1. Introduction
Loneliness is an emerging public health concern [
1], with older adults being at elevated risk of loneliness due to old-age-related vulnerabilities such as living alone, loss of family, and chronical disease [
2]. Loneliness can be defined as an “unpleasant experience that occurs when a person’s network of social relations is deficient in some important way, either quantitatively or qualitatively” [
3], and it is characterized by negative emotions, such as sadness and anger [
4]. Older migrants are a sub-group of older people who might have different experiences of loneliness from ageing locals, since migration often influences the trajectories of social connections with family, friends, and communities [
5]. The ethnic cultural background of older migrants is important in determining their participation and belonging patterns in the host society, which have important implications for their loneliness [
6]. However, there are few comparative studies that distinguish between the experiences of migrants who are more or less culturally different from the ageing local population.
In the current study, we explore how culture is related to experiences of loneliness among older migrants by focusing on three groups of ageing adults: cultural migrants, similar-culture migrants, and ageing locals. The categorization of these three groups of older migrants is based on Hofstede’s Individualism Index [
7]. The main research objectives are to (1) compare these three groups in terms of their loneliness levels, and (2) to unravel how different influencing factors, such as the social environment, social relationships, coping strategies, and personal characteristics, are associated with loneliness across the three groups.
4. Measurements
Dependent variable. Loneliness was measured using the four-item UCLA loneliness scale [
37]. The participants were asked to indicate the frequency of their feelings of lack of companionship, being left out, isolation, and feeling in tune with people around them based on a Likert scale ranging from one (never) to five (always). The UCLA scale was reliable, with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.84.
Independent variables. The social environment, social situation, coping strategies, and personal characteristics were used as independent variables for predicting loneliness.
First, the
social environment includes social capital, discrimination, and ageism. Social capital was measured using a seven-item scale [
38] with items including “People in this neighborhood are willing to help their neighbors” and “This is a close-knit or tight neighborhood where people generally know one another”. The responses ranged from one (strongly disagree) to five (strongly agree) (α = 0.82). Discrimination was measured using the short version of the everyday discrimination scale, which includes five statements [
39]. One of the five statements was “You are treated with less courtesy or respect than other people”. The participants were asked to rate their experience of discrimination on an ascending scale from one (never) to seven (almost every day) (α = 0.77). Subsequently, they were asked to indicate the main reason for the experience of discrimination: ancestry or national origins, gender, race, age, religion, height, weight, physical attractiveness, physical disability, mental health, sexual orientation, education or income, or other, and they could choose multiple reasons. For this study, we simply averaged across all the items to obtain a score of the extent to which the participants experienced discrimination across all of those identities. Ageism was measured by asking the participants to what extent they agreed or disagreed with the statement that old-age was a time of loneliness using a scale from one (strongly disagree) to five (strongly agree).
Second, the
social situation was measured in terms of marital status (1 = married [including those in civil partnerships] or cohabiting, 2 = divorced [including formerly in a civil partnership, which is now legally dissolved] or separated [but still legally married], 3 = widowed [including surviving partner from a civil partnership], 4 = single [never married or never in a civil partnership], 5 = in a relationship [and not living together]), having child/children or not (1 = yes, 0 = no), and relational mobility. Relational mobility was measured based on the participants’ responses to twelve statements [
29]. They were asked to think about their immediate community and to indicate to what extent they agreed with each item by using a 1–6 scale (1 = strongly disagree, 6 = strongly agree, α = 0.90). An example item is “Even if these people were not completely satisfied with the group they belonged to, they would usually stay with it anyway”.
Third, the coping strategies for loneliness were measured by asking the respondents about their self-perceived solutions that were effective at reducing feelings of loneliness. More precisely, they were asked “Please think about which of these possible solutions to loneliness you, or others you know, have found to be effective at reducing feelings of loneliness. Please select all that apply.” The respondents were given 20 possible solutions to choose from: (1) joining a club; (2) finding activities that distract you when you are on your own; (3) dedicating time to work, study, or hobbies; (4) looking for a new job; (5) setting out to introduce yourself to all your neighbors; (6) moving to a new area; (7) re-engaging with your church, mosque, or equivalent; (8) telling someone else that you’re feeling lonely; (9) talking to friends and family about your feelings; (10) deciding to invite people to be friends without fearing rejection; (11) setting out to look for the good in every person you meet; (12) deliberately starting a conversation with anyone you interact with, e.g., in shops; (13) seek counselling; (14) finding new non-social activities and pastimes; (15) finding new friends; (16) using the internet for support; (17) carrying on as normal and waiting for the feeling to pass; (18) giving yourself time to think about why you’re feeling lonely; (19) finding new social activities and pastimes; and (20) changing your thinking to be more positive. The respondents were also given the choice of “I do not know any solutions”. The solutions numbered 1, 4–7, 10, 12, 15, 16, and 19 were labeled as active coping strategies because they relate to efforts that are aimed at initiating new social contacts. The remaining 10 strategies were labeled as passive coping because they are about finding activities that temporarily distract people from loneliness. In the analysis, the variable of active coping was computed using the sum of the active solutions, while the variable of passive coping was computed using the sum of the passive solutions. Non-coping was measured by the response to the choice of “I do not know” (1 = not being aware of any solutions, 0 = being aware of at least one solution).
Fourth, demographic variables were also collected. Age, gender, years of education received, employment status, income, and self-perceived health were included as personal characteristic variables. The respondents were asked to indicate their age in years and choose their gender from four choices (male, female, other, prefer not to say). Regarding gender, those who answered “other” or “prefer not to say” were excluded due to the small sample size and the potential ambiguity of such responses. Gender was coded (1 = male, 0 = female). Education was measured in the number of years of education that they had completed from primary school to college or post-graduate study. The respondents were also asked to indicate their employment status (full-time in work, part-time in work, full-time student, part-time student, non-paid work, and unemployed). Employment status was coded (1 = full-time work or part-time work, 0 = full-time or part-time student, non-paid work or unemployed). Income was measured by asking how they feel their needs are met by the financial resources that they have (i.e., money), and they could answer either very well, fairly well, or poorly. Income was coded (1 = fairly well or very well, 0 = poorly). Self-perceived health was measured by asking how they would rate their current health status if 10 represents the healthiest that they could be and 0 represents the poorest health.
Analytical Strategy
To answer the first research question concerning the possible variance in loneliness and the influencing factors among these three groups, statistical analyses of one-way ANOVA and Chi-square tests were used to determine the differences in loneliness, social environment variables, social situation variables, coping strategies, and personal characteristics. When conducting the bivariate analysis, we included the z-test with adjusted p-values according to the Bonferroni correction to limit the potential for type I errors (α = 0.005). To answer the other research question concerning the associations between loneliness and its influencing factors, multiple linear regressions were performed to unravel the associations between the different influencing factors and loneliness among the three groups of non-migrants, cultural migrants, and similar-culture migrants.
5. Results
5.1. Sample Characteristics
Table 1 presents the characteristics of the three groups: non-migrants (N = 1084), cultural migrants (N = 239), and similar-culture migrants (N = 841). Most of the sample was female: 72.8% of non-migrants, 73.6% of cultural migrants, and 76.8% of similar-culture migrants were women. Concerning education, the non-migrants had received an average of 17 years of education (SD = 4.5), while the cultural migrants had received 17.7 years on average (SD = 5.0) and the similar-culture migrants 18.0 years (SD = 5.5). The majority of all three groups were retired (51.9% of non-migrants, 45.2% of cultural migrants, and 44.6% of similar-culture migrants). Most people thought that their needs were met fairly well or very well by their available financial means: 87.5% of non-migrants, 79.9% of cultural migrants, and 83.9% of similar-culture migrants.
The bivariate analyses showed significant differences in personal characteristics between the three groups. The cultural migrants (M = 62.2, SD = 5.7) were slightly younger than the non-migrants (M = 63.1, SD = 6.2) and similar-culture migrants (M = 63.6, SD = 6.7) (p < 0.05). The non-migrants had received the least years of education (M = 17.0, SD = 4.5) compared with the cultural migrants (M = 17.7, SD = 5.0) and similar-culture migrants (M = 18, SD = 5.5) (p < 0.01). The cultural migrants have the lowest income level: 20.1% rated “poorly” compared with 12.5% of non-migrants and 16.1% of similar-culture migrants (p < 0.05). There was no significant statistical difference between the groups in terms of gender, employment, and self-perceived health.
5.2. No Significant Difference in Loneliness among the Non-Migrants, Cultural Migrants, and Similar-Culture Migrants
The three groups’ loneliness levels according to the UCLA scale were not significantly different from each other. Thus, Hypothesis 1—cultural migrants have a higher loneliness level than those from a similar cultural background—was rejected. The non-migrants (N = 1084) and cultural migrants (N = 239) had the same loneliness score of 2.6 (SD = 1.2), while the similar-culture migrants (N = 841) had a loneliness score of 2.7 (SD = 1.2). This finding means that older migrants in the BBC loneliness data, regardless of their cultural origin, have similar levels of loneliness.
5.3. Social Capital, Discrimination, and Ageism All Significantly Associated with Loneliness across the Three Groups
Table 2 shows the results of the linear regressions for loneliness. The linear regression model of loneliness accounts for 37.3% of the variance for the group of non-migrants, 27.3% for the cultural migrants, and 29.8% for the similar-culture migrants. The effect sizes of the regression models of all the predictors on the dependent variable of loneliness for the three groups are 0.59, 0.38, and 0.42, respectively. The effect sizes are all large based on the guidelines for interpretation of Cohen’s f2, indicating that 0.02 is a small effect, 0.15 is a medium effect, and 0.35 is a large effect [
40].
Social capital, discrimination, and ageism, the three elements of the social environment in our study, are all significantly associated with loneliness in older migrants regardless of their cultural background. Thus, Hypothesis 2 was rejected as all three elements of the social environment were significant predictors of loneliness across the three groups regardless of their migration background. Higher social capital appears to act as a protective factor for loneliness, because it shows a negative association for members of all three groups: non-migrants (β = −0.21, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−0.28, −0.12]), cultural migrants (β = −0.27, p < 0.005, 95% CI [−0.48, −0.05]), and similar-culture migrants (β = −0.13, p < 0.005, 95% CI [−0.25, −0.03]).
Experiencing discrimination is a risk factor for loneliness in older people regardless of their migration background: non-migrants (β = 0.22,
p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.16, 0.29]), cultural migrants (β = 0.18,
p < 0.005, 95% CI [0.02, 0.34]), and similar-culture migrants (β = 0.11,
p < 0.005, 95% CI [0.03, 0.20]). The top four reasons that the respondents gave for the feeling of being discriminated against were age, gender, education, and ancestry and race (
Table 1). There exist between-group differences in terms of the reasons for feeling discriminated against. More non-migrants felt they were being discriminated against because of age (57.5%), gender (40.8%), and education (33.6%) compared with cultural migrants (44.2%, 38.1%, and 27.5%, respectively) and similar-culture migrants (46.3%, 33.7%, and 26.4%, respectively). Significantly more cultural migrants (37.3%) gave the reason of ancestry and race for the feeling of being discriminated against compared with the non-migrants (17.2%) and similar-culture migrants (27.6%). Ageism was significantly associated with higher levels of loneliness in all three groups: non-migrants (β = 0.26,
p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.20, 0.31]), cultural migrants (β = 0.30,
p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.16, 0.43]), and similar-culture migrants (β = 0.30,
p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.22, 0.36]).
5.4. Social Situation Acts as a Protective Factor for Loneliness in Non-Migrants and Similar-Culture Migrants
Regarding the social situation, being married or cohabitating was negatively associated with loneliness for only the non-migrants (β = −0.32, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−0.44, −0.20]) and similar-culture migrants (β = −0.32, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−0.47, −0.18]). Relational mobility showed negative correlation with the non-migrants (β = −0.18, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−0.27, −0.10]) and similar-culture migrants (β = −0.13, p < 0.005, 95% CI [−0.23, −0.10]). Neither of these two factors were significant predictors of loneliness in the cultural migrants. Thus, Hypothesis 3 was supported by the statistical results.
5.5. Coping Strategies: Active Coping and Non-Coping Are Significantly Associated with Loneliness
Active coping showed a negative association with loneliness in all three groups: non-migrants (β = −0.04, p < 0.005, 95% CI [−0.07, 0.00]), cultural migrants (β = −0.08, p < 0.005, 95% CI [−0.15, 0.00]), and similar-culture migrants (β = −0.04, p < 0.005, 95% CI [−0.08, 0.01]). Thus, adopting an active coping strategy, such as joining a club and finding new social activities, appeared protective against loneliness. The passive coping strategies showed no significant association across all three groups. Meanwhile, non-coping (i.e., not being aware of any coping strategies) was positively associated with loneliness in all three groups: non-migrants (β = 0.84, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.65, 1.07]), cultural migrants (β = 0.60, p < 0.005, 95% CI [0.08, 1.10]), and similar-culture migrants (β = 0.75, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.51, 0.99]). These findings led to the rejection of the fourth hypothesis that different coping strategies influenced differently for different groups of older people.
The top three active coping strategies adopted by the respondents in the sample were as follows: (1) joining a club (52.1% of non-migrants, 47.1% of cultural migrants, and 48.6% of similar-culture migrants); (2) deliberately starting conversations with anyone you interact with (e.g., in shops) (45.7%, 44.4%, and 48%, respectively); and (3) finding new social activities and pastimes (42.5%, 46.2%, and 43.4%, respectively). The top three passive coping strategies were: (1) dedicating time to work (63.5%, 57.3%, and 46%, respectively); (2) looking for a new job (57.3%, 58.2%, and 56.7%, respectively); and (3) changing thinking to be more positive (46.0%, 50.4%, and 44.1%, respectively). However, not all the respondents were able to engage in active or passive coping strategies. Some 9.5% of non-migrants, 8.2% of cultural migrants, and 9.6% of similar-culture migrants indicated that they were not aware of any coping strategies. There were statistically fewer similar-culture migrants (58.6%) who adopted the passive coping strategies of dedicating time to work, study, or hobbies compared with non-migrants (63.5%) and cultural migrants (64.3%) (x2 = 5.8; df = 2; p < 0.05).
5.6. Health, Income, and Education Show Significant Association
Three variables concerning personal characteristics were significantly associated with loneliness: self-perceived health, income, and education. Self-perceived health was negatively associated with loneliness for the non-migrants (β = −0.07, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−0.10, −0.05]), cultural migrants (β = −0.05, p < 0.005, 95% CI [−0.11, 0.00]), and similar-culture migrants (β = −0.08, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−0.11, −0.05]). Higher income was protective against loneliness for the cultural migrants (β = −0.42, p < 0.005, 95% CI [−0.79, −0.06]) and similar-culture migrants (β = −0.33, p < 0.005, 95% CI [−0.52, −0.12]), but not for the non-migrants. Education was negatively associated with loneliness in the group of non-migrants (β = −0.01, p < 0.01, 95% CI [−0.03, 0.00]), but not in the two groups of older people with a migration background.
6. Discussion
This study explored how culture is related to experiences of loneliness among older migrants. We focused on three groups of ageing adults (cultural migrants, similar-culture migrants, and non-migrants) and explored (1) whether the loneliness level differed among these three groups of older people and (2) what factors are associated with their experiences of loneliness. This section discusses the research findings, limitations of the study, directions for future studies, and practical interventions for loneliness.
We found no significant difference in the loneliness levels among older migrants based on the individualist-collectivist cultural indicator, as defined by Hofstede [
7]. Contrary to what was expected (more cultural difference leading to higher loneliness), migrants’ culture of origin (or their cultural background) is not associated with their loneliness level. Rather, the most influential factors in terms of predicting loneliness were individual resources (as measured in terms of coping strategies), social situation (i.e., marital status and relational mobility), and the social environment of the receiving society (as measured in social capital, discrimination, and ageism).
Concerning individual resources, being aware of active coping strategies focused on initiating new social contacts is protective against loneliness across all three groups. This finding is consistent with existing literature confirming the beneficial role of active coping strategies that directly deal with the stressor at hand [
41]. In terms of social status, being married or cohabitating is correlated with lower levels of loneliness, which accords with previous literature stating that marriage or cohabitation is positively associated with subjective well-being [
42]. Our finding also shows that higher levels of relational mobility, or opportunities for more active engagement in close relationships, protects against loneliness. This can be explained by the fact that those engaged more actively in close relationships are also people with more social resources, and those resources can mediate the negative consequences of loneliness [
43]. We also found that being childless or lacking meaningful social contact with offspring for older adults is not significantly correlated with loneliness across cultures, which is consistent with existing literature stating that childlessness does not necessarily lead to higher levels of loneliness [
44,
45]. The explanation for this could be that older adults have “filled the possible gap due to their childlessness with other activities or persons” [
45]. These activities can be playing sports, travelling, or engaging in establishing other meaningful social networks with godchildren, nephews, nieces, or the children of friends [
45].
Our study highlights the important role of the social environment for the ageing population, regardless of their migration background. This can be explained by the theory of environment gerontology, which emphasizes the person–environment relationship as decisive in ageing outcomes [
46]. A favorable social environment with high social capital, where interpersonal trust and reciprocity create stronger ties among kin, friends, neighbors, and strangers alike [
47], can help older adults buffer the negative experience of loneliness. The positive association of discrimination and ageism with loneliness confirms existing research findings that discrimination and ageism can result in social exclusion from support networks and be detrimental to individual well-being [
22,
47].
This study shows that the social environment plays a prominent role in understanding health outcomes such as loneliness. This finding merits a shift from the individual-based deficiency model to a society-based deficiency model. The determining factor for how older migrants from another culture feel in a new environment is more about the characteristics of the receiving society than about the individual migrant’s characteristics: whether they experience discrimination, whether trust between people exists, and whether they are treated negatively due to their advanced age (ageism) are what matter in predicting loneliness. Even though discrimination has no place in public opinion, until this day, discrimination toward older migrants continues to affect older migrants negatively, leading to inequality and disparity in health and social care [
48]. How to address these structural barriers should be at the top of the agenda for policymakers in charge of health issues for the ageing population with a migration background. In this sense, public health policymakers can support initiatives that support an anti-discrimination and favorable social environment in the receiving society, where older migrants can thrive and achieve maximum well-being.
This overall finding challenges the unidimensional definition of loneliness only from the perspective of deficiency in social contact [
3]. Our results support current efforts among social gerontological researchers [
49] to broaden the loneliness research toward a multi-dimensional perspective by including structural factors, such as social environment variables, to establish a more holistic picture. Apart from being influenced by individual-level factors such as the number of social contacts, we have shown that loneliness can also be a consequence of an unfavorable social environment.
The second research objective was to explore the associations of different influencing factors with loneliness among older adults from different cultural backgrounds. We found that older migrants from different cultural backgrounds vary in their protective factors toward loneliness. This highlights the importance of categorizing older adults based on their cultural background when devising intervention programs. For instance, the social situation, as measured by marital status and relational mobility, can protect non-migrants and similar-culture migrants from loneliness, although this protective effect was not seen in the group of cultural migrants. This finding informs several suggestions for future interventions to reduce loneliness levels based on older people’s cultural background. For non-migrants and similar-culture migrants, ameliorating their social situation might be an interventional tool for decreasing their loneliness. For example, we would expect, based on our findings, that an intervention focused on increasing meaningful contact, such as having a partner, and encouraging relational mobility would work for those non-migrants and similar-culture migrants experiencing loneliness. For all older adults, regardless of their cultural background, loneliness interventions could focus on increasing knowledge of active coping strategies, such as joining a club and finding new social activities. This is in line with previous research confirming the role of new social contacts in reducing older migrants’ reports of loneliness [
50].
This study is not without limitations. First, there are potential issues with the sample composition. Since the recruitment for the study was announced in English, only migrants who had no local language barrier could participate in the research. This means that older migrants who have not mastered English well enough to listen to BBC News or read English newspapers were excluded from the research. Most likely, the ones who experience language barriers are the ones who are excluded from meaningful social relationships [
51] and have heightened levels of loneliness compared with those without a migration background [
25]. This limitation also points to the opportunity for future studies to conduct qualitative interviews with older adults with language barriers (possibly the most socially excluded ones) to get deeper insights into their loneliness experiences. Not being able to speak the local language (i.e., having the language barrier) can lead to poor quality social relationships [
51,
52] and exclusion from social participation in the larger society [
21]. Second, we have a rather small sample size, especially for the group of cultural migrants, in our study, which restricts the interest to test the possible differences in the predictive power of the independent variables (e.g., everyday discrimination) across the three groups. Third, the unequal sample sizes across the three groups of older people do result in reduced statistical power. However, since equal sample sizes are not a perquisite for conducting one-way ANOVA and Chi-square tests, and as our three groups have roughly the same variance [
53] (pp. 375–394), we still believe in the outcome of our statistical analysis. Despite these limitations, the strength of the study also lies in BBC Loneliness Experiment, with its broad coverage of topics related to loneliness, such as the social environment, coping, and social situation, and with its respondents from various backgrounds.