Environmental Concern, Income, and Nature Experience in India
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Background
2.1. An Indian EKC of Environmental Concern
2.2. Conceptual Framework
2.3. Environmentalism of the Poor as Nature Experience
3. Empirical Approach
3.1. Statistical Approach
+ γ11 × Nature connection * Income + γ12 × Nature connection * Group
+ δ11 × Nature connect * Income * Group
II: Nature connection = β21 × Past farming + β22 × Income + β23 × Group
+ γ21 × Past farming * Income + γ22 × Past farming * Group
+ δ21 × Past farming * Income * Group
= (β21 + (β23 + γ22) × 1 + (β22 + γ21 + δ21 * 1 ) * Income) * (β11 + (β23 + γ22) × 1
+ (β12 + γ11 + δ11 * 1) * Income)
3.2. Measurement
3.3. Data
4. Results
4.1. Graphical and Descriptive Analysis
4.2. Path Model
4.3. Robustness
5. Discussion and Conclusion
5.1. General implications for income-concern theory
5.2. Details of Nature Experience’s Influence
5.3. Pollution behavior and bridging policies
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Reliability (omega-t): 0.55 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Full Sample | Farmers | Switchers | Non-Farmers | |
eco-crisis | 0.79 | 0.80 | 0.71 | 0.71 |
balance of nature | 0.82 | 0.84 | 0.79 | 0.76 |
limits to growth | 0.30 | 0.24 | 0.33 | 0.68 |
anti-anthropocentrism | 0.34 | 0.37 | 0.40 |
Appendix B
Appendix B. Structural Equation Regressions including Robustness | Raw Data, Latent Variable Model, Reduced Dependent, N = 1203 | Error Imputation, Latent Variable Model, N = 1210 | Raw Data, Parceled Dependent, N = 1203, Population Weights | Raw Data, Latent Variable Model, N = 1106, Group = Switchers (vs. Farmers) | Raw Data, Latent Variable Model, N = 450, Group = Switchers (vs. Non-Farmers) | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Path 2: environmental concern (NEP) ~ | ||||||||||||||||
past farming | 0.066 | (0.027) | * | 0.029 | (0.008) | *** | 0.084 | (0.044) | . | 0.082 | (0.035) | * | ||||
group | 0.125 | (0.038) | ** | 0.013 | (0.009) | 0.196 | (0.058) | ** | 0.084 | (0.025) | ** | −0.029 | (0.041) | |||
nature connection * group | −0.136 | (0.040) | ** | −0.026 | (0.009) | ** | −0.094 | (0.053) | . | −0.317 | (0.072) | *** | −0.266 | (0.064) | *** | |
nature connection | 0.270 | (0.037) | *** | 0.031 | (0.009) | ** | 0.246 | (0.055) | *** | 0.330 | (0.041) | *** | 0.299 | (0.053) | *** | |
nature connection * assets | −0.001 | (0.051) | −0.021 | (0.010) | * | 0.137 | (0.104) | |||||||||
nature connection * assets * group | −0.124 | (0.051) | * | 0.035 | (0.012) | ** | −0.168 | (0.072) | * | −0.173 | (0.079) | * | −0.142 | * | ||
assets * group | 0.027 | (0.041) | −0.008 | (0.010) | 0.005 | (0.051) | ||||||||||
religious nature experience | −0.009 | (0.037) | −0.003 | (0.009) | 0.062 | (0.043) | −0.037 | (0.041) | 0.076 | (0.059) | ||||||
recreational nature experience | 0.029 | (0.025) | 0.016 | (0.006) | * | 0.030 | (0.030) | 0.028 | (0.027) | −0.009 | (0.035) | |||||
aesthetic nature experience | 0.023 | (0.022) | 0.014 | (0.006) | * | −0.014 | (0.027) | 0.030 | (0.024) | 0.014 | (0.033) | |||||
educational nature experience | 0.019 | (0.031) | −0.010 | (0.011) | 0.016 | (0.042) | 0.016 | (0.033) | 0.075 | (0.046) | ||||||
abstract thinking | −0.014 | (0.025) | −0.008 | (0.006) | 0.008 | (0.043) | −0.010 | (0.027) | 0.044 | (0.039) | ||||||
time preference | −0.013 | (0.024) | −0.005 | (0.006) | 0.024 | (0.032) | −0.023 | (0.026) | 0.014 | (0.038) | ||||||
knowledge | −0.091 | (0.026) | *** | −0.042 | (0.010) | *** | −0.151 | (0.050) | ** | −0.105 | (0.029) | *** | −0.032 | (0.038) | ||
altruism | 0.058 | (0.022) | * | 0.006 | (0.005) | 0.063 | (0.029) | * | 0.060 | (0.025) | * | 0.054 | (0.033) | |||
extraversion (inverted) | −0.010 | (0.026) | 0.028 | (0.008) | *** | −0.027 | (0.038) | −0.004 | (0.029) | −0.008 | (0.037) | |||||
agreeableness | −0.055 | (0.030) | . | −0.014 | (0.007) | . | −0.110 | (0.060) | . | −0.053 | (0.033) | −0.050 | (0.044) | |||
conscientousness | 0.005 | (0.025) | 0.015 | (0.006) | * | −0.015 | (0.028) | 0.008 | (0.027) | 0.012 | (0.036) | |||||
openness | −0.091 | (0.024) | *** | 0.008 | (0.006) | −0.103 | (0.026) | *** | −0.104 | (0.026) | *** | −0.117 | (0.034) | ** | ||
neuroticism | 0.013 | (0.023) | 0.017 | (0.006) | ** | 0.041 | (0.033) | 0.009 | (0.025) | 0.048 | (0.033) | |||||
age | 0.039 | (0.029) | 0.006 | (0.007) | 0.046 | (0.036) | 0.044 | (0.031) | 0.026 | (0.043) | ||||||
sex | −0.090 | (0.027) | ** | −0.017 | (0.007) | * | −0.013 | (0.027) | −0.101 | (0.029) | ** | −0.016 | (0.037) | |||
caste (inverted) | 0.018 | (0.027) | −0.008 | (0.007) | 0.050 | (0.036) | 0.002 | (0.030) | −0.060 | (0.040) | ||||||
education | 0.131 | (0.049) | ** | 0.038 | (0.014) | ** | 0.071 | (0.067) | 0.136 | (0.053) | * | 0.079 | (0.078) | |||
assets | −0.030 | (0.044) | −0.007 | (0.010) | −0.023 | (0.069) | −0.012 | (0.047) | −0.091 | (0.123) | ||||||
village + religious FE | ON | ON | OFF | (only rel) | (only rel) | |||||||||||
Path 1: nature connection ~ | ||||||||||||||||
past farming | 0.002 | (0.021) | −0.047 | (0.025) | . | −0.001 | (0.035) | −0.009 | (0.026) | |||||||
group | −0.033 | (0.030) | −0.027 | (0.035) | −0.030 | (0.066) | −0.007 | (0.019) | ||||||||
past farming * group | 0.045 | (0.016) | ** | 0.092 | (0.019) | *** | 0.041 | (0.033) | 0.251 | (0.101) | * | |||||
religious nature experience * group | 0.064 | (0.035) | . | 0.155 | (0.042) | *** | 0.125 | (0.052) | * | −0.071 | (0.055) | |||||
past farming * assets | 0.028 | (0.028) | −0.011 | (0.034) | 0.027 | (0.042) | −0.002 | (0.026) | ||||||||
past farming * assets * group | −0.068 | (0.023) | ** | −0.045 | (0.028) | . | −0.053 | (0.029) | . | −0.055 | (0.028) | * | ||||
recreational nature experience | 0.086 | (0.019) | *** | 0.094 | (0.023) | *** | 0.079 | (0.035) | * | 0.095 | (0.020) | *** | ||||
Path 1.3: recreational nature experience ~ | ||||||||||||||||
religious nature experience | −0.057 | (0.044) | −0.016 | (0.043) | −0.037 | (0.082) | −0.044 | (0.046) | ||||||||
past farming | 0.296 | (0.029) | *** | 0.287 | (0.029) | *** | 0.287 | (0.051) | *** | 0.308 | (0.036) | *** | ||||
aesthetic nature experience | 0.030 | (0.026) | 0.073 | (0.025) | ** | −0.028 | (0.044) | 0.034 | (0.027) | |||||||
aesthetic nature experience * group | 0.062 | (0.025) | * | 0.026 | (0.025) | 0.083 | (0.045) | . | 0.018 | (0.044) | ||||||
~ | ||||||||||||||||
Path 1.2: aesthetic nature experience ~ | ||||||||||||||||
past farming | 0.247 | (0.032) | *** | 0.253 | (0.033) | *** | 0.312 | (0.077) | *** | 0.193 | (0.040) | *** | ||||
religious nature experience | 0.494 | (0.047) | ** | 0.390 | (0.049) | *** | 0.565 | (0.068) | *** | 0.519 | (0.050) | *** | ||||
religious nature experience * assets | −0.153 | (0.051) | ** | −0.095 | (0.051) | . | −0.115 | (0.095) | −0.157 | (0.054) | ** | |||||
Path 1.1: religious nature experience ~ | ||||||||||||||||
past farming | 0.151 | (0.019) | *** | 0.150 | (0.019) | *** | 0.066 | (0.057) | 0.196 | (0.023) | *** | |||||
~ | ~ | |||||||||||||||
group ~ | ||||||||||||||||
abstract thinking | −0.037 | (0.019) | * | −0.033 | (0.019) | . | −0.016 | (0.031) | 0.074 | (0.046) | ||||||
time preference | −0.062 | (0.018) | ** | −0.056 | (0.018) | ** | −0.118 | (0.030) | *** | −0.027 | (0.046) | |||||
agreeableness | −0.073 | (0.023) | ** | −0.083 | (0.022) | *** | −0.089 | (0.038) | * | 0.065 | (0.053) | |||||
knowledge | 0.050 | (0.019) | * | 0.050 | (0.019) | * | 0.027 | (0.032) | −0.093 | (0.046) | * | |||||
education | 0.068 | (0.027) | * | 0.069 | (0.027) | * | 0.183 | (0.044) | *** | 0.111 | (0.063) | . | ||||
nature connection * assets * group ~ | ||||||||||||||||
nature connection * assets | 0.640 | (0.024) | *** | 0.571 | (0.024) | *** | 0.922 | (0.050) | *** | |||||||
RMSEA | 0.073 | 0.069 | 0.092 | 0.204 | 0.141 | |||||||||||
SRMR | 0.043 | 0.045 | 0.037 | 0.042 | 0.054 |
References and Note
- Dinda, S. Environmental Kuznets Curve Hypothesis: A Survey. Ecol. Econ. 2004, 49, 431–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Murthy, K.B.V.; Bhasin, N. Environmental Kuznets Curve: CO2 emissions, pollution havens and type of economic development. In Emerging Dynamics of Sustainability in Multinational Enterprises; McIntyre, J.R., Ivanaj, S., Ivanaj, V., Narayan Kar, R., Eds.; Edward Elgar Publishing: Cheltenham, UK, 2016; pp. 209–231. [Google Scholar]
- Munasinghe, M. Is environmental degradation an inevitable consequence of economic growth: Tunneling through the environmental Kuznets curve. Ecol. Econ. 1999, 29, 89–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roca, J. Do individual preferences explain the Environmental Kuznets curve? Ecol. Econ. 2003, 45, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gifford, R.; Nilsson, A. Personal and social factors that influence pro-environmental concern and behaviour: A review. Int. J. Psychol. J. Int. Psychol. 2014, 49, 141–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fairbrother, M. Rich People, Poor People, and Environmental Concern: Evidence across Nations and Time. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 2013, 29, 910–922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guha, R.; Martinez-Alier, J. Varieties of Environmentalism: Essays North and South; Earthscan: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Martinez-Alier, J. The Environmentalism of the Poor; Edward Elgar Publishing: Cheltenham, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Dunlap, R.E.; Mertig, A.G. Global Concern for the Environment: Is Affluence a Prerequisite? J. Soc. Issues 1995, 51, 121–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koger, S. Psychological and Behavioral Aspects of Sustainability. Sustainability 2013, 5, 3006–3008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dwivedi, R. Environmental Movements in the Global South. Int. Sociol. 2016, 16, 11–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Widdop Quinton, H.; Khatun, F. Childhoodnature Alternatives: Adolescents in India, Nepal, and Bangladesh Explore Their Nature Connectedness. In Research Handbook on Childhoodnature: Assemblages of Childhood and Nature Research; Cutter-Mackenzie, A., Malone, K., Barratt Hacking, E., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2018; pp. 1–32. [Google Scholar]
- Nisbet, E.K.; Zelenski, J.M.; Murphy, S.A. The Nature Relatedness Scale. Environ. Behav. 2008, 41, 715–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diao, X.; McMillan, M.; Rodrik, D. The Recent Growth Boom in Developing Economies: A Structural Change Perspective. Natl. Bureau Econ. Res. 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hayes, A.F. An Index and Test of Linear Moderated Mediation. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2015, 50, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nair, J. The Promise of the Metropolis: Bangalore’s Twentieth Century; Oxford University Press: New Delhi, India, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Dunlap, R.E.; van Liere, K.D.; Mertig, A.G.; Jones, R.E. Measuring Endorsement of the New Ecological Paradigm: A Revised NEP Scale. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 425–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gambhir, S. Environmental Kuznets Curve: An Empirical Analysis in the Indian Context. PRAGATI J. Indian Econ. 2017, 4, 44–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pisano, I.; Lubell, M. Environmental Behavior in Cross-National Perspective. Environ. Behav. 2017, 49, 31–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Summers, N.; VanHeuvelen, T. Heterogeneity in the Relationship between Country-Level Affluence and Environmental Concern. Soc. Forces 2017, 96, 329–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bravo, G.; Marelli, B. Micro-foundations of the Environmental Kuznets Curve hypothesis: An empirical analysis. Int. J. Innov. Sustain. Dev. 2007, 2, 36–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duroy, Q.M.H. Testing the affluence hypothesis: A cross-cultural analysis of the determinants of environmental action. Soc. Sci. J. 2008, 45, 419–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lange, H.; Meier, L.; Anuradha, N.S. Highly Qualified Employees in Bangalore, India: Consumerist Predators? In The New Middle Classes; Lange, H., Meier, L., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2009; pp. 281–298. [Google Scholar]
- Mawdsley, E. India’s Middle Classes and the Environment. Dev. Chang. 2004, 35, 79–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Upadhya, C. India’s ‘new Middle Class’ and the Globalising City: Software Professionals in Bangalore, India. In The New Middle Classes; Lange, H., Meier, L., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2009; pp. 253–269. [Google Scholar]
- Chatterjee, D.P. Oriental Disadvantage versus Occidental Exuberance. Int. Sociol. 2008, 23, 5–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plassmann, F.; Khanna, N. Household Income and Pollution. J. Environ. Dev. 2016, 15, 22–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Franzen, A.; Meyer, R. Environmental Attitudes in Cross-National Perspective: A Multilevel Analysis of the ISSP 1993 and 2000. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 2010, 26, 219–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunlap, R.E.; York, R. The Globalization of Environmental Concern and The Limits of The Postmaterialist Values Explanation: Evidence from Four Multinational Surveys. Sociol. Q. 2008, 49, 529–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guha, R.; Gadgil, M. The Use and Abuse of Nature; Oxford University Press: New Delhi, India, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Pao, H.; Tsai, C. Multivariate Granger causality between CO2 emissions, energy consumption, FDI (foreign direct investment) and GDP (gross domestic product): Evidence from a panel of BRIC (Brazil, Russian Federation, India, and China) countries. Energy Policy 2011, 36, 685–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kanjilal, K.; Ghosh, S. Environmental Kuznet’s curve for India: Evidence from tests for cointegration with unknown structuralbreaks. Energy Policy 2013, 56, 509–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Angelsen, A.; Larsen, H.O.; Lund, J.F.; Smith-Hall, C.; Wunder, S. (Eds.) Measuring Livelihoods and Environmental Dependence: Methods for Research and Fieldwork; Routledge: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- World Bank. GDP per Capita, PPP (Current International $): India (1990–2016); World Bank: Washington, DC, USA, 2016; Available online: https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/NY.GDP.PCAP.PP.CD?end=2016&locations=IN&start=1990&view=chart (accessed on 16 October 2018).
- Chawla, L. Significant Life Experiences Revisited: A review of research on sources of environmental sensitivity. Environ. Educ. Res. 1998, 4, 369–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tam, K.-P. Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: Similarities and differences. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 34, 64–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, J.L.; Green, J.D.; Reed, A. Interdependence with the environment: Commitment, interconnectedness, and environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 173–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaiser, F.G.; Hartig, T.; Brügger, A.; Duvier, C. Environmental Protection and Nature as Distinct Attitudinal Objects. Environ. Behav. 2011, 45, 369–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schultz, P.W. Empathizing with Nature: The Effects ofPerspective Taking on Concern for Environmental Issues. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 391–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corral-Verdugo, V.; Carrus, G.; Bonnes, M.; Moser, G.; Sinha, J.B.P. Environmental Beliefs and Endorsement of Sustainable Development Principles in Water Conservation. Environ. Behav. 2008, 40, 703–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shrapnel, M.; Davie, J. The influence of personality in determining farmer responsiveness to risk. J. Agric. Educ. Ext. 2001, 7, 167–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shaver, P.R.; Brennan, K.A. Attachment Styles and the “Big Five” Personality Traits: Their Connections with Each Other and With Romantic Relationship Outcomes. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 18, 536–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scannel, L.; Gifford, R. Comparing the Theories of Interpersonal and Place Attachment. In Place Attachment: Advances in Theory, Methods and Applications; Manzo, L.C., Devine-Wright, P., Eds.; Taylor & Francis: Abingdon, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Mikulincer, M.; Shaver, P.R. The attachment behavioral system in adulthood: Activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 35, 53–152. [Google Scholar]
- Giusti, M.; Barthel, S.; Marcus, L. Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool Children in Stockholm. Child. Youth Environ. 2014, 24, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barr, R.F.; Mcconaghy, N. Conditioning in Relation to Conceptual Thinking. Br. J. Psychiatry 1972, 121, 299–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yaghoobi, A.; Mohammadzade, S.; Chegini, A.A.; Yarmohammadi Vasel, M.; Zoghi Paidar, M.R. The Relationship Between Attachment Styles, Self-Monitoring and Cybercrime in Social Network Users. Int. J. High Risk Behave. Addict. 2016, 5, e27785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haushofer, J.; Fehr, E. On the psychology of poverty. Science (N. Y.) 2014, 344, 862–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carvalho, L.S. Poverty and Time Preference; RAND Working Paper Series WR-759; RAND: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Andreoni, J. Impure Altruism and Donations to Public Goods: A Theory of Warm-Glow Giving. Econ. J. 1990, 100, 464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mani, A.; Mullainathan, S.; Shafir, E.; Zhao, J. Poverty Impedes Cognitive Function. Science 2013, 341, 976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dawson, J.F.; Richter, A.W. Probing three-way interactions in moderated multiple regression: Development and application of a slope difference test. J. Appl. Psychol. 2006, 91, 917–926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raubenheimer, J. An item selection procedure to maximise scale reliability and validity. SA J. Ind. Psychol. 2004, 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van de Vijver, F.; Tanzer, N.K. Bias and equivalence in cross-cultural assessment: An overview. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée/Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 2004, 54, 119–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hawcroft, L.J.; Milfont, T.L. The use (and abuse) of the new environmental paradigm scale over the last 30 years: A meta-analysis. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 143–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Khan, A.; Khan, M.N.; Adil, M. Exploring the New Ecological Paradigm (NEP) Scale in India: Item Analysis, Factor Structure and Refinement. Asia-Pac. J. Manag. Res. Innov. 2012, 8, 389–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schultz, P.W.; Gouveia, V.V.; Cameron, L.D.; Tankha, G.; Schmuck, P.; Franěk, M. Values and their Relationship to Environmental Concern and Conservation Behavior. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2016, 36, 457–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blackwell, M.; Honaker, J.; King, G. A Unified Approach to Measurement Error and Missing Data: Overview and Applications. Sociol. Methods Res. 2015, 46, 303–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bögeholz, S. Nature experience and its importance for environmental knowledge, values and action: Recent German empirical contributions. Environ. Educ. Res. 2006, 12, 65–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Witt, A.H. Pathways to Environmental Responsibility: A Qualitative Exploration of the Spiritual Dimension of Nature Experience. J. Stud. Relig. Nat. Cult. 2013, 7, 154–186. [Google Scholar]
- Pohl, D.; Schrenk, M. Naturerfahrungen und Naturzugänge von Kindern. In Bildung für Nachhaltige Entwicklung—Ergebnisse Empirischer Untersuchungen; Schrenk, M., Holl-Giese, W., Eds.; Dr. Kovac: Hamburg, Germany, 2005; pp. 33–46. [Google Scholar]
- Perkins, H.E. Measuring love and care for nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 455–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Indian Market Research Society of India (MRSI). Socio-Economic Classification 2011. The SEC System; MRSI: Mumbai, India, 2011; Available online: http://imrbint.com/research/The-New-SEC-system-3rdMarch2011.pdf (accessed on 16 October 2018).
- Howe, L.D.; Galobardes, B.; Matijasevich, A.; Gordon, D.; Johnston, D.; Onwujekwe, O.; Patel, R.; Webb, E.A.; Lawlor, D.A.; Hargreaves, J.R. Measuring socio-economic position for epidemiological studies in low- and middle-income countries: A methods of measurement in epidemiology paper. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2012, 41, 871–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gosling, S.D.; Rentfrow, P.J.; Swann, W.B. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. J. Res. Personality 2003, 37, 504–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Noftle, E.E.; Shaver, P.R. Attachment dimensions and the big five personality traits: Associations and comparative ability to predict relationship quality. J. Res. Personal. 2006, 40, 179–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCrae, R.R.; Kurtz, J.E.; Yamagata, S.; Terracciano, A. Internal consistency, retest reliability, and their implications for personality scale validity. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. Off. J. Soc. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 15, 28–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frederick, S. Time preference and personal identity. In Time and Decision: Economic and Psychological Perspectives on Intertemporal Choice; Loewenstein, G., Read, D., Baumeister, R.F., Eds.; Russell Sage Foundation: New York, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 89–113. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, R.O.; Ackermann, K.A.; Handgraaf, M. Measuring Social Value Orientation. SSRN J. 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Podsakoff, P.M.; MacKenzie, S.B.; Lee, J.-Y.; Podsakoff, N.P. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 879–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stratified, Random Sample of 1200 HH-heads. Survey Data: 2 Rural-urban Transects: Bangalore, India, 2016–2017; Our survey data were collected as part of a large project that is still under way. Project regulations require that access remains restricted to project members for further analysis during the duration of the project. Data are however available on an internal repository and will be made accessible to referees upon request alongside the computer code used for the analysis presented here.
- Kraas, F. Megacities; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Simon, D. Urban Environments: Issues on the Peri-Urban Fringe. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 2008, 33, 167–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffmann, E.; Jose, M.; Nölke, N.; Möckel, T. Construction and Use of a Simple Index of Urbanisation in the Rural–Urban Interface of Bangalore, India. Sustainability 2017, 9, 2146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Solon, G.; Haider, S.; Wooldridge, J. What Are We Weighting For? National Bureau of Economic Research: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Nijman, J. India’s Urban Future. Am. Behav. Sci. 2015, 59, 406–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sridhar, K.S.; Reddy, A.V.; Srinath, P. Is it Push or Pull? Recent Evidence from Migration into Bangalore, India. J. Int. Migr. Integr. 2013, 14, 287–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cross, S.E.; Hardin, E.E.; Gercek-Swing, B. The What, How, Why, and Where of Self-Construal. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 15, 142–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bratman, G.N.; Hamilton, J.P.; Hahn, K.S.; Daily, G.C.; Gross, J.J. Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual prefrontal cortex activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 8567–8572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morewedge, C.K. It Was a Most Unusual Time: How Memory Bias Engenders Nostalgic Preferences. J. Behav. Dec. Making 2013, 26, 319–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amel, E.; Manning, C.; Scott, B.; Koger, S. Beyond the roots of human inaction: Fostering collective effort toward ecosystem conservation. Science 2017, 356, 275–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Group Size (Weighted) | Farmers N = 754 (491) | Switchers N = 359 (567) | Nonfarmers N = 97 (152) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
New Environmental Paradigm | 0.738 | (0.163) | 0.775 | (0.133) | 0.796 | (0.145) |
Past farming | 0.822 | (0.340) | 0.911 | (0.207) | 0.000 | (0.000) |
Nature connection | 0.785 | (0.222) | 0.809 | (0.252) | 0.726 | (0.275) |
Religious nature experience | 0.565 | (0.383) | 0.619 | (0.363) | 0.643 | (0.399) |
Recreational nature experience | 0.636 | (0.450) | 0.706 | (0.435) | 0.419 | (0.475) |
Aesthetic nature experience | 0.561 | (0.433) | 0.639 | (0.416) | 0.317 | (0.436) |
Educational nature experience | 0.343 | (0.338) | 0.479 | (0.352) | 0.308 | (0.279) |
Knowledge | 0.297 | (0.458) | 0.219 | (0.416) | 0.262 | (0.450) |
Abstraction | 0.199 | (0.400) | 0.098 | (0.299) | 0.110 | (0.320) |
Agreeableness | 0.367 | (0.282) | 0.350 | (0.288) | 0.308 | (0.281) |
Intraversion | 0.624 | (0.379) | 0.634 | (0.367) | 0.664 | (0.388) |
Openness | 0.407 | (0.368) | 0.433 | (0.390) | 0.490 | (0.425) |
Conscientousness | 0.625 | (0.365) | 0.575 | (0.406) | 0.497 | (0.369) |
Neuroticism | 0.413 | (0.367) | 0.395 | (0.390) | 0.471 | (0.412) |
Social Value Orientation | 0.261 | (0.193) | 0.157 | (0.166) | 0.220 | (0.196) |
Time preference (impatient = 0) | 0.355 | (0.375) | 0.313 | (0.311) | 0.428 | (0.392) |
Age (0 = 03, 1 = 94) | 0.497 | (0.146) | 0.436 | (0.153) | 0.425 | (0.162) |
Gender (female = 0) | 0.767 | (0.424) | 0.566 | (0.498) | 0.598 | (0.502) |
Caste (highest = 0, lowest = 7 ) | 0.203 | (0.225) | 0.190 | (0.220) | 0.203 | (0.242) |
Education (years, highest = 12) | 0.127 | (0.105) | 0.189 | (0.125) | 0.166 | (0.113) |
Assets | 0.518 | (0.163) | 0.611 | (0.233) | 0.573 | (0.228) |
Path 2: Environmental Concern (NEP) ~ | Path 1: Nature Connection ~ | Path 1.3: Recreational Nature Experience ~ | Path 1.2: Aesthetic Nature Experience ~ | Path 1.1: Religious Nature Experience ~ | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
past farming | 0.066 | (0.027) | * | 0.002 | (0.021) | 0.296 | (0.029) | *** | 0.247 | (0.032) | *** | 0.151 | (0.019) | *** | |
group | 0.125 | (0.038) | ** | −0.033 | (0.030) | ||||||||||
nature connection * group (past farming for path 1) | −0.136 | (0.040) | ** | 0.045 | (0.016) | ** | |||||||||
nature connection | 0.270 | (0.037) | *** | ||||||||||||
nature connection * assets (past farming for path 1) | −0.001 | (0.051) | 0.028 | (0.028) | |||||||||||
nature connection * assets * group (past farming for path 1) | −0.124 | (0.051) | * | −0.068 | (0.023) | ** | |||||||||
assets * group | 0.027 | (0.041) | |||||||||||||
religious nature experience | −0.009 | (0.037) | −0.057 | (0.044) | 0.494 | (0.047) | ** | ||||||||
religious nature experience * group | 0.064 | (0.035) | . | ||||||||||||
religious nature experience * assets | −0.153 | (0.051) | ** | ||||||||||||
recreational nature experience | 0.029 | (0.025) | 0.086 | (0.019) | *** | ||||||||||
aesthetic nature experience | 0.023 | (0.022) | 0.030 | (0.026) | |||||||||||
aesthetic nature experience * group | 0.062 | (0.025) | * | ||||||||||||
abstract thinking | −0.014 | (0.025) | |||||||||||||
time preference | −0.013 | (0.024) | |||||||||||||
knowledge | −0.091 | (0.026) | *** | ||||||||||||
altruism | 0.058 | (0.022) | * |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bettin, J.; Wollni, M. Environmental Concern, Income, and Nature Experience in India. Sustainability 2019, 11, 346. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11020346
Bettin J, Wollni M. Environmental Concern, Income, and Nature Experience in India. Sustainability. 2019; 11(2):346. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11020346
Chicago/Turabian StyleBettin, Johannes, and Meike Wollni. 2019. "Environmental Concern, Income, and Nature Experience in India" Sustainability 11, no. 2: 346. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11020346
APA StyleBettin, J., & Wollni, M. (2019). Environmental Concern, Income, and Nature Experience in India. Sustainability, 11(2), 346. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11020346