Next Article in Journal
An Empirical Framework to Sustain Value Generation with Project Risk Management: A Case Study in the IT Consulting Sector
Next Article in Special Issue
Modeling the Influence of Paternalistic Leadership and Personality Characteristics on Alienation and Organizational Culture in the Aviation Industry of Pakistan: The Mediating Role of Cohesiveness
Previous Article in Journal
Examining Social Sustainability in Organizations
Previous Article in Special Issue
Temperament, Character and Cognitive Emotional Regulation in the Latent Profile Classification of Smartphone Addiction in University Students
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Influence of Psychological Distance on the Challenging Moral Decision Support of Sports Majors in Internet of Things and Machine Learning

1
Gdansk University of Physical Education and Sport, 999038 Gdansk, Poland
2
Chengdu Sport University, Chengdu 610041, China
3
Jinan University, Guangzhou 510632, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2022, 14(19), 12115; https://doi.org/10.3390/su141912115
Submission received: 12 August 2022 / Revised: 8 September 2022 / Accepted: 13 September 2022 / Published: 25 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Organizational Behavior and Psychological Research for Sustainability)

Abstract

:
This work intends to examine the influence of different dimensions of psychological distance on the moral decision-making of sports college students in sports dilemmas under different learning pressure conditions, and to further investigate the relationship between psychological distance and moral decision-making. The research on the influencing factors of moral decision-making of sports majors can effectively help to understand the moral cognition level of the group, and provide a reference for the interpretation of athletes’ moral anomie behavior, thereby enriching the content of the moral quality education of athletes. This work intends to study the impact of psychological distance on the moral decision support of sports college students in the context of the Internet of Things (IoT) and machine learning. Psychological distance in the machine learning environment may affect individuals’ understanding and cognition of events and, to a certain extent, can change students’ cognition and judgment of events. IoT and machine learning environments are chosen as the foundation. The learning pressure of college students majoring in physical education is a variable. A questionnaire survey and experimental design are used to test the influence of different degrees of learning pressure, social distance, and spatial distance on the moral decision-making of physical education college students in the sports dilemma. The dimensions of the psychological distance of physical education (PE) students are analyzed under different stress conditions and their impact on the moral decision-making of PE students. This experiment adopts a mixed experimental design of 3 learning stresses (no stress vs. moderate stress vs. high stress) × 2 social distances (self vs. others) × 2 spatial distances (Beijing vs. France). The results show that the main effect of social distance is significant. When the self is the decision-making subject, individuals tend to make more moral decisions. There is a significant interaction between social distance and learning pressure. In a stress-free and high-stress environment, individuals make a significant increase in the number of moral decisions when faced with self-centered decision-making. Now, moral decision-making and its consequences are important for college students majoring in sports. The results of their moral decision-making in the field of education directly reflect the image of the individual and even the institution.

1. Introduction

Research Background and Motivations

With the rapid development of computer and network technology, the Internet of Things (IoT) is constantly changing human beings’ way of life, thinking, values, and even personal interests, hobbies, and entertainment. This technology creates a new living environment, namely the network, in the virtual society. In the virtual world, changes in people’s real and emotional experiences, feelings, and ideological and moral concepts are a problem that cannot be ignored [1]. The behavioral choices and corresponding consequences of sports students are particularly serious. Their behavior affects the school’s reputation [2]. In such situations, the tension of learning is indispensable. Psychological distance also affects the cognitive process of individuals. Whether a person is affected by different psychological distances in a learning environment is also the focus of scholars’ research [3].
At present, in the study of ethical psychology, people gradually tend to discuss the “nature of morality” [4]. In recent years, there has been an increasing number of related studies on the nature of morality. Among them, the research on the nature of students’ morality is the most common. College students are the young talents of China ‘s future construction and the backbone of the future construction of the motherland. The moral quality and moral development of college students will have a great impact on the moral level of Chinese society in the future. Therefore, it is of great practical significance to study the moral decision-making of college students. In the study of ethical psychology, people tend to discuss “the essence of morality”. However, the nature of morality has always been an important proposition in the field of morality. College students are the backbone of China’s socialist construction. The level of their moral development reflects the highest moral level of a society in a certain sense and is the “wind vane” of a whole society. Therefore, discussing the essence and law of moral education for college students will directly affect the basic task of “cultivating morality and cultivating people” [5]. College students in the new century should follow the internal ethical foundation when facing ethical issues, or they should reflect on the changing ethical environment in combination with the actual situation. Under the premise of following ethical principles, students should make appropriate adjustments and take other actions. The discussion of such issues can not only provide a new reference for the psychology of moral policy formulation, but also help to understand the mechanism of its generation and open new working ideas and methods for solving the dilemma of moral education [6].
This work studies the impact of psychological distance on the moral decision support of sports college students in the context of IoT and machine learning. The effects of different learning stress levels on students’ psychological distance and moral decision-making are explored. Additionally, the influence of social and spatial distance on students’ practical decision-making is explored. This work has positive significance for promoting the psychological and moral healthy development of physical education college students.
Research question: This work mainly focuses on college students majoring in sports, to explore the moral decision-making results of individuals in dilemma situations under different dimensions of time pressure and psychological distance. In addition, this experiment also compares whether there will be significant differences in the moral performance of college students majoring in physical education in the context of life dilemmas and sports dilemmas to analyze the overall moral quality of the group more completely, rather than just making a moral example in the field of their own concern, while ignoring the performance in life.
Question 1: Two dimensions (time pressure and no time pressure) are set to analyze the impact of time pressure on the moral decision-making of sports majors in different situations.
Question 2: When the variable of psychological distance (spatial distance) is added, the influence of time pressure on moral decision-making is discussed.
Question 3: When the variable of psychological distance (social distance) is added, the impact of time pressure on moral decision-making is discussed.
Research hypothesis:
Hypothesis 1.
Time pressure affects moral decision-making in dilemma situations. Under the condition of no time pressure, there are more deontological choices of nonsports majors and sports majors. Under high time pressure, nonsports majors and sports majors make more utilitarian choices.
Hypothesis 2.
Dilemma type affects moral decision-making in dilemma situations. In life dilemmas, nonsports majors and sports majors choose more deontological outcomes. In the sports dilemma, nonsports majors and sports majors have more utilitarian choices.
Hypothesis 3.
Time pressure affects decision time. The decision-making time of nonsports majors and sports-major college students will be accelerated under high-time-pressure conditions. Nonsports majors and sports majors tend to have a longer decision-making speed without time pressure.

2. Literature Review

In the application research of machine learning in mental health, Senders et al. believed that machine learning is the process of automatically analyzing or predicting new and unknown data by discovering the laws of many existing data points and information [7]. The father of machine learning, TOM M. Mitchell, gave a formal definition of machine learning. Suppose that Performance (P) is used to evaluate the performance of a computer program on a certain task (T). If the program uses Experience (E) to improve the performance of T, that is, the improvement of P, the program mainly learns T. Many scholars have tried to use machine learning to predict complex psychological problems of individuals, such as predicting stress disorders and anxiety disorders [8]. Chu and Yang found that thousands of effective features can only predict the possibility of suicide. They built a suicide recognizer for Weibo social media using a multi-layer perceptron algorithm, which can evaluate the possibility of the suicide of Weibo users in real-time with a prediction accuracy of about 94% [9]. The survey of Aldahiri et al. used the data of 40,736 older people from different regions and used the existing diagnostic codes through machine learning. The future of Alzheimer’s disease in the elderly was predicted. In the following year, the predicted performance could reach 75% or higher [10]. Kassab et al. used four different machine learning algorithms and multiple logistic regression models to predict the treatment effect of cognitive-behavioral therapy on children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. The results showed that the machine learning algorithm performed well in children’s psychological prediction, with an average accuracy of about 80%. The best predictors of treatment effectiveness are the onset and duration of mental illness [11].
The sports field has begun to try to use internet technology to monitor various physiological parameters of the human body in real-time. Ferrag et al. found that the instrumentation required in this type of field usually requires high experimental conditions and is expensive. In sports and health, the application of internet technology to health development has initially formed a certain influence. In the current information and communication technology, intelligent perception technology is an emerging technology. Fitness products such as smartphones and various wearable smart bracelets, such as sensors and GPS on smartphones, can monitor and guide ordinary people’s exercise [12]. Jim et al. built a health counseling system based on wearable sensors. Portable and lightweight sensors allow users to obtain more precise motion monitoring [13]. The results of Lee et al. on the effect of internet technology on mental health can provide a useful reference for college physical education teachers to guide extracurricular sports activities. Wearable smartphones can be used to obtain information conveniently and quickly, such as the user’s geographic location, exercise status, time, and physiological indicators [14]. Artificial intelligence, machine learning, big data, and other related technologies can be used to analyze and process the user’s basic data and the data collected by the sensor, which can evaluate the user’s action status and guide the action correctly [15].
Beverly pointed out that moral decision-making can be a responsive decision in the context of either a real or hypothetical moral dilemma (when a responsive choice is required, accompanied by certain moral rules or principles). It can be a morally acceptable decision about behavior judgments of sexuality or the moral character of others (including judgments about individuals, groups, or institutions) [16]. Desioli et al. believed that the current research on moral science tends to understand the intrinsic nature of human morality from a broader perspective, and carries out more process research from the aspects of moral evolutionary bases, how to make moral decisions, and the construction and maintenance of norms [17]. Hadit believed that intuition directly leads individuals to make corresponding moral judgments. This is a cognition, not a reasoning process. Thus, the main idea of the theory is that moral judgments arise from quick moral intuitions, often accompanied by a slow process of reasoning, as shown in Figure 1. Individual intuition leads to judgment. Discussing the reasons for judgment is to find reasons from the results to justify one’s decision. However, the ethical decision-making and personal reflection aspects of this process are almost nonexistent [18].
Moral decision is the last link of moral choice, that is, the final decision made by the subject in the process of moral choice. That is to say, when the actor is faced with a variety of possible ways to choose, the final decision on the degree of good and evil and the morality of these possible behavioral ways is the final decision [19], that is, the result of moral decision-making under the balance of pros and cons. The study of moral decision-making has always been accompanied by disputes of schools, mainly because the measurement of moral decision-making is usually associated with moral dilemma situations, and individuals are often in the contradiction between rational analysis and perceptual intuition. These two ways of thinking also correspond to different decision-making principles, namely, the principle of utilitarianism and the principle of altruism. Beverly, on the other hand, argued that moral decision-making can be either a responsive decision about how to behave in a real or hypothetical moral dilemma (when a choice of response is needed, accompanied by certain moral rules or principles), or a judgment about the moral acceptability of behavior or the moral qualities of others (including judgments about individuals, groups, or institutions) [20]. Hadit believed that it is moral intuition that directly leads individuals to make corresponding moral judgments, which is a kind of cognition, not a reasoning process. Thus, the main point of the theory is that moral judgments are caused by quick moral intuitions, followed by a slow process of moral reasoning when necessary [21]. After analyzing and integrating the research results of Hadt et al., Greene proposed a dual-processing theoretical model of moral decision-making from the perspective of cognitive neuroscience. Its theory discusses the decision-making tendency of consequentialism and deontology; the former is driven by cognitive process, while the latter is driven by emotion. In addition, he divided moral judgment into two categories, personal and nonpersonal, i.e., personal is driven by social emotional responses (e.g., in a footbridge dilemma, an individual is required to push another person through physical touch to save another person, and the individual is more directly affected by his or her own emotions), while nonpersonal is less driven by emotional responses and more determined by cognitive processes (e.g., in a trolley dilemma, an individual is required to pull the lever). In his research, “cognition” is not an information processing process, but is defined as a process that contrasts with emotion [22]. In specific studies, Greene also used the dilemma to test the dual-processing theory, and came to the following conclusion that in the trolley dilemma, the cognitive process can compete with the emotional process, prompting people to recognize individual harmful moral violations; that is, in the case of low emotional response, people tend to be consequential. In a situation like the footbridge dilemma, individuals experience a large number of negative emotional reactions, causing individuals to disapprove of harmful behaviors; that is, in the case of high emotional reactions, people tend to make deontological decisions [23]. Regarding the study of psychological distance, Trope and Liberman, through experimental support, introduced the concept of “psychological distance” in the field of psychology for the first time, and defined psychological distance as the range formed by individuals extending outward along four different dimensions: time, space, social relations, and hypothetical with their own time and position as reference points [24]. Another widely used definition is from a social psychology perspective. Agnew focused on explaining the interpersonal distance dimension of psychological distance, that is, social distance. He believed that individuals are based on the collected social information and evaluate and judge the distance between themselves and external individuals after comprehensive analysis. However, the evaluation results are not always perceived by individuals, and may only occur subconsciously [25]. Gong’s research confirms that when the set dilemma occurs when both spatial and temporal distances are far away, the individual ‘s decision-making results tend to be more utilitarian than when the psychological distance is close, and thus more choices are made to maximize benefits, even if the corresponding costs are greater [26].
As for the study of moral decision-making of college students majoring in physical education, students in physical education colleges and universities, and college students majoring in physical education in other colleges and universities, because many of them have received physical training since childhood, the proportion of cultural knowledge and ideological education is much less than that of students in ordinary colleges and universities, and their personal values are mostly judged by the results obtained in the competition or by winning or losing. In the long run, this has led to the emphasis on utilitarianism by this group. In life, it is mostly self-centered and interests first, with too much emphasis on results in competition regardless of moral norms or other people’s feelings. Then, with such obvious differences in the values of the two groups, the researchers began to compare and analyze their performance in the moral dilemma. Escolar-Chua first measured the moral judgment performance of two groups of college students and found that basketball players had lower moral judgment scores than nonsports individuals in four life and sports dilemmas [27]. For the higher utilitarian decision-making tendency of basketball players, Stanger explained it as a collective project where basketball players belong. Even if they violate the rules, the punishment they receive is not always personal, so the price paid is within an acceptable range, and they dare to choose the choice of low risk and high return [28]. There are many studies on the exploration of time pressure, among which some scholars pay attention to the decision-makers’ decision-making bias under time pressure and distraction in the hypothetical car purchase problem. It is proved that if individuals want to make some judgments involving personal investment under time pressure or distraction, they will rely heavily on negative evidence in the event [29]. Zakay’s understanding of time pressure is that when the time allocated is less than the time required for decision-making or is considered to be required for decision-making, it may cause time tension and may damage the best performance of the decision-making process. As a background variable, time pressure affects the nature of the decision-making process, especially the strategy of decision-makers [30].

3. Research Methodology

3.1. Research Objectives

The purpose is to examine the influence of different dimensions of psychological distance on the moral decision-making of sports college students in sports dilemmas under different learning pressure conditions and to investigate the relationship between psychological distance and moral decision-making. Combined with previous experience, learning pressure levels of different dimensions are added in this experiment, that is, moderate learning pressure. Stress is divided into three dimensions to explore in more detail the impact of different levels of learning stress on moral decision-making [31].

3.2. Research Methods and Subjects

(1) Investigation method
Questionnaire method. College students who majored in sports in different countries are selected, and the importance of each factor and the reference order for career decision-making are measured to provide data for follow-up research.
Experimental Method. The information display board technology and Mouselab Web tool are used, and a career decision information board is compiled. A combination of computer programs and web technology is used to verify the differences in the decision-making process of college students under different psychological distances. The research object of this experiment is obtained through sampling and is randomly selected from the physical education majors of 10 universities. Each university selects 30 students of physical education majors as the research object, and 30 students of each school include 15 students in school and 15 students who have graduated within 3 years. From these 300 objects, 103 college students in school and 97 college students who have graduated within 3 years are selected, including 100 boys and 100 girls, and the ratio of boys to girls is 1:1. Finally, 191 pieces of data are collected, including 180 pieces of valid data. As the survey object is obtained by sampling, there is a certain error. The confidence level of the experiment is 95%. According to the sampling error equation, the experimental error is calculated to be ±11.71324%.
(2) Subject of investigation
The specific distribution of the subjects is shown in Table 1.
Design of the experiment. The experiment adopts the 2 (time distance: July 1, 2022, one year later) × 2 (social distance: good friends, strangers) × 2 (student type: college students, junior high school students) × 6 (situational stories: 1~6) hybrid design. Among them, temporal distance, social distance, and student type are between-subject variables. Situational stories are within-subject variables. The dependent variable is the score of moral assessment of moral dilemma stories, that is, the score of moral judgment.

4. Experimental Design and Performance Evaluation

4.1. Experimental Materials (or Datasets Collection)

This experiment adopts a mixed design of 3 (study learning pressure: high, medium, no) × 2 (spatial distance: Beijing, France) × 2 (social distance: self, others). Learning pressure is a between-subject variable. Spatial and social distance are both within-subject variables. The dependent variable is the number of times subjects made moral decisions. In the early stage of the experiment, it is judged and verified whether the selected dimension is effective for the experiment.

4.2. Experimental Environment

Schedule an experiment. In the sports scenario, 12 moral materials are selected. The situational content and decision-making remain unchanged, and only social and spatial distances are added to the original title.
Original title: In an important football game, you are fighting for possession of the ball with a good opponent who has suffered a leg injury. You know you can get the ball by kicking the player’s injured leg without being punished by the referee.
Will you please do this?
A. Yes B. No
The title after the adaptation is: (please imagine the following events in Beijing or France) In an important football match, you (others) are fighting for possession of the ball with a good player who has a leg injury. You (the other person) know you can kick the player’s injured leg without being punished by the referee to gain possession.
Will you (or anyone else) do this?
A. Yes B. No
Manipulation of psychological distance mainly focuses on two dimensions, social and spatial distance. The initiation of psychological distance is generally reflected in the instruction language. Therefore, a preliminary test is conducted before the formal experiment to confirm that the distance of the psychological distance selected in the formal experiment can be effectively activated.
The questionnaire is conducted in the form of online distribution, and a total of 50 results are collected.
Psychological distance distinguishes between near and far. There are four questions below and no correct answer. Please start from your own actual situation, imagine, and perceive how far it is from you, and make the one that you think is closest to you. A seven-point scale is used to select a psychological distance.
Social distancing: 1. I am participating in competition; 2. He (she) is participating in a competition.
Spatial distance: 1. I participate in a game in Beijing; 2. I participate in a game in France.
Statistical results show that at least 90% of individuals perceive psychological distance as expected. Thus, the pre-test results proved that manipulating social and spatial distance is effective.

4.3. Parameters Setting

Before the start of the experiment, the subjects should be informed in advance that they will participate in a decision-making experiment in a movement dilemma situation. The steps are as follows:
(1)
The stress group without learning
After the subjects are ready, the instruction language presented is as follows: welcome to participate in this experiment! In order to familiarize yourself with the experimental process, please understand the instructions before entering this experiment. After the “+” gaze point appears on the computer screen, please focus on the center of the computer screen. Next, the segment context description will appear. Immediately after each situation has been read, press the space bar to enter the question page and make a “yes” or “no” choice. If you think the answer is “yes,” press the “Y” key on your keyboard. If you think the answer is “no”, press the “N” key. The descriptions are hypothetical situations, and please imagine as much as possible based on the information in the question statement. There is no absolute right or wrong answer to the question. I hope you can choose according to your own ideas! After understanding the above instructions, please press the “Q” key to enter the formal experiment [32].
(2)
(Medium/High) Study Pressure Group
After the subjects are ready, the instruction language presented is as follows: welcome to participate in this experiment! In order to familiarize you with the experimental process, please understand the instructions before entering this experiment. After the “+” gaze point appears on the computer screen, please focus on the center of the computer screen. Then, a situation description will appear on the screen (normal college students need 8 s to read it completely, but in this experiment, the presentation time is only 4 s (or 6 s), so please pay attention. After that, the page will automatically jump to the answer page and ask you to make a “yes” or “no” choice. If you think the answer is “yes,” please press the “Y” key on your keyboard. If you think the answer is “no,” press the “N” key. The descriptions are all hypothetical situations. Please imagine as much as possible based on the information in the question statement. There is no absolute right or wrong answer to the question, and I hope you can choose according to your own ideas! After understanding the above instruction, now, please press the “Q” key to enter the formal experiment [33].

4.4. Performance Evaluation

Learning pressure, social distance, and spatial distance are independent variables. The number of moral decisions made by the subjects is used as the dependent variable, and a three-way repeated-measures ANOVA is performed. Under each experimental condition, the results of the subjects making moral decisions are descriptive statistics, as shown in Table 2. Table 3 presents the variance analysis of learning pressure × social distance × spatial distance on utilitarian decision-making.
The main effect of social distance is very significant (F = 12.017, p < 0.01), indicating a significant difference in the decision-making choices of the subjects when the decision-making body is themselves, and the decision-making body is others. The subjects made moral decisions with the self as the main body significantly higher than others. The main effect of learning pressure is not significant (F = 0.820, p = 0.206), and the main effect of spatial distance is not significant (F = 0.884, p = 0.355). The interaction between learning pressure and social distance is significantly different (F = 4.703, p < 0.05). There is a significant difference in the interaction of learning pressure and spatial distance (F = 8.776, p < 0.06). The interaction between social and spatial distance is not significant (F = 14.52, p = 0.236). The interactions of learning pressure, social distance, and spatial distance are also not significant (F = 0.830, p = 0.540). Study pressure and social distance are further analyzed by simple effects. The results show that there are significant differences in moral decision-making between the far and near dimensions of social distance, regardless of the condition of no learning (F = 11.128, p < 0.05) or high learning pressure (F = 10.043, p < 0.05). The utilitarian decisions made by individuals with the self as the decision-making subject are significantly higher than those made by others. Under moderate learning pressure, there is no significant difference in decision-making results between self and others (F = 0.850, p = 0.412); that is, under moderate learning pressure, social distance does not affect individuals’ moral decision-making. The pressure value is 1–3 points, indicating that the pressure value is increasing. The pressure values under different conditions are shown in Figure 2.
In Figure 2, the further simple-effects analysis of learning pressure and spatial distance found that in the condition of moderate learning pressure (F = 4.475, p < 0.05), there are significant differences in the moral decision-making of subjects in the far and near dimensions of spatial distance. The moral decisions made by individuals in Beijing as the background of the event are significantly higher than those in France. In the absence of learning pressure, there is no significant difference in decision-making results between Beijing and France (F = 3.813, p = 0.058). Similarly, under high learning pressure, there is no significant difference in the decision-making results of the subjects in the two dimensions of spatial distance (F = 3.813, p = 0.150). In the absence of learning pressure and high learning pressure, spatial distance does not affect individuals’ moral decision-making. Figure 3 shows the interaction of learning pressure and spatial distance.

4.5. Discussion

This work explores the influence of two dimensions of psychological distance based on learning pressure: spatial distance and social distance, on moral decision-making. After the four levels of psychological distance and different levels of learning and pressure are combined, the change in an individual’s decision-making behavior in the motor dilemmas is analyzed [34].
Experimental result 1 is that the main effect of social distance is significant. Individuals with closer social distance tend to make utilitarian decisions when making decisions. When social distancing is greater, individuals are more inclined to make deontological decisions.
Experimental result 2 is that spatial distance affects moral decision-making. When the background of the dilemma decision-making situation is Beijing, individuals tend to make utilitarian decisions. When the background of the dilemma decision-making situation is France, which is far away in space, individuals tend to make deontological decisions. Individuals tend to be harsher in their judgments about events at greater spatial distances, so it is easier to judge them as immoral. However, in the research on spatial distance and moral judgment and decision-making, individuals tend to adopt stricter standards for evaluating events or behaviors with close spatial distance, and it is easier to make deontological decisions.
Experimental result 3 is that the interaction between learning pressure and social distance is significant, and the simple effect of social distance is significant at a low level of learning pressure (i.e., when there is no pressure). The simple effect is significant at high levels of learning stress. However, the simple effect is not significant at the medium level of learning stress. When the subject of the event is the individual self, individuals tend to make moral decisions under both stress-free and high-stress conditions. When the decision subject is changed to someone the individual is unfamiliar with, the number of moral decisions is significantly reduced [35].
Changes in social and spatial distance dimensions can alter individuals’ tendency to make moral decisions under different learning stress conditions. In the socially close dilemma, individuals with no and high learning pressure increase the number of moral decisions. In the spatially distant dilemma, individuals under moderate learning pressure increase the number of moral decisions. The data suggest that spatial and social distancing influences the propensity to make moral decisions. Previous studies have also confirmed that time pressure will speed up individual decision-making time. The experiment shows that the decision-making time of the fresh PE student group under high time pressure (1077.93/2302.98 ms) is faster than that under no time pressure (1994.83/2363.63 ms). The nonsports group also had less decision-making time in the sports dilemma than without time pressure. However, the decision-making time of the nongraduating sports student group increased slightly in the life dilemma under the condition of high time pressure. The reason may be that the scenes described in the life dilemma are closely related to personal job search and other people’s lives [36]. Due to the existence of time pressure, individuals may not have enough information about the description of the predicament, but they cannot easily make the choice to decide their fate, so the thinking time may be a little longer. In addition, in the plight of life, sports college students have the least reaction time under high time pressure, indicating that when individuals face life difficulties, they need to consider the moral rules and need to weigh more information than in the sports situation, so the information processing process is shorter to make decisions faster. It takes the longest time for ordinary professional college students to make decisions without pressure, indicating that due to the lack of background knowledge of sports competitions, individuals are not very clear about the severity of each moral anomie behavior [37]. They have to grasp all the information mentioned in the dilemma, and then weigh which choice has the highest return ratio between winning the game with fouls and obeying the rules but losing the game, so their thinking process is more complicated and takes longer decision-making time.

5. Conclusions

The results show that in the context of IoT and machine learning, there is no significant difference in the moral decision-making of college students in general majors and sports majors. The education received by the college students majoring in sports is more about the spirit of sports competition. Their achievement motivation is generally higher, and they are more familiar with sports rules. Based on this cognitive background, ordinary professional college students are more likely to make more harmful behaviors in the sports dilemma, that is, to choose result-oriented decisions. College students majoring in sports are less likely to exhibit some behaviors that violate the spirit of competition, such as passive competition and doping. Their decision-making is not just about the outcome but more about the ethical trade-offs involved in the event. Under high study pressure, both general and sports college students choose to make more utilitarian decisions, and the decision-making tendency in sports dilemmas is higher than that in life dilemmas. Under no learning pressure, both general and sports college students choose to make more deontological decisions. The decision-making tendency in sports dilemmas is higher than that in life dilemmas. This is consistent with the conclusions of previous studies. Without considering other factors, individuals are under high learning pressure and are more inclined to result-oriented utilitarian decision-making. The factor of learning pressure is added to produce a certain sense of urgency in the individual decision-making process. Whether it is a sports major or an ordinary professional college student, the cognitive processing process cannot be carried out completely, thus ignoring the disadvantages brought by breaking the rules. They only notice the egoistic nature of the results. Therefore, individuals under high learning stress are more egoistic in their choices than students under no stress.

5.1. Research Contribution

From a realistic point of view, the thinking and analysis of the current situation of moral education in contemporary colleges and universities systematically analyze the guiding ability of moral education in colleges and universities under the new media environment. The effectiveness and pertinence of moral education are improved. Thus, this work is also very meaningful in practical work. The new media has a wide range of readers in colleges and universities, and it profoundly impacts the moral education of colleges and universities. Therefore, this work analyzes and excavates the value of theory and practice and moral education in colleges and universities.
The experimental results support hypothesis one and hypothesis two, and partially support hypothesis three. Graduated sports college students have a stronger tendency. Graduated PE students and fresh PE students have similar ways of thinking about moral cognition, so when facing the dilemma of life, everyone will choose the result of altruism. However, due to the sports dilemma, the education received by college students majoring in physical education is more about the spirit of sports competition, their achievement motivation is generally higher, and they are more familiar with the rules of sports. Therefore, based on this cognitive background, there are fresh college students majoring in physical education who are more likely to make more harmful behaviors in the sports dilemma, that is, to choose result-oriented decision-making. When they make decisions, they not only consider the results, but also weigh the ethics involved in the event. The experimental results show that under no time and high time pressure, when the self is the decision-making subject, utilitarian decision-making is preferred; especially under no time pressure, the utilitarian choice is highest, but when the decision-making subject is others, individuals are more likely to make deontological decisions. This may be because the situation we face is related to ourselves, with a higher situation involved; thus, considering the problem may be taken with more care, may be more comprehensive, and may be more susceptible to the results, so individuals tend to make utilitarian choices. However, when the spatial distance is closer, the higher the time pressure an individual perceives, the easier it is to make deontological decisions. Simply, the less important information an individual pays attention to and the less complete the cognitive process, the more the individual tends to be conservative rather than risk foul. However, if the decision-making scene occurs at a long distance, even if the information is insufficient, the individual will choose to make a risky decision, which may be because the individual is more tolerant of injury and violations in the long-distance sports scene.

5.2. Future Works and Research Limitations

(1)
Disadvantages
  • The research on psychological distance only uses two dimensions, social and spatial distance. The actual psychological distance also includes two aspects, temporal distance and hypothetical, and the scope is not comprehensive enough.
  • The selected subjects are college students majoring in sports, and their representation is not particularly strong. Such students have less competition experience and are less aware of sportsmanship than professional athletes.
  • The timeliness of the control of learning pressure is not particularly strong. A learning pressure questionnaire should be added to test whether the subjects’ learning pressure manipulation during the experiment is effective.
(2)
Future research directions
  • The four dimensions of psychological distance will be deeply explored for moral decision-making, especially the influence of sports on moral decision-making.
  • More representative subjects will be selected. Additionally, the analysis of the content of the network environment will be strengthened. Study pressure and whether to apply IoT technology will be researched. The exploration of sports ethics’ anomie behavior is more appropriate and pertinent.
  • The sports dilemma of sports majors in different types will be discussed more deeply. The tolerance of sports college students and even athletes to moral misconduct will be analyzed and improved.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, X.Z. and M.L.; methodology, L.W.; software, Q.X.; validation, X.Z. and M.L.; formal analysis, X.Z.; investigation, M.L.; data curation, X.Z. and L.W.; writing—original draft preparation, X.Z.; writing—review and editing, M.L.; visualization, X.Z.; supervision, M.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by Gdansk University of Physical Education and Sport Ethics Committee (protocol code 2234/35 and date of approval 2022.5).

Informed Consent Statement

This article does not contain any experiments with human participants or animals performed by any of the authors. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors without undue reservation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Wang, C.; Du, C. Optimization of physical education and training system based on machine learning and Internet of Things. Neural Comput. Appl. 2022, 34, 9273–9288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Nenkov, G.Y.; Morrin, M.; Maille, V.; Rank-Christ Man, T.; Lwin, M.O. Sense and sensibility: The impact of visual and auditory sensory input on marketplace morality. J. Bus. Res. 2019, 95, 428–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Mohammadian, M.; Sedighi, M.; Serkani, M. Selection of Internet of things (IOT) applications in education industry using the Best-Worst Multi-Criteria Decision-Making Method. Technol. Educ. J. (TEJ) 2022, 16, 363–386. [Google Scholar]
  4. Ju, Y.; You, M. The outrage effect of personal stake, dread, and moral nature on fine dust risk perception moderated by media use. Health Commun. 2021, 36, 866–876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Selinske, M.; Garrard, G.; Bekessy, S.; Gordon, A.; Kusmanof, A.; Fidler, F. Revisiting the promise of conservation psychology. Conserv. Biol. 2018, 32, 1464–1468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Azimi, I.; Pahikkala, T.; Rahmani, A.M.; Niela Vilén, H.; Axelin, A.; Liljeberg, P. Missing data resilient decision-making for healthcare IoT through personalization: A case study on maternal health. Future Gener. Comput. Syst. 2019, 96, 297–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Senders, J.T.; Staples, P.C.; Karhade, A.V.; Zaki, M.M.; Gormley, W.B.; Broekman, M.L.; Smith, T.R.; Arnaout, O. Machine learning and neurosurgical outcome prediction: A systematic review. World Neurosurg. 2018, 109, 476–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Yun, G.; Ravi, R.V.; Jumani, A.K. Analysis of the teaching quality on deep learning-based innovative ideological political education platform. Prog. Artif. Intell. 2022, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chu, H.; Yang, J.Z. Emotion and the psychological distance of climate change. Sci. Commun. 2019, 41, 761–789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Aldahiri, A.; Alrashed, B.; Hussain, W. Trends in using IoT with machine learning in health prediction system. Forecasting 2021, 3, 181–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kassab, M.; DeFranco, J.; Laplante, P. A systematic literature review on Internet of things in education: Benefits and challenges. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 2020, 36, 115–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Ferrag, M.A.; Shu, L.; Choo KK, R. Fighting COVID-19 and future pandemics with the Internet of Things: Security and privacy perspectives. IEEE/CAA J. Autom. Sin. 2021, 8, 1477–1499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Jim, H.S.; Hoogland, A.I.; Brownstein, N.C.; Barata, A.; Dicker, A.P.; Knoop, H.; Johnstone, P.A. Innovations in research and clinical care using patient-generated health data. CA A Cancer J. Clin. 2020, 70, 182–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Hernández-Ortega, B. Don’t believe strangers: Online consumer reviews and the role of social psychological distance. Inf. Manag. 2018, 55, 31–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Lee, K.; Romzi, P.; Hanaysha, J.; Alzoubi, H.; Alshurideh, M. Investigating the impact of benefits and challenges of IOT adoption on supply chain performance and organizational performance: An empirical study in Malaysia. Uncertain Supply Chain Manag. 2022, 10, 537–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Theodorakis, I.G.; Painesis, G. The impact of psychological distance and construal level on consumers’ responses to taboos in advertising. J. Advert. 2018, 47, 161–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Liu, Q.; Zhang, X.; Huang, S.; Zhang, L.; Zhao, Y. Exploring consumers’ buying behavior in a large online promotion activity: The role of psychological distance and involvement. J. Theor. Appl. Electron. Commer. Res. 2020, 15, 66–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Yudkin, D.A.; Pick, R.; Hur, E.Y.; Liberman, N.; Trope, Y. Psychological distance promotes exploration in search of a global maximum. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2019, 45, 893–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Bode, L. Political news in the news feed: Learning politics from social media. Mass Commun. Soc. 2016, 19, 24–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Bg, A.; Alra, B.; Pel, C. Moral decision-making and moral development: Toward an integrative framework. Dev. Rev. 2018, 49, 80–100. [Google Scholar]
  21. Haidt, J. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychol. Rev. 2001, 108, 814–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Greene, J.D.; Nystrom, L.E.; Engell, A.D.; Darley, J.M.; Cohen, J.D. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron 2004, 44, 389–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Greene, J.D. The secret joke of Kant’s soul. In Moral Psychology; The Neuroscience of Morality: Emotion, Brain Disorders, and Development; Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Ed.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2008; pp. 35–80. [Google Scholar]
  24. Liberman, N.; Trope, Y. Trav ersing psychological distance. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 364–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Agnew, C.R.; Loving, T.J.; Le, B.; Goodfriend, W. Thinking close: Measuring relational closeness as perceived self-other inclusion. In Handbook of Closeness & Intimacy; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2004; pp. 103–115. [Google Scholar]
  26. Gong HIliev, R.; Sachdeva, S. Consequences are far away: Psychological distance affects modes of moral decision making. Cognition 2012, 24–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Escolar-Chua, R.L. Moral sensitivity, moral distress, and moral courage among baccalaureate Filipino nursing students. Nurs. Ethics 2018, 25, 458–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Stanger, N.; Kavussanu, M.; Boardley, I.; Ring, C. The Influence of Moral Disengagement and Negative Emotion on Antisocial Sport Behavior. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 2013, 2, 117–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Zhang, Y.; Yin, H.; Zheng, L. Investigating academic dishonesty among Chinese undergraduate students: Does gender matter. Assess. Eval. High. Educ. 2018, 43, 812–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ariely, D.; Zakay, D. A timely account of the role of duration in decision making. Acta Psychol. 2001, 108, 187–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ellemers, N.; Van Der Toorn, J.; Paunov, Y.; Van Leeuwen, T. The psychology of morality: A review and analysis of empirical studies published from 1940 through 2017. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 23, 332–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Wade-Benzoni, K.A. Legacy motivations & the psychology of intergenerational decisions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2019, 26, 19–22. [Google Scholar]
  33. Chen, H.S.; Mitchell, R.K.; Brigham, K.H.; Howell, R.; Steinbauer, R. Perceived psychological distance, construal processes, and abstractness of entrepreneurial action. J. Bus. Ventur. 2018, 33, 296–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Muthukrishna, M.; Bell, A.V.; Henrich, J.; Curtin, C.M.; Gedranovich, A.; McInerney, J.; Thue, B. Beyond Western, Educated, Industrial, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) psychology: Measuring and mapping scales of cultural and psychological distance. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 31, 678–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Zheng, Y.; Ke, H. The adoption of scale space hierarchical cluster analysis algorithm in the classification of rock-climbing teaching evaluation system. J. Ambient. Intell. Human. Comput. 2020, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Zheng, Y.; Zhou, Y.; Lai, Q. Effects of twenty-four move shadow boxing combined with psychosomatic relaxation on depression and anxiety in patients with type-2 diabetes. Psychiatr. Danub. 2015, 27, 174. [Google Scholar]
  37. Xu, D.; Zheng, Y.; Jia, Y. The Bibliometric Analysis of the Sustainable Influence of Physical Education for University Students. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 592276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Social Intuition Model of Moral Decision-Making.
Figure 1. Social Intuition Model of Moral Decision-Making.
Sustainability 14 12115 g001
Figure 2. Interaction between learning pressure and social distance.
Figure 2. Interaction between learning pressure and social distance.
Sustainability 14 12115 g002
Figure 3. Interaction of learning pressure and spatial distance.
Figure 3. Interaction of learning pressure and spatial distance.
Sustainability 14 12115 g003
Table 1. Distribution of subjects.
Table 1. Distribution of subjects.
GenderObject 1 (College Students Majoring in Physical Education within Three Years of Graduation)Object 2 (Graduating College Students Majoring in Physical Education)
boys5743
girls4654
Table 2. Mean and standard deviation of moral decision-making under the two dimensions of psychological distance with different learning pressure levels (M ± SD).
Table 2. Mean and standard deviation of moral decision-making under the two dimensions of psychological distance with different learning pressure levels (M ± SD).
Beijing SelfOthers
no study pressure1.41 ± 1.0550.34 ± 0.815
middle study stress1.01 ± 0.5151.21 ± 0.774
high study pressure0.92 ± 1.1640.66 ± 0.725
France SelfOthers
No study pressure0.88 ± 1.0611.13 ± 0.991
middle study stress1.14 ± 1.0610.61 ± 0.736
high study pressure1.28 ± 1.0340.41 ± 0.506
Table 3. Variance analysis of learning pressure × social distance × spatial distance on utilitarian decision-making.
Table 3. Variance analysis of learning pressure × social distance × spatial distance on utilitarian decision-making.
Source of VariationSum of Squared Deviations from the MeanDegrees of FreedomMean VarianceStatisticsInspection Level
Time pressure0.63520.3180.230.805
Social distance7.4 7.212.0190.001
Spatial distance0.559 0.5560.8850.356
Time pressure × social distance5.433 2.8174.7020.012
Time pressure × spatial distance6.21123.0074.7750.015
Social distance × spatial distance0.810.8011.470.232
Time pressure × social distance × spatial distance0.04310.0190.040.96
Errors23.157410.552
Note: The data in the table are obtained by data analysis according to the experimental results.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zong, X.; Wang, L.; Xie, Q.; Lipowski, M. The Influence of Psychological Distance on the Challenging Moral Decision Support of Sports Majors in Internet of Things and Machine Learning. Sustainability 2022, 14, 12115. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141912115

AMA Style

Zong X, Wang L, Xie Q, Lipowski M. The Influence of Psychological Distance on the Challenging Moral Decision Support of Sports Majors in Internet of Things and Machine Learning. Sustainability. 2022; 14(19):12115. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141912115

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zong, Xingxing, Lian Wang, Qingyuan Xie, and Mariusz Lipowski. 2022. "The Influence of Psychological Distance on the Challenging Moral Decision Support of Sports Majors in Internet of Things and Machine Learning" Sustainability 14, no. 19: 12115. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141912115

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop