Next Article in Journal
Research on Spatial Correlation Characteristics and Their Spatial Spillover Effect of Local Government Debt Risks in China
Previous Article in Journal
Effect of Twisted Fin Array in a Triple-Tube Latent Heat Storage System during the Charging Mode
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Three Levels of Green Exercise, Physical and Social Environments, Personality Traits, Physical Activity, and Engagement with Nature on Emotions and Attention

Department of Landscape Architecture, National Chin-Yi University of Technology, Taichung 41170, Taiwan
Sustainability 2021, 13(5), 2686; https://doi.org/10.3390/su13052686
Submission received: 4 February 2021 / Revised: 24 February 2021 / Accepted: 26 February 2021 / Published: 2 March 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Social Ecology and Sustainability)

Abstract

:
The current study examined the effects of the three levels of green exercise on people’s psychological health using a randomized trial with a pretest and posttest design and further explored which variables of the physical environment (thermal comfort, noise, and air pollution), social environment (the number of companions and crowdedness), personality traits, physical activity (intensity and frequency), and engagement with nature may help explain experiences during the three levels of green exercise using a cross-sectional approach. Field studies were conducted to test the study’s hypotheses. The participants were 95 students from a technology university in Taiwan. The experiment comprised a 15-min green exercise in a park. No significant differences were found in emotions and attention between the three levels of green exercise. However, a 15-min green exercise of any level significantly improved emotions and attention. Furthermore, fatigue was significantly and negatively associated with daily transportation-related physical activity, agreeableness, and engagement with nature. Moreover, the total mood disturbance was significantly and negatively associated with engagement with nature and daily transportation-related physical activity. The degree of engagement with nature played a pivotal role in green exercise. This study provided the evidence that quantified engagement with nature is beneficial for quantified psychological health for the first time.

1. Introduction

Globally, approximately 31% of adults lacked sufficient physical activity in 2008, around 3.2 million deaths each year are attributable to insufficient physical activity [1]. Moreover, three diseases that received the most attention globally are depression, cardiovascular disease, and AIDS in 2020. Among these, depression is the number one cause of human disability and number two cause of the overall burden of society. People in modern society certainly face increasingly serious challenges of physical and psychological health [2]. Research has shown that physical activity benefits not only physical health [3] but also psychological health [4]. The former certainly includes cardiovascular disease [5,6] and the latter particularly includes depression [7,8], as indicated by the results of meta-analyses. Studies have also shown that people engaging in physical activity require positive environmental stimulation to receive comprehensive benefits from their physical activity of choice [9]. For example, a laboratory study showed that sensory stimulation of visual, auditory, and olfactory of natural environments tended to improve emotions of participants conducting green exercise compared to sensory occlusion in terms of a reduction in tension, fatigue, and confusion and an increase in vigor [10]. Immersion in natural environments when taking physical activity is critical for people to receive benefits [11].
Two major theories explore the benefits of exposure to nature: The Stress Reduction Theory (SRT) [12] and the Attention Restoration Theory (ART) [13]. Both theories suggest that thousands of years of evolutionary processes taking place in nature has enabled mankind to respond adaptively to natural elements with beneficial responses [14]. SRT focuses on emotional and physiological recovery from arousal in response to stress [15], while ART emphasizes the benefits of exposure to nature on attention depleted by continuous use and is restored by temporarily not being used due to the ability of nature to fascinate humans [13]. In general, lack of attention decreases cognitive clarity and effective functioning, specifically which results in irritability, impulsiveness, impatience, reduced tolerance for frustration, and increased likelihood of taking risks [13,16,17,18]. ART sees humans as active and purposive restoration seekers, who take goal-directed behaviors to pursue restorative experiences, while SRT seems to emphasize more people’s direct, psycho-physiological responses to nature [19]. Though the focus of both theories is not on the physical activity, they provide theoretical foundations to explore the psychological health of physical activity taking in natural environments.
As such, “green exercise”, “a direct form of engagement that describes physical activity with a simultaneous exposure to nature” [20], may be a creative solution of the modern challenges of physical and psychological health. According to the degree of engagement with nature, green exercise can further be divided into the following three levels: (1) Viewing nature: This refers to people being visually involved with the environment, such as viewing it from a window or looking at “nature surrogates” (i.e., pictures, posters, and videos of nature); (2) being in the presence of nearby nature: This refers to people being accidentally or incidentally involved with the environment, but relatively passively because their primary interests are activities such as walking, cycling, and talking in a park, not nature itself; and (3) active participation and involvement with nature: This refers to people purposefully engaging with the environment, such as through gardening, farming, and camping [4,21]. Therefore, green exercise is easily practical in everyday life and does not necessarily require organized, planned, purposeful, special, and repetitive activities “out in the wild” to maintain or promote physical fitness, such as exercise or sports [22].
Current empirical research has largely focused on the first level of green exercise (viewing nature) [23]. Studies on the second level of green exercise (being in the presence of nearby nature) have primarily focused on walking or jogging [24,25]. As for studies focusing on the third level of green exercise (active participation and involvement with nature), emphasis has been placed on gardening activities, “nature experiences,” and “outward bound experiences.” Few studies have examined all three levels concurrently [26]. Only six studies [11,27,28,29,30,31] have been published on this subject, which compared only the effects of two levels of green exercise, but not all three. In brief, the second level of green exercise (being in the presence of nearby nature) was found to have more positive influences than did the first level of green exercise (viewing nature) in terms of energy [11,27] and relaxation [31]. The third level of green exercise (active participation and involvement with nature) was also found to have a greater effect of stress restoration than the first level of green exercise (viewing nature) [30]. Furthermore, research on green exercise has rarely considered the degree of engagement with nature or empirically examined its effects on people [26], even though the degree of engagement with nature is the criterion of the categorization of three levels of green exercise. Therefore, the first objective of the present study was to examine the effects of all three levels of green exercise on people’s psychological health to address the gap in the literature. Moreover, given that most studies have focused on the effects of engaging in the same activities in different environments, this study examined the effects on people’s psychological health when engaged in the three levels of green exercise in the same environment. The author was interested in the within-environment (i.e., the same environment) difference rather than the between-environment (i.e., different environment) difference. Specifically, this study explored whether and how different forms of physical activity in the same green space along with the three levels of engagement with nature affect emotions and attention [12,13].
In addition, scholars have suggested that weather should be considered when discussing green exercise [11], as well as noise and air quality [29], taking physical activity alone or with others [32], social interaction [33], duration and intensity of physical activities [34], and physical fitness [35]. As such, social ecologists have provided a comprehensive view of physical activity, arguing that physical activity is influenced by interactions among the physical environment, social environment, and the participants’ personality traits [36,37]. However, although green exercise is one type of physical activity, research on green exercise has not examined the influence of personality traits. Therefore, the second objective of this study was to address this gap in the literature by examining the influence of the physical environment, social environment, personality traits, physical activity, and particularly engagement with nature across all three levels of green exercise in the same environment, to explore which variables may help to explain people’s experiences during the three levels of green exercise. Specifically, the present study comprehensively examined the effects of thermal comfort, noise, and air pollution of the physical environment; the number of companions and crowdedness of the social environment; the personality traits of the participants; the intensity of the physical activities and the frequency of physical activities; and the degree of engagement with nature on emotions and attention during the three levels of green exercise. The investigated physical activities included the experimental green exercise as well as other physical activity performed in daily life. Nevertheless, to explore the mechanism of green exercise with respect to psychological health or the pathways of these various factors are beyond the scope of this study.
Based on the abovementioned objectives, this study proposed two hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Higher levels of green exercise have greater benefits to participants’ emotions and attention than lower levels of green exercise [11,27,30,31] and accordingly.
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
The degree of engagement with nature has greater benefits for participants’ emotions and attention than does physical activity, physical and social environments, and personality traits [9,11].
However, these two directional hypotheses are arguable because no research has explored the effects of all of these factors simultaneously on participants’ psychological health. Given that this study explores the influence of the physical environment, social environment, physical activity, and personality traits on people’s psychological health, a literature review is provided below.

2. Physical Environment and Psychological Health

Environmental psychology often regards weather, noise, and air pollution as stressors in the physical environment [38,39]. Weather can affect people’s comfort in outdoor environments [40] as well as their satisfaction and recreational behaviors [41]. In general, weather changes people’s preference for natural environments [42,43]. Research on specific atmospheric conditions has shown that temperature, relative humidity, radiant temperature, wind speed, and thermal comfort affect people’s moods outdoors [28]. With the increasing awareness of the greenhouse effect, the thermal comfort of the outdoor environment has received increasing attention [44,45]. Research has also found that thermal comfort affects preferences, physiological responses, and attention [46]. Moreover, noise affects people’s emotions, mainly causing annoyance [39] and anger [47], general psychological processes such as attention and short-term memory [48], and even psychoneurotic disorders [49]. Air pollution also causes negative emotions [39] such as depression and mood disorder [50] and also influences attention [51]. The effects of particulate matter (PM) and ultrafine particulate matter on the brain and central nervous system have also received considerable attention in recent years [50]. Nevertheless, studies of the effects of outdoor thermal comfort [52,53] and recent exposure to air pollution [54,55] on psychological health remain inadequate.

3. Social Environment and Psychological Health

Crowdedness is often discussed in environmental psychology as a key stressor in the social environment [38,39]. Chen [56] defined a sense of crowdedness as having too much stimulation, too many restrictions on behavior, too much disliked social contact or interference, and inappropriate resources, all of which make people feel the high-density influence. People usually have negative emotions when in an overcrowded space, which affects their physical, cognitive, and behavioral responses [38]. Furthermore, an inverted u-shaped function between presence of people and perceived restorativeness has been found for urban parks. This suggests that having some people nearby is more beneficial for psychological health than a crowded space or an isolated space in the natural environment [57]. Human interaction in social environments also includes companions. Having companionship during activity can reduce a sense of isolation [58] and encourage a person to adhere to walking activities [59]. People generally experience greater enjoyment in outdoor group walks than when walking outdoors alone [60]. Three studies using self-report questionnaires investigated the intermediating relationships between social environment and green exercise with respect to psychological health. One study found that shortage of social support and loneliness partly mediated the relationship but not social cohesion or social contact [61]. The other found no such relationship in terms of social interaction, social cohesion, and loneliness [62]. Another found that social cohesion and loneliness mediated the relationship [63]. Given the inconsistent results and method bias of self-reports of these three studies [63], further explorations of the relationships between green exercise, social environment, and psychological health are needed.

4. Physical Activity and Psychological Health

The benefits of physical activity and exercise on psychological health include improving mood, anxiety, depression, self-perception, self-esteem, and cognitive functioning [4,33]. Meta-analyses further showed that exercise can be an effective healing treatment for depression [7,8] and is beneficial to cognitive functioning [64]. Attention is closely related to cognitive functioning particularly in terms of the ART [13]. Another meta-analysis further indicated that both during and following exercise, exercise-induced arousal improved cognitive performance such as speeded mental processing and enhanced memory storage and retrieval [65]. Moreover, a meta-analysis focusing on green exercise also showed that: (1) It significantly improved self-esteem and total mood disturbance (TMD), with a moderate effect size; (2) the improvements in self-esteem and mood, and the activity duration showed a u-shaped relationship; and (3) the relationship between the improvement in self-esteem and activity intensity showed a linearly negative correlation, whereas the improvement in mood and activity intensity showed a u-shaped relationship [66]. In addition to activity duration and intensity, the effects of green exercise on psychological health, particularly attention, of other factors of physical activity, such as physical fitness and activity frequency [35], have seldom been examined simultaneously.

5. Personality Traits, Physical Activity, and Psychological Health

Most researchers have argued that there is a significant correlation between the activity type that people engage in and their personality traits [67,68]. Extraversion was found to be positively correlated with exercise participation and regular exercise, and emotional stability was positively correlated with regular exercise [69]. It was found that those with greater personality traits of emotional stability, extraversion, openness to learning, agreeableness, cautiousness, and responsibility had greater motivation to exercise [70]. Meta-analyses also showed that: (1) Extraversion, conscientiousness, and openness all had a positive correlation with physical activity; (2) neuroticism had a negative correlation with physical activity [71,72]; (3) lower neuroticism and higher conscientiousness were correlated with more physical activity and with less physical inactivity and static behavior; and (4) extraversion and openness were correlated with more physical activity and less physical inactivity. However, extraversion and openness are not related to most static behaviors such as watching television [73].
Individuals with some personality traits also tend to have a sense of belonging to the natural world. Studies showed that extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness were positively associated with the concept that nature and humanity are one, while neuroticism was negatively associated with that concept, as measured by various scales [74,75,76]. Specifically, openness was found to have the largest correlation with the Connectedness to Nature [74] and the Inclusion of Nature in Self [76]. Moreover, openness and agreeableness were found to have the largest and the second largest correlation, respectively, with the Nature Relatedness, Commitment to Nature, Connectedness to Nature, Connectivity with Nature, Emotional Affinity toward Nature, and Environmental Identity [75,76]. Therefore, people with the personality traits of extraversion, conscientiousness, openness, and agreeableness are more likely to take physical activity in natural environments.
Moreover, the relationship between personality traits and psychological health has long been hypothesized since the ancient Greece [77]. Meta-analyses showed that: In general, high levels of neuroticism and low levels of extraversion, conscientiousness, and agreeableness were positively related to mood disorders [78]; and, specifically, high neuroticism and low conscientiousness were positively related to depressive disorders [79]. Moreover, it was found that: (1) Extraversion and agreeableness were positively related to mental health [80] and optimism [81]; and (2) extraversion, agreeableness, and openness were positively related to vitality [82,83]. Nevertheless, personality traits, physical activity, and psychological health are seldom investigated simultaneously. Given that personality traits are related to both physical activity and psychological health, it is necessary to further examine which one has greater explanatory power for the psychological health effects of engaging in green exercise in the same natural environment.

6. Research Methods

6.1. Research Design

This study is part of a larger research project funded by a government agency. Although this research project involved human subjects, the funding agency did not require this research project to be submitted to the Institutional Review Board (IRB). Nevertheless, we (the author and research assistants) adhered to the Declaration of Helsinki on human research ethics revised in 1975, when conducting this study. H1 (differences in emotions and attention between three green exercise levels) was tested using a randomized trial with a pretest and posttest design in which each of the participants and his or her companions were randomly assigned to a real environment to engage in one of the three levels of green exercise as an experimental treatment. Thus, the unit of randomization was the individual participant. Given that we adopted a rigorous randomized trial to maintain the internal validity of this study, the participants were not allowed to choose their preferred activities of engagement with nature. H2 (greater effects of engagement with nature on emotions and attention than other factors) was further examined using a cross-sectional approach to collect additional data on the physical environment (thermal comfort, noise, and air pollution), the social environment (number of companions and crowdedness), and personality traits (Figure 1), which could not be controlled in this field study. The physical activity variables included activity intensity, limb activity, and activity frequency. The participants were not aware of their level of green exercise was designated. However, the research assistant was aware of the participant assignments and she also assessed the outcomes.

6.2. Experimental Procedure

Each participant’s experimental session lasted approximately 70 min, which included times for explaining the instructions, putting on physical activity instruments, reading and signing consent forms, completing questionnaires and scales (pretest), performing attention tests (pretest), calibrating instruments, taking the treatment, completing scales (posttest), performing attention tests (posttest), and removing the instruments, which were all conducted at the experimental environment (Figure 2). A research assistant was with the participants during the experimental procedures except for when the participants were taking the treatment. Before the official experiment, the research assistant had gone over the entire procedure three times with one male and two female volunteer students individual by individual to familiarize herself with the experiment. The same research assistant then conducted all experimental sessions to maintain the consistency of intervention and data collection, who also took photos and notes of the participants to make sure that each experimental condition was provided as planned.

6.3. Experimental Setting

The experimental environment was a forest park in Taichung, Taiwan, which spans 11.4 hectares. Overall, the park comprises 3.7 hectares of trees, which accounts for more than 30% of the total area. To ensure that the participants conducted the activities in the same location, a small area was assigned in the park as a common activity site for the experiment, which covered approximately 0.24 ha (56 × 67 × 15 × 80 m; Figure 3 and Figure 4). The experiment was conducted from 2 December 2015 to 23 February 2016, 8 a.m. to 5 p.m., to fit the participants’ school schedules. The average temperature during this period was 20.50 °C (SD = 3.405).

6.4. Experimental Treatment

The experimental treatment for each participant lasted 15 min for only one activity bout of one session, a time that was selected because studies have shown that one bout of 15-min green exercise is beneficial for physical and psychological health [11,84]. We randomly assigned each of the participants to one of three groups that corresponded to the three levels of green exercise based on a random number generator on the internet. That is, the interventions were administered individual by individual. The instruction given to the participants was that we would like to know their activity experience in the forest park. In the first-level group (viewing nature), participants were instructed to sit on a long chair and view the surrounding scenery. In the second-level group (being in the presence of nearby nature), participants were instructed to act freely (e.g., chatting, standing, walking, and jogging), but could not sit, lie, or rest. In the third-level group (active participation and involvement with nature), participants were instructed to collect at least seven natural elements that they considered special, such as stones, flowers, fruits, branches, leaves, and insects [26], similar to scavenging done by early humans [85]. Previous research showed that this experimental treatment resulted in significant difference in the degree of engagement with nature and in the levels of physical activity between the three levels of green exercise [26]. These interventions did not cause important adverse events and/or side effects.

6.5. Participants and Companions

The participants were recruited from a technology university in Taiwan via flyers. For safety reasons and because this study explored the effects of physical activity on people in an outdoor environment, the inclusion criterion for participants was general good health. Exclusion criteria for participants were injuries, pain, asthma [60], or allergies (e.g., to sunlight, air pollution, and plants). We also encouraged the participants to invite their relatives and friends to accompany them. However, they could not bring their spouse or romantic partner [60]. This was because romantic relationships may have impacts on emotional and cognitive well-being [86]. The invited companions were not required to complete any questionnaire, wear any instrument, or provide any information. Given that the companions were at their own will to accompany the participants, the number of companions could not be controlled by this study. We recruited 101 Taiwanese college students as the participants, of which 95 (41 men and 54 women) were valid, with an average age of 20.47 years (standard deviation [SD] = 1.236), average height of 165.53 cm (SD = 8.805), average weight of 56.96 kg (SD = 11.391), and average body mass index of 20.67 (SD = 2.963). Furthermore, 39 participants were accompanied by between one and five companions.
In the end, 33 participants participated in the first-level activity (viewing nature), 13 of which brought companions; 31 participated in the second-level activity (being in the presence of nearby nature), 13 of which brought companions; and 31 participated in the third-level activity (active participation and involvement with nature), 13 of which brought companions. The results of statistical analyses showed no significant differences between the three participant groups in terms of college attended, years of study, with or without companions, companion types, age, height, weight, and body mass index, except for gender (χ2 = 7.998, p = 0.018, df = 2). Neither the participants nor their companions received compensation. Data from six participants were ruled invalid because their physical activity instruments recorded incomplete data. We did not exclude any participants, nor did we lose any of them during any stage of the experimental procedures. Since the participants attended only one activity bout of one session, their nonattendance days/rate was zero.

6.6. Research Variables and Measurements

The present study contained 2 major independent variables to examine H1 (differences in emotions and attention between three green exercise levels), 10 major predictor variables to examine H2 (greater effects of engagement with nature on emotions and attention than other factors), and 2 major dependent variables to examine both H1 and H2. A summary of these variables and their measurements is presented in Table 1.

6.6.1. Three Levels of Green Exercise (Independent and Predictor Variable)

Engagement with nature refers to people’s external attention to [9] and involvement with nature in physical, psychological, and spiritual ways [26]. A questionnaire that included eight items (vision, hearing, smell, taste, touch, body and limbs, cognition, and spirituality) was used to measure the participants’ engagement with nature on a 7-point scale. A sample item is: To what extent was your interaction or engagement with nature in terms of vision. The higher the total score, the higher was the degree of engagement with nature [26].
In order to measure the physical activity during the experiment, we used a global positioning system (GPS) watch (Forerunner 405, Garmin, Taipei, ROC) worn on the participants’ nondominant hand to continually record their position, movement, and duration (4 Hz sample rate) of activity. The positioning accuracy was within 10 m. In combination with a geographic information system software (ArcGIS, Environmental Systems Research Institute, Redlands, CA, USA), we calculated the body movement speeds of the participants (total distance covered divided by time: m/s). In addition, we used a MicroMini-Motionlogger Actigraph watch (Ambulatory Monitoring Inc., Ardsley, NY, USA) with proportional integrating measure mode worn on the wrist of participants’ dominant hand to objectively record hand activity (16 Hz sample rate, 2–3 Hz bandwidth, no unit of measurement) [87]. The device has good reliability and validity [88,89]. By using an Intelligent Device for Energy Expenditure and Activity (IDEEA) 3 (MiniSun, Fresno, CA, USA), we continually measured participants’ limb activity (type %), posture (type %, m/m), posture change (number), gait (type %), and energy expenditure (kcal/m) during the experiment (64 Hz sample rate). The empirical results demonstrated that (1) the IDEEA accurately identified participants’ physical activity types with an accuracy of >98%. The correlation coefficient between estimated walking and running speeds and actual speed was 0.986 [90]. (2) The intraclass correlation reliability between the measured gait and force plate measurements was 0.784 (stride) and 0.998 (rhythm) [91]. (3) The IDEEA accurately estimated the energy consumed during physical activities, with an accuracy of more than 95% [92].

6.6.2. Personality Traits (Predictor Variable)

Personality traits refer to people’s unique characteristic patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors [93], which we measured using the Chinese version of the Big Five Test. Huang [94] translated the Big Five Test, developed by John et al. [95], into Chinese. The Chinese version of the Big Five Test comprised of 44 questions on a 5-point scale, 16 of which were reverse items: The higher the score, the more apparent was the personality trait. Huang [94] also collected the data of 157 participants in Taiwan using a questionnaire to test the Chinese version of the Big Five Test. The results showed that it had good internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.75), as did each personality trait dimension (Cronbach’s α = 0.80 for extraversion, 0.71 for agreeableness, 0.78 for conscientiousness, 0.79 for neuroticism, and 0.77 for openness). Sample items include: I am an outgoing and sociable person; I like to cooperate with others; I make plans and follow through with them; I get nervous easily; and I am curious about many different things.

6.6.3. Social Environment (Predictor Variable)

Human interactions in social environments often include companions, who accompany individuals in their activities. We recorded the number of companions in the questionnaire. In addition, Manning et al. [96] suggested that research on the degree of crowdedness should be conducted using a visual method rather than the traditional method of asking respondents about the number of people they encounter. Therefore, panoramic photographs were taken every five minutes at the center of the experimental site to record the number of people and vehicles (i.e., cars, motorcycles, and bicycles). Thus, each participant had four sets of data on crowdedness from the beginning of the experiment to the end, whereas nonparticipants and vehicles were tallied only once.

6.6.4. Physical Environment (Predictor Variable)

Thermal comfort, which is defined as the condition of satisfaction provided by the thermal environment [97], comprises environmental conditions (temperature, relative humidity, wind speed, and average radiant temperature) and human conditions (metabolic heat and clothing quantity). This study followed Standard 55 of the American Society of Heating, Refrigerating and Air-Conditioning Engineers and Standard 7726 of the International Organization for Standardization [98] to measure the environmental conditions during the experiment using appropriate instruments (R.M. Young-41382 (measuring range: 0–100% sRH, accuracy at 23 °C: ±1% RH, response time: 10 s; calibrated measuring range: −50 to 50 °C, accuracy at 23 °C: ±0.3 °C, response time: 10 s), R.M. Young-03002 (wind speed range: 0–50 m/s, azimuth range: 360° mechanical, 352° electrical, wind speed accuracy: ±0.5 m/s, wind direction accuracy: ±5°, anemometer threshold: 1.1 m/s, vane threshold: 1.3 m/s), R.M. Young Company, Traverse City, MI, USA, and TRH-301 (range: 0–100% RH, 0–100 °C, accuracy: ±2% RH, ±0.3 °C, compensation: ±0.008% RH/°C, response time: <15 s), TECPEL Co. Ltd., New Taipei City, ROC) set up at the experimental site. We used questionnaires to survey the participants’ physical conditions (sex, height, weight, age, clothing, and physical activity) on site. Subsequently, we used RayMan software [99,100] to calculate two thermal comfort indices, physiologically equivalent temperature (PET) and standard effective temperature (SET).
In general, noise is unwanted sound. During the experiment, we measured the ambient sound volume in decibels (dBA) every five minutes at the center of the experimental site using a decibel meter (DT-805, (range: 30–130 dB, accuracy: ±1.4 dB, frequency: 31.5–8 kHz), SHENZHEN EVERBEST MACHINERY INDUSTRY CO., LTD., Shenzhen, China). Therefore, each participant had four items of data measured on noise from the start of the experiment to the end, and the mean value was then calculated.
Air pollutants are defined as substances in the air that directly or indirectly impair people’s health or environmental quality [101]. We adopted air quality data of the Taichung City Air Quality Station (Environmental Protection Administration, EPA) closest to the experimental site. The data were collected hourly and included SO2, CO, O3, PM10, PM2.5, NOx, NO, and NO2.

6.6.5. Daily Physical Activity (Predictor Variable)

The physical activity of participants over the seven days before the experiment was measured using the Chinese version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ), which features 27 questions that cover job, transport, housework, recreation, and sitting. The level of physical activity was calculated as frequency × time × metabolic equivalent per week. Liou [102] obtained approval of the World Health Organization (WHO) to translate the IPAQ into Chinese. A test found that: (1) The content validity index of the Chinese and English versions was more than 98%, (2) the internal rank correlation between the degree of similarity of the languages and meanings was 0.72–0.93, (3) the test-retest reliability was 0.78, and (4) the criterion validity with respect to the three-dimensional space accelerator was 0.31–0.52 [103]. Example questions included: Don’t count the walks you have mentioned about work and traffic. In the past seven days, how many days did you use walking as leisure or exercise, with each time lasting more than 10 min? When you use walking as leisure or exercise, how much time does this type of walking usually take up a day? Please consider again only those activities that lasted more than 10 min. In the past seven days, how many days did you do perform strenuous and moderately strenuous activities during your leisure time? How many hours do you usually spend a day doing strenuous and moderately strenuous leisure activities? Moreover, we used questionnaires to survey how many times the participants had engaged in physical activity in the seven days before the experiment. This was because extraversion was positively correlated with regular exercise, which was positively correlated with emotional stability [69].

6.6.6. Emotions (Dependent Variable)

Emotions are broadly defined as the feeling dimension of people [104], and in this study, they were measured using the Chinese version of the Profile of Mood States (POMS) both before and after the experimental treatment. Although moods last longer than emotions in general [105], mood states and emotions were viewed relatively equivalent in this study. Chang and Lu [106] used back translation to translate the POMS short form [107] into Chinese. The internal consistency reliability of the Chinese version was 0.71–0.93, and the total explained variance of validity was 65.56%. Moreover, exploratory factor analysis showed that it had good reliability and validity. Hsu et al. [108] then conducted confirmatory factor analysis to prove the construct validity of the Chinese version of the POMS, which included vitality, self-esteem, confusion, fatigue, anger, tension, and depression. Each question of the POMS was measured on a 5-point scale, and the higher the score, the stronger the emotion. Example questions included: Energetic, dignified, confused, fatigued, furious, nervous, and hopeless. In addition, we used TMD to assess the participants’ overall emotional state. We calculated the score from the sum of the five negative emotion scores in the Chinese version of the POMS, minus the scores of the two positive emotions, and added the constant 100 [109]. The higher the TMD score, the more disturbed, irritated, or disordered was each participant.

6.6.7. Attention (Dependent Variable)

Attention is the concentration of consciousness or the allocation of limited cognitive processing resources [110], which in this study was objectively measured using the spatial span forward (SSF) and digit span backward (DSB) tests from the Wechsler Memory Scale (third edition) both before and after the experimental treatment. The SSF is a visual memory span test conducted using a spatial memory span board, on which has 10 numbered small blocks. The research assistant touched blocks (from two to nine) in a specific order at a speed of approximately one second per block, and participants must then identify the blocks in the same order. When participants perform the DSB test, they must accurately reverse the order of the digital strings (from two to eight) dictated by the research assistant and repeat them. The test was shown to have good reliability and validity [111]. Before the official test, participants were allowed two practice runs. Their attention was calculated as the maximum number of correct answers before two consecutive incorrect answers [112]. The higher the score, the greater was the participant’s attention.

6.7. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using the PASW Statistics for Windows, version 22.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). The collected data did not have any missing values. Outliers were limited, only four for both fatigue pretest and posttest, two for tension pretest and three for tension posttest, four for both depression pretest and posttest, and one for both SSF pretest and posttest. Nevertheless, the analyses were by intent to treat. That is, all data were included for analyses after participant allocation. The estimate of treatment effect is generally conservative for intent to treat because of dilution due to noncompliant participants [113]. Appendix A Table A1 is the descriptive statistics and the distribution of the collected data. When the data did not meet the premises of statistical analyses, such as normality, homogeneity, sphericity, or covariance matrix, they were transformed [98].
H1, Higher levels of green exercise have greater benefits to participants’ emotions and attention than lower levels of green exercise, was examined using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). MANOVA was used because it is a statistical test of more than one dependent variable based on their optimal linear combination [98]. That is, MANOVA can simultaneously consider the relationship of all dependent variables to examine whether a significant difference exists in the experimental treatment, and then, analyze whether the experimental treatment has significant differences in terms of individual dependent variables. Further, the observed power of the MANOVA was reported. The Bonferroni correction was used for post hoc pair comparisons because there were three levels of green exercise as the experimental treatment in consideration of the experiment-wise error-rate.
H2, the degree of engagement with nature has greater benefits for participants’ emotions and attention than does physical activity, physical and social environments, and personality traits, was examined using multiple linear regression analyses. Nevertheless, predictor variables that are highly correlated with others must be excluded to prevent collinearity and to fit the premises of the regression models [114]. Moreover, given that the a priori power for the determined sample size for the regression analyses is dependent on the number of the predictors, which should consider the collinearity problem, this study calculated the post hoc power using the predictors without the collinearity using the G*Power 3.1.9.2 program (Heinrich-Heine-Universität, Düsseldorf, Germany).

7. Results

7.1. Scale Reliability

We conducted internal consistency reliability analyses for the Big Five Test, IPAQ, POMS (both pretest and posttest), and the degree of engagement with nature. The results showed that (1) the Big Five Test had relatively acceptable reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.65), (2) the IPAQ had less than ideal reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.574), (3) the POMS had good reliability for the pretest and posttest (Cronbach’s α = 0.929 and 0.880, respectively), and (4) the degree of engagement with nature had good reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.765) [115]. Since the internal consistency reliability of these scales was not very low, the data of these scales were not removed from the analysis.

7.2. Hypothesis 1 (Differences in Emotions and Attention between Three Green Exercise Levels)

We set the SSF, DSB, seven emotions in the POMS, and TMD as dependent variables; the levels of green exercise as an independent variable and independent factor; and the test time (pretest and posttest) as independent variables and a dependent factor (repeated measures) to conduct a two-way mixed-design MANOVA. The results of the MANOVA showed the following: (1) The interaction between green exercise levels and test time reached significance only in the SSF test and entailed a moderate effect size (F(2,89) = 3.700, p = 0.029, ηp2 = 0.077); the 95% confidence interval (CI) for the difference did not include 0. Follow-up tests showed that for the second-level green exercise (being in the presence of nearby nature), the SSF posttest (M = 9.323) was significantly greater than the pretest (M = 8.774) with an observed power of 0.554, and the 95% CI for the difference did not include 0, whereas for the third-level green exercise (active participation and involvement with nature), the SSF posttest (M = 10.000) was also significantly greater than the pretest (M = 8.452) with an observed power of 0.975, and the 95% CI for the difference did not include 0. (2) The main effects of green exercise levels were nonsignificant (F(2,89) ≤ 1.482, p ≥ 0.233, ηp2 ≤ 0.032). (3) The main effects of test time for SSF, vitality, confusion, fatigue, anger, tension, depression, and TMD all reached significance (F(1,89) ≥ 5.704, p ≤ 0.019, ηp2 ≥ 0.060) with observed powers at least greater than 0.656 and entailed at least a moderate effect size. Moreover, none of the 95% CIs for the difference included 0, and all posttests were greater than the pretests (Table 2; Appendix A Table A2).

7.3. Hypothesis 2 (Greater Effects of Engagement with Nature on Emotions and Attention Than Other Factors)

The predictor variables with collinearity were eliminated [114]. As a result, 23 predictor variables (the Big Five Test, IPAQ, the degree of engagement with nature, number of companions, activity frequency, MicroMini-Motionlogger Actigraph hand activity, IDEEA all posture speed, IDEEA numbers for ascending and descending stairs, crowdedness in terms of the number of vehicles, noise, O3, SO2, and two dummy variables for the three green exercise levels) were appropriate for conducting multiple linear regression analyses without collinearity. Next, we used the changed values (posttest values minus pretest values) of the SSF, DSB, TMD, and the seven emotions of the POMS as dependent variables to conduct multiple linear regression analyses. The results showed that: (1) Fatigue reached significance (F(21,73) = 1.761, p = 0.040, R2 = 0.336, R2 Adjusted = 0.145) with a post hoc power of 0.981, the significant predictor variables were transportation-related physical activity (beta = −0.337, t(94) = −2.96, p = 0.004), agreeableness (beta = −0.234, t(94) = −2.186, p = 0.032), and the degree of engagement with nature (beta = −0.227, t(94) = −2.126, p = 0.037; Table 3; Appendix A Table A3). (2) The TMD reached significance (F(21,73) = 1.769, p = 0.039, R2 = 0.337, R2 Adjusted = 0.147) with a post hoc power of 0.982; the significant predictor variables were the degree of engagement with nature (beta = −0.319, t(94) = −2.994, p = 0.004) and transportation-related physical activity (beta = −0.265, t(94) = −2.332, p = 0.022; Table 3; Appendix A Table A4).

8. Discussion

The results did not support H1, which stated that higher levels of green exercise would have greater benefits to participants’ emotions and attention than would lower levels. Six studies have compared the effects of two levels of green exercise, with most finding varying effects between different environments [11,27,28,30,31]. However, no differences were found in terms of the effects within similar environments [11]. Therefore, future studies should examine whether different environments (i.e., between-environment difference), different levels of green exercise (i.e., within-environment difference), or the interaction between environments and green exercise levels have greater effects on people. Moreover, the classification of environments should not be limited to the dichotomy of natural and manmade environments [84]. More crucially, the present study found that regardless of the level of green exercise participants engaged in, as little as 15 min was beneficial for improving their emotions, such as vitality, confusion, fatigue, anger, tension, depression, and total mood disturbance, and attention in terms of spatial span forward test. This result is consistent with both other empirical research findings [11,28,84] and SRT [12] and ART [13]. This result demonstrates the importance of green exercise for psychological health, particularly with respect to its convenient practicality in daily life.
The results partially supported H2, which stated that the degree of engagement with nature would have greater benefits for participants’ emotions and attention than physical activity, physical and social environments, and personality traits. Fatigue was significantly and negatively associated with transportation-related physical activity, agreeableness, and the degree of engagement with nature (in a descending order) and the TMD was significantly and negatively associated with the degree of engagement with nature and transportation-related physical activity (in a descending order). Therefore, when people engage in green exercise, the more direct and profound the interaction with nature is, the more likely it is to reduce negative emotions. This is the first time that empirical research has directly shown that quantified engagement with nature is beneficial for quantified psychological health (decreased fatigue and total mood disturbance). Furthermore, this is consistent with previous findings in that environmentally oriented activities [116], positive environmental stimuli [9], and immersion in natural environments [11] are crucial for people to receive benefits from physical activity. Therefore, green exercise should be considered a method of locking people’s physical activities and environments into an ongoing positive relationship [117]. Furthermore, engagement with nature appears to be the key to integrating people’s physical activities and environments.
H2 (greater effects of engagement with nature on emotions and attention than other factors) failed to find significant results related to attention. This may be because attention was measured by SSF and DSB, which are focused on working memory [112]. There is a broad consensus that attention and working memory are closely linked [118]. However, a recent systematic review study of ART indicated no current agreements on what constitutes a measurement of attention as proposed by the ART. Therefore, an optimal testing protocol of attention, in the view of the ART, should include working memory as measured by SSF and DSB [112], cognitive flexibility as measured by the Trail Making Test B [119], and alerting, orienting, and executive control as measured by the Attention Network Task [120]. Therefore, future research on the ART may need to apply a set of tests to measure attention [121].
H1 (Higher levels of green exercise have greater benefits to participants’ emotions and attention than lower levels of green exercise) and H2 (The degree of engagement with nature has greater benefits for participants’ emotions and attention than does physical activity, physical and social environments, and personality traits) appeared to be similar. However, H1 focused on examining the effect of the experimental treatment of the three green exercise levels operationalized simultaneously by not only types of physical activity but also levels of engagement with nature. This research found no significant differences between the three green exercise levels. By contrast, H2 focused on further exploring the effect of the physical environment, social environment, personality traits, physical activity, engagement with nature, and the three green exercise levels, respectively. This research found engagement with nature played an important role. While the results of the examinations of H1 and H2 appeared to be at odds at first, they were in agreement that the three green exercise levels as the dependent variable did not influence emotions and attention as indicated by MANOVAs and the three green exercise levels as the predictor variables were not significantly related to emotions and attention as indicated by multiple linear regressions. Therefore, engagement with nature may be more influential than the three green exercise levels. Alternatively, the types of physical activity used to operationalize the three green exercise levels may be too limited. For example, the first-level green exercise of viewing nature is not necessarily limited to sitting, standing still, or lying down. Additionally, the second-level green exercise of being in the presence of nearby nature can have various levels of physical activity, such as walking and jogging, which were found to have different effects on emotions and attention [84]. Future research on green exercise should further investigate the influence of physical activity and engagement with nature on people. The framework of green exercise proposed by Han and Wang [26], which includes the dimensions of the natural environment, physical activities, and engagement with nature, may serve as a guidance to further investigate the main effect of each dimension and the interactions of these three dimensions on people.
Surprisingly, physical activity related to transportation rather than physical activity in green exercise was associated with the reduction in negative emotions. The WHO [122] recommends at least 150 min of accumulated moderate-intensity physical activity each week, including leisure, occupation, transportation, household chores, play, games, sports, and planned exercise. Therefore, physical activity related to transportation possibly played a pertinent role in relieving the negative emotions of participants in this study because they were college students who had engaged in little occupation, household chores, play, games, sports, or planned exercise. The means (and SDs in parentheses) for job, transportation, housework, recreation, and sitting in the IPAQ were 13.61 (46.37), 21.38 (36.02), 17.44 (107.73), 24.73 (37.97), and 17.07 (8.59), respectively. The physical activity level of transportation was the second highest and that of recreation was the highest. Nevertheless, given that the reliability of the Chinese version of the IPAQ was less than ideal (Cronbach’s α = 0.574), the results in this study require replicating and may not be generalizable to other participant groups or regions.
Agreeableness refers to the degree to which a person is polite, trustworthy, friendly, and easy to communicate with [123]. A study found that agreeableness had a significant and negative correlation with burnout [124]. Another study found that agreeableness had a significant and negative correlation with fatigue but a significant and positive correlation with emotional stability [125]. Moreover, some researchers claimed that agreeable people have relatively high effortful control and are less likely to be influenced by mood-disordered outcomes [126]. However, because the abovementioned three studies were not focused on the relationships between agreeableness and emotions when performing green exercise, future studies may extend the focus from the effects of agreeableness in an interpersonal context to those of agreeableness in terms of the interaction between people and the environment.
The present study examined the effects on people engaged in three levels of green exercise in the same natural environment. The variance among the stressors in the environment, such as thermal comfort (PET: M = 22.096 °C, SD = 6.932; SET: M = 14.760, SD = 5.562), noise (M = 47.83 dBA, SD = 2.397), and degree of crowdedness (M = 48.053, SD = 20.746), may not be large enough to reveal their influence. Moreover, PET around 22 °C is regarded as slightly cool for the people in Taiwan [127] and 47 dBA is much lower than the standard (60 dBA) proposed by the Noise Control Act [128]. Different environments may have greater variances. Additionally, the air pollution was based on hourly data from an air quality station approximately eight km from the experimental site. Future studies should consider using on-site instruments to measure real-time air quality. Moreover, some scholar may argue that companions could confound the experimental treatment of the participants’ the degree of engagement with the natural environment. A previous study showed that both the interaction between the three-level green exercise and the presence of companions on engagement with nature and the main effect of the presence of companions on engagement with nature were not significant [26]. Although this study is the first to examine the effect of the number of companions on psychological health when engaging in green exercise without a dichotomy (i.e., the presence or absence of companions) [60,129,130], the variance in the number of companions was small (M = 0.611, SD = 0.96). Furthermore, the quality of such accompanying relationships should be considered in future studies, such as emotional support [39].

9. Conclusions

Engaging in 15 min of green exercise of any level improved participants’ emotions and attention. Therefore, people can easily choose viewing nature, being in the presence of nearby nature, or active participation and involvement with nature according to their convenience or physical ability in everyday life to enhance their psychological health. Policymakers, government officials, environmental planners and designers, and the general public should not only prevent the existing natural environments from destruction but also create more natural settings to allow people having more opportunities of viewing nature, being nearby nature, or active involvement with nature. Nonetheless, it should be noted that plants are generally regarded as a representation of nature [131,132]. Having plants nearby indoors or outdoors is certainly feasible. Plants can provide people many sensory stimulations, such as visual, olfactory, tactile, taste, and even auditory.
Moreover, the degree of engagement with nature played a pivotal role in green exercise. Engagement with nature is likely the crucial element of green exercise, which integrates people’s physical activities and environments into an ongoing positive relationship [117]. Studies on green exercise should consider the framework of green exercise proposed by Han and Wang [26] to further examine the effects of natural environment, physical activities, and engagement with nature on people. Moreover, a comprehensive view of the social ecology should be adopted to explore physical activity, which is an interaction among the physical environment, social environment, and participants’ personality traits [36,37], which may prove both useful and productive for green exercise to meet the increasingly serious challenge of physical and psychological health of the modern society [2].

Funding

This study was funded by the Ministry of Science and Technology in Taiwan (MOST 104-2410-H-167-009).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The author thanks Y.-T. Chang and H.-L. Chu for data collection, organization, and analyses.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares that there is no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Descriptive statistics and distribution of the collected data.
Table A1. Descriptive statistics and distribution of the collected data.
VariableNumberMin.Max.MeanS.D.SkewnessKurtosis
Physiologically Equivalent TemperatureOverall955.335.922.0966.932−0.117−0.34
Green
exercise
Level 13310.734.823.2796.1370.208−0.594
Level 2315.335.419.9587.640.162−0.514
Level 331835.922.9746.706−0.4060.342
Standard Effective TemperatureOverall952.826.214.765.565−0.092−0.78
Green
exercise
Level 1338.126.216.2584.7740.321−0.622
Level 2312.822.612.5976.2610.161−1.267
Level 3316.323.915.3295.09−0.107−0.862
Noise
(dB)
Overall9544.1356.3347.832.3970.80.595
Green
exercise
Level 13344.5356.3347.4452.6021.5093.037
Level 23144.452.448.211.9980.618−0.207
Level 33144.1352.6547.862.5440.373−0.878
Crowdedness
–People
Overall9522412114.73782.6041.6272.17
Green
exercise
Level 1332241293.63674.8332.8269.869
Level 23142344128.77477.691.3251.2
Level 33130346123.16192.7581.2840.454
Crowdedness–AutomobileOverall951210138.78917.3671.5563.283
Green
exercise
Level 133139440.12117.921.1151.53
Level 231148737.12915.0221.313.231
Level 3311210139.03219.2882.0715.193
Crowdedness–MotorcycleOverall950275.2955.3451.482.356
Green
exercise
Level 1330113.8483.3180.562−0.714
Level 2310275.2266.3441.9283.945
Level 3310186.9035.7060.653−0.995
Crowdedness
−Bicycle
Overall950223.9683.8021.584.354
Green
exercise
Level 1330114.033.2060.564−0.531
Level 2310144.2583.8980.781−0.094
Level 3310223.6134.3492.72210.096
Crowdedness
Totalvehicle
Overall951313748.05320.7641.8524.971
Green
exercise
Level 133221134819.3491.4373.098
Level 2312210346.61318.7591.563.445
Level 3311313749.54824.3972.1876.321
SO2
(ppb)
Overall950.18.93.1331.9020.6260.051
Green
exercise
Level 1330.26.62.7741.7280.516−0.259
Level 2310.27.93.2451.9310.590.021
Level 3310.18.93.3872.0430.6710.21
CO
(ppm)
Overall950.31.50.6710.291.3350.952
Green
exercise
Level 1330.331.40.6460.2561.5952.415
Level 2310.31.50.6580.2881.3221.379
Level 3310.41.470.7070.3271.1930.165
NOx
(ppb)
Overall95139331.49516.8921.6272.331
Green
exercise
Level 133137327.74214.2761.5942.496
Level 231149332.20719.5311.9083.553
Level 331187134.58116.4721.2820.272
NO
(ppb)
Overall951.8618.61511.1382.9369.023
Green
exercise
Level 1331.8486.9169.3783.58413.503
Level 23126110.05514.0412.9798.642
Level 3312.1358.9689.7521.7611.761
NO2
(ppb)
Overall95114522.8578.2980.686−0.223
Green
exercise
Level 133114520.8068.2481.0420.837
Level 231114022.1727.8240.442−0.654
Level 331144525.5488.3140.718−0.559
PM10
(μg/m3)
Overall951.716554.14838.251.1811.134
Green
exercise
Level 1331616562.27644.3140.88−0.002
Level 231511846.03625.4990.461.116
Level 3311.715953.85241.431.311.185
PM2.5
(μg/m3)
Overall9528029.04718.440.9420.126
Green
exercise
Level 13327230.62120.7540.763−0.638
Level 23135825.75914.1290.5650.014
Level 33178030.82119.981.0630.234
O3
(ppb)
Overall952.25225.94712.955−0.006−0.699
Green
exercise
Level 1332.44928.64815.058−0.467−1.105
Level 2312.25025.15910.997−0.0980.211
Level 3312.25223.98412.3180.5030.179
Movement Speed
(m/s)
Overall950.0021.4460.2740.2251.9077.203
Green
exercise
Level 1330.0020.1830.0680.0371.2832.817
Level 2310.0941.4460.4070.2652.3757.61
Level 3310.1740.7060.3620.1190.8741.137
Exercise FrequencyOverall95071.3051.3371.4643.07
Green
exercise
Level 133041.1821.1850.9470.193
Level 231051.1291.2041.4522.78
Level 331071.6131.5851.5513.438
Total Mood Disturbance
−Pretest
Overall9569152100.22117.8870.389−0.237
Green
exercise
Level 1337013199.75815.3790.026−0.473
Level 2316914399.96819.3050.479−0.345
Level 33173152100.96819.3860.484−0.102
Total Mood Disturbance
−Posttest
Overall956013787.48414.9230.6040.176
Green
exercise
Level 1336710788.12111.578−0.142−1.05
Level 2316613788.03217.6780.6780.195
Level 3316012486.25815.4920.8530.166
The Big Five
–Extraversion
Overall95163725.7054.1990.321−0.169
Green
exercise
Level 133203726.6364.0840.461−0.255
Level 231163324.6454.071−0.0140.036
Level 331193525.7744.3340.55−0.437
The Big Five
–Agreeableness
Overall95264231.6213.3040.4580.224
Green
exercise
Level 133264231.2733.8590.8420.585
Level 231273630.7742.9180.273−1.389
Level 331274132.8392.7210.3091.816
The Big Five
–Conscientiousness
Overall95203427.6843.068−0.119−0.343
Green
exercise
Level 133203427.4243.527−0.189−0.467
Level 231223327.8392.8410.007−0.616
Level 331223427.8062.8330.06−0.14
The Big Five
–Neuroticism
Overall95172924.2212.799−0.363−0.314
Green
exercise
Level 133182923.9392.957−0.338−0.467
Level 231182924.8062.272−0.7111.481
Level 331172923.9353.087−0.037−0.728
The Big Five
–Openness
Overall95234633.9474.2340.0550.162
Green
exercise
Level 133254133.6064.286−0.231−0.79
Level 231274433.5814.0890.305−0.119
Level 331234634.6774.3620.1051.692
CompanionOverall95050.6110.962.4077.82
Green
exercise
Level 133050.6971.2622.6027.043
Level 231030.5810.8071.3471.351
Level 331020.5480.7230.952−0.378
Spatial Span Forward
–Pretest
Overall955138.5891.653−0.197−0.064
Green
exercise
Level 1335138.5451.872−0.2280.172
Level 2316128.7741.586−0.246−0.51
Level 3316128.4521.502−0.09−0.018
Spatial Span Forward
–Posttest
Overall954149.3681.863−0.0610.513
Green
exercise
Level 1334128.8181.878−0.5030.533
Level 2315149.3231.8690.1180.85
Level 331813101.6930.573−0.937
Digit Span Backward
–Pretest
Overall952148.6322.8320.143−0.757
Green
exercise
Level 1332148.7273.43−0.089−1.193
Level 2314138.3552.2880.191−0.743
Level 3314148.8062.6880.462−0.516
Digit Span Backward
–Posttest
Overall953149.1163.007−0.322−0.755
Green
exercise
Level 1333148.973.432−0.447−1.022
Level 2313148.8062.96−0.046−0.747
Level 3314149.5812.579−0.211−0.561
Engagement
with Nature
Overall9584931.6637.506−0.3930.569
Green
exercise
Level 133194931.6066.4510.5760.779
Level 23184228.9678.611−0.542−0.03
Level 331204734.4196.530−0.3380.275
International Physical Activity QuestionnaireOverall952109394.224132.4795.33835.968
Green
exercise
Level 13321.97268.575.37754.4121.7944.923
Level 23121093112.5197.3864.41521.589
Level 33113.5584.496.011111.6793.08811.978
Profile of Mood States(Vitality)–PretestOverall9502211.8956.356−0.229−0.837
Green
exercise
Level 13312212.4246.892−0.197−1.207
Level 23102211.9355.680−0.4340.191
Level 33102211.2906.553−0.181−0.992
Profile of Mood States(Vitality)–PosttestOverall9502412.8006.657−0.378−0.891
Green
exercise
Level 13302213.5156.433−0.516−0.838
Level 23102312.1617.221−0.182−1.065
Level 33102412.6776.447−0.478−0.580
Profile of Mood States(Self-esteem)–PretestOverall950168.4743.590−0.5620.013
Green
exercise
Level 1330167.7884.106−0.308−0.523
Level 2310148.7743.364−0.6790.283
Level 3310158.9033.208−0.6641.056
Profile of Mood States(Self-esteem)–PosttestOverall950168.6004.106−0.347−0.635
Green
exercise
Level 13301584.402−0.435−0.985
Level 2310168.7424.313−0.278−0.571
Level 3312169.0973.590−0.126−0.559
Profile of Mood States(Confusion)–PretestOverall950237.1685.6350.586−0.517
Green
exercise
Level 1330187.6975.3820.351−1.120
Level 2310237.2906.5480.693−0.527
Level 3310196.4844.9920.615−0.007
Profile of Mood States(Confusion)–PosttestOverall950143.1893.8101.1390.384
Green
exercise
Level 1330124.2123.6210.516−0.619
Level 2310142.8064.1591.6501.910
Level 3310122.4843.5201.4751.174
Profile of Mood States(Fatigue)–PretestOverall950236.9586.0790.790−0.132
Green
exercise
Level 13301464.0540.096−0.852
Level 2310176.1615.9730.648−0.961
Level 3310238.7747.5840.556−1.012
Profile of Mood States(Fatigue)–PosttestOverall950223.2003.9452.0895.991
Green
exercise
Level 1330122.7883.0591.1761.158
Level 2310163.0973.5151.8294.830
Level 3310223.7425.0922.1004.989
Profile of Mood States(Anger)–PretestOverall950132.0533.1601.6371.966
Green
exercise
Level 1330101.8182.7891.7912.443
Level 2310122.4523.6771.3170.508
Level 3310131.9033.0371.9844.665
Profile of Mood States(Anger)–PosttestOverall950120.6531.7974.05519.368
Green
exercise
Level 133070.6061.3913.44114.028
Level 2310120.8712.4873.71714.558
Level 331050.4841.3382.9357.760
Profile of Mood States(Tension)–PretestOverall950123.2323.1400.9920.232
Green
exercise
Level 1330113.2733.0340.802−0.198
Level 2310123.3553.4891.1770.756
Level 3310103.0652.9770.962−0.044
Profile of Mood States(Tension)–PosttestOverall95081.3792.1051.4921.127
Green
exercise
Level 133071.6672.2451.098−0.219
Level 231081.5162.2641.5531.510
Level 331070.9351.7502.1404.355
Profile of Mood States(Depression)–PretestOverall950101.1792.1142.2184.875
Green
exercise
Level 1330101.1822.3382.4065.915
Level 231071.4192.1571.4821.201
Level 331090.9351.8433.17612.113
Profile of Mood States(Depression)–PosttestOverall95070.4631.2363.33211.737
Green
exercise
Level 133050.3640.9943.65215.111
Level 231070.6451.5392.9899.720
Level 331050.3871.1453.42711.445
Intelligent Device for Energy Expenditure and Activity
All posture speed
Overall620.202198.775.54367.4960.276−1.338
Green
exercise
Level 2310.202198.772.05465.9500.358−1.285
Level 3310.206194.579.03369.9190.205−1.404
Intelligent Device for Energy Expenditure and Activity
Numbers for ascending and descending stairs
Overall62056682.67790.2442.73212.392
Green
exercise
Level 231324574.93566.5460.9880.305
Level 331056690.419109.5762.84611.384
Table A2. Statistics of MANOVA.
Table A2. Statistics of MANOVA.
Multivariate Tests
EffectValueFHypothesis dfError dfSig.Partial Eta SquaredNoncent.
Parameter
Observed Power
Between-SubjectsInterceptPillai’s Trace0.983506.8909.00081.0000.0000.9834562.0091.000
Wilks’ Lambda (λ)0.017506.8909.00081.0000.0000.9834562.0091.000
Hotelling’s Trace56.321506.8909.00081.0000.0000.9834562.0091.000
Roy’s Largest Root56.321506.8909.00081.0000.0000.9834562.0091.000
TreatmentPillai’s Trace0.2481.28918.000164.0000.2010.12423.1941.000
Wilks’ Lambda (λ)0.7671.27918.000162.0000.2080.12423.0201.000
Hotelling’s Trace0.2861.26918.000160.0000.2150.12522.8431.000
Roy’s Largest Root0.1821.6579.00082.0000.1130.15414.9151.000
Within-
Subjects
TimePillai’s Trace0.61914.6019.00081.0000.0000.619131.4111.000
Wilks’ Lambda (λ)0.38114.6019.00081.0000.0000.619131.4111.000
Hotelling’s Trace1.62214.6019.00081.0000.0000.619131.4111.000
Roy’s Largest Root1.62214.6019.00081.0000.0000.619131.4111.000
Time * TreatmentPillai’s Trace0.2041.03518.000164.0000.4240.10218.6230.711
Wilks’ Lambda (λ)0.8021.05118.000162.0000.4060.10518.9270.720
Hotelling’s Trace0.2401.06818.000160.0000.3890.10719.2190.728
Roy’s Largest Root0.2061.8769.00082.0000.0670.17116.8830.788
ANOVA
SourceMeasureType III Sum of SquaresdfMean
Square
FSig.Partial Eta SquaredNoncent.
Parameter
Observed Power
TimeSpatial Span ForwardSphericity Assumed31.392131.39216.5210.0000.15716.5210.980
Greenhouse-Geisser31.392131.39216.5210.0000.15716.5210.980
Huynh-Feldt31.392131.39216.5210.0000.15716.5210.980
Lower-bound31.392131.39216.5210.0000.15716.5210.980
Digit Span BackwardSphericity Assumed10.190110.1903.2740.0740.0353.2740.433
Greenhouse-Geisser10.190110.1903.2740.0740.0353.2740.433
Huynh-Feldt10.190110.1903.2740.0740.0353.2740.433
Lower-bound10.190110.1903.2740.0740.0353.2740.433
VitalitySphericity Assumed49.690149.6905.7040.0190.0605.7040.656
Greenhouse-Geisser49.690149.6905.7040.0190.0605.7040.656
Huynh-Feldt49.690149.6905.7040.0190.0605.7040.656
Lower-bound49.690149.6905.7040.0190.0605.7040.656
Self-esteemSphericity Assumed0.52110.5210.2020.6540.0020.2020.073
Greenhouse-Geisser0.52110.5210.2020.6540.0020.2020.073
Huynh-Feldt0.52110.5210.2020.6540.0020.2020.073
Lower-bound0.52110.5210.2020.6540.0020.2020.073
ConfusionSphericity Assumed775.5881775.58865.7390.0000.42565.7391.000
Greenhouse-Geisser775.5881775.58865.7390.0000.42565.7391.000
Huynh-Feldt775.5881775.58865.7390.0000.42565.7391.000
Lower-bound775.5881775.58865.7390.0000.42565.7391.000
FatigueSphericity Assumed720.8271720.82762.5620.0000.413625621.000
Greenhouse-Geisser720.8271720.82762.5620.0000.413625621.000
Huynh-Feldt720.8271720.82762.5620.0000.413625621.000
Lower-bound720.8271720.82762.5620.0000.413625621.000
AngerSphericity Assumed94.422194.42230.6150.0000.25630.6151.000
Greenhouse-Geisser94.422194.42230.6150.0000.25630.6151.000
Huynh-Feldt94.422194.42230.6150.0000.25630.6151.000
Lower-bound94.422194.42230.6150.0000.25630.6151.000
TensionSphericity Assumed169.1141169.11468.1000.0000.43368.1001.000
Greenhouse-Geisser169.1141169.11468.1000.0000.43368.1001.000
Huynh-Feldt169.1141169.11468.1000.0000.43368.1001.000
Lower-bound169.1141169.11468.1000.0000.43368.1001.000
DepressionSphericity Assumed23.737123.73716.6160.0000.15716.6160.981
Greenhouse-Geisser23.737123.73716.6160.0000.15716.6160.981
Huynh-Feldt23.737123.73716.6160.0000.15716.6160.981
Lower-bound23.737123.73716.6160.0000.15716.6160.981
Total Mood DisturbanceSphericity Assumed8111.14918111.14979.8640.0000.47379.8641.000
Greenhouse-Geisser8111.14918111.14979.8640.0000.47379.8641.000
Huynh-Feldt8111.14918111.14979.8640.0000.47379.8641.000
Lower-bound8111.14918111.14979.8640.0000.47379.8641.000
Time * TreatmentSpatial Span ForwardSphericity Assumed14.06227.0313.7000.0290.0777.4000.666
Greenhouse-Geisser14.06227.0313.7000.0290.0777.4000.666
Huynh-Feldt14.06227.0313.7000.0290.0777.4000.666
Lower-bound14.06227.0313.7000.0290.0777.4000.666
Digit Span BackwardSphericity Assumed1.97120.9860.3170.7290.0070.6330.099
Greenhouse-Geisser1.97120.9860.3170.7290.0070.6330.099
Huynh-Feldt1.97120.9860.3170.7290.0070.6330.099
Lower-bound1.97120.9860.3170.7290.0070.6330.099
VitalitySphericity Assumed12.58026.2900.7220.4890.0161.4440.169
Greenhouse-Geisser12.58026.2900.7220.4890.0161.4440.169
Huynh-Feldt12.58026.2900.7220.4890.0161.4440.169
Lower-bound12.58026.2900.7220.4890.0161.4440.169
Self-esteemSphericity Assumed0.43820.2190.0850.9190.0020.1700.063
Greenhouse-Geisser0.43820.2190.0850.9190.0020.1700.063
Huynh-Feldt0.43820.2190.0850.9190.0020.1700.063
Lower-bound0.43820.2190.0850.9190.0020.1700.063
ConfusionSphericity Assumed11.32225.6610.4800.6200.0110.9600.126
Greenhouse-Geisser11.32225.6610.4800.6200.0110.9600.126
Huynh-Feldt11.32225.6610.4800.6200.0110.9600.126
Lower-bound11.32225.6610.4800.6200.0110.9600.126
FatigueSphericity Assumed34.975217.4881.5180.2250.0333.0360.315
Greenhouse-Geisser34.975217.4881.5180.2250.0333.0360.315
Huynh-Feldt34.975217.4881.5180.2250.0333.0360.315
Lower-bound34.975217.4881.5180.2250.0333.0360.315
AngerSphericity Assumed1.45920.7290.2360.7900.0050.4730.086
Greenhouse-Geisser1.45920.7290.2360.7900.0050.4730.086
Huynh-Feldt1.45920.7290.2360.7900.0050.4730.086
Lower-bound1.45920.7290.2360.7900.0050.4730.086
TensionSphericity Assumed2.08621.0430.4200.6580.0090.840.116
Greenhouse-Geisser2.08621.0430.4200.6580.0090.840.116
Huynh-Feldt2.08621.0430.4200.6580.0090.840.116
Lower-bound2.08621.0430.4200.6580.0090.840.116
DepressionSphericity Assumed0.78020.3900.2730.7620.0060.5460.092
Greenhouse-Geisser0.78020.3900.2730.7620.0060.5460.092
Huynh-Feldt0.78020.3900.2730.7620.0060.5460.092
Lower-bound0.78020.3900.2730.7620.0060.5460.092
Total Mood DisturbanceSphericity Assumed97.650248.8250.4810.6200.0110.9610.126
Greenhouse-Geisser97.650248.8250.4810.6200.0110.9610.126
Huynh-Feldt97.650248.8250.4810.6200.0110.9610.126
Lower-bound97.650248.8250.4810.6200.0110.9610.126
Error (Time)Spatial Span ForwardSphericity Assumed169.118891.900
Greenhouse-Geisser169.118891.900
Huynh-Feldt169.118891.900
Lower-bound169.118891.900
Digit Span BackwardSphericity Assumed276.979893.112
Greenhouse-Geisser276.979893.112
Huynh-Feldt276.979893.112
Lower-bound276.979893.112
VitalitySphericity Assumed775.375898.712
Greenhouse-Geisser775.375898.712
Huynh-Feldt775.375898.712
Lower-bound775.375898.712
Self-esteemSphericity Assumed229.262892.576
Greenhouse-Geisser229.262892.576
Huynh-Feldt229.262892.576
Lower-bound229.262892.576
ConfusionSphericity Assumed1050.0138911.798
Greenhouse-Geisser1050.0138911.798
Huynh-Feldt1050.0138911.798
Lower-bound1050.0138911.798
FatigueSphericity Assumed1025.4408911.522
Greenhouse-Geisser1025.4408911.522
Huynh-Feldt1025.4408911.522
Lower-bound1025.4408911.522
AngerSphericity Assumed274.489893.084
Greenhouse-Geisser274.489893.084
Huynh-Feldt274.489893.084
Lower-bound274.489893.084
TensionSphericity Assumed221.015892.483
Greenhouse-Geisser221.015892.483
Huynh-Feldt221.015892.483
Lower-bound221.015892.483
DepressionSphericity Assumed127.144891.429
Greenhouse-Geisser127.144891.429
Huynh-Feldt127.144891.429
Lower-bound127.144891.429
Total Mood DisturbanceSphericity Assumed9038.97189101.561
Greenhouse-Geisser9038.97189101.561
Huynh-Feldt9038.97189101.561
Lower-bound9038.97189101.561
Tests of Within-Subjects Effects
SourceMeasureTimeType III Sum
of Squares
dfMean
Square
FSig.Partial Eta SquaredNoncent.
Parameter
Observed Power
TimeSpatial Span ForwardLinear31.392131.39216.5210.0000.15716.5210.980
Digit Span BackwardLinear10.190110.1903.2740.0740.0353.2740.433
VitalityLinear49.690149.6905.7040.0190.0605.7040.656
Self-esteemLinear0.52110.5210.2020.6540.0020.2020.073
ConfusionLinear775.5881775.58865.7390.0000.42565.7391.000
FatigueLinear720.8271720.82762.5620.0000.41362.5621.000
AngerLinear94.422194.42230.6150.0000.25630.6151.000
TensionLinear169.1141169.11468.1000.0000.43368.1001.000
DepressionLinear23.737123.73716.6160.0000.15716.6160.981
Total Mood DisturbanceLinear8111.14918111.14979.8640.0000.47379.8641.000
Time * TreatmentSpatial Span ForwardLinear14.06227.0313.7000.0290.0777.4000.666
Digit Span BackwardLinear1.97120.9860.3170.7290.0070.6330.099
VitalityLinear12.58026.2900.7220.4890.0161.4440.169
Self-esteemLinear0.43820.2190.0850.9190.0020.1700.063
ConfusionLinear11.32225.6610.4800.6200.0110.9600.126
FatigueLinear34.975217.4881.5180.2250.0333.0360.315
AngerLinear1.45920.7290.2360.7900.0050.4730.086
TensionLinear2.08621.0430.4200.6580.0090.8400.116
DepressionLinear0.78020.3900.2730.7620.0060.5460.092
Total Mood DisturbanceLinear97.650248.8250.4810.6200.0110.9610.126
Error (Time)Spatial Span ForwardLinear169.118891.900
Digit Span BackwardLinear276.979893.112
VitalityLinear775.375898.712
Self-esteemLinear229.262892.576
ConfusionLinear1050.0138911.798
FatigueLinear1025.4408911.522
AngerLinear274.489893.084
TensionLinear221.015892.483
DepressionLinear127.144891.429
Total Mood DisturbanceLinear9038.97189101.561
Tests of Between-Subjects Effects
SourceMeasureType III Sum
of Squares
dfMean
Square
FSig.Partial Eta SquaredNoncent.
Parameter
Observed Power
InterceptSpatial Span Forward14,926.764114,926.7643499.1200.0000.9753499.1201.000
Digit Span Backward14,572.208114,572.2081017.7950.0000.9201017.7951.000
Vitality27,848.781127,848.781363.5170.0000.803363.5171.000
Self-esteem13,540.176113,540.176490.2360.0000.846490.2361.000
Confusion4812.43514,812.435137.6900.0000.607137.6901.000
Fatigue4749.81814749.818115.3640.0000.565115.3641.000
Anger320.9321320.93230.8220.0000.25730.8221.000
Tension974.4591974.45979.4230.0000.47279.4231.000
Depression115.3681115.36824.4880.0000.21624.4880.998
Total Mood Disturbance1,613,889.30811,613,889.3083609.4920.0000.9763609.4921.000
TreatmentSpatial Span Forward7.57723.7880.8880.4150.0201.7760.199
Digit Span Backward11.81425.9070.4130.6630.0090.8250.115
Vitality10.39025.1950.0680.9340.0020.1360.060
Self-esteem60.238230.1191.0900.3400.0242.1810.236
Confusion79.139239.5701.1320.3270.0252.2640.244
Fatigue122.069261.0351.4820.2330.0322.9650.309
Anger9.31524.6580.4470.6410.0100.8950.121
Tension11.33325.6670.4620.6320.0100.9240.123
Depression5.32122.6610.5650.5710.0131.1290.141
Total Mood Disturbance61.876230.9380.0690.9330.0020.1380.060
ErrorSpatial Span Forward379.662894.266
Digit Span Backward1274.2518914.317
Vitality6818.2348976.609
Self-esteem2458.1528927.620
Confusion3110.6518934.951
Fatigue3664.3408941.172
Anger926.7008910.412
Tension1091.9658912.269
Depression419.303894.711
Total Mood Disturbance39,794.01089447.124
* This means the interaction of time and treatment.
Table A3. Statistics of multiple linear regression analysis on fatigue.
Table A3. Statistics of multiple linear regression analysis on fatigue.
Model Summary
ModelRR SquareAdjusted R
Square
Standard Error
of the Estimate
Change Statistics
R Square ChangeF Changedf1df2Sig. F Change
10.5800.3360.1454.55060.3361.76121730.040
ANOVA
ModelSum of SquaresdfMean SquareFSig.
1Regression765.7322136.4631.7610.040
Residual1511.7007320.708
Total2277.43294
Table A4. Statistics of multiple linear regression analysis on total mood disturbance.
Table A4. Statistics of multiple linear regression analysis on total mood disturbance.
Model Summary
ModelRR SquareAdjusted R
Square
Standard Error
of the Estimate
Change Statistics
R Square ChangeF Changedf1df2Sig. F Change
10.5810.3370.14713.40510.3371.76921730.039
ANOVA
ModelSum of SquaresdfMean SquareFSig.
1Regression6674.55821317.8361.7690.039
Residual13,117.86473179.697
Total19,792.42194

References

  1. World Health Organization. Physical Inactivity: A Global Public Health Problem. 2020. Available online: https://www.who.int/dietphysicalactivity/factsheet_inactivity/en/ (accessed on 20 July 2020).
  2. World Health Organization. World Health Report; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  3. Maas, J.; Verheij, R.A. Are health benefits of physical activity in natural environments used in primary care by general practitioners in The Netherlands? Urban For. Urban Green. 2007, 6, 227–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Pretty, J.; Peacock, J.; Sellengs, M.; Griffen, M. The mental and physical health outcomes of green exercise. Int. J. Environ. Health Res. 2005, 15, 319–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Dibben, G.O.; Dalal, H.M.; Taylor, R.; Doherty, P.; Tang, L.H.; Hillsdon, M. Cardiac rehabilitation and physical activity: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Heart 2018, 104, 1394–1402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Li, J.; Siegrist, J. Physical activity and risk of cardiovascular disease: A meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2012, 9, 391–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Cooney, G.M.; Dwan, K.; Greig, C.A.; Lawlor, D.A.; Rimer, J.; Waugh, F.R.; McMurdo, M.; Mead, G.E. Exercise for depression. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2013, 9, CD004366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Krogh, J.; Hjorthøj, C.; Speyer, H.; Gluud, C.; Nordentoft, M. Exercise for patients with major depression: A systematic review with meta-analysis and trial sequential analysis. BMJ Open 2017, 7, e014820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Harte, J.L.; Eifert, G.H. The effects of running, environment, and attentional focus on athletes’ catecholamine and cortisol levels and mood. Psychophysiology 1995, 32, 49–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Wooller, J.-J.; Barton, J.; Gladwell, V.F.; Micklewright, D. Occlusion of sight, sound and smell during Green Exercise influences mood, perceived exertion and heart rate. Int. J. Environ. Health Res. 2016, 26, 267–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kinnafick, F.-E.; Thogersen-Ntoumani, C. The effect of the physical environment and levels of activity on affective states. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 36, 241–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Ulrich, R.S. Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment. In Behavior and Natural Environments; Altman, I., Wohlwill, J.F., Eds.; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 85–125. [Google Scholar]
  13. Kaplan, R.; Kaplan, S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  14. Ulrich, R.S.; Parsons, R. Influences of passive experiences with plants on individual well-being and health. In Role of Horticulture in Human Well-Being and Social Development; Relt, P., Ed.; Timber Press: Oregon, Portland, 1992; pp. 93–105. [Google Scholar]
  15. Ulrich, R.S.; Simons, R.F.; Losito, B.D.; Fiorito, E.; Miles, M.A.; Zelson, M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 201–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Hartig, T.; Mang, M.; Evans, G.W. Restorative effects of natural environment experiences. Environ. Behav. 1991, 23, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hartig, T.; Korpela, K.; Evans, G.W.; Garling, T. A measure of restorative quality in environments. Scand. Hous. Plan. Res. 1997, 14, 175–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kaplan, S.; Bardwell, L.V.; Slakter, D.B. The museum as a restorative environment. Environ. Behav. 1993, 26, 725–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Han, K.-T. A review: Theories of restorative environments. J. Ther. Hortic. 2001, 12, 30–43. [Google Scholar]
  20. Rogerson, M.; Brown, D.K.; Sandercock, G.; Wooller, J.J.; Barton, J. A comparison of four typical green exercise environments and prediction of psychological health outcomes. Perspect. Public Health 2016, 136, 171–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Keniger, L.E.; Gaston, K.J.; Irvine, K.N.; Fuller, R. What are the benefits of interacting with nature? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10, 913–935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Caspersen, C.; Powell, K.; Christensen, G. Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: Definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health Rep. 1985, 100, 126–131. [Google Scholar]
  23. Han, K.-T. Restorative Nature: An Overview of the Positive Influences of Natural Landscapes on Humans; Lambert Academic Publishing: Saarbrücken, Germany, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  24. Bowler, D.E.; Buyung-Ali, L.M.; Knight, T.M.; Pullin, A.S. A systematic review of evidence for the added benefits to health of exposure to natural environments. BMC Public Health 2010, 10, 456–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Thompson Coon, J.T.; Boddy, K.; Stein, K.; Whear, R.; Barton, J.; Depledge, M.H. Does participating in physical activity in outdoor natural environments have a greater effect on physical and mental wellbeing than physical activity indoors? A systematic review. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2011, 45, 1761–1772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Han, K.-T.; Wang, P.-C. Empirical examinations of effects of three-level green exercise on engagement with nature and physical Activity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Plante, T.G.; Cage, C.; Clements, S.; Stover, A. Psychological benefits of exercise paired with virtual reality: Outdoor exercise energizes while indoor virtual exercise relaxes. Int. J. Stress Manag. 2006, 13, 108–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Park, B.J.; Furuya, K.; Kasetani, T.; Takayama, N.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Relationship between psychological responses and physical environment in forest settings. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2011, 102, 24–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Tyrväinen, L.; Ojala, A.; Korpela, K.; Lanki, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kagawa, T. The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Han, K.-T. Influence of passive versus active interaction with indoor plants on the restoration, behavior, and knowledge of students at a junior high school in Taiwan. Indoor Built Environ. 2018, 27, 818–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Lin, W.; Chen, Q.; Jiang, M.; Zhang, X.; Liu, Z.; Tao, J.; Wu, L.; Xu, S.; Kang, Y.; Zeng, Q. The effect of green space behaviour and per capita area in small urban green spaces on psychophysiological responses. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2019, 192, 103637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Marselle, M.R.; Irvine, K.N.; Warber, S.L. Walking for well-being: Are group walks in certain types of natural environments better for well-being than group walks in urban environments? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10, 5603–5628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Barton, J.; Griffin, M.; Pretty, J. Exercise-, nature-, and socially interactive-based initiatives improve mood and self-esteem in the clinical population. Perspect. Public Health 2012, 132, 89–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Marselle, M.R.; Irvine, K.N.; Lorenzo-Arribas, A.; Warber, S.L. Does perceived restorativeness mediate the effects of perceived biodiversity and perceived naturalness on emotional well-being following group walks in nature? J. Environ. Psychol. 2016, 46, 217–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Mackay, G.J.S.; Neill, J.T. The effect of “green exercise” on state anxiety and the role of exercise duration, intensity, and greenness: A quasi-experimental study. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2010, 11, 238–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sallis, J.F.; Owen, N. Ecological models. In Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice; Glanz, K., Lewis, K.M., Rimer, B.K., Eds.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1997; pp. 402–403. [Google Scholar]
  37. Stokols, D. Establishing and maintaining health environment. Am. Psychol. 1992, 47, 6–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Gifford, R. Environmental Psychology: Principles and Practice; Allyn & Bacon: Needham Heights, MA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  39. Halpern, D. Mental Health and the Built Environment: More than Bricks and Mortar? Taylor & Francis Ltd.: London, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  40. Stathopolos, T.; Wu, H.; Zacharias, J. Outdoor human comfort in an urban climate. Energy Build. 2004, 35, 297–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gómez Martín, M.B. Weather, climate and tourism: A geographical perspective. Ann. Tour. Res. 2005, 32, 571–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hipp, J.A.; Ogunseitan, O.A. Effect of environmental conditions on perceived psychological restorativeness of coastal parks. J. Environ. Psychol. 2011, 31, 421–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. White, M.P.; Cracknell, D.; Corcoran, A.; Jenkinson, G.; Depledge, M.H. Do preferences for waterscapes persist in inclement weather and extend to sub-aquatic scenes? Landsc. Res. 2014, 39, 339–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Fang, Y.-C.; Lin, Y.-J. Influences of microclimate on user’s thermal comfort in urban park. J. Landsc. 2010, 16, 77–99. [Google Scholar]
  45. Wu, Y.-J. Environmental Thermal Comfort and Visitors’ Adaptive Behavior in Urban Greenway-A Case Study of Jing-Guo Greenway in Taichung City. Master’s Thesis, University of Chung Hsing, Taichung, China, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  46. Ni, S.-C. The Influences of Thermal Comfort on Physiological Effect, Preference, and Attention Restoration. Unpublished Master’s Thesis, National Taiwan University, Taipei, China, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  47. Bronzaft, A.L. Noise pollution: A hazard to physical and mental well-being. In Handbook of Environmental Psychology; Bechtel, R.B., Churchman, A., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 499–510. [Google Scholar]
  48. Parsons, R.J. Environmental psychophysiology. In Handbook of Psychophysiology; Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L.G., Berntson, G.G., Eds.; Cambridge: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 752–788. [Google Scholar]
  49. Noise (II) The Third Public Nuisance of the Living Environment. Available online: http://www.ch.com.tw/index.aspx?sv=ch_fitness&chapter=AIA900602 (accessed on 17 February 2021).
  50. Calderón-Garcidueñas, L.; Calderón-Garcidueñas, A.; Torres-Jardón, R.; Avila-Ramírez, J.; Kulesza, R.J.; Angiulli, A.D. Air pollution and your brain: What do you need to know right now. Prim. Health Care Res. Dev. 2015, 16, 329–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Bullinger, M. Psychological effects of air pollution on healthy residents-a time-series approach. J. Environ. Psychol. 1989, 9, 103–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lenzholzer, S.; Koh, J. Immersed in microclimatic space: Microclimate experience and perception of spatial configurations in Dutch squares. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2010, 95, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Lenzholzer, S.; Nikolopoulou, M. Foreword to the special issue on subjective approaches to thermal perception. Int. J. Biometeorol. 2020, 64, 167–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Saenen, N.D.; Provost, E.B.; Viaene, M.K.; Vanpoucke, C.; Lefebvre, W.; Vrijens, K.; Roels, H.A.; Nawrot, T.S. Recent versus chronic exposure to particulate matter air pollution in association with neurobehavioral performance in a panel study of primary schoolchildren. Environ. Int. 2016, 95, 112–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Sunyer, J.; Suades-González, E.; García-Esteban, R.; Rivas, I.; Pujol, J.; Alvarez-Pedrerol, M.; Forns, J.; Querol, X.; Basagaña, X. Traffic-related Air Pollution and Attention in Primary School Children: Short-term Association. Epidemiology 2017, 28, 181–189. [Google Scholar]
  56. Chen, S.-Y. Relationship between Crowding and Outdoor Recreation Experience: A Social-psychological Approach. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, National Taiwan University, Taipei, China, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  57. Nordh, H.; Alalouch, C.; Hartig, T. Assessing restorative components of small urban parks using conjoint methodology. Urban For. Urban Green. 2011, 10, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Richardson, C.R.; Faulkner, G.; McDevitt, J.; Skrinar, G.S.; Hutchinson, D.S.; Piette, J.D. Integrating physical activity into mental health services for persons with serious mental illness. Psychiatr. Serv. 2005, 56, 324–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Wendel-Vos, W.; Droomers, M.; Kremers, S.; Brug, J.; van Lenthe, F. Potential environmental determinants of physical activity in adults: A systematic review. Obes. Rev. 2007, 8, 425–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Johansson, M.; Hartig, T.; Staats, H. Psychological benefits of walking: Moderation by company and outdoor environment. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 2011, 3, 261–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Maas, J.; van Dillen, S.M.E.; Verheij, R.A.; Groenewegen, P.P. Social contacts as a possible mechanism behind the relation between green space and health. Health Place 2009, 15, 586–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Zijlema, W.L.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Smith, G.; Cirach, M.; Martinez, D.; Dadvand, P.; Gascon, M.; Jones, M.; Gidlow, C.; Hurst, G.; et al. The relationship between natural outdoor environments and cognitive functioning and its mediators. Environ. Res. 2017, 155, 268–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Berg, M.M.V.D.; Van Poppel, M.; Van Kamp, I.; Ruijsbroek, A.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Gidlow, C.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J.; Gražulevičiene, R.; Van Mechelen, W.; Kruize, H.; et al. Do Physical Activity, Social Cohesion, and Loneliness Mediate the Association Between Time Spent Visiting Green Space and Mental Health? Environ. Behav. 2019, 51, 144–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Etnier, J.L.; Salazar, W.; Landers, D.M. The influence of physical fitness and exercise upon cognitive functioning: A meta-analysis. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1997, 9, 249–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Lambourne, K.; Tomporowski, P. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis. Brain Res. 2010, 1341, 12–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Barton, J.; Pretty, J. What is the best dose of nature and green exercise for improving mental health? A multi-study analysis. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2010, 44, 3947–3955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Hills, P.; Argyle, M. Musical and religious experiences and their relationship to happiness. Pers. Individ. Differ. 1998, 25, 91–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Kirkcaldy, B.D.; Furnham, A. Extraversion, neuroticism, psychoticism and recreational choice. Pers. Individ. Differ. 1991, 12, 737–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Paunonen, S.V. Big-five factors of personality and replicated predictions of behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 84, 411–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Huang, C.H. A Study of the Influences of Personaity, Fitness Motivation and Exercise Participation Behavior on the Quality of Sexual Life. Unpublished Master’s Thesis, National Cheng Kung University, Tainan, China, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  71. Rhodes, R.E.; Smith, N.E.I. Personality correlates of physical activity: A review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 2006, 40, 958–965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Wilson, K.E.; Dishman, R.K. Personality and physical activity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2015, 72, 230–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Sutin, A.R.; Stephan, Y.; Luchetti, M.; Artese, A.; Oshio, A.; Terracciano, A. The five-factor model of personality and physical inactivity: A meta-analysis of 16 samples. J. Res. Pers. 2016, 63, 22–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Markowitz, E.M.; Goldberg, L.R.; Ashton, M.C.; Lee, K. Profiling the “Pro-Environmental Individual”: A personality perspective. J. Pers. 2012, 80, 81–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. Nisbet, E.K.; Zelenski, J.M.; Murphy, S.A. The Nature Relatedness Scale: Linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environ. Behav. 2009, 41, 715–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Tam, K.-P. Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: Similarities and differences. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 34, 64–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Alizadeh, Z.; Feizi, A.; Rejali, M.; Afshar, H.; Keshteli, A.H.; Adibi, P. The predictive value of personality traits for psychological problems (stress, anxiety and depression): Results from a large population based study. J. Epidemiol. Glob. Health 2018, 8, 124–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Malouff, J.M.; Thorsteinsson, E.B.; Schutte, N.S. The relationship between the five factor model of personality and symptoms of clinic disorders: A meta-analysis. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2005, 27, 101–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Kotov, R.; Gamez, W.; Schmidt, F.; Watson, D. Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2010, 136, 768–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Carver, C.S.; Scheier, M.F. Dispositional optimism. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 293–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Milligan, M. Optimism and the Five-Factor Model of Personality, Coping, and Health Behavior. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  82. Engin Deniz, M.; Satici, A.S. The relationships between Big Five personality traits and subjective vitality. An. De Psicol. 2017, 33, 218–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Ryan, R.M.; Frederick, C. On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. J. Pers. 1997, 65, 529–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Han, K.-T. The effect of nature and physical activity on emotions and attention while engaging in green exercise. Urban For. Urban Green. 2017, 24, 5–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. Hunting and scavenging by early humans: The state of the debate. J. World Prehist. 2002, 16, 1–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Kansky, J. What’s love got to do with it?: Romantic relationships and well-being. In Handbook of Well-Being; Diener, E., Oishi, S., Tay, L., Eds.; DEF Publishers: Salt Lake City, UT, USA, 2018; pp. 1–24. [Google Scholar]
  87. Calkins, M.; Szmerekovsky, J.G.; Biddle, S. Effect of Increased Time Spent Outdoors on Individuals with Dementia Residing in Nursing Homes. J. Hous. Elder. 2007, 21, 211–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Ancoli-Israel, S.; Cole, R.; Alessi, C.; Chambers, M.; Moorcroft, W.; Pollak, C. The role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep 2003, 26, 342–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Stone, K.L.; Ancoli-Israel, S. Actigraphy. In Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine; Kryger, M., Ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  90. Zhang, K.; Werner, P.; Sun, M.; Pi-Sunyer, F.X.; Boozer, C. Measurement of human daily physical activity. Obes. Res. 2003, 11, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  91. Maffiuletti, N.A.; Gorelick, M.; Kramers-de Quervain, I.; Bizzini, M.; Munzinger, J.P.; Tomasetti, S.; Stacoff, A. Concurrent validity and intrasession reliability of the IDEEA accelerometry system for the quantification of spatiotemporal gait parameters. Gait Posture 2008, 27, 160–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Zhang, K.; Pi-Sunyer, F.X.; Boozer, C.N. Improving energy expenditure estimation for physical activity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2004, 36, 883–889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Costa, P.T., Jr.; McCrae, R.R. Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychol. Assess. 1992, 4, 5–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Huang, Y.-C. The Relationship between Personality, Sales Skill, Selling-customer Orientation and Sales Performance of Part-time Salespeople. Unpublished Master’s Thesis, National Sun Yat-sen University, Kaohsiung, China, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  95. John, O.P.; Donahue, E.M.; Kentle, R.L. The Big Five Inventory--Versions 4a and 54; Institute of Personality and Social Research: Berkeley, CA, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  96. Manning, R.E.; Lime, D.W.; Friemund, W.A. Use of visual research methods to measure standards of quality for parks and outdoor recreation. J. Leis. Res. 2004, 36, 552–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Brager, G.S.; Fountain, M.E.; Benton, C.C.; Arens, E.A.; Bauman, F.S. A comparison of methods for assessing thermal sensation and acceptability in the field. In Proceeding of Thermal Comfort: Past, Present and Future; Oseland, N.A., Ed.; British Research Establishment: Garston, Hertfordshire, UK, 1993; pp. 17–39. [Google Scholar]
  98. Wu, M.-L.; Chang, Y.-R. SPSS and Statistical Analysis Application; Wunan Press: Taipei, China, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  99. Matzarakis, A.; Rutz, F.; Mayer, H. Modelling radiation fluxes in simple and complex environments—Application of the RayMan model. Int. J. Biometeorol. 2007, 51, 323–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Matzarakis, A.; Rutz, F.; Mayer, H. Modelling radiation fluxes in simple and complex environments: Basics of the RayMan model. Int. J. Biometeorol. 2010, 54, 131–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Air Pollution Control Act. Available online: https://law.moj.gov.tw/LawClass/LawAll.aspx?pcode=o0020001 (accessed on 17 February 2021).
  102. Liou, Y.-M. Development and Verification of Validity and Reliability of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire Taiwan Version. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, National Taiwan University, Taipei, China, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  103. Liou, Y.-M.; Wu, J.-S. The appraisal and questionnaire of physical activity. Taiwan. Assoc. Diabetes Educ. Newsl. Sept. 2009, 11–17. [Google Scholar]
  104. Simon, H.A. Comments. In Affect and Cognition; Clark, M.S., Fiske, S.T., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1982; pp. 333–342. [Google Scholar]
  105. Ekman, P. Expression and the Nature of Emotion. In Approaches to Emotion; Scherer, K., Ekman, P., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1984; pp. 319–343. [Google Scholar]
  106. Chang, Y.-C.; Lu, J.-H. The revised report of Profile of Mood State (POMS). Sports Exerc. Res. 2001, 3, 47–55. [Google Scholar]
  107. McNair, D.M.; Lorr, M.; Droppleman, L.F. Revised Manual for the Profile of Mood States; Educational and Industrial Testing Service: San Diego, CA, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  108. Hsu, P.-Y.; Chang, Y.-C.; Lu, J.-H. The re-revised Profile of Mood States (PMOS). Sports Exerc. Res. 2003, 5, 85–95. [Google Scholar]
  109. Heuchert, J.P.; McNair, D.M. Profile of Mood States 2nd Edition-Youth. Poms 2-Y Assess. Rep. Derrick Smith 2012, 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  110. Anderson, J.R. Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications, 6th ed.; Worth Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  111. Lezak, M.D. Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  112. Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1955. [Google Scholar]
  113. Gupta, S.K. Intention-to-treat concept: A review. Perspect. Clin. Res. 2011, 2, 109–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Chiou, H.-J. Quantitative Research and Statistical Analysis in Social and Behavioral Sciences; Wunan: Taipei, China, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  115. DeVellis, R.F. Scale Development Theory and Applications; Sage: London, UK, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  116. Carrus, G.; Scopelliti, M.; Lafortezza, R.; Colangelo, G.; Ferrini, F.; Salbitano, F.; Agrimi, M.; Portoghesi, L.; Semenzato, P.; Sanesi, G. Go greener, feel better? The positive effects of biodiversity on the well-being of individuals visiting urban and peri-urban green areas. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015, 134, 221–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Bamberg, J.; Hitchings, R.; Latham, A. Enriching green exercise research. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2018, 178, 270–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Oberauer, K. Working memory and attention–a conceptual analysis and review. J. Cogn. 2019, 2, 36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  119. Partington, J.; Leiter, J.E.; Graydon, R. Partington’s Pathways Test. Psychol. Serv. Cent. J. 1949, 1, 11–20. [Google Scholar]
  120. Fan, J.; McCandliss, B.D.; Sommer, T.; Raz, A.; Posner, M.I. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002, 14, 340–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Stevenson, M.P.; Schilhab, T.; Bentsen, P. Attention restoration theory II: A systematic review to clarify attention processes affected by exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health-Pt B Crit. Rev. 2018, 21, 227–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. World Health Organization. Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health. Available online: http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/44399/1/9789241599979_eng.pdf (accessed on 25 May 2018).
  123. Tok, S.; Morali, S.L. Trait emotional intelligence, the big five personality dimensions and academic success in physical education teacher candidates. Soc. Behav. Pers. 2009, 37, 921–932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Takemura, K.; Hasegawa, M.; Tamura, S.; Takishita, Y.; Matsuoka, T.; Iwawaki, Y.; Yamanaka, R. The association between burnout syndrome and personality in Japanese nursing students. Int. J. Recent Sci. Res. 2015, 6, 5545–5549. [Google Scholar]
  125. Kitamura, H.; Shindo, M.; Tachibana, A.; Honma, H.; Someya, T. Personality and resilience associated with perceived fatigue of local government employees responding to disasters. J. Occup. Health 2013, 55, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  126. Ode, S.; Robinson, M.D. Agreeableness and the self-regulation of negative affect: Findings involving the neuroticism/somatic distress relationship. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2007, 43, 2137–2148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  127. Lin, T.-P.; Matzarakis, A. Tourism climate and thermal comfort in Sun Moon Lake, Taiwan. Int. J. Biometeorol. 2008, 52, 281–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Noise Control Act. Available online: https://law.moj.gov.tw/LawClass/LawAll.aspx?pcode=o0030001 (accessed on 17 February 2021).
  129. Staats, H.; Hartig, T. Alone or with a friend: A social context for psychological restoration and environmental preferences. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 199–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Staats, H.; van Germerden, E.; Hartig, T. Preference for restorative situations: Interactive effects of attentional state, activity-in-environment and social context. Leis. Sci. 2010, 32, 401–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Bringslimark, T.; Hartig, T.; Patil, G.G. The psychological benefits of indoor plants: A critical review of the experimental literature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 422–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Smardon, R.C. Perception and aesthetics of the urban environment: Review of the role of vegetation. Landsc. Urban Plan. 1988, 15, 85–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Research structure.
Figure 1. Research structure.
Sustainability 13 02686 g001
Figure 2. Timeline of the experimental procedure.
Figure 2. Timeline of the experimental procedure.
Sustainability 13 02686 g002
Figure 3. Aerial photograph of the experimental site.
Figure 3. Aerial photograph of the experimental site.
Sustainability 13 02686 g003
Figure 4. Photographs of the experimental site at eye level, facing (a) east, (b) west, (c) south, and (d) north.
Figure 4. Photographs of the experimental site at eye level, facing (a) east, (b) west, (c) south, and (d) north.
Sustainability 13 02686 g004
Table 1. Research variables and measurements.
Table 1. Research variables and measurements.
Personality Traits(predictor variable)
Personality characteristics: the self-reported Big Five Test (questionnaire)
SocialEnvironment(predictor variable)
Number of companions: number of companions accompanying the participants in the green exercise (questionnaire)
Crowdedness: the number of other people and vehicles during the green exercise (photo records)
PhysicalEnvironment(predictor variable)
Thermal comfort (physiologically equivalent temperature, PET; standard effective temperature, SET): environmental conditions (weather instruments: temperature, humidity, wind speed, and average radiant temperature); and human conditions (questionnaire: metabolic heat and clothing quantity)
Noise: the mean value of environmental sound during green exercise (decibel meter)
Air pollution: SO2, CO, O3, PM10, PM2.5, NOX, NO, and NO2 (Air Quality Station)
Daily PhysicalActivity(predictor variable)
Physical activity: self-reported Chinese version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ), which assesses physical activity over the past 7 days (questionnaire)
Activity frequency: self-reported frequency of physical activity over the past 7 days (questionnaire)
ExperimentalTreatment (independent and predictor variable)
Engagement with nature: self-reported degree of engagement with nature (questionnaire)
Physical activity: body movement speed (global positioning system watch, GPS), hand activity (MicroMini-Motionlogger Actigraph), limb activity, posture, posture change, gait, and energy expenditure (Intelligent Device for Energy Expenditure and Activity, IDEEA)
Emotions and Attention (dependent variable)
Emotions: the self-reported Profile of Mood States (POMS) and total mood disturbance (TMD) (questionnaire)
Attention: spatial span forward (SSF) test and digit span backward (DSB) test
Table 2. Summarized results of the two-way mixed-design multivariate analysis of variance.
Table 2. Summarized results of the two-way mixed-design multivariate analysis of variance.
EffectVariableFPηp2Observed PowerMean (SD)Mean Diff.95% Conf. Int.
for the Diff.
Post hoc
Lower BoundUpper Bound
Treatment a
* Time
SSF
Interaction
3.7000.0290.0770.666
L10.5290.4720.0160.109Pre8.545 (1.872)Pre-Post−1.0370.491
Post8.181 (1.878)
L24.6860.038 *0.1350.554Pre8.774 (1.586)Pre-Post−1.066−0.031Post > Pre
Post9.323 (1.869)
L316.4340.000 ***0.3540.975Pre8.452 (1.502)Pre-Post−2.328−0.768Post > Pre
Post1.000 (1.693)
Time
(pre-post)
SSF16.5210.0000.1570.980Pre8.600 (0.176)Pre-Post−1.231−0.423Post > Pre
Post9.427 (0.190)
Vigour5.7040.0190.0600.656Pre11.792 (0.675)Pre-Post−1.905−0.175Post > Pre
Post12.832 (0.688)
Confusion65.7390.0000.4251.000Pre7.173 (0.597)Pre-Post3.1025.116Pre > Post
Post3.063 (0.390)
Fatigue62.5620.0000.4131.000Pre7.065 (0.633)Pre-Post2.9964.957Pre > Post
Post3.104 (0.415)
Anger3.6150.0000.2561.000Pre2.039 (0.334)Pre-Post0.9191.949Pre > Post
Post0.605 (0.188)
Anxiety68.1000.0000.4331.000Pre3.262 (0.334)Pre-Post1.4572.381Pre > Post
Post1.344 (0.221)
Depression16.6160.0000.1570.981Pre1.152 (0.224)Pre-Post0.3681.069Pre > Post
Post0.433 (0.129)
TMD79.8640.0000.4731.000Pre100.368 (1.900)Pre-Post10.33416.243Pre > Post
Post87.079 (1.537)
a Treatment L1 denotes Level 1 Green Exercise, L2 denotes Level 2 Green Exercise, and L3 denotes Level 3 Green Exercise. The significant results from the follow-up tests of the ANOVAs used a family-wise error rate of 0.05/10 = 0.005. *, *** in interaction denotes significance levels at 0.05, and 0.001, respectively.
Table 3. Results of multiple linear regressions (only significant relationships are shown).
Table 3. Results of multiple linear regressions (only significant relationships are shown).
PredictorDepend Variable: FatigueDepend Variable: TMD
B (SE)BetatpB(SE)Betatp
Constant17.439 (14.95) 1.1660.24759.820 (44.039) 1.3580.179
Transportation-related physical activity−0.046 (0.016)−0.337−2.960.004 **−0.107 (0.046)−0.265−2.3320.022 *
Engagement with nature−0.149 (0.070)−0.227−2.1260.037 *−0.617 (0.206)−0.319−2.9940.004 **
Agreeableness−0.348 (0.159)−0.234−2.1860.032 *
F 1.761 * 1.769 *
R2 0.336 0.337
∆R2 0.145 0.147
Power 0.981 0.982
*, ** in interaction denotes significance levels of 0.05 and 0.01°.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Han, K.-T. Effects of Three Levels of Green Exercise, Physical and Social Environments, Personality Traits, Physical Activity, and Engagement with Nature on Emotions and Attention. Sustainability 2021, 13, 2686. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13052686

AMA Style

Han K-T. Effects of Three Levels of Green Exercise, Physical and Social Environments, Personality Traits, Physical Activity, and Engagement with Nature on Emotions and Attention. Sustainability. 2021; 13(5):2686. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13052686

Chicago/Turabian Style

Han, Ke-Tsung. 2021. "Effects of Three Levels of Green Exercise, Physical and Social Environments, Personality Traits, Physical Activity, and Engagement with Nature on Emotions and Attention" Sustainability 13, no. 5: 2686. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13052686

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop