and pollution level, skyline beauty, sky ratio, water ratio, and proportion of man-made objects have a direct and significant impact on the OSBS of the panorama shot. Therefore, in landscape design and planning, due consideration should be given to the handling and presentation of these landscape elements to enhance aesthetic outcomes. Designers can elevate the landscape aesthetics by accentuating these crucial elements, thus crafting more appealing, beautiful, and inviting outdoor spaces.
  • The results of the empirical case study indicate that the overall effect of the positive viewpoint on the OSBS is greater than that of the negative viewpoint. Therefore, in landscape design, emphasizing positive viewpoints and employing strategies to enhance landscape aesthetics can yield superior results, augmenting the allure and affinity of the landscape.
  • Visual hierarchy, cloud ratio, type of vegetation, and proportion of man-made objects play a key role in mediating the influence of PVSB on the OSBS, and their influence is greater. Visual hierarchy reflects the richness and hierarchy of elements in the scene, which in turn has a positive influence on the overall beauty of the viewer. Visual hierarchy reflects the richness and hierarchy of elements in the scene, which in turn positively influences the viewer’s OSBS. In addition, visual hierarchy also mediates the OSBS through the negative viewpoint of scenic beauty, although the degree of its mediating influence is relatively low. In landscape design and planning, it is essential to pay attention to and effectively leverage the role of these intermediary factors to enhance the quality and overall aesthetic appeal of the landscape.
These findings have important implications for landscape beauty and scene perception evaluation and provide a scientific basis for landscape design, urban planning, and environmental management. In future practice, we should place emphasis on the treatment and presentation of landscape elements, prioritize the cultivation of positive viewpoints, and effectively utilize the role of intermediary factors. By doing so, we can create more captivating, beautiful, and public-friendly landscape spaces, fostering sustainable development and improvements in urban ecological environments. This study provided new perspectives and methodologies to further advance research in landscape aesthetics evaluation and landscape planning. Future research can further explore other potential influencing factors and delve into the interrelationships among the factors to further improve the understanding and evaluation methods of OSBS.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.L., Y.C. and Q.Z.; methodology, Q.Z., Y.C. and B.L.; software, Y.C. and Q.Z.; validation, B.L., Q.Z. and Y.C.; formal analysis, Y.C., Q.Z. and B.L.; investigation, Y.C., Q.Z. and B.L.; resources, B.L., Y.C. and Q.Z.; data curation, Y.C. and Q.Z.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.C., Q.Z. and B.L.; writing—review and editing, B.L., Q.Z., and Y.C.; visualization, Q.Z. and Y.C.; supervision, B.L.; project administration, B.L.; funding acquisition, B.L.. Y.C. and Q.Z. contributed equally to this manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Humanities and Social Science Fund of Chinese Ministry of Education (Number: 22YJA760034).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Qin, X.; Fang, M.; Yang, D.; Wangari, V.W. Quantitative evaluation of attraction intensity of highway landscape visual elements based on dynamic perception. Environ. Impact Assess. Rev. 2023, 100, 107081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Chen, G.; Sun, X.; Yu, W.; Wang, H. Analysis Model of the Relationship between Public Spatial Forms in Traditional Villages and Scenic Beauty Preference Based on LiDAR Point Cloud Data. Land 2022, 11, 1133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Li, J.; Zhang, Z.; Jing, F.; Gao, J.; Ma, J.; Shao, G.; Noel, S. An evaluation of urban green space in Shanghai, China, using eye tracking. Urban For. Urban Green. 2020, 56, 126903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Wang, Y.; Yang, G.; Lu, Y. Evaluation of urban wetland landscapes based on a comprehensive model—A comparative study of three urban wetlands in Hangzhou, China. Environ. Res. Commun. 2023, 53, 035004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. López-Martínez, F. Visual landscape preferences in Mediterranean areas and their socio-demographic influences. Ecol. Eng. 2017, 104, 205–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Luckmann, K.; Lagemann, V.; Menzel, S. Landscape Assessment and Evaluation of Young People: Comparing Nature-Orientated Habitat and Engineered Habitat Preferences. Environ. Behav. 2013, 45, 86–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Sun, M.; Zhang, X.; Ryan, C. Perceiving tourist destination landscapes through Chinese eyes: The case of South Island, New Zealand. Tour. Manag. 2015, 46, 582–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Akten, M.; Celik, M. Evaluation of visual landscape perception for Incilipinar and Adalet Park cases. J. Food Agric. Environ. 2013, 11, 1532–1538. [Google Scholar]
  9. Yazici, K.; Asur, F. Assessment of landscape types and aesthetic qualities by visual preferences (Tokat, Turkey). J. Environ. Prot. Ecol. 2021, 22, 340–349. [Google Scholar]
  10. Jacques, D. Neuroaesthetics and landscape appreciation. Landsc. Res. 2021, 46, 116–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bao, Y.; Yang, T.; Lin, X.; Fang, Y.; Wang, Y.; Pöppel, E.; Lei, Q. Aesthetic Preferences for Eastern and Western Traditional Visual Art: Identity Matters. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Tang, I.C.; Sullivan, W.C.; Chang, C.Y. Perceptual Evaluation of Natural Landscapes: The Role of the Individual Connection to Nature. Environ. Behav. 2015, 47, 595–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Munoz-Pedreros, A. Landscape evaluation: An environmental management. Rev. Chil. Hist. Nat. 2004, 77, 139–156. [Google Scholar]
  14. Li, X.; Li, L.; Wang, X.; Lin, Q.; Wu, D.; Dong, Y.; Han, S. Visual quality evaluation model of an urban river landscape based on random forest. Ecol. Indic. 2021, 133, 108381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Yamashita, S. Perception and evaluation of water in landscape: Use of Photo-Projective Method to compare child and adult residents’ perceptions of a Japanese river environment. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2002, 62, 3–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Chen, X.; Wang, Y.; Huang, T.; Lin, Z. Research on Digital Experience and Satisfaction Preference of Plant Community Design in Urban Green Space. Land 2022, 11, 1411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Zhang, H.; Qiu, M.; Li, L.; Lu, Y.; Zhang, J. Exploring the dimensions of everyday soundscapes perception in spatiotemporal view: A qualitative approach. Appl. Acoust. 2021, 181, 108149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Petrova, E.G.; Mironov, Y.V.; Aoki, Y.; Matsushima, H.; Ebine, S.; Furuya, K.; Petrova, A.; Takayama, N.; Ueda, H. Comparing the visual perception and aesthetic evaluation of natural landscapes in Russia and Japan: Cultural and environmental factors. Prog. Earth Planet. Sci. 2015, 2, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Cottet, M.; Vaudor, L.; Tronchère, H.; Roux-Michollet, D.; Augendre, M.; Brault, V. Using gaze behavior to gain insights into the impacts of naturalness on city dwellers’ perceptions and valuation of a landscape. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 60, 9–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Svobodova, K.; Vojar, J.; Sklenicka, P.; Filova, L. Presentation Matters: Causes of Differences in Preferences for Agricultural Landscapes Displayed via Photographs and Videos. Space Cult. 2018, 21, 259–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Chen, B.; Adimo, O.A.; Bao, Z. Assessment of aesthetic quality and multiple functions of urban green space from the users’ perspective: The case of Hangzhou Flower Garden, China. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2009, 93, 76–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Tanaka, K.; Kato, Y.; Mizuno, A.; Mikawa, M.; Fujisawa, M. Dynamic grass color scale display technique based on grass length for green landscape-friendly animation display. Sci. Rep. 2023, 13, 260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Stamps, A.E. Use of Static and Dynamic Media to Simulate Environments: A Meta-Analysis. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2010, 111, 355–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Luo, J.; Zhao, T.; Cao, L.; Biljecki, F. Semantic Riverscapes: Perception and evaluation of linear landscapes from oblique imagery using computer vision. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2022, 228, 104569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Gyurkovich, M.; Pieczara, M. Using Composition to Assess and Enhance Visual Values in Landscapes. Sustainability 2021, 13, 4185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Smith, E.L.; Bishop, I.D.; Williams, K.J.; Ford, R.M. Scenario Chooser: An interactive approach to eliciting public landscape preferences. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2012, 106, 230–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Guan, F.; Fang, Z.; Wang, L.; Zhang, X.; Zhong, H.; Huang, H. Modelling people’s perceived scene complexity of real-world environments using street-view panoramas and open geodata. ISPRS J. Photogramm. Remote Sens. 2022, 186, 315–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Pardo-García, S.; Mérida-Rodríguez, M. Measurement of visual parameters of landscape using projections of photographs in GIS. Comput. Environ. Urban Syst. 2017, 61, 56–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Meitner, M.J. Scenic beauty of river views in the Grand Canyon: Relating perceptual judgments to locations. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2004, 68, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Brivio, E.; Serino, S.; Cousa, E.N.; Zini, A.; Riva, G.; De Leo, G. Virtual reality and 360 degrees panorama technology: A media comparison to study changes in sense of presence, anxiety, and positive emotions. Virtual Real. 2021, 25, 303–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Higuera-Trujillo, J.L.; López-Tarruella Maldonado, J.; Llinares-Millán, C. Psychological and physiological human responses to simulated and real environments: A comparison between Photographs, 360° Panoramas, and Virtual Reality. Appl. Ergon. 2017, 65, 398–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Chen, W. On boat: A magnificent panorama of River Basin in Tang Dynasty. Heliyon 2023, 9, e12771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ritter, K.A.; Chambers, T.L. Three-dimensional modeled environments versus 360 degree panoramas for mobile virtual reality training. Virtual Real. 2022, 26, 571–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kusahara, M. The panorama in Meiji Japan: Horizontal and vertical perspectives. Early Popul. Vis. Cult. 2020, 18, 400–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Pasewaldt, S.; Semmo, A.; Trapp, M.; Dollner, J. Multi-perspective 3D panoramas. Int. J. Geogr. Inf. Sci. 2014, 28, 2030–2051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Ventura, J.; Höllerer, T. Structure and motion in urban environments using upright panoramas. Virtual Real. 2013, 17, 147–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Sun, L.; Wang, J.; Xie, Z.; Li, R.; Wu, X.; Wu, Y. Evaluating the Street Greening with the Multiview Data Fusion. J. Sens. 2021, 2021, 2793474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Zhang, Y.; Li, S.; Fu, X.; Dong, R. Quantification of urban greenery using hemisphere-view panoramas with a green cover index. Ecosyst. Health Sustain. 2021, 7, 1929502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. De Carvalho, M.R.; Freire, R.C.; Nardi, A.E. Virtual reality as a mechanism for exposure therapy. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 112, 220–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Vavrouchová, H.; Fukalová, P.; Svobodová, H.; Oulehla, J.; Pokorná, P. Mapping Landscape Values and Conflicts through the Optics of Different User Groups. Land 2021, 10, 1306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Karmanov, D.; Hamel, R. Evaluations of Design Gardens by Students of Landscape Architecture and Non-design Students: A Comparative Study. Landsc. Res. 2009, 34, 457–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Cavagnaro, E.; Staffieri, S.; Carrieri, A.; Burns, K.; Chen, N.; Fermani, A. Profiling for sustainable tourism: Young travellers’ self-transcendence values and motivations. Eur. J. Tour. Res. 2021, 28, 2810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mohsin, A.; Lengler, J.; Chaiya, P. Does travel interest mediate between motives and intention to travel? A case of young Asian travellers. J. Hosp. Tour. Manag. 2017, 31, 36–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Buffa, F. Young Tourists and Sustainability. Profiles, Attitudes, and Implications for Destination Strategies. Sustainability 2015, 7, 14042–14062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Berto, R.; Massaccesi, S.; Pasini, M. Do eye movements measured across high and low fascination photographs differ? Addressing Kaplan’s fascination hypothesis. J. Environ. Psychol. 2008, 28, 185–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Yao, Y.; Zhu, X.; Xu, Y.; Yang, H.; Wu, X.; Li, Y.; Zhang, Y. Assessing the visual quality of green landscaping in rural residential areas: The case of Changzhou, China. Environ. Monit. Assess. 2012, 184, 951–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Swanwick, C. Society’s attitudes to and preferences for land and landscape. Land Use Policy 2009, 26, S62–S75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Kaltenborn, B.P.; Bjerke, T. Associations between environmental value orientations and landscape preferences. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2002, 59, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Gao, Y.; Zhang, T.; Zhang, W.; Meng, H.; Zhang, Z. Research on visual behavior characteristics and cognitive evaluation of different types of forest landscape spaces. Urban For. Urban Green. 2020, 54, 126788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Rogge, E.; Nevens, F.; Gulinck, H. Perception of rural landscapes in Flanders: Looking beyond aesthetics. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2007, 82, 159–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Bulut, Z.; Yilmaz, H. Determination of landscape beauties through visual quality assessment method: A case study for Kemaliye (Erzincan/Turkey). Environ. Monit. Assess. 2008, 141, 121–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Duzgunes, E.; Demirel, O. Evaluation of rural areas in terms of landscape quality: Salacik Village (Trabzon/Turkey) example. Environ. Monit. Assess. 2015, 187, 310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Li, Q.; Huang, J.; Wang, C.; Lin, H.; Zhang, J.; Jiang, J.; Wang, B. Land Development Suitability Evaluation of Pingtan Island Based on Scenario Analysis and Landscape Ecological Quality Evaluation. Sustainability 2017, 9, 1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Arriaza, M.; Cañas-Ortega, J.; Cañas-Madueño, J.; Ruiz-Aviles, P. Assessing the visual quality of rural landscapes. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2004, 69, 115–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Ozhanci, E.; Yilmaz, H. Visual assessment of rural landscape with different characters. Forestist 2019, 69, 44–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Akalin, A.; Yildirim, K.; Wilson, C.; Kilicoglu, O. Architecture and engineering students’ evaluations of house facades: Preference, complexity and impressiveness. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 124–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Aletta, F.; Kang, J.; Astolfi, A.; Fuda, S. Differences in soundscape appreciation of walking sounds from different footpath materials in urban parks. Sustain. Cities Soc. 2016, 27, 367–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Xu, W.; Zhao, J. Investigating Visual Aesthetic Fatigue in Urban Green Spaces. Int. J. Environ. Res. 2023, 17, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Kousar, S.; Shabbir, A. Analysis of environmental degradation mechanism in the nexus among energy consumption and poverty in Pakistan. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 2021, 28, 27528–27541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Briki, W. Passion, Trait Self-Control, and Wellbeing: Comparing Two Mediation Models Predicting Wellbeing. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Hu, S.; Tong, W.; Mao, K. Study on Highway Landscape Environment Assessment and Grading Method. Sustainability 2023, 15, 4904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Sowinska-Swierkosz, B. Index of Landscape Disharmony (ILDH) as a new tool combining the aesthetic and ecological approach to landscape assessment. Ecol. Indic. 2016, 70, 166–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Sezen, I.; Yilmaz, S. Visual assessment for the evaluation of Erzurum-Bayburt-Of highway as scenic road. Sci. Res. Essays 2010, 5, 366–377. [Google Scholar]
  64. Wang, Z.; Sun, H.; Li, J. Research on Architectural Color and Visual Comfort in Historic Landscape Areas. Buildings 2023, 13, 1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lin, W.; Mu, Y.; Zhang, Z.; Wang, J.; Diao, X.; Lu, Z.; Guo, W.; Wang, Y.; Xu, B. Research on cognitive evaluation of forest color based on visual behavior experiments and landscape preference. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0276677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Wang, Z.; Li, M.; Zhang, X.; Song, L. Modeling the scenic beauty of autumnal tree color at the landscape scale: A case study of Purple Mountain, Nanjing, China. Urban For. Urban Green. 2020, 47, 126526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Cheng, Y.; Tan, M. The quantitative research of landscape color: A study of Ming Dynasty City Wall in Nanjing. Color Res. Appl. 2018, 43, 436–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Yan, J.; Du, S.; Zhang, J.; Yu, W. Analyzing Transregional Vernacular Cultural Landscape Security Patterns with a Nature–Culture Lens: A Case Study of the Yangtze River Delta Demonstration Area, China. Land 2023, 12, 661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Sang, K.; Fontana, G.L.; Piovan, S.E. Assessing Railway Landscape by AHP Process with GIS: A Study of the Yunnan-Vietnam Railway. Remote Sens. 2022, 14, 603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Geng, M.; Hong, L.; Ma, K.; Wang, K. Evolution of Urban Public Space Landscape in Tianjin Port City. J. Coast. Res. 2020, 104, 142–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Jeon, J.Y.; Jo, H.I. Effects of audio-visual interactions on soundscape and landscape perception and their influence on satisfaction with the urban environment. Build. Environ. 2020, 169, 106544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Mo, L.; Chen, J.; Xie, Y. Assessment of landscape resource using the scenic beauty estimation method at compound ecological system. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 2021, 28, 5892–5899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Lin, L.; Homma, R.; Iki, K. Preferences for a lake landscape: Effects of building height and lake width. Environ. Impact Assess. Rev. 2018, 70, 22–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Scolozzi, R.; Schirpke, U.; Detassis, C.; Abdullah, S.; Gretter, A. Mapping Alpine Landscape Values and Related Threats as Perceived by Tourists. Landsc. Res. 2015, 40, 451–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Franco, D.; Franco, D.; Mannino, I.; Zanetto, G. The impact of agroforestry networks on scenic beauty estimation—The role of a landscape ecological network on a socio-cultural process. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2003, 62, 119–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Zhang, N.; Zheng, X.; Wang, X. Assessment of Aesthetic Quality of Urban Landscapes by Integrating Objective and Subjective Factors: A Case Study for Riparian Landscapes. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2022, 9, 735905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Peng, S.-H.; Han, K.-T. Assessment of Aesthetic Quality on Soil and Water Conservation Engineering Using the Scenic Beauty Estimation Method. Water 2018, 10, 407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Ren, X.; Wei, P.; Wang, Q.; Sun, W.; Yuan, M.; Shao, S.; Zhu, D.; Xue, Y. The effects of audio-visual perceptual characteristics on environmental health of pedestrian streets with traffic noise: A case study in Dalian, China. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1122639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Tan, X.H.; Li, X.H.; Peng, Y.L. Aesthetic Evaluation of Plant Landscape Based on Principal Factor Analysis and Sbe in Wetland Park—A Case Study of Jinlong Lake Wetland Park (China). J. Environ. Eng. Landsc. Manag. 2021, 29, 40–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Ng, C.F. Perception and Evaluation of Buildings: The Effects of Style and Frequency of Exposure. Collabra Psychol. 2020, 6, 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Han, J.; Lee, S. Verification of Immersive Virtual Reality as a Streetscape Evaluation Method in Urban Residential Areas. Land 2023, 12, 345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Zhang, Z.; Gao, Y.; Zhou, S.; Zhang, T.; Zhang, W.; Meng, H. Psychological Cognitive Factors Affecting Visual Behavior and Satisfaction Preference for Forest Recreation Space. Forests 2022, 13, 136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Krause, C.L. Our visual landscape: Managing the landscape under special consideration of visual aspects. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2001, 54, 239–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Fu, S.B.; Lv, F. Farmer’s Eco-culture for sustainable development: A qualitative approach of understanding the rural art design. Pak. J. Agric. Sci. 2023, 60, 235–244. [Google Scholar]
  85. Zeng, X.; Zhong, Y.; Yang, L.; Wei, J.; Tang, X. Analysis of Forest Landscape Preferences and Emotional Features of Chinese Forest Recreationists Based on Deep Learning of Geotagged Photos. Forests 2022, 13, 892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Zhao, J.; Luo, P.; Wang, R.; Cai, Y. Correlations between aesthetic preferences of river and landscape characters. J. Environ. Eng. Landsc. Manag. 2013, 21, 123–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Schirpke, U.; Tasser, E.; Lavdas, A.A. Potential of eye-tracking simulation software for analyzing landscape preferences. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0273519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Study area.
Figure 1. Study area.
Sustainability 15 11458 g001
Figure 2. Content of filtered panoramic photos. Note: 20 photographs represent 20 different urban and rural landscapes.
Figure 2. Content of filtered panoramic photos. Note: 20 photographs represent 20 different urban and rural landscapes.
Sustainability 15 11458 g002
Figure 3. Organizing students’ experimental evaluation.
Figure 3. Organizing students’ experimental evaluation.
Sustainability 15 11458 g003
Figure 4. Hypothetical model.
Figure 4. Hypothetical model.
Sustainability 15 11458 g004
Figure 5. Positive view screenshots and negative view screenshots of 20 scenes. Note: Serial numbers 1–20 represent photographs taken from positive viewpoints and 21–40 represent photographs taken from negative viewpoints.
Figure 5. Positive view screenshots and negative view screenshots of 20 scenes. Note: Serial numbers 1–20 represent photographs taken from positive viewpoints and 21–40 represent photographs taken from negative viewpoints.
Sustainability 15 11458 g005aSustainability 15 11458 g005b
Figure 6. Correlation heat map. Note: Significance levels: “**” ≤ 0.001,“*” ≤ 0.05.
Figure 6. Correlation heat map. Note: Significance levels: “**” ≤ 0.001,“*” ≤ 0.05.
Sustainability 15 11458 g006
Figure 7. Direct effect results affecting the OSBS.
Figure 7. Direct effect results affecting the OSBS.
Sustainability 15 11458 g007
Figure 8. Mediating effect results affecting the OSBS. Note: Significance levels: *** ρ ≤ 0.001, ** ρ ≤ 0.05. m means indirect effect coefficient. OSBD means SBE value of the scene; PVSBD means SBE value of the positive viewpoint; NVSBD means SBE value of the negative viewpoint.
Figure 8. Mediating effect results affecting the OSBS. Note: Significance levels: *** ρ ≤ 0.001, ** ρ ≤ 0.05. m means indirect effect coefficient. OSBD means SBE value of the scene; PVSBD means SBE value of the positive viewpoint; NVSBD means SBE value of the negative viewpoint.
Sustainability 15 11458 g008
Table 1. Experimental organization scheme of landscape perception evaluation cited.
Table 1. Experimental organization scheme of landscape perception evaluation cited.
CategoryNumber of PersonsGroup CompositionEvaluation Type
Expert Group9University professors and experts in environmental design and visual designObjective evaluation
Student Group152Undergraduate students of environmental design and other art and design majors in the School of Architecture and Art of Central South UniversitySubjective evaluation
Table 2. Experimental organization scheme of landscape perception evaluation cited.
Table 2. Experimental organization scheme of landscape perception evaluation cited.
VariableAbbreviationScoring
12345
Overall environmentB1
Visual harmony of the scenery [38]C1extremely conflictedmore conflictedmoderatemore harmoniousExtremely harmonious
Color Beauty [39]C2extremely gray and monotonemore gray and monotonemoderatemore vivid and richextremely vivid and rich
Historic ambience [40,41]C3extremely thinrelatively thinmoderaterelatively thickextremely thick
Sense of order and regularityC4extremely disorganizedmore disorganizedmoderatemore regularextremely regular
Visual hierarchy [42]C5extremely thinrelatively thinmoderaterelatively richextremely rich
Visual area depthC6very short distanceshort distancemoderatelong distancevery long distance
Dirt and pollution level [40,42,43]C7extremely clean relatively cleanmoderaterelatively dirtyextremely dirty
SkyB2
Skyline beautyC8very weak beautyweak beautymoderatestrong beautyvery strong beauty
Sky ratio [44]C90–10%10–20%20–33%33–45%45–100%
Cloud ratioC10all cloudyovercast, cloudysunny, cloudysunny, lightly cloudysunny, no clouds
Native elementsB3
Vegetation ratio [45]C110–5%5–25%25–50%50–75%75–100%
Percentage of exposed soil [46]C120–5%5–25%25–50%50–75%75–100%
Type of vegetation [42,45,47]C13Very few species relatively few speciesmoderate relatively rich in speciesextremely rich in species
Water ratio [48]C140–5%5–25%25–50%50–75%75–100%
Man-made elementsB4
Overall beauty of the man-made object [49]C15extremely uglyugliermoderatemore beautifulextremely beautiful
Proportion of man-made objects [39,44,47]C160–5%5–25%25–50%50–75%75–100%
Near-natural degree of flooring material [50]C170–5%5–25%25–50%50–75%75–100%
Table 3. Test results of the passed multicollinearity problem.
Table 3. Test results of the passed multicollinearity problem.
Indicator CodeBetatTolerancesVIF
C10.1747.5120.3932.546
C20.1396.0210.3982.516
C30.1165.6490.5021.992
C40.0492.3210.4772.096
C80.0613.070.5441.838
C150.1085.1130.4722.117
C50.0090.3520.2983.359
C7−0.104−4.4020.3782.648
C9−0.144−5.8160.3462.887
C100.0320.9880.2044.899
C130.0080.3330.3622.76
C140.093.0360.2394.19
C16−0.004−0.1670.4562.191
C17−0.027−1.3480.5151.944
Table 4. Direct effect results affecting OSBS.
Table 4. Direct effect results affecting OSBS.
Indicator CodePVSBDNVSBDOSBD (without
Mediation)
C10.144 ***0.054 **0.174 ***
C20.17 ***0.061 **0.139 ***
C30.020.123 ***0.116 ***
C40.1 ***0.052 **0.049 **
C50.115 ***−0.113 ***0.009
C7−0.003−0.025−0.104 ***
C80.133 ***0.103 ***0.061 **
C9−0.157 ***0.077 **−0.144 ***
C100.12 ***−0.0360.032
C13−0.109 ***0.072 **0.008
C140.112 ***0.076 **0.09 **
C150.087 ***0.162 ***0.108 ***
C160.128 ***0.015−0.004
C170.0120.051 **−0.027
PVSBD 0.122 ***
NVSBD 0.062 ***
Note: Significance levels: *** ρ ≤ 0.001, ** ρ ≤ 0.05. OSBD means SBE value of the scene; PVSBD means SBE value of the positive viewpoint; NVSBD means SBE value of the negative viewpoint.
Table 5. Mediating effect results affecting OSBS.
Table 5. Mediating effect results affecting OSBS.
IS to OSBD (with
Mediation)
Indirect EffectTotal
Effect
VAF
Mediator VariableMediator Variable
PVSBDNVSBDPVSBDNVSBD
C10.018 ***0.003 *0.195 ***9.2%1.5%
C20.021 ***0.004 *0.164 ***12.8%2.4%
C30.0020.008 **0.126 **1.6%6.3%
C40.012 ***0.003 *0.064 ***18.8%4.7%
C50.014 ***−0.007 **0.0346.7%23.3%
C70.001−0.0020.1060.0%1.9%
C80.016 ***0.006 **0.083 ***19.3%7.2%
C9−0.019 ***0.005 **0.168 **11.3%3.0%
C100.015 **−0.0020.049 **30.6%4.1%
C13−0.013 ***0.004 **0.025 **52.0%16.0%
C140.014 **0.005 *0.109 ***12.8%4.6%
C150.011 **0.01 ***0.129 ***8.5%7.8%
C16−0.016 ***0.0010.021 ***76.2%4.8%
C170.0010.003 *0.0313.2%9.7%
Note: Significance levels: *** ρ ≤ 0.001, ** ρ ≤ 0.05, * ρ ≤ 0.1. OSBD means SBE value of the scene; PVSBD means SBE value of the positive viewpoint; NVSBD means SBE value of the negative viewpoint. When VAF < 20%, there is no mediation effect; when 20% < VAF < 80%, it is judged as a “partial mediation” effect, and when VAF > 80%, it is judged as a “full mediation” effect.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chen, Y.; Zhong, Q.; Li, B. Positive or Negative Viewpoint Determines the Overall Scenic Beauty of a Scene: A Landscape Perception Evaluation Based on a Panoramic View. Sustainability 2023, 15, 11458. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151411458

AMA Style

Chen Y, Zhong Q, Li B. Positive or Negative Viewpoint Determines the Overall Scenic Beauty of a Scene: A Landscape Perception Evaluation Based on a Panoramic View. Sustainability. 2023; 15(14):11458. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151411458

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chen, Yue, Qikang Zhong, and Bo Li. 2023. "Positive or Negative Viewpoint Determines the Overall Scenic Beauty of a Scene: A Landscape Perception Evaluation Based on a Panoramic View" Sustainability 15, no. 14: 11458. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151411458

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop