Next Article in Journal
Proposed Environmental Risk Management Elements in a Carpathian Valley Basin, within the Roşia Montană European Historical Mining Area
Next Article in Special Issue
Role of Callous and Unemotional (CU) Traits on the Development of Youth with Behavioral Disorders: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Journal
Influence of ESGC Indicators on Financial Performance of Listed Pharmaceutical Companies
Previous Article in Special Issue
Effects of the Educational Use of Music on 3- to 12-Year-Old Children’s Emotional Development: A Systematic Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

On the Rocky Road to Independence: Big Five Personality Traits and Locus of Control in Polish Primary School Students during Transition into Early Adolescence

1
Institute of Psychology, Maria Curie-Skłodowska University in Lublin, Głęboka 45, 20-612 Lublin, Poland
2
Department of Psychology, Jan Kochanowski University in Kielce, Krakowska 11, 25-029 Kielce, Poland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(9), 4564; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18094564
Submission received: 23 February 2021 / Revised: 3 April 2021 / Accepted: 16 April 2021 / Published: 25 April 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Childhood Education and Care)

Abstract

:
This article reports the results of a survey of 455 Polish primary school sixth-graders experiencing changes in the education system. The goal of the study was to identify the relationships between the Big Five personality traits, measured with the picture-based personality survey for children (PBPS-C) and locus of control, determined using the locus of control questionnaire (LOCQ). The results lead to the conclusion that primary school students do not have an established locus of control of either success or failure. There are also no significant differences between boys and girls in the way they interpret the causes of situations and events that happen to them. Boys, compared to girls, scored significantly higher on traits related to seeking and enjoying the company of others. On the other hand, girls exhibited significantly higher levels of traits responsible for increased anxiety than boys. The personality traits that correlated the strongest with locus of control were Conscientiousness, Openness to Experience, and Agreeableness. A regression model showed that locus of control of success was significantly affected by two traits: Extraversion and Conscientiousness. Locus of control of failure was significantly predicted by Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness (positively), and Neuroticism (negatively). Regression model with gender as a moderator of relationships between personality traits and locus of control turned out to be insignificant.

1. Introduction

The aim of this study was to analyse personality traits and locus of control of students in the last (sixth) grade of primary school, who were entering adolescence. At the time of the study, the participants were the first generation of students taught in the new education system refashioned by the new education reform in Poland [1]. Before 2017, the Polish system of primary and secondary education had been divided into three levels: six-grade primary schools, three-grade middle schools, and three-grade secondary schools. In 2016, primary school education was extended from six to eight years, and since that time, until 2019, middle schools have been being phased out. As research has repeatedly shown [2], experiences of social change in children’s living environment, including those related to the transition to a next stage of education, are associated with increased stress induced by new requirements and the need to adjust to a new peer group and new teachers [3,4]. Stress resulting from the transition to another stage of education may be additionally aggravated by the hardships of adolescence. The moment of passage from the safe period of childhood to adolescence is connected with numerous challenges related to biological maturation and new social roles and expectations [1,4,5]. Some early adolescents are reluctant to abandon the familiar stability and security of childhood, on the one hand, but on the other, they excitedly enter the more uncertain, more complex, and definitely more dangerous world of adolescence. Emotionally speaking, this is a very demanding process. The curiosity of the new and the growing need for independence motivate young people to take up new challenges on a completely unknown ground. At the same time, however, the uneasiness and sometimes even fear of giving up the comfort of parental care and not having to participate fully in social life makes it difficult for them to part with childhood [6]. Early adolescence is a very important moment in everyone’s life. Students are compelled to cope with the pressures and possibilities associated with becoming an adolescent. The physiological changes occurring in young people’s bodies affect their self-image and may lower their self-esteem and mood [7,8,9]. This is largely due to the neurohormonal changes associated with the pubertal growth spurt. The emotional swing adolescents go through is a consequence of the imbalance between the heightened activity in subcortical emotional processing systems and the immaturity of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) which regulates self-control [10]. For this reason, individuals who enter adolescence have ambivalent emotions, easily go from one extreme to the next, and show exaggerated emotional reactions that are often inappropriate to the context. Puberty opens the world to students in many alluring but also threatening ways [11]. During adolescence prominent developmental transformations are seen in the PFC and limbic brain regions, alterations that include an apparent shift in the balance between mesocortical and mesolimbic dopamine systems. Developmental changes in these stressor-sensitive regions, which are critical for attributing incentive salience to different events and stimuli, likely contribute to the unique characteristics of adolescence [12,13].
Students in early adolescence are at risk of experiencing impairments in personality development [14] and show increased vulnerabilities in behavioural and psychiatric morbidity [5,15]. The period of entering adolescence is also a period of rapid physical growth, the onset of sexual maturation, increase in risk taking and sensation seeking, increased self-consciousness, early experimentation with substance use, and growing rates of accidents, reckless behaviour, depression, anxiety, and eating disorders [12,13,15,16]. It has been claimed that the roots of many adult-onset health problems can be traced back to patterns of behaviour that can be identified in adolescence [5]. Therefore, the period of transition into adolescence reveals complex issues that have enormous implications for public health. Given this background, we decided to survey students in the last grade of primary school who experienced changes in the education system in Poland.
Adolescence is a period of an intensive development of personality, including locus of control, which allows young people to interpret the events in which they participate in their everyday lives and assign meaning to them. Personality traits and locus of control play an important role in predicting successful completion of compulsory education, which, in turn, paves the way for future professional success [17,18,19]. It is important to note that there are also other variables which are crucial for educational achievements and successful adaptation to new demands and roles. Among many, there is evidence that parental involvement, teacher support, and general cognitive abilities [20], as well as goal orientations, popularity, and learning strategies [21], are important factors that shape personal adjustment and educational outcomes of early adolescents. Individual history of so-far education and negative life events also play a crucial role in successful adjustment and the way how adolescents realize their educational duties [22].
By measuring personality traits and locus of control in students, one can partially estimate the level of their adaptation to the new situation and the way they interpret the events they experience [23]. The literature on the subject shows that some personality traits importantly affect the ways early adolescents cope with stress and solve everyday problems. For instance, individuals who are high on Neuroticism have an increased tendency to ruminate about unpleasant events and avoid new and unpredictable situations. By contrast, persons with high levels of Extraversion are more likely to use active problem-solving strategies and interpret new situations as challenges rather than threats [24,25,26]. As for locus of control, Borecka-Biernat [4] has observed that early adolescents who have an internal locus of control are more likely to use active strategies for solving social conflicts.

1.1. Personality Traits in Early Adolescence

Currently, one of the most popular personality trait models used in psychological studies of children and adolescents is McCrae’s and Costa [27,28,29] Big Five model. The authors of this model define personality as a dimension of individual differences relating to traits that are responsible for the characteristic ways people feel, think, and behave. According to Costa and McCrae, personality traits are internal dispositions that mould people’s self-image, sense of self-efficacy, and personal goals [30]. They identify five universal dimensions of personality: Extraversion, Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, and Openness to Experience [27,28]. Persons with high levels of Extraversion are sociable, spontaneously show their emotions, and easily establish new interpersonal relationships. Individuals who are high on Neuroticism have mood swings, feel anxious, often for no reason, and use defensive coping strategies. Conscientious people like order, are well-organized, hard-working, and capable of postponing gratification. Agreeable individuals are compassionate and caring, avoid conflict, and strive to have harmonious relationships with other people. Persons who are open to experience show mental flexibility, cognitive curiosity, versatile interests, and readiness to analyse the complex determinants of their own and other people’s decisions and emotions [28].
Costa and McCrae believe that personality traits are biologically determined and are manifested in the ways even very young children respond to various situations. Personality is strongly influenced by the functions of the PFC (Conscientiousness and Openness to Experience) [31] and by motivation systems that organize responses to rewards and punishments and that drive approach and avoidance behaviour (Extroversion, Neuroticism and Agreeableness) [32]. Bouchard and Loehin [33] also argue that the Big Five traits are biologically based, indicating that the genetic factor explains about 50% of the variance in the Big Five personality traits. Temperamental traits observed in early childhood, such as positive versus negative affect, level of activity, or effortful control, allow one to predict the development of the Big Five traits in later stages of childhood and adolescence [34,35,36]. Costa and McCrae note that personality development is not complete until the early thirties [37], as personality may change due to factors apart from intrinsic maturation [38]. Researchers note that personality traits are also an effect of socialization [39,40,41] and may alter in certain periods of life, especially under the influence of changes in the external environment, assumption of new social roles, teaching, upbringing, and significant life changes. It has been pointed out that the moment of transition from childhood to adolescence abounds in changes in various spheres of young people’s functioning (both biological and socio-emotional), which lead to vital changes in personality [42,43].
Despite the fact that personality traits are most often analysed in older adolescents and adults, existing findings show that studies of younger children are also warranted. For middle childhood, personality has been considered a potent predictor of concurrent and later adaptation [44]. Already Freud noted that personality traits formed in late childhood exerted an influence on personality adjustment in later developmental periods, including adolescence and adulthood [45]. In this light, it is not surprising that personality traits of ever younger individuals are studied more and more often [44]. Nonetheless, the literature on developmental changes in the Big Five personality traits in younger children is scanty [46].
In a study by Suldo, Minch, and Hearon [47], all Big Five personality traits were significant predictors of life satisfaction in adolescents, with the reservation that Agreeableness was associated with life satisfaction only in girls. Very similar findings were obtained by Weber and Huebner [48], who studied 344 American early adolescents. In this group of teenagers, Neuroticism correlated negatively—and Extraversion, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness positively—with general life satisfaction. In addition, Marcionetti and Rossier [49] showed that Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Neuroticism were associated with life satisfaction in Swiss adolescents. Additionally, they demonstrated that satisfaction with life was connected with high self-efficacy and high self-esteem.
Today, personality psychologists are particularly interested in the developmental changes in the Big Five personality traits occurring during adolescence [50,51,52]. Pullmann, Raudsepp, and Allik [51], who conducted a longitudinal study in a sample of 2650 Estonian adolescents aged 12–18 years, observed the greatest developmental changes in personality traits in the youngest group of respondents (between 12 and 14 years of age). During the 2 year study, the researchers recorded a significant increase in the level of Extraversion in girls and a decrease in the levels of Neuroticism and Agreeableness in both genders.
Research also shows that personality traits are associated with intelligence, school achievement, adjustment, coping, willingness to engage in risk-taking behaviours, and social competence. Graziano and Ward [53], who studied a group of early adolescents, showed that Conscientiousness played an important role in successful school adjustment. Gullone and Moore [54] found that in a sample of Australian teenagers aged from 11 to 18, Extraversion and Agreeableness were negatively associated with the willingness to take risk. Heaven and Ciarrochi [17], who analysed relationships of personality traits and intelligence with school achievement among Australian adolescents, additionally showed that Openness to Experience was a good predictor of school achievement, though only in respondents with higher levels of intelligence. Medvedova [55] observed that, in early adolescents, Neuroticism correlated positively with avoidance coping, and Extraversion was positively associated with support seeking and task-oriented coping. Shiner [44] demonstrated that Extraversion and Agreeableness were predictors of social competence, pro-social behaviour and peer acceptance in 10 year olds.

1.2. Locus of Control in Early Adolescence

Late childhood and early adolescence are considered to be crucial for the development of action orientations and for conceptualisations of person–environment transactions [56,57]. For this reason, locus of control develops intensively in these periods of life. Locus of control in Rotter’s theory of social learning [58] is the degree to which people believe they have control over the causes of various events they experience in their lives [59]. Locus of control is a person’s belief about their ability to control the course of events in which they participate [60]. The theoretical construct of locus of control is a continuum that runs from internal to external. People with a strong internal locus of control believe that they have control over both positive and negative events in their lives and that these events are primarily a result of their own actions, features, and competence. They think they have influence on the course of events in their surroundings and feel responsible for their decisions. By contrast, people with a strong external locus of control are more likely to believe that events and reinforcements (rewards and punishment) in their lives are due to factors that are beyond their control, such as bad luck, chance, or unfavourable circumstances [58,61].
Locus of control may be different for different spheres of human life. For instance, a person who believes that their social success is due to their own mental characteristics may at the same time think that their educational or occupational failures are caused by bad luck or other people’s bad intentions. Research shows that students with an internal locus of control are less likely to perceive negative events in their lives as being due to external factors. They are more likely to take action to remedy the difficult situation, for example, by looking to others for support [62], which is especially important in the case of students who transition from one stage of education to the next. Locus of control develops in childhood under the educational influences of the family and school, which stimulate the development of a sense of responsibility for the actions one takes [56]. It is also a result of the child’s increasing independence and their growing ability to actively influence events and derive satisfaction from the effects of their actions. One of the most important factors that may induce developmental changes in locus of control is a change of social environment, including a transition to the next stage of education. Rotter’s is a personality-dominant approach to locus of control. Another approach, introduced in 1977 by Stern and Manifold, treats internal locus of control as a social value. The theory of the norm of internality puts emphasis on social judgements which favour internality as a way of interpreting different events [63]. Social superiority of internal students results from the fact that social appraisers in schools prefer them to externals. The norm of internality, as many researchers suggest, is particularly salient in education as it plays an important role in the assessment of children’s academic potential [64,65,66]. According to this theory, preference for internal causal explanations is learnt at schools where children discover that it is a socially desirable norm [66]. Dubois and Le Poultier [67] conducted studies in which teachers had to predict which fictitious students would pass into the next grade. They also obtained information about the social class of the pupils’ families (lower versus higher) and the children’s level of academic achievements (low versus average). The children filled out an internality questionnaire (with an external versus internal orientation), and the teachers had to make predictions concerning their success in passing to the next grade. Despite important cues concerning family status and academic potential, children who gave more internal explanations received more favourable judgements from the teachers.
Chubb, Fertmani, and Ross [68] note that the development of locus of control in adolescence reflects the challenges and difficulties faced by adolescents. These challenges are related to the need to reconcile learning with free-time activities and adapt to the new peer environment when passing to the next stage of education. In a study of early adolescents, Krampen [56] observed that parental approval and attention to the child’s positive behaviour predicted an internal locus of control. Kaya [69] found that locus of control may also depend on a person’s sociometric status. Students classified as popular and controversial were characterized by a significantly more internal locus of control compared to students with other statuses. It also turns out that participation in sport is conducive to the development of an internal locus of control in early adolescence [57]. Kulas [70] studied changes in students’ locus of control in late childhood and early adolescence over several years of their school life. Locus of internal control decreased as students passed from grade three to grade four of primary school and then increased between grades four and seven. The shift towards a more external locus of control may have been associated with the novelty of the situation related to the transition to grade four, in which there is a notable change in the structure of educational requirements. According to Kulas, adjustment to the new situation and the prospect of it remaining stable for the next several years promoted the development of an internal locus of control in the students he surveyed. Many authors also point to the relationship between locus of control in adolescents and such variables as educational achievement [66,67,71], stress coping [72], personal resilience [73], and social support [74]. Butler-Sweeney [72] demonstrated that internal locus of control was associated with the task-focused coping style and higher self-esteem in girls. Cazan and Dumitrescu [73], on the other hand, found that internal locus of control was related to psychological resilience in adolescents. Mendolia and Walker [18] discovered a positive relationship between Conscientiousness and achievement of long-term life goals and a negative association between low self-esteem and an external locus of control in teenage youth. These researchers point out that the organizational effort young people invest in implementing their plans significantly reduces the possibility of them dropping out from school or work. Young people who have a low self-esteem and an external locus of control are more at risk of leaving school or work.

1.3. Personality Traits and Locus of Control in Early Adolescence

Although personality traits and locus of control are separate theoretical constructs, they share some common features. One of them is that they allow to predict how an individual will behave in an unknown situation. This knowledge can help professionals plan effective teaching and educational activities for young people. While personality traits are biologically based and determined, the locus of control, which is a construct derived from the theory of social learning, is shaped by external experiences and social requirements. Personality traits, being internal dispositions and tendencies to engage in or withdraw from particular activities and to interpret various situations in specific ways, may secondarily lead to the development, in children and adolescents, of a sense of responsibility (or a lack thereof) for various situations they experience directly. Firstly, personality traits may predispose young people to seek activities and ways of spending time that suit their individual preferences. Secondly, they can also influence the mode in which teenagers interpret reward and failure situations as being within or beyond their personal control [42,43,75]. What is important here is cognitive assessment of the situation with respect to one’s resources and estimation of the probability of success or failure. A person who feels they have the resources needed to complete a task is likely to undertake the task. By contrast, a person who is not so sure about their resources is likely to avoid new and unpredictable situations associated with a risk of failure [76].
Personality is strongly influenced by motivation systems that organise responses to rewards and punishments and that drive approach and avoidance behaviours [36]. Personality traits influence the processes of assigning motivational meanings to various stimuli and situations [77]. Extraverts tend to seek rewarding situations and see new and ambiguous situations as challenges and opportunities for reward [36,78,79]. In addition to looking for and engaging in potentially rewarding situations, extraverted individuals derive satisfaction from their successes longer than do individuals with elevated Neuroticism levels. Neurotics are characterized by increased sensitivity to punishment [75,78,79], and when they fail, they tend to ruminate and have intrusive thoughts [80,81]. Failures do not demotivate extraverts as strongly as they do to neurotics. For this reason, it has been pointed out that people who are extraverted and open to experience cope better with stress and are less likely than people with high levels of Neuroticism to avoid situations that are associated with taking on challenges and a threat of losing positive self-esteem in the event of failure [76,78].
These findings are important from the point of view of children and adolescents’ developing sense of influence over various life events. Getting involved in situations that may potentially bring various benefits can be conducive to developing an internal locus of control. Conversely, avoiding uncertain situations and withdrawing from the risk associated with making decisions in new and ambiguous circumstances may result in the perpetuation of a sense of lack of influence on what is happening and may incite young people to attribute the causes of events to factors that are beyond their control, such as bad luck or unfavourable conditions.

1.4. Gender Differences in Personality Traits and Locus of Control

Limura and Taku [82] found, in a study of Japanese teenagers, that girls scored higher on Conscientiousness than boys. Very similar results were obtained by Klimstra et al. [83] in a sample of early adolescents. The girls who participated in their study had higher levels of Openness to Experience than their male counterparts. In Polish studies of adolescents, girls and boys have been observed to differ in terms of the Big Five personality traits. Lickiewicz [84], in a study involving 236 Polish teenagers, found that girls had higher levels of Extraversion, Openness to Experience, and Agreeableness compared to boys. Czerniawska [85], who studied a group of 200 middle school and secondary school students, observed that boys scored higher on Neuroticism than girls, while girls had higher Openness to Experience scores compared to boys. Branje, van Lieshout, and Gerris [50] demonstrated on the basis of a longitudinal study of 285 Dutch adolescents that Extraversion and Openness to Experience decreased with age in boys, while the opposite was true of Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness to Experience in girls. An intercultural study carried out by De Bolle et al. [86] in a sample of 4850 adolescents aged from 12 to 17 years showed that girls around the age of 14 exhibited a higher level of Neuroticism than boys in the same age group. This is consistent with the results of research on the levels of Neuroticism in adult women and men, in which female respondents obtained higher scores than their male counterparts. In De Bolle et al.’s study, girls also scored higher on Openness to Experience and Conscientiousness than boys, regardless of age. These differences were independent of culture, which suggests that the differences in personality traits between girls and boys may be universal. In his longitudinal study, Soto [87] obtained similar results in a group of teenage girls. Akhtar and Saxena [88] found that adolescent boys were more internal than girls. Similarly, Kulas [70] observed that adolescent girls were characterized by a significantly more external locus of control compared to boys, which confirms the assumption documented in the literature about higher external locus of control rather in women than in men [89]. The shift towards a more external locus of control in early adolescent girls may be due to the psycho-physiological changes associated with the pubertal growth spurt. Young people may interpret the necessity to accept the changes occurring in their bodies as either opportunities or threats. Girls are more likely than boys to see these changes as giving rise to confusion and diminishing their confidence about the activities they engage in [90,91].

1.5. The Present Study

The variables analysed in the present article play an important role in the contemporary psychological discussion on the possibility of predicting young people’s educational achievements [18,92,93] and satisfaction with life [47,94,95], as well as shaping their professional careers [19]. A review of previous research on Costa and McCrae’s [27] Big Five personality traits and Rotter’s [58] concept of locus of control allowed us to formulate the following research questions:
1.
What are the differences in personality trait configurations between primary school girls and boys?
2.
What are the differences in locus of control of success and failure between primary school girls and boys?
3.
What is the relationship between personality traits and locus of control of success and failure in primary school sixth grade students?
In order to answer these research questions, we formulated three hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Girls are more likely than boys to have higher levels of Openness to Experience, Neuroticism, and Conscientiousness.
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
Girls are more likely than boys to have an external locus of control of success and failure.
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
In the whole group, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness are associated with internal locus of control of successes and failures. Neuroticism is associated with external locus of control of successes and failures. It was presumed that gender is a moderator of relationships between personality traits and locus of control.
Research on differences in personality traits and locus of control between boys and girls is sometimes inconsistent. Given these previous findings, we assumed that the girls and boys participating in the present study would differ in personality traits and locus of control. In light of the research, we also assumed different relationships between personality traits and locus of control as different personality traits may have different influence on the locus of control. Some traits promote internal, while others promote external locus of control in adolescents [78,79,96,97].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedure

The sample consisted of primary school sixth graders from the city of Lublin, Poland. A total of 455 students took part in the study, including 232 girls and 223 boys, min 12 and max 13 years old. The mean age of the respondents was M = 12.43, and standard deviation was SD = 0.50. Surveys were conducted in four primary schools in Lublin with paper–pencil method by authors of this article. Schools were selected randomly from the list of public schools from the website of the Lublin’s Board of Education. Non-public schools were not taken into consideration. Questionnaires were completed in groups during one meeting with each class. In total, students from 29 classes were examined. The study procedure complied with ethical principles regarding confidentiality, anonymity, and voluntary participation. Consent had been obtained from school principals and parents, and school psychologists/counsellors had been consulted before the survey was conducted. The participants were informed about the scientific purpose of the survey and that they had the right to withdraw from participation at any time. The students filled out a personal data sheet, providing their age and gender details, and two self-report questionnaires: the picture-based personality survey for children (PBPS-C) by Maćkiewicz and Cieciuch [98,99] and the locus of control questionnaire (LOCQ) by Krasowicz and Kurzyp-Wojnarska [100]. The participants took 45 min to complete the survey. As a token of appreciation for their participation in the study, each class received feedback via their homeroom teacher (form tutor) regarding their locus of control scores. All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. The present study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee for Scientific Research of Maria Curie-Skłodowska University, Lublin, Poland (No. 29).

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Picture-Based Personality Survey for Children (PBPS-C)

The Big Five personality traits were measured using the PBPS-C by Maćkiewicz and Cieciuch [98]. It is worth mentioning that currently in Poland there are no instruments comparable to PBPS-C for measuring the Big Five personality traits in early adolescents. Different language versions of this test are successfully used to measure personality traits of children and adolescents in many European countries, among others, Italy [101], Germany [102], France [103], Greece [104], and Spain [105]. It was assumed that the children surveyed, who were transitioning from childhood to early adolescence, could use mental operations that allowed them to accurately assess their various personality characteristics by comparing themselves to the personality characteristics of other people [46]. Following Caspi, Roberts, and Shiner [106], Maćkiewicz and Cieciuch assumed that personality traits and their development in children should be measured with a tool that went beyond the classical questionnaire format based on written descriptions of personality traits. Their picture-based survey takes into account the cognitive abilities of children in the periods of late childhood and early adolescence, who have not yet fully developed the ability to think abstractly. PBPSC is comprised of 15 items, each of which consists of two pictures showing the same character behaving in different ways. Above each pair of pictures, there is a brief caption describing a certain situation, e.g., “When it rains”. The participants are instructed to read the caption and then choose the one picture that best represents the way they would behave in this situation: “Think how you usually behave in a situation like that”. The subjects are told that they should think whether, in the given situation, they are more likely to behave like the character in the picture on the right or on the left. Then, the subjects rate on a five-point scale how similar the character’s behaviour is to their own by ticking an appropriate box (rating from 1—“very similar” to 5—“not similar at all”). The respondents’ choices are compared with the scoring key. The overall score for each scale ranges from 3 to 15 points. Because this instrument has no norms, the scores are interpreted by comparing to the theoretical mean M = 9.0 and the theoretical mean standard deviation SD = 2.0. The validity of PBPS-C was determined using confirmatory factor analysis of Sarisa and Gallhofer’s data quality indicator, defined as the product of validity and reliability. The values of this indicator for the individual PBPS-C scales are as follows: Extraversion 0.86; Agreeableness 0.88; Conscientiousness 0.83; Neuroticism 0.76; and Openness to Experience 0.77. PBPSC has a satisfactory data quality indicator value, which means it can be used in scientific research on the Big Five personality traits of children and adolescents [98] (pp. 77–79). The scale is available in two versions: for younger children in grades 1–3 of primary school and for older children in grades 4–6 of primary school. In the present study, the second version of the questionnaire was used. According to Jean Piaget [107], early adolescence involves a shift from concrete to abstract thinking. This means that thinking becomes hypothetical and allows individuals to solve verbal-conceptual problems. It is worth emphasizing, however, that the cognitive changes occur gradually in adolescents, have different pace, and are characterized by interindividual differences (such as socioeconomical status of the family, history of hospitalization or so far educational achievements etc.).

2.2.2. Locus of Control Questionnaire (LOCQ)

The locus of control variable was measured using the LOCQ by Krasowicz and Kurzyp-Wojnarska [100], a Polish version of Rotter’s I-E instrument based on his theory of Rotter’s social learning and locus of control [54]. The LOCQ and I-E differ in the way the scores are interpreted: in the LOCQ, the higher the score, the more internal the person’s locus of control is (reversely to I-E). At the time of the study, in the school year 2016/2017, the LOCQ had been the up-to-date version of the instrument, before the revised version (LOCQ-R) was issued in 2017 [108]. The test measures locus of control of the consequences of behaviour, a construct described in Rotter’s social learning theory. The LOCQ consists of 46 items, 36 of which are diagnostic for locus of control. The minimum and maximum raw scores are 0 and 18, respectively, for the success and failure scales, and 0 and 36 for the generalized locus of control scale. Half of the diagnostic items regard situations of success and the other half—situations of failure; the sum of the scores on those items is a measure of a generalized locus of control, i.e., a general tendency to attribute more internal or more external causes to various events. The higher score, the higher internal locus of control. The respondent is addressed directly and asked about situations he or she has experienced. External factors responsible for success or failure include a wide range of situations associated with parents, peers, school, etc. Internal factors, on the other hand, include the respondent’s own behaviours and efforts [100].
There are two versions of the LOCQ: one for girls and one for boys. They only differ in the grammatical form of the questions (the Polish language has gendered verb and adjective forms); the content of the questions is the same. After reading an item, the respondent chooses one of two alternatives (a or b), the one that best describes his/her own locus of control in the situation depicted by the item. Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for the individual scales are: α = 0.40 (success), α = 0.54 (failure), and α = 0.62 (global). Despite the low values of the coefficients, we decided to use the LOCQ in the survey because at the time, no other tests for measuring locus of control of success and failure in adolescents aged 12–13 years were available in Polish.
The global score is the sum of the scores obtained on the success and failure subscales and is a measure of a generalized locus of control, not differentiated into locus of control in situations of success and failure. It provides information on a person’s generalized tendency to attribute responsibility for various events to internal or external factors. Subscale scores give a more detailed picture of the typical ways a person interprets the causes of events related to successes and failures.

2.3. Data Analysis

The data obtained in the survey were interpreted quantitatively on the basis of raw scores for both the LOCQ and the PBPS-C. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS v.25. First, normal distribution of the scores was tested using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. The distribution of the variables did not deviate from the normal distribution. In further analyses, a parametric Student’s t-test of significance of differences was used, and the effect size was measured using Hedge’s g coefficient. Correlation analysis was performed using Pearson’s r, and a regression analysis was carried out in which personality traits were predictors, and locus of control in situations of successes and failures were dependent variables. Moreover, the analysis of interaction effect of gender on relationships between personality traits and locus of control was carried out. Before Student’s t-test was applied, homogeneity of variance of data obtained for the first and second research question had been tested using Levene’s F-test. The F-test was statistically non-significant.

3. Results

The analyses of the results are presented in the same order as the research questions, i.e., first, the significances of differences are given separately for girls’ and boys’ scores; then, the results of the analysis of correlations between the investigated variables for the entire sample are presented; and finally, the results of the regression analysis, with personality traits as predictors and locus of control as the dependent variable, are shown.
The results of comparisons of significance of differences between personality trait scores in the samples of girls and boys are shown in Table 1.
The data indicates that girls differ statistically significantly from boys in terms of Extraversion and Neuroticism, but the effect size for both these personality traits is small. Girls are more prone to anxiety than boys, while boys are more open to making new friends and spending time in the company of other people. Table 2 shows the respondents’ mean locus of control scores for the success, failure, and global scales.
There are no statistically significant differences in locus of control of success and failure and the generalized locus of control between the boys and the girls surveyed. Both boys’ and girls’ locus of control scores are in the average range. Table 3 shows the results of the analysis of correlations between the Big Five personality traits and locus of control (generalized and related to success and failure) for the entire sample.
All the correlations between the participants’ PBPS-C and LOCQ scores are statistically significant. Extraversion correlates moderately positively with locus of control of success, locus of control of failure, and generalized locus of control. Negative weak correlations are observed between Neuroticism and the three types of locus of control. Openness to Experience correlates moderately with locus of control of success and generalized locus control, and there is also a weak relationship between Openness to Experience and locus of control of failure. Conscientiousness correlates moderately with locus of control of success and generalized locus of control, and weakly with locus of control of failure. The last personality trait, Agreeableness, correlates weakly with locus of control of success and locus of control of failure, and moderately with generalized locus of control. Table 4 shows the results of the analysis of correlations between the Big Five personality traits and locus of control related to success and failure.
Data presented in Table 4 indicate that there is no significant interaction effect for gender and relationships between personality traits and locus of control. Model with interaction effect of gender fits the data F (11, 443) = 8.78; p < 0.001; however, introduction of gender to the model did not increase percentage of explained variance of locus of control (1%).
Table 5 presents the results of the multiple regression analysis in which personality traits were predictors and locus of control (of success and failure) were dependent variables. The regression model fits the data as follows: locus of control of success (F (5, 449) = 17.86, p < 0.001) and locus of control of failure (F (5, 449) = 17.87, p < 0.001). The regression coefficients show that locus of control of success is associated positively with Extroversion, Openness to Experience, and Conscientiousness. Locus of control of failure, on the other hand, is associated positively with Extroversion, Openness to Experience, and Agreeableness, and negatively with Neuroticism. All the coefficients indicate that there is a significant but weak relationship between personality traits and locus of control of success and failure. The model tested explains a low percentage of variance in each personality trait (R2 = 17%). The values of test of variance inflation factor (VIF) coefficient for all personality traits are less than 2, which means that there is low relationships between personality traits.

4. Discussion

There are no systematic studies on the associations between personality traits and locus of control in students entering adolescence, especially those who experience changes in educational arrangements, for instance, due to an educational reform [74,109]. Given this gap in research, we decided to carry out the study reported in this article.
In the first hypothesis (H1), we assumed that girls were more likely than boys to have higher levels of Openness to Experience, Neuroticism, and Conscientiousness. The results partially confirm this hypothesis. Costa and McCrae [27,28] have demonstrated that women, on average, have higher Neuroticism levels than men. It has been noted, however, that gender differences in this trait appear already in early adolescence [86,87,110], and our study confirms this finding. This is explained in many ways: among other things, by differences in socialization patterns between girls and boys, as well as the influence of culture and social expectations on the roles they play. Therefore, in Western cultures, the differences in Neuroticism between men and women are more pronounced, possibly due to the egalitarian division of roles between women and men. Women, who take on many different life roles, experience more stress [28,110,111,112,113]. As for the hypothesis that girls should score higher than boys on Openness to Experience and Conscientiousness, our results did not confirm it. Thus, they are in contrast to other studies, including those involving Polish youth [83,84,85]. It is worth noting, however, that the discrepancy between the results may be due to the fact that previous studies had been conducted before 2010, with adolescents who had not had the same educational experiences as the present participants, who had to face the challenges posed by the education reform of 2016. Moreover, personality was measured in those studies using different questionnaires, which makes it impossible to compare the results. In this present study, boys were more extroverted than girls. This finding is similar to that obtained by Branje et al. [114]. It is worth noting that, from a theoretical point of view, Extroversion includes an energy component connected with increased activity, positive emotions, and excitement seeking. This energy component can be seen as conceptually opposite to anhedonia, a central feature of depression [86]. It has been pointed out that in early adolescence, girls, compared to boys, are more at risk of having mood swings and depressive symptoms, which is due to neurohormonal changes. These changes may also explain the higher Extroversion levels in young boys [86,115,116].
Additional analyses conducted in reference to the second hypothesis, in which we assumed that girls are more likely than boys to have an external locus of control of success and failure, demonstrated that there were no differences between girls and boys in this trait. This observation is inconsistent with the results reported by Kulas [70] who showed, in a longitudinal study of younger adolescents, that girls, but not boys, experienced a shift towards a more external locus of control. Wade [117] also observed that girls were more external than boys, although his subjects were older adolescents. It is worth noting, however, that our results confirm those obtained by Manger and Eikeland [118], who found no differences in the locus of control between girls and boys aged 14 and 15.
In the third hypothesis, we assumed for the whole group, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness are associated with internal locus of control of successes and failures. Neuroticism is associated with external locus of control of successes and failures. Additionally, we presumed that gender is a moderator of relationships between personality traits and locus of control. The correlation analysis demonstrated that Extraversion was moderately positively correlated with an internal locus of control of successes and failures and generalized control in all the students surveyed, as was expected. Extraverted behaviours associated with the readiness to form interpersonal relationships and derive satisfaction from them contribute to the sense of agency and internal control over the events in which one participates. It has been pointed out that adolescence is a period of intensive development of social relationships [119], and Extraversion may positively influence the formation of an internal locus of control, because it enables gaining satisfaction from peer relationships. Moreover, extraverts are prone to seek rewarding situations. In addition to looking for and engaging in potentially rewarding situations, extraverted individuals derive satisfaction from their successes longer than do individuals with elevated Neuroticism [36,78,79]. In turn, it may lead to the development of self-efficacy and internal locus of control. Moreover, in this present study, weak negative correlations were observed between Neuroticism and the three types of locus of control. This result is consistent with previous studies, which indicate strong interdependence of neuroticism and external locus of control [120,121,122]. Most likely, heightened anxiety, which is a core of neuroticism, and a tendency to worry coexist with a shift towards a more external locus of control, because, when experiencing difficult emotions, people are more likely to resort to strategies protecting their self-image [86,121,123]. For this reason, they may fail to see their personal influence on the various events they participate in, and they are more likely to look for the causes of failure in external factors.
We found that Openness to Experience correlated moderately with locus of control of success and generalized locus control, and weakly, but significantly, with locus of control of failure. Openness to Experience has been demonstrated to promote the use of diverse problem-solving strategies and in-depth analysis of complex causes of events, human intentions, and feelings. The ability to change a strategy of action when it turns out to be ineffective increases the likelihood of success and may secondarily contribute to the development of an internal locus of control of success [96,124]. The ability to analyse the complex causes of failure may, in turn, foster introspection and allow individuals to contemplate the motives or intentions which have led to the unfavourable outcome.
In our study, Conscientiousness was found to correlate moderately with locus of control of success and generalized locus of control and weakly with locus of control of failure. Conscientiousness, which involves the ability to work hard and continue one’s efforts with patience, has been demonstrated to play an important role in the successful completion of compulsory education [125,126]. School is one of the basic areas of activity of young people, which is why educational successes and failures influence their quality of life [127,128,129]. The more conscientious a person is, the more likely they are to achieve success and then reap the benefits associated with it, such as maintaining positive affect, receiving social gratification (praise) from teachers or parents, etc. In this study, Conscientiousness was found to be conducive to developing an internal locus of control in situations associated with the achievement of success. This hypothesis was also confirmed.
It was expected that Agreeableness would correlate with an internal locus of control. The results indicated that Agreeableness, correlated weakly with locus of control of success and locus of control of failure, and moderately with a generalized locus of control. In light of the research, Agreeableness is broadly considered a key facet of adjustment, motivation to maintain positive interpersonal relations, mental health, and socioemotional competence [97]. In a longitudinal study of a sample of early adolescents, Jensen-Campbell et al. [130] showed that Agreeableness was associated with both peer acceptance and friendship. Moreover, it protected children from victimization by peers. Because the quality of peer relationships is an important component of general life satisfaction in young people, agreeable persons may find it easier to function in a peer group and are more likely to develop an internal locus of control [131].
The regression model with personality traits as predictors and locus of control as the explained variable was well fitted to the data and showed that Extraversion and Conscientiousness were important predictors of locus of control of success. On the other hand, locus of control of failure was significantly predicted by Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness (a positive predictor), and Neuroticism (a negative predictor). This result is in line with the studies quoted above which indicate that the individual personality traits play a role in the achievement of important educational and non-educational goals, and thus the development of a locus of control in students. Development of an internal locus of control of success is most strongly affected by Extraversion and Conscientiousness. These traits have the greatest impact on the quality of life of young people in two ways: Extraversion is conducive to deriving satisfaction from peer relationships, and Conscientiousness—from educational success. Both these areas are important aspects of young people’s everyday functioning [128,131]. In the case of failures, all traits, except for Neuroticism, were found to promote the development of an internal locus of control. This is probably due to the fact that increased anxiety, negative affect, and rumination do not foster a sense of responsibility for failure events, which may protect individuals against losing self-esteem [80,81]. Further analysis concerning gender revealed that there is no significant interaction effect for gender and relationships between personality traits and locus of control. Gender differentiates some personality traits but not the relationships between personality traits and locus of control.

5. Conclusions

The focus of this present study was on the analysis of personality traits and locus of control in students entering the period of early adolescence at a socially important moment of an introduction of a major educational reform in Poland.
The present results may be influenced by factors related to the introduction of the education reform of 2016 and the specific character of Polish educational policy and its social reception by young people. Nevertheless, we believe that they provide universal scientific insights into the psychosocial functioning of early adolescents experiencing changes in the education system. The present analyses have practical implications, as they can help specialists in education and psychologists understand the possible causes of young people’s adjustment difficulties or difficulties in meeting school requirements. They can also help students discover new ways of achieving important goals while fully using their personality dispositions and provide support in planning educational programmes aimed at teaching students responsibility and encouraging active participation in everyday educational and non-educational activities. Students who experience stress and are lost in a new educational situation may feel they have no control over what they experience, which may lead to the development of an external locus of control [74].
The present results can be used to plan measures and interventions aimed at supporting their adjustment to the new school environment and requirements. Education reforms are put in place all over Europe with varied success, as described in the literature [132,133,134,135,136,137,138,139,140,141]. Supposedly, the profile of scores obtained in young people in other countries could be different from the one obtained in this study. However, this only testifies to the potential of the present study and the possibility it provides of comparing the results in a wider context.

6. Limitations

An important limitation of our study is the fact that the participants were surveyed in only one city in eastern Poland. Therefore, the findings cannot be generalized to the whole population of students experiencing the changes imposed by the education reform in Poland. No detailed information was collected on the socioeconomic status of the students’ families and variables such as parental education or monthly income. These variables should be controlled for in a future study. Likewise, we did not control for parent–children relations, the current students’ achievement level, their sociometric status in the classroom, or their need for psychological support. These factors are important in successful adaptation to new environment and further educational outcomes. Results presented in this study pertain to two variables which are important in predicting academic success but are not the only ones underlying the quality of this process. In addition to taking into account the abovementioned variables in future research, it would also be interesting to examine students’ self-descriptions of their personality traits and compare them with assessments made by their parents and teachers. Studies of this type are very popular because they give a fuller picture of the personality functioning of young people seen from various perspectives [50,107]. With regard to locus of control, some studies show that it should be analysed in specific domains of young people’s life, such as the school environment or peer relationships [118]. In this present study, both successes and failures included a variety of events associated with the respondents’ school life and out-of-school activity. Future investigations should focus on particular spheres of functioning. Due to the interindividual differences in the changes that take place during early adolescence and the complex conditions of the development of personality traits, there is no universal signpost telling adolescents and their parents what life path to follow. Rather, it is young people themselves who begin to seek their own development path with increasing awareness and in keeping with their preferences and dispositions. Due to the fact that personality’s development is a long-term process, longitudinal research with at least a few waves during early, middle, and late adolescence period are necessary. To obtain a bigger and more accurate picture of these changes, future research should take into account other variables and analyse the relationships between them and the development of personality traits and locus of control in adolescents.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.F. and B.Ł.; methodology, S.F. and B.Ł.; software, S.F. and B.Ł.; validation, B.Ł. and S.F.; formal analysis, B.Ł. and S.F.; investigation, B.Ł. and S.F.; resources, S.F.; data curation, S.F.; writing—original draft preparation, S.F. and B.Ł.; writing—review and editing, S.F. and B.Ł.; visualization, B.Ł. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee for Scientific Research of University of Maria Curie-Skłodowska, Faculty of Education and Psychology (protocol code 29, 2019).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ustawa z Dnia 14 Grudnia 2016 r. Prawo OśWiatowe. Dziennik Ustaw 2017, Poz. 59. Available online: https://isap.sejm.gov.pl/isap.nsf/download.xsp/WDU20170000059/T/D20170059L.pdf (accessed on 10 January 2021).
  2. Sirsch, U. The impending transition from primary to secondary school: Challenge or threat? Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2003, 27, 385–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Anderson, L.W.; Jacobs, J.; Schramm, S.; Splittgerber, F. School transitions: Beginning of the end or a new beginning? Int. J. Educ. Res. 2000, 33, 325–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Borecka-Biernat, D. Coping strategies in adolescents in situations of social conflict and the locus of control. Pol. Forum Psychol. 2020, 25, 157–169. [Google Scholar]
  5. Dahl, R.E.; Gunnar, M.R. Heightened stress responsiveness and emotional reactivity during pubertal maturation: Implications for psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 2009, 21, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Pickhardt, C. Surviving Your Child’s Adolescence: How to Understand, and Even Enjoy, the Rocky Road to Independence; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  7. Strauss, R.S. Childhood obesity and self-esteem. Pediatrics 2000, 105, e15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Williams, J.M.; Currie, C. Self-esteem and physical development in early adolescence: Pubertal timing and body image. J. Early Adolesc. 2000, 20, 129–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Natsuaki, M.N.; Biehl, M.C.; Ge, X. Trajectories of depressed mood from early adolescence to young adulthood: The effects of pubertal timing and adolescent dating. J. Res. Adolesc. 2009, 19, 47–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hare, T.A.; Tottenham, N.; Galvan, A.; Voss, H.U.; Glover, G.H.; Casey, B.J. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 63, 927–934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Block, J.; Robins, R.W. A longitudinal study of consistency and change in self-esteem from early adolescence to early adulthood. Child Dev. 1993, 64, 909–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Spear, L.P. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2000, 24, 417–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Casey, B.J.; Jones, R.M. Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: Implications for substance use disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2010, 49, 1189–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cohen, P.; Chen, H.; Crawford, T.N.; Brook, J.S.; Gordon, K. Personality disorders in early adolescence and the development of later substance use disorders in the general population. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007, 88, S71–S84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Corbett, B.A.; Muscatello, R.A.; Horrocks, B.K.; Klemencic, M.E.; Tanguturi, Y. Differences in Body Mass Index (BMI) in Early Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder Compared to Youth with Typical Development. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Casey, B.J.; Getz, S.; Galvan, A. The adolescent brain. Dev. Rev. 2008, 28, 62–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Heaven, P.C.L.; Ciarrochi, J. When IQ is not everything: Intelligence, personality and academic performance at school. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 53, 518–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Mendolia, S.; Walker, I. Youth unemployment and personality traits. IZA J. Labor Econ. 2015, 4, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Ng-Knight, T.; Schoon, I. Can locus of control compensate for socioeconomic adversity in the transition from school to work? J. Youth Adolesc. 2017, 46, 2114–2128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Karbach, J.; Gottschling, J.; Spengler, M.; Hegewald, K.; Spinath, F.M. Parental involvement and general cognitive ability as predictors of domain-specific academic achievement in early adolescence. Learn. Instr. 2013, 23, 43–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Veas, A.; Castejón, J.L.; Gilar, R.; Miñano, P. Academic achievement in early adolescence: The influence of cognitive and non-cognitive variables. J. Gen. Psychol. 2015, 142, 273–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Larson, R.; Ham, M. Stress and “storm and stress” in early adolescence: The relationship of negative events with dysphoric affect. Dev. Psychol. 1993, 29, 130–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Gomez, R. Locus of control and type A behavior pattern as predictors of coping styles among adolescents. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1997, 23, 391–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Larsen, R.J. Neuroticism and selective encoding and recall of symptoms: Evidence from a combined concurrent-retrospective study. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 62, 480–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hoffman, M.A.; Levy-Shiff, R.; Malinski, D. Stress and adjustment in the transition to adolescence: Moderating effects of neuroticism and extroversion. J. Youth Adolesc. 1996, 25, 161–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kardum, I.; Krapić, N. Personality traits, stressful life events, and coping styles in early adolescence. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2001, 30, 503–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Costa, P.T., Jr.; McCrae, R.R. A contemplated revision of the NEO Five-Factor Inventory. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2004, 36, 587–596. [Google Scholar]
  28. Costa, P.T.; McCrae, R.R. Personality in adulthood: A six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO Personality Inventory. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 54, 853–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. McCrae, R.R.; Costa, P.T. Personality in Adulthood: A Five-Factor Theory Perspective, 2nd ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  30. McCrae, R.R.; Costa, P.T. Toward a new generation of personality theories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. In The Five-Factor Model Personality: Theoretical Perspectives; Wiggins, J., Ed.; The Guildford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 51–87. [Google Scholar]
  31. DeYoung, C.G.; Peterson, J.B.; Higgins, D.M. Sources of openness/intellect: Cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. J. Personal. 2005, 73, 825–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Corr, P.J.; DeYoung, C.G.; McNaughton, N. Motivation and personality: A neuropsychological perspective. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2013, 7, 158–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Bouchard, T.J., Jr.; Loehin, J.C. Genes, evolution, and personality. Behav. Genet. 2001, 31, 243–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Gartstein, M.A.; Rothbart, M.K. Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav. Dev. 2003, 26, 64–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hane, A.A.; Fox, N.A.; Henderson, H.A.; Marshall, P.J. Behavioral reactivity and approach-withdrawal bias in infancy. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 44, 1491–1496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Caspi, A.; Silva, P.A. Temperamental qualities at age three predict personality traits in young adulthood. Longitudinal evidence from birth cohort. Child Dev. 1995, 66, 486–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Costa, P.T., Jr.; McCrae, R.R. Stability and change in personality from adolescence through adulthood. In The Developing Structure Temperament and Personality from Infancy to Adulthood; Halverson, C.F., Jr., Kohnstamm, G.A., Martin, R.P., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1994; pp. 139–150. [Google Scholar]
  38. Specht, J.; Egloff, B.; Schmukle, S.C. Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 101, 862–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Srivastava, S.; John, O.P.; Gosling, S.D.; Potter, J. Development of personality in early and middle adulthood: Set like plaster or persistent change? J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 84, 1041–1053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Scollon, C.N.; Diener, E. Love, work, and changes in extraversion and neuroticism over time. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 91, 1152–1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Göllner, R.; Roberts, B.W.; Damian, R.I.; Lüdtke, O.; Jonkmann, K.; Trautwein, U. Whose “storm and stress” is it? Parent and child reports of personality development in the transition to early adolescence. J. Personal. 2017, 85, 376–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Denissen, J.J.; van Aken, M.A.; Penke, L.; Wood, D. Self-regulation underlies temperament and personality: An integrative developmental framework. Child Dev. Perspect. 2013, 7, 255–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Van den Akker, A.L.; Deković, M.; Asscher, J.; Prinzie, P. Mean-level personality development across childhood and adolescence: A temporary defiance of the maturity principle and bidirectional associations with parenting. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 107, 736–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Shiner, R.L. Linking childhood personality with adaptation: Evidence for continuity and change across time into late adolescence. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 78, 310–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Strachey, J. The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud. New Introductory Lectures on Psycho-Analysis and Other Work; The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psycho-Analysis: London, UK, 1964; pp. 1–267. [Google Scholar]
  46. Measelle, J.R.; John, O.P.; Ablow, J.C.; Cowan, P.A.; Cowan, C.P. Can children provide coherent, stable, and valid self-reports on the big five dimensions? A longitudinal study from ages 5 to 7. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 89, 90–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Suldo, S.M.; Minch, D.R.; Hearon, B.V. Adolescent life-satisfaction and personality characteristics: Investigating Relationships Using a Five Factor Model. J. Happiness Stud. 2015, 16, 965–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Weber, M.; Huebner, S. Early adolescents’ personality and life satisfaction: A closer look at global vs. domain-specific satisfaction. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 83, 31–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Marcionetti, J.; Rossier, J. Global life satisfaction in adolescence: The role of personality traits, self-esteem, and self-efficacy. J. Individ. Differ. 2016, 37, 135–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Branje, S.J.; Van Lieshout, C.F.; Gerris, J.R. Big Five personality development in adolescence and adulthood. Eur. J. Personal. 2007, 21, 45–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Pullmann, H.; Raudsepp, L.; Allik, J. Stability and change in adolescents’ personality: A longitudinal study. Eur. J. Personal. 2006, 20, 447–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Roskam, I.; Nils, F. Predicting intra-individual academic achievement trajectories of adolescents nested in class environment: Influence of motivation, implicit theory of intelligence, self-esteem and parenting. Psychol. Belg. 2007, 47, 119–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Graziano, W.G.; Ward, D. Probing the Big Five in Adolescence: Personality and Adjustment during a Developmental Transition. J. Personal. 1992, 60, 425–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Gullone, E.; Moore, S. Adolescent Risk-Taking and the Five Factor Model of Personality. J. Adolesc. 2000, 23, 393–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Medvedova, L. Personality dimensions—“Little five”—and their relationships with coping strategies in early adolescence. Studia Psychol. 1998, 40, 261–265. [Google Scholar]
  56. Krampen, G. Perceived childrearing practices and the development of locus of control in early adolescence. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 1989, 12, 177–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bang, H.; Chang, M.; Lee, C.; Kim, S.; Taliaferro, A. Sport Participation, Locus of Control, and Academic Performance among Early Adolescents: Racial and Linguistic Status Differences. Sociol. Spectr. 2019, 39, 20–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Rotter, J.B. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol. Monogr. 1966, 80, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Lefcourt, H.M. Locus control. In Measures Social Psychological Attitudes; Robinson, J.P., Shaver, P.R., Wrightsman, L.S., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1991; Volume 1, pp. 413–499. [Google Scholar]
  60. Drwal, R.Ł. Poczucie kontroli jako wymiar osobowości-podstawy teoretyczne, techniki badawcze i wyniki badań. In Adaptacja Kwestionariuszy Osobowości. Wybrane Zagadnienia i Techniki; Drwal, R.Ł., Brzozowski, P., Oleś, P., Eds.; PWN: Warsaw, Poland, 1995; pp. 199–227. [Google Scholar]
  61. Ajzen, I. Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 32, 665–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Vinokur, A.; Seizer, M.L. Desirable versus undesirable life events: The relationship to stress and mental disorders. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1975, 39, 329–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Dubois, N.; Beavois, J.L. The social value of internal explanations and the norm of internality. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2008, 2, 1737–1752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Bressoux, P.; Pansu, P. Norme d’internalité et activités évaluatives en milieu scolaire. Revue Française de Pédagogie 1998, 122, 19–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Bressoux, P.; Pansu, P. Effet de Contexte, Valeur d’Internalité et Jugement Scolaire. L’orientation Sc. Et Prof. 2001, 30. Available online: https://journals.openedition.org/osp/5133#text (accessed on 31 October 2020). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Pansu, P.; Dubois, N.; Dompnier, B. Internality-norm theory in educational contexts. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2008, 23, 385–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Dubois, N.; Le Poultier, F. Internalité et évaluation scolaire. Perspect. Cogn. Et Conduites Soc. 1991, 3, 153–166. [Google Scholar]
  68. Chubb, N.H.; Fertman, C.I.; Ross, J.L. Adolescent self-esteem and locus of control: A longitudinal study of gender and age differences. Adolescence 1997, 32, 113–129. [Google Scholar]
  69. Kaya, A. Sociometric status, depression, and locus of control among Turkish early adolescents. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 2007, 35, 1405–1414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Kulas, H. Locus of control in adolescence: A longitudinal study. Adolescence 1996, 31, 721–729. [Google Scholar]
  71. Huebner, E.S.; Ash, C.H.; Laughlin, J.E. Life experiences, locus of control, and school satisfaction in adolescence. Soc. Indic. Res. 2001, 55, 167–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Butler-Sweeney, J. The Relationship among Locus of Control, Coping Style, Self-Esteem and Cultural Identification in Female Adolescents. 2007. Available online: https://scholarship.shu.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=2664&context=dissertations (accessed on 14 December 2020).
  73. Cazan, A.M.; Dumitrescu, S.A. Exploring the relationship between adolescent resilience, self-perception and locus of control. Rom. J. Exp. Appl. Psychol. 2016, 7, 283–286. [Google Scholar]
  74. Cauce, A.M.; Hannan, K.; Sargeant, M. Life stress, social support, and locus of control during early adolescence: Interactive effects. Am. J. Community Psychol. 1992, 20, 787–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Gomez, R.; Holmberg, K.; Bounds, J.; Fullarton, C.; Gomez, A. Neuroticism and extraversion as predictors of coping styles during early adolescence. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1999, 27, 3–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Schneider, T.R.; Rench, T.A.; Lyons, J.B.; Riffle, R.R. The influence of neuroticism, extraversion and openness on stress responses. Stress Health 2012, 28, 102–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Fishman, I.; Ng, R.; Bellugi, U. Do extraverts process social stimuli differently from introverts? Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 2, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Gallagher, D.J. Extraversion, neuroticism and appraisal of stressful academic events. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1990, 11, 1053–1057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Gallagher, D.J. Personality, coping, and objective outcomes: Extraversion, neuroticism, coping styles, and academic performance. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1996, 21, 421–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Hervas, G.; Vazquez, C. What else do you feel when you feel sad? Emotional overproduction, neuroticism and rumination. Emotion 2011, 11, 881–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Slavish, D.C.; Sliwinski, M.J.; Smyth, J.M.; Almeida, D.M.; Lipton, R.B.; Katz, M.J.; Graham-Engeland, J.E. Neuroticism, rumination, negative affect, and sleep: Examining between-and within-person associations. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2018, 123, 217–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Limura, S.; Taku, K. Gender differences in relationship between resilience and big five personality traits in Japanese adolescents. Psychol. Rep. 2018, 121, 920–931. [Google Scholar]
  83. Klimstra, T.A.; Hale, W.W., III; Raaijmakers, Q.A.; Branje, S.J.; Meeus, W.H. Maturation of personality in adolescence. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 96, 898–912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Lickiewicz, J. Cechy osobowości i inteligencja emocjonalna uczniów liceum a profil klasy. Ann. Univ. Mariae Curie-Skłodowska Sectjo J. Pedagog. Psychol. 2005, 18, 47–58. [Google Scholar]
  85. Czerniawska, E. “Wielka Piątka” a aktywność strategiczna i osiągnięcia w uczeniu się uczniów gimnazjum i liceum. Psychol. Rozw. 2008, 13, 71–84. [Google Scholar]
  86. De Bolle, M.; De Fruyt, F.; MaCrae, R.R.; Löckenhoff, C.E.; Costa, P.T., Jr.; Aguilar-Vafaie, M.E.; Ahn, C.K.; Ahn, H.N.; Alcalay, L.; Allik, J.; et al. The emergence of sex differences in personality traits in early adolescence: A cross-sectional, cross-cultural study. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 108, 171–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  87. Soto, C.J. The little six personality dimensions from early childhood to early adulthood: Mean-level age and gender differences in parents’ reports. J. Personal. 2016, 84, 409–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Akhtar, A.; Saxena, S. Gender differences in locus of control. Indian J. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 5, 45–49. [Google Scholar]
  89. Sherman, A.C.; Higgs, G.E.; Williams, R.L. Gender differences in the locus of control construct. Psychol. Health 1997, 12, 239–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Brooks-Gunn, J.; Reiter, E.O. The role of pubertal processes. In At the Threshold: The Developing Adolescent; Feldman, S.S., Elliott, G.R., Eds.; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1990; pp. 16–53. [Google Scholar]
  91. Murdey, I.D.; Cameron, N.; Biddle, S.J.; Marshall, S.J.; Gorely, T. Pubertal development and sedentary behaviour during adolescence. Ann. Hum. Biol. 2004, 31, 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Furnham, A.; Monsen, J. Personality traits and intelligence predict academic school grades. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2009, 19, 28–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. John, O.P.; Caspi, A.; Robins, R.W.; Moffitt, T.E.; Stouthamer-Loeber, M. The “Little Five”: Exploring the nomological network of the five-factor model of personality in adolescent boys. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 160–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Gutiérrez, J.L.G.; Jiménez, B.M.; Hernández, E.G.; Puente, C.P. Personality and subjective well-being: Big five correlates and demographic variables. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2005, 38, 1561–1569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Tanksale, D. Big Five Personality Traits: Are they really important for the subjective well-being of Indians? Int. J. Psychol. 2017, 50, 64–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. McCrae, R.R.; Costa, P.T. Conceptions and correlates of openness to experience. In Handbook Personality Psychology; Hogan, R., Johnson, J., Briggs, S., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1997; pp. 825–847. [Google Scholar]
  97. Kochanska, G.; Kim, S. Children’s early difficulty and agreeableness in adolescence: Testing a developmental model of interplay of parent and child effects. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 1556–1564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Maćkiewicz, M.; Cieciuch, J. Jak mierzyć cechy Wielkiej Piątki u dzieci? Prace nad Obrazkowym Pomiarem Cech Osobowości Dzieci (OPCO-D). Psychol. Rozw. 2012, 17, 69–82. [Google Scholar]
  99. Maćkiewicz, M.; Cieciuch, J. Pictorial Personality Traits Questionnaire for Children (PPTQ-C)—A New Measure of Children’s Personality Traits. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. Krasowicz, G.; Kurzyp-Wojnarska, A. Kwestionariusz do Badania Poczucia Kontroli (KBPK). Podręcznik; Polskie Towarzystwo Psychologiczne: Warsaw, Poland, 1990; pp. 1–39. [Google Scholar]
  101. Barabanelli, C.; Caprara, G.V.; Rabasca, A.; Pastorelli, C. A Questionnaire for Measuring the Big Five in Late Childhood. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2003, 34, 645–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Bleidorn, W.; Ostendorf, F. Ein Big Five-Inventarfür Kinder und Jungendliche. Diagnostica 2009, 55, 160–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Olivier, M.; Herve, M. The Big Five Questionnaire for Children (BFQ-C): A French validation on 8- to 14-year-old children. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 87, 55–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Kokkinos, C.M.; Markos, A. The Big Five Questionnaire for Children (BFQ-C). Factorial invariance across sex and age in a Greek sample of preadolescents. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2015, 33, 1–5. [Google Scholar]
  105. Del Barrio, V.; Carrasco, M.; Holgado, F. Factor Structure Invariance in the Children’s Big Five Questionnaire. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2006, 22, 158–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Caspi, A.; Roberts, B.W.; Shiner, R. Personality development: Stability and change. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2005, 56, 453–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Piaget, J. Understanding Causality; WW Norton Company: New York, NY, USA, 1974. [Google Scholar]
  108. Krasowicz-Kupis, G.; Wojnarska, A. Kwestionariusz do Badania Poczucia Kontroli—Wersja Zrewidowana. Podręcznik; Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP: Warsaw, Poland, 2017; pp. 5–37. [Google Scholar]
  109. Swearingen, E.M.; Cohen, L. Measurement of adolescents’ life events: The Junior High Life Experiences Survey. Am. J. Community Psychol. 1985, 13, 69–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Sharma, B.B. Gender differences in adolescent neuroticism. J. Humanit. Soc. Sci. 2013, 9, 118–122. [Google Scholar]
  111. Soto, C.J.; John, O.P.; Gosling, S.D.; Potter, J. Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big-Five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 330–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  112. McCrae, R.R.; Terracciano, A. Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 88, 547–561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  113. Bleidorn, W.; Klimstra, T.A.; Denissen, J.J.; Rentfrow, P.J.; Potter, J.; Gosling, S.D. Personality maturation around the world: A cross-cultural examination of social-investment theory. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 24, 2530–2540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Branje, S.J.; van Lieshout, C.F.; van Aken, M.A. Relations between Big Five personality characteristics and perceived support in adolescents’ families. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 86, 615–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  115. Saluja, G.; Iachan, R.; Scheidt, P.C.; Overpeck, M.D.; Sun, W.; Giedd, J.N. Prevalence of and risk factors for depressive symptoms among young adolescents. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2004, 158, 760–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  116. Christodoulou, G.; Majmundar, A.; Chou, C.P.; Pentz, M.A. Anhedonia, screen time, and substance use in early adolescents: A longitudinal mediation analysis. J. Adolesc. 2020, 78, 24–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Wade, J.T. An examination of locus of control/fatalism for Blacks, Whites, boys, and girls over a two-year period of adolescence. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 1996, 24, 239–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Manger, T.; Eikeland, O.J. On the relationship between locus of control, level of ability and gender. Scand. J. Psychol. 2000, 41, 225–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Underwood, M.K.; Rosen, L.H. Social Development: Relationships in Infancy, Childhood, and Adolescence; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  120. Joe, V.C. Review of the internal–external control construct as a personality variable. Psychol. Rep. 1971, 28, 619–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Judge, T.A.; Erez, A.; Bono, J.E.; Thoresen, C.J. Are measures of self-esteem, neuroticism, locus of control, and generalized self-efficacy indicators of a common core construct? J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 83, 693–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Vittengl, J.R. Who pays the price for high neuroticism? Moderators of longitudinal risks for depression and anxiety. Psychol. Med. 2017, 47, 1794–1805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  123. Furnham, A. Lay understandings of defence mechanisms: The role of personality traits and gender. Psychol. Health Med. 2012, 17, 723–734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. George, J.M.; Zhou, J. When openness to experience and conscientiousness are related to creative behavior: An interactional approach. J. Appl. Psychol. 2001, 86, 513–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Richardson, M.; Abraham, C. Conscientiousness and achievement motivation predict performance. Eur. J. Personal. 2009, 23, 589–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Dumfart, B.; Neubauer, A.C. Conscientiousness is the most powerful noncognitive predictor of school achievement in adolescents. J. Individ. Differ. 2016, 37, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Wang, M.T.; Holcombe, R. Adolescents’ perceptions of school environment, engagement, and academic achievement in middle school. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2010, 47, 633–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Rezende, L.F.M.D.; Azeredo, C.M.; Silva, K.S.; Claro, R.M.; França-Junior, I.; Peres, M.F.T.; Eluf-Neto, J. The role of school environment in physical activity among Brazilian adolescents. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0131342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  129. González-Carrasco, M.; Sáez, M.; Casas, F. Subjective Well-Being in Early Adolescence: Observations from a Five-Year Longitudinal Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 8249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Jensen-Campbell, L.A.; Adams, R.; Perry, D.G.; Workman, K.A.; Furdella, J.Q.; Egan, S.K. Agreeableness, extraversion, and peer relations in early adolescence: Winning friends and deflecting aggression. J. Res. Personal. 2002, 36, 224–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Oberle, E.; Schonert-Reichl, K.A.; Zumbo, B.D. Life satisfaction in early adolescence: Personal, neighborhood, school, family, and peer influences. J. Youth Adolesc. 2011, 40, 889–901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Alfirević, N.; Pavičić, J.; Relja, R. School management innovation and principal support systems: Toward the agenda for Croatian school reform. Econ. Res. 2016, 29, 1150–1164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  133. Bertola, G.; Checchi, D. Sorting and private education in Italy. In Education, Training and Labour Market Outcomes in Europe; Bertola, G., Checchi, D., Eds.; Palgrave Macmillan: London, UK, 2004; pp. 69–108. [Google Scholar]
  134. Birzea, C. Educational reform and power struggles in Romania. Eur. J. Educ. 1996, 31, 97–107. [Google Scholar]
  135. Boyd-Barrett, O. Educational reform in Spain and in the UK: A comparative perspective. In Education Reform in Contemporary Spain; Boyd-Barret, O., O’Malley, P., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2002; pp. 65–78. [Google Scholar]
  136. Brauns, H.; Steinmann, S. Educational reform in France, West-Germany and the United Kingdom: Updating the CASMIN educational classification. Zuma Nachr. 1999, 23, 7–44. [Google Scholar]
  137. Hanson, E.M. School-Based Management and Educational Reform: Cases in the USA and Spain. 1998. Available online: https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED336832.pdf (accessed on 13 October 2020).
  138. Koc, Y.; Isiksal, M.; Bulut, S. Elementary school curriculum reform in Turkey. Int. Educ. J. 2007, 8, 30–39. [Google Scholar]
  139. Koustourakis, G. The new educational policy for the reform of the curriculum and the change of school knowledge in the case of Greek compulsory education. Int. Stud. Sociol. Educ. 2007, 17, 131–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Mitter, W. Educational reform in West and East Germany in European perspective. Oxf. Rev. Educ. 1990, 16, 333–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Remesal, A. Educational reform and primary and secondary teachers’ conceptions of assessment: The Spanish instance, building upon Black and Wiliam. Curric. J. 2007, 18, 27–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Descriptive statistics, Student’s t-test, and effect size for boys’ (n = 223) and girls’ (n = 232) PBPS-C scores.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics, Student’s t-test, and effect size for boys’ (n = 223) and girls’ (n = 232) PBPS-C scores.
Personality TraitBoysGirlstpHedge’s g
MSDMSD
Extraversion12.022.4211.482.962.1060.036 **0.20
Neuroticism7.952.908.962.48−3.9990.001 ***0.37
Openness to Experience9.282.679.542.54−1.0850.2780.10
Conscientiousness10.142.759.922.670.8700.3850.08
Agreeableness11.102.4511.502.450.8690.0810.16
Note: ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics, Student’s t-test and effect size for boys’ (n = 223) and girls’ (n = 232) LOCQ scores.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics, Student’s t-test and effect size for boys’ (n = 223) and girls’ (n = 232) LOCQ scores.
Locus of ControlBoysGirlstp
MSDMSD
Success12.022.8411.832.850.6960.487
Failure11.803.6111.473.430.9950.320
Generalized23.815.6123.285.491.0130.312
Table 3. Results of analysis of correlations between PBPS-C and LOCQ scores for the entire sample (n = 455).
Table 3. Results of analysis of correlations between PBPS-C and LOCQ scores for the entire sample (n = 455).
Personality TraitLocus of Control
SuccessFailureGeneralized
ExtraversionPearson’s r0.32 **0.34 **0.33 **
p0.0010.0010.001
NeuroticismPearson’s r−0.23 **−0.24 **−0.27 **
p0.0010.0010.001
Openness to ExperiencePearson’s r0.31 **0.25 **0.30 **
p0.0010.0010.001
ConscientiousnessPearson’s r0.32 **0.17 **0.27 **
p0.0010.0010.001
AgreeablenessPearson’s r0.21 **0.28 **0.30 **
p0.0010.0010.001
Note: ** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Results of regression analysis with gender as a moderator of relationships between personality traits and locus of control in the situations of successes and failures.
Table 4. Results of regression analysis with gender as a moderator of relationships between personality traits and locus of control in the situations of successes and failures.
PredictorsLocus of Control
SuccessFailure
β SEtp β SEtp
Model 1Constance 1.125.5230.001 *** 1.383.4900.001 ***
Gender0.010.120.2160.8290.030.160.7620.446
Extraversion0.130.052.7300.007 **0.150.063.2690.001 ***
Neuroticism−0.080.05−1.6760.094−0.110.06−2.2930.022 **
Openness to Experience0.140.053.0150.003 **0.160.063.3960.001 ***
Conscientiousness0.230.055.0090.001 ***0.040.060.9470.344
Agreeableness0.080.051.6870.0920.200.074.1830.001 ***
Model 2Constance 1.135.4570.001 *** 1.343.6340.001 ***
Extroversion0.110.052.3230.021 **0.130.062.8000.005 **
Neuroticism−0.060.05−1.3110.191−0.110.06−2.2740.023 **
Openness to Experience0.140.053.0450.002 **0.170.063.5770.001 ***
Conscientiousness0.240.055.1210.001 ***0.050.060.9960.320
Agreeableness0.080.051.7080.0880.190.074.1140.001 ***
Gender0.010.130.3180.7510.040.160.8030.422
Gender × Extraversion−0.080.13−1.7920.074−0.030.17−0.5590.577
Gender × Neuroticism−0.010.14−0.1730.8630.080.171.6840.093
Gender × Openness to Experience0.030.130.6130.5400.050.161.1230.262
Gender × Conscientiousness−0.030.13−0.6070.5440.0010.170.0020.998
Gender × Agreeableness−0.060.13−1.2390.216−0.0030.17−0.0650.948
R2 = 0.17; ΔR2 = 0.01R2 = 0.18; ΔR2 = 0.01
Note: ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Results of regression analysis with personality traits as predictors and locus of control as dependent variable.
Table 5. Results of regression analysis with personality traits as predictors and locus of control as dependent variable.
Locus of Control
Personality TraitsSuccessFailure
β SEtpVIF β SEtpVIF
Extraversion0.120.052.7610.006 **1.100.150.063.3460.001 ***1.10
Neuroticism−0.080.05−1.7470.0811.13−0.120.06−2.4750.014 *1.13
Openness to Experience0.140.053.0100.003 **1.210.150.063.3500.001 ***1.21
Conscientiousness0.230.055.0210.001 ***1.240.050.060.9670.3341.24
Agreeableness0.080.051.6750.0951.140.190.074.1230.001 ***1.14
R2 = 0.17Adj R2 = 0.17 R2 = 0.17Adj R2 = 0.17
Note: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Filipiak, S.; Łubianka, B. On the Rocky Road to Independence: Big Five Personality Traits and Locus of Control in Polish Primary School Students during Transition into Early Adolescence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 4564. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18094564

AMA Style

Filipiak S, Łubianka B. On the Rocky Road to Independence: Big Five Personality Traits and Locus of Control in Polish Primary School Students during Transition into Early Adolescence. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(9):4564. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18094564

Chicago/Turabian Style

Filipiak, Sara, and Beata Łubianka. 2021. "On the Rocky Road to Independence: Big Five Personality Traits and Locus of Control in Polish Primary School Students during Transition into Early Adolescence" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 9: 4564. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18094564

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop