Next Article in Journal
Decrease in All-Cause 30-Day Mortality after Bacteraemia over a 15-Year Period: A Population-Based Cohort Study in Denmark in 2000–2014
Previous Article in Journal
Comparison of Official and Friendly Matches through Acceleration, Deceleration and Metabolic Power Measures: A Full-Season Study in Professional Soccer Players
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Association of Meteorological Factors with Cognitive Function in Older Adults

1
School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518052, China
2
Institute of Mental Health, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518052, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(11), 5981; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115981
Submission received: 6 May 2021 / Revised: 25 May 2021 / Accepted: 27 May 2021 / Published: 2 June 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Aging)

Abstract

:
Individual and meteorological factors are associated with cognitive function in older adults. However, how these two factors interact with each other to affect cognitive function in older adults is still unclear. We used mixed effects models to assess the association of individual and meteorological factors with cognitive function among older adults. Individual data in this study were from the database of China Family Panel Studies. A total of 3448 older adults from 25 provinces were included in our analysis. Cognitive functions were measured using a memory test and a logical sequence test. We used the meteorological data in the daily climate dataset of China’s surface international exchange stations, and two meteorological factors (i.e., average temperature and relative humidity) were assessed. The empty model showed significant differences in the cognitive scores of the older adults across different provinces. The results showed a main impact of residence (i.e., urban or rural) and a significant humidity–residence interaction on memory performance in older adults. Specifically, the negative association between humidity and memory performance was more pronounced in urban areas. This study suggested that meteorological factors may, in concert with individual factors, be associated with differences in memory function in older adults.

1. Introduction

The aging population has rapidly increased worldwide. By 2050, the global population aged 60 years and older is expected to total 2 billion, up from 900 million in 2015 [1]. Normal or pathological (e.g., mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease) cognitive decline associated with aging is often accompanied by reduced quality of life [2,3,4]. Moreover, cognitive impairment in older age has caused substantial financial and societal burdens. For example, direct and indirect costs for individuals with cognitive impairment have reached nearly $244 billion annually worldwide [5]. Thus, a better understanding of the factors that influence the speed of the cognitive decline over aging is warranted.
Multiple factors have been associated with cognitive function, such as age, gender, educational and nutritional level, socio-economic status, physical activity/sedentary behaviors, and chronic disease conditions. Some researchers have investigated the preventable effects of lifestyle behaviors (e.g., physical activity and prolonged sitting) on cognitive function in older age, showing that adopting healthy behaviors reduced the speed of the cognitive decline across aging [6,7,8]. In addition to these individual and lifestyle risk factors, meteorological and environmental factors have also attracted scientific attention.
Previous studies have shown that environmental factors such as urbanization and traffic (e.g., highway construction) have been associated with cognitive function in older age [9,10,11,12]. For example, living in cities appears to reduce the risk of cognitive impairment, although the exact reasons are unclear [12,13]. This is not only because of the individual factors (e.g., higher occupational complexity) of urban elderly, notably, it has been suggested that the effect of this environmental factor on cognitive function can be explained by its influence on microclimate and ecology. Indeed, studies have shown that meteorological factors such as precipitation, sunshine, and other natural phenomena related to atmospheric movement may influence cognitive function [14,15,16]. In particular, among these multiple meteorological indicators, temperature (i.e., air temperature) and humidity have been highlighted because they are thought to represent the most stable, common, and sensitive meteorological dimensions [17,18]. Other meteorological indicators such as ground temperature and atmospheric pressure are essentially following the variation of temperature and humidity, so they have serious multicollinearity problems. Therefore, it is meaningful to explore the influence of temperature and humidity on cognitive function in older age.
However, evidence of the associations of temperature and humidity with age-related cognitive decline is mixed. Some studies suggest that extreme (either cold or heat) temperature influences the rate of cognitive decline across aging (e.g., alertness, decision-making ability, and memory) [19,20]. However, other studies find no evidence of an association between temperature and cognitive function [21,22]. Moreover, some studies point out that humidity is a risk factor for the physiological and cognitive function in old age [23,24], but related research is still lacking. This lack of consistency may result from various factors. First, most studies have only investigated the influence of a single meteorological factor on cognitive function, which may have biased the associations observed. Second, study often relied on small sample sizes (n < 30) [21,22]. Third, the statistical approach used in previous studies (i.e., traditional regression model, which ignores the nesting of data, greatly reducing the practical significance of the research results) was not particularly suited to accurately estimate the associations between meteorological factors (i.e., a macro level factor) and cognitive functions (a micro level factor), while accounting for individual factors. It has been pointed out in the literature that meteorological factors, such as temperature and humidity, have different effects on demographic factors (e.g., gender and age) [25,26,27]. Therefore, it is necessary to explore the interaction between meteorological factors and individual factors.
To fill this knowledge gap, the objective of the current study was to investigate, using an analytical strategy suited to examine the multiple levels of influence (i.e., micro and macro), the associations of temperature and relative humidity with the level of and change in cognitive function across aging. We used the monthly average temperature and relative humidity of each province in China to measure the meteorological factors. Cognitive functions were measured using cognitive tests. Age, gender, residence (urban or rural), and chronic disease (e.g., diabetes and hypertension) in older age were used as individual variables. In addition, as mentioned above, the traffic level influences people’s cognitive function through its impact on microclimate and ecology [9,10], and therefore, we use highway construction as a covariate.
We used a mixed model, as shown in Figure 1, to test the hypotheses that the variables at the cross-level interactions had main and moderating effects on cognitive function in older age. We hypothesized that individual factors would be associated with cognitive function level and change across aging (H1a and H1b). Moreover, we hypothesized that temperature and humidity would be associated with cognitive function level and change across aging (H2a and H2b). Finally, we hypothesized that the association between individual factors and cognitive functions’ level of and change across aging would be moderated by temperature and humidity (H3a and H3b).

2. Methods

2.1. Data Sources and Participants

In this study, data were extracted from the China Family Panel Studies (CFPS), a national large-scale screening dataset with provinces (or autonomous regions/municipalities) as the unit. Cognitive function was assessed in 2016 using a battery of tests, including a memory test and a logical sequence test. Geographic location and interview date were used to accurately match cognitive scores with the local meteorological data during the month cognitive tests were administered. Specifically, the meteorological data in this study were from the daily climate dataset of China’s surface international exchange stations (Surface_Climate Data_China_ Multiple Elements_Daily Value Data_Climate Exchange Station, SCCMDC, V3.0). The dataset contained daily records of meteorological conditions from 166 monitoring stations in China. As the covariate, highway data in traffic came from the China Statistical Yearbook of 2017, the data of which are compiled by the National Bureau of statistics of China (http://www.stats.gov.cn, 22 February 2021).
We included data for participants aged 60 years or above, who provide demographic information including gender, age, residence, years of education (years of education ≤6 means having received basic education, excluding illiteracy, and >6 means reaching the level of junior high school or above) and chronic diseases (excluding mild cognitive impairment and different forms of dementia), and who completed the cognitive tests. We excluded participants who did not have information on the interview date and on their provinces of living. A total of 3448 older adults from 25 provinces (or autonomous regions/municipalities) participated in the study. The demographic information of all the participants is shown Table 1.

2.2. Variable Measurement

2.2.1. Individual Level Variables

The individual level variables were age, gender, residence (i.e., urban or rural), and chronic disease. The information was extracted from the demographic information recorded in the CFPS. In this study, gender, residence, and chronic diseases were used as categorical variables, and age was used as a continuous variable.

2.2.2. Provincial Level Variables

At the provincial level, we mainly focused on two meteorological variables, monthly average temperature and relative humidity in 2016. The statistical regulations were as follows: (1) the daily average temperature and relative humidity were the average values of four time observations (2:00 a.m., 8:00 a.m., 2:00 p.m., 8:00 p.m.) per day for a month; (2) when a certain fixed time value was not measured, the daily average value of corresponding elements was not measured.
The hourly observation data from the ground automatic station uploaded in real time had passed the station quality control. The National Meteorological Information Center of China inputs the observation data and station quality control codes uploaded in real time into the real-time database for data service (http://data.cma.cn, 9 September 2020).
We matched provincial level meteorological data with CFPS samples in the following way. First, we extracted the month and the location (on the provincial level) the participants were interviewed from the CFPS. Second, the monthly average values of temperature and humidity of each province in the corresponding month were extracted from SCCMDC. Finally, meteorological data were matched with the cognitive scores of subjects in CFPS according to dates and provinces.

2.2.3. Outcome Variables

The memory test and the logical sequence test in CFPS were used to measure cognitive function. The prototypes of the two tests are from the health and retirement study (HRS). In the memory test, the interviewer read 10 common words (such as mountain, rice, river, etc.) to the participant. After hearing all 10 words, the participants recalled the words read by the interviewer. The score of this recall was called the immediate recall score. After a few minutes, the interviewer would ask the participant to recall the 10 words again. The score of this recall was called the delayed recall score. The score of the memory test was the total number of words correctly answered by the older adult, and the order was not required.
The logical sequence test used two stages of an adaptability test. This design was based on the item response theory (IRT) [28]. In the first stage, the interviewees answered three series of questions, and the number of correct answers (0 to 3) in the first stage was obtained. In the second stage, the system selected the corresponding group of questions from four groups according to the number and distributed them to the interviewees. The difficulty of four groups of questions was graded, in the first stage, individuals with more correct answers would receive more difficult tests in the second stage. The score calculated by the Rasch model was directly provided in CFPS (http://www.isss.pku.edu.cn/cfps, 16 August 2020).

2.2.4. Covariate

Highway construction as a provincial level was used as a covariate. It refers to the total length of public transit (such as bus lanes, etc.) and rail transit in a province. We used the China Statistical Yearbook of 2017, the data of which were compiled by the National Bureau of Statistics of China, to calculate the average highway mileage of 25 provinces, and matched traffic data according to the matching principles of the meteorological data and CFPS individual data.

2.3. Data Analysis

In this study, a mixed effect model (MEM) was used to evaluate the effects of different level variables on cognitive performance of older adults. Statistical assumptions associated with MEM (i.e., normality of the residuals, linearity, multicollinearity, and undue influence) were checked and were met for all models (Supplementary Materials Figure S1, Table S1).
The cognitive score (the memory test and logical sequence test were analyzed separately) of CFPS was used as the outcome variable ( Y i j ). In accordance with previous studies [29,30], the model was established by the following steps.
Firstly, an empty model was established to test whether there was significant difference in cognitive scores of the older adults in different provinces. The total variance of the outcome variable was divided into two parts, the between-province variance ( υ 0 j ) and within-province variance ( ε i j ). Equation (1) indicated that the cognitive scores of each older adult individual ( Y i j ) could be estimated by the average cognitive scores of the older adults in all provinces ( γ 00 ), between-province variance ( υ 0 j ), and within-province variance ( ε i j ).
                                        Y i j = γ 00 + υ 0 j + ε i j
Secondly, we established an individual level model to test the influence of individual level variables on cognitive scores. As shown in Equation (2), we added four variables at the individual level. The cognitive scores of each older adult individual ( γ i j ) could be estimated by the average cognitive scores of the older adults in all provinces ( γ 00 ), age ( γ 10 ), gender ( γ 20 ), residence ( γ 30 ), and chronic disease ( γ 40 ), and between-province variance ( υ 0 j ) and within-province variance ( ε i j ).
Y i j = γ 00 + γ 10 a g e j + γ 20 g e n d e r j + γ 30 r e s i d e n c e j + γ 40 c h r o n i c   d i s e a s e j + υ 0 j + ε i j
Thirdly, we established a model including both the individual level and provincial level to test the effects of individual level variables and provincial level variables on cognitive scores of the older adults, concurrently. The provincial level variables included temperature ( γ 01 ) and relative humidity ( γ 02 ). In addition, a control variable was added, traffic condition ( γ 03 ). Therefore, as shown in Equation (3), the cognitive score of each older adult individual ( Y i j ) could be estimated by the average cognitive scores of the older adults in all provinces ( γ 00 ) and the effects of individual level variables ( γ 10 ,   γ 20 , γ 30 and γ 40 ), provincial level variables ( γ 01 ,   γ 02 and γ 03 ), and variances ( υ 0 j and ε i j ).
Y i j = γ 00 + γ 10 a g e j + γ 20 g e n d e r j + γ 30 r e s i d e n c e j +   γ 40 c h r o n i c   d i s e a s e j + γ 01 t e m p e r a t u r e j + γ 02 h u m i d i t y j + γ 03 t r a f f i c j + υ 0 j + ε i j
Finally, we established a full model, including main and moderating effects across levels. We tested the interaction between meteorological factors and demographic factors. As shown in Equation (4), ( γ 11 ) represented the moderating effect of the provincial level variable (temperature) on the relationship between individual level variable (age) and the outcome variable (cognitive score). Similarly, ( γ 12 ,   γ 13 ,   γ 21 ,   γ 22 ,   γ 23 ,     γ 31 ,     γ 32 ,   γ 33 ,   γ 41 , γ 42 and γ 43 ) represented the moderating effects of provincial level variables on the relationship between individual level variables and the outcome variable.
Y i j = γ 00 + γ 10 a g e j + γ 20 g e n d e r j + γ 30 r e s i d e n c e j   +   γ 40 c h r o n i c   d i s e a s e j   + γ 01 t e m p e r a t u r e j + γ 02 h u m i d i t y j + γ 03 t r a f f i c j   + γ 11 a g e j × t e m p e r a t u r e j + γ 12 a g e j × h u m i d i t y j + γ 13 a g e j × t r a f f i c j   + γ 21 g e n d e r j × t e m p e r a t u r e j + γ 22 g e n d e r j × h u m i d i t y j + γ 23 g e n d e r j × t r a f f i c j   + γ 31 r e s i d e n c e j × t e m p e r a t u r e j + γ 32 r e s i d e n c e j × h u m i d i t y j + γ 33 r e s i d e n c e j × t r a f f i c j   + γ 41 c h r o n i c   d i s e a s e j × t e m p e r a t u r e j + γ 42 c h r o n i c   d i s e a s e j × h u m i d i t y j   + γ 43 c h r o n i c   d i s e a s e j × t r a f f i c j + υ 0 j + ε i j
We took age, temperature, humidity, and highway construction as continuous variables. We set gender as a categorical variable, with male marked as 1 and female marked as 2. Chronic diseases (including diabetes, hypertension, and chronic cardiovascular disease) are divided into suffering (marked as 1) or not (marked as 0), and residence was distinguished by living in urban (marked as 1) or rural areas (marked as 2). IBM (Amonk, New York, NY, USA) SPSS statistics (v19.0) was used for data analysis.

3. Results

3.1. The Memory Test

3.1.1. The Empty Model

The results in Table 2 show that there were significant differences in memory scores of the older adults from different provinces. We calculated intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) considering both within-province variance and between-province variance. The ICC considering both within-province variance and between-province variance was equal to 0.072, which indicated that 7.2% of the memory test scores variation of the older adults was associated with provincial characteristics. The design effect was approximately equal to 1 + (average cluster size − 1) × intraclass correlation (ICC).

3.1.2. The Individual Level Model

The individual level model assessed the association of the memory test scores with age, gender, residence, and chronic disease.
The results showed that older age (γ10 = −0.112, t = −13.464, p < 0.001), being male (γ20 = 0.311, t = 2.999, p = 0.003) and living in rural areas (γ30 = −0.582, t = −5.450, p < 0.001) were associated with lower memory test scores (Table 3 and Table 4).
The empty versus individual level models were assessed on the basis of the Restricted Maximum Likelihood (REML) test. Results showed that the individual model significantly improved the fit to the data compared to the empty model (Table 2).

3.1.3. The Individual Level and Provincial Level Model

In this model, we investigated the effects of individual and provincial level variables on the memory test scores of older adults. As the results shown in Table 4, older age (γ10 = −0.112, t = −13.488, p < 0.001), being male (γ20 = 0.317, t = 3.053, p = 0.002) and living in rural areas (γ30 = −0.598, t = −5.602, p < 0.001) were associated with lower memory test scores, after adjustment for the provincial level variable.
Moreover, among the variables at the provincial level, humidity (γ02 = −0.048, t = −2.685, p = 0.008) was associated with memory scores of the older adults. Higher humidity was associated with lower memory test scores. The results of the REML test for the individual level and provincial level model are shown in Table 3.

3.1.4. Interactive Effects Between Individual and Residential Variables on Cognitive Performance

As shown in Table 5, a positive moderating effect was found between the residence of the older adults and the average relative humidity of the provinces (γ32 = 0.045, t = 2.197, p = 0.028).
According to the humidity data in this study, we further divided the humidity into three levels, low-level humidity with less than 70% (480 cases), middle-level humidity with 70–79.9% (2110 cases), and high-level humidity with more than 80% (858 cases). Simple effect analysis showed that whether living in urban or rural areas, the memory scores of the older adults in high humidity were significantly lower (p < 0.001). The memory scores of the older adults living in urban areas decreased more significantly in high humidity than the older adults living in rural areas (p < 0.001) (shown in Figure 2). This result was still significant (t = 2.086, p = 0.37) when the other interactions were deleted from the model.
These results partially confirmed the H3; the relative humidity had a moderating effect on the relationship between residence (urban or rural) and cognitive performance of the older adults. The results of the REML test for the full model are shown in Table 2.

3.2. The Logical Sequence Test

3.2.1. The Empty Model

The results in Table 6 show that there were significant differences in logical sequence scores of the older adults from different provinces. We calculated ICC considering both within-province variance and between-province variance. The ICC considering both within-province variance and between-province variance was equal to 0.061, which indicated that 6.1% of the logical sequence test scores variation of the older adults was associated with provincial characteristics.

3.2.2. The Individual Level Model

The individual level model assessed the association of the logical sequence test scores with age, gender, residence, and chronic disease.
The results showed that older age (γ10 = −0.098, t = −9.269, p < 0.001), being female (γ20 = −1.037, t = −7.805, p < 0.001), and living in rural areas (γ30 = −0.736, t = −5.391, p < 0.001) were associated with lower logical sequence test scores (Table 7 and Table 8). However, the effect of suffering a chronic disease appeared to be positive (γ40 = 0.270, t = 1.981, p = 0.048).
The empty versus individual level models were assessed on the basis of REML test. Results showed that the individual model significantly improved the fit to the data compared to the empty model (Table 6).
According to Cohen (1988), when the ICC is less than 0.059, it is not necessary to use the mixed model [31,32]. In addition, as shown in Table 6, in the level 1 and 2 model and the full model, the ICC values were 0.055 and 0.049, respectively, indicating that the differences attributable to different provinces were too small. In addition to the size of ICC, in a logical sequence test, the design effect values of the level 1 and level 2 model and the full model were 8.53 and 7.71, respectively. The fitting degree of these models was too low to use the mixed model for further analysis. Therefore, we used a simpler single-layer linear regression analysis method., which was more convenient and the results were reliable. For the full model results, see Supplementary Materials Table S2.

4. Discussion

In this study including 3448 older adults, we used mixed effects models and found that living in rural areas and high-level humidity areas were associated with lower memory test performance (Table 7). Furthermore, we found that the strength of the negative association between the areas of residence and memory performance was moderated by humidity. In particular, this negative association was significantly more pronounced in urban areas compared to rural areas of residence. This study adds to the previous literature by revealing the association of individual and meteorological factors with cognitive function among older adults (Table 8).
The moderating effect of humidity on the relationship between residence and memory function in older age was illustrated by the association between high level humidity and memory function of urban older adults. Whether living in urban or rural areas, the memory scores of the older adults in high-humidity areas were significantly lower. This result is consistent with the studies by Vasmatzidis et al. and Trezza et al., which found that the memory function of people acutely exposed to high humidity is significantly lower than that of people exposed to low humidity [33,34]. In addition to memory function, some studies have found that a high relative humidity (70%) environment (e.g., subtropical climate) can reduce the cognitive ability of individuals [35], such as psychomotor speed and visual learning [33,35], making it more difficult for them to think clearly and reducing their alertness [36]. This may be due to the influence of relative humidity on the body’s ability of thermal sensation and thermoregulation. High humidity reduces the evaporation of human sweat, resulting in greater heat stress, which leads to the decline of cognitive function [33,37]. Older adults are less adaptable to climate change [38]. One possible explanation is that with adult aging, the regulatory capacity of older adults, such as cardiovascular function, gradually decreases, so the older adults are more vulnerable to the interference of heat stress [26,39]. Heat stress increases neuronal requirements for cognitive tasks in older adults [40,41] and disrupts brain functional connectivity [42,43]. Interestingly, we also found that the main effects of gender differences both in memory and logical sequence tests, which were consistent with previous views [44]. Although no significant interaction between gender and meteorological factors was found in this study, some studies suggested that due to the different physiological structures of males and females (such as weight and surface area), the adaptability to meteorological factors was also different [27,45]. It reminds us that in future cognitive interventions for the elderly, different functions need to focus on different gender groups.
Importantly, in this study, we found that in high humidity, the memory scores of the older adults living in urban areas were significantly lower than those living in rural areas. Previous studies have provided a possible explanation that long-term exposure to air pollution (e.g., particulate matter) is associated with cognitive impairment in the elderly [46,47,48]. Compared with rural areas, air pollution and other environmental problems in cities are more serious and meteorological factors such as high humidity play an important role in the elevation of air pollutant concentration [49]. Previous studies have shown that the impact of traffic conditions on the microenvironment is related to the distance from the residence to the highway [46]. This largely due to more intensive highway construction in cities; no matter in the design, construction, and operation stages, it will have a negative impact on the local micro ecological environment [9]. Moreover, high humidity conditions are more likely to cause pollution of atmospheric particles [50]. In addition, because of the urban heat island effect, a large number of studies have shown that in a high-heat and high-humidity environment, human cognitive function will be significantly reduced [34,35,51].
However, in the logical sequence test, we found that the performance differences of the older adults in different provinces were not significantly related to meteorological factors, but more related to the individual factors (such as age, gender, etc.). The results of previous studies are not consistent. Some studies show that temperature is related to mathematical ability, while others have not [52,53,54]. Another study points out that this inconsistency may be due to the lack of attention to gender differences in previous studies on temperature and mathematical ability [25]. At present, the research on the relationship between meteorological factors and mathematical ability, especially for older adults, needs to be verified by follow-up studies.
In this study, we used a mixed model to test the correlation between meteorological factors and cognitive function of older adults, and also examined the micro and macro multi-level effects. The limitations of this study were that we mainly investigated the influence of temperature and humidity on the cognitive function of older adults, while atmospheric pressure, precipitation, and other meteorological variables, as well as the air quality and the degree of greening in the city would also have an impact on the local microclimate. The meteorological data used in this study was accurate to the month, and we conceptualized the effects of humidity on cognitive outcomes as a contributor to a more chronic process of cognitive decline. In future research, using longitudinal datasets will help us to compare the acute exposure and cumulative effects of meteorological factors on cognitive function. In addition, we could further investigate the comprehensive effects of climate, environment, and ecology, using socio-economic status, formal education, home, and work environments (i.e., whether outfitted with air conditioners and dehumidifiers) as covariates or predictors in the equations to predict cognitive differences and report more accurate results.

5. Conclusions

In this study, 3448 older adults in CFPS were selected to assess the association of individual and meteorological factors with cognitive function by using mixed effects models. The main findings show that the negative association between humidity and memory performance is more pronounced in urban areas than in rural areas. This study explains how meteorological factors play a role in the influence of demographic factors on the cognitive function of older adults, and provides a reference for older adults to choose a more suitable residence of retirement.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph18115981/s1, Figure S1: Normal P-P plot of regression standardized residual, Table S1: Analysis of multicollinearity by stepwise regression, Table S2: The effects of individual level and provincial level variable in the full model.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.J.; methodology, Y.Q., L.Z. and C.J.; software, Y.Q.; validation, Y.Q. and K.W.; formal analysis, Y.Q. and C.J.; investigation, Y.Q.; resources, Y.Q. and K.W.; data curation, Y.Q. and K.W.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.Q.; writing—review and editing, D.W. and C.J.; visualization, Y.Q.; supervision, C.J.; project administration, C.J.; funding acquisition, C.J. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by Key Research Projects of Colleges and Universities in Guangdong Province (2018WZXDM015), and a Shenzhen Basic Research Grant (2019SHIBS0003).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are openly available in the following websites: (1) The China Family Panel Studies (http://isss.pku.edu.cn); (2) The National Meteorological Information Center of China (http://data.cma.cn); (3) The National Bureau of statistics of China (http://www.stats.gov.cn).

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the School of Psychology and the Institute of Mental Health (IMeH) of Shenzhen University. The data obtained in this study benefited from the China Family Panel Studies (CFPS) project implemented by the Institute of Social Science Survey (ISSS) of Peking University and the National Bureau of Statistics of China. In addition, the authors express their thanks to all the elderly and their families who participated in the interviews.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. WHO. Ageing and Health; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bajaj, J.S.; Duarte-Rojo, A.; Xie, J.J.; Acharya, C.; Wade, J.B.; Robles, C.; Vargas, H.E. Minimal Hepatic Encephalopathy and Mild Cognitive Impairment Worsen Quality of Life in Elderly Patients With Cirrhosis. Clin. Gastroenterol. Hepatol. 2020, 18, 3008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Landeiro, F.; Mughal, S.; Walsh, K.; Nye, E.; Morton, J.; Williams, H.; Consortium, R. Health-related quality of life in people with predementia Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment or dementia measured with preference-based instruments: A systematic literature review. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2020, 12, 154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Muir, S.W.; Gopaul, K.; Odasso, M.M.M. The role of cognitive impairment in fall risk among older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Age Ageing 2012, 41, 299–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Alzheimer’s Association. Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 2020, 16, 391–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Kraft, E. Cognitive function, physical activity, and aging: Possible biological links and implications for multimodal interventions. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2012, 19, 248–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Muller, A.M.; Blandford, A.; Yardley, L. The conceptualization of a Just-In-Time Adaptive Intervention (JITAI) for the reduction of sedentary behavior in older adults. mHealth 2017, 3, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Olanrewaju, O.; Kelly, S.; Cowan, A.; Brayne, C.; Lafortune, L. Physical Activity in Community Dwelling Older People: A Systematic Review of Reviews of Interventions and Context. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0168614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Jiao, L. Impact of Highway Construction on Ecology and Environment and Its Countermeasure. Res. Soil Water Conserv. 2005, 12, 193–194, 217. [Google Scholar]
  10. Peng, C.; Lin, F.; Zhong, Z.; Zhe, Z.; Feixiong, Y. Fuzzy Comprehensive Assessment for Impact on Ecology Environment of Construction of Extra-long Highway Tunnels in Mountain Area. IOP Conf. Ser. Earth Environ. Sci. 2019, 304, 032032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Robbins, R.N.; Scott, T.; Joska, J.A.; Gouse, H. Impact of urbanization on cognitive disorders. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2019, 32, 210–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Xu, H.; Dupre, M.E.; Gu, D.; Wu, B. The impact of residential status on cognitive decline among older adults in China: Results from a longitudinal study. BMC Geriatr. 2017, 17, 107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Saenz, J.L.; Downer, B.; Garcia, M.A.; Wong, R. Cognition and Context: Rural-Urban Differences in Cognitive Aging Among Older Mexican Adults. J. Aging Health 2018, 30, 965–986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Aron, A.R. The Climate Crisis Needs Attention from Cognitive Scientists. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019, 23, 903–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Finlay, J.; Khan, A.; Gronlund, C.; Sol, K.; Jang, J.; Melendez, R.; Clarke, P. Weather Woes? Exploring Potential Links between Precipitation and Age-Related Cognitive Decline. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 9011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Singh, P.K.; Dhiman, R.C. Climate change and human health: Indian context. J. Vector Borne Dis. 2012, 49, 55–60. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  17. Gutiérrez, D.; Wilson, R.J. Intra- and interspecific variation in the responses of insect phenology to climate. J. Anim. Ecol. 2021, 90, 248–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Lemonsu, A.; Amossé, A.; Chouillou, D.; Gaudio, N.; Haouès-Jouve, S.; Hidalgo, J.; Tudoux, B. Comparison of microclimate measurements and perceptions as part of a global evaluation of environmental quality at neighbourhood scale. Int. J. Biometeorol. 2020, 64, 265–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Dai, L.; Kloog, I.; Coull, B.A.; Sparrow, D.; Spiro, A., III; Vokonas, P.S.; Schwartz, J.D. Cognitive function and short-term exposure to residential air temperature: A repeated measures study based on spatiotemporal estimates of temperature. Environ. Res. 2016, 150, 446–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Leavitt, V.; Sumowski, J.; Wylie, G.; Chiaravalloti, N.; DeLuca, J. Warmer outdoor temperature is associated with decreased cognitive efficiency in multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. J. 2012, 18, 153. [Google Scholar]
  21. Muller, M.D.; Gunstad, J.; Alosco, M.L.; Miller, L.A.; Updegraff, J.; Spitznagel, M.B.; Glickman, E.L. Acute cold exposure and cognitive function: Evidence for sustained impairment. Ergonomics 2012, 55, 792–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Schlader, Z.J.; Gagnon, D.; Adams, A.; Rivas, E.; Cullum, C.M.; Crandall, C.G. Cognitive and perceptual responses during passive heat stress in younger and older adults. Am. J. Physiol.-Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2015, 308, R847–R854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Magalhaes, M.G.; Gragnani, A.; Veiga, D.F.; Blanes, L.; Galhardo, V.A.C.; Kallas, H.; Ferreira, L.M. Risk factors for pressure ulcers in hospitalized elderly without significant cognitive impairment. Wounds Compend. Clin. Res. Pract. 2007, 19, 20–24. [Google Scholar]
  24. Sunwoo, Y.; Chou, C.; Takeshita, J.; Murakami, M.; Tochihara, Y. Physiological and subjective responses to low relative humidity in young and elderly men. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2006, 25, 229–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Chang, T.Y.; Kajackaite, A. Battle for the thermostat: Gender and the effect of temperature on cognitive performance. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0216362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Huang, J.; Chen, Y.; Jones, P.; Hao, T. Heat stress and outdoor activities in open spaces of public housing estates in Hong Kong: A perspective of the elderly community. Indoor Built Environ. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Lan, L.; Lian, Z.; Liu, W.; Liu, Y. Investigation of gender difference in thermal comfort for Chinese people. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2008, 102, 471–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Chen, P.; Ding, S.L.; Lin, H.J.; Zhou, J. Item selection strategies of CAT under polytomous scored item response model. Acta Psychol. Sinia 2006, 38, 461–467. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  29. Chien, S.-P.; Wu, H.-K. Examining influences of science teachers’ practices and beliefs about technology-based assessment on students’ performances: A hierarchical linear modeling approach. Comput. Educ. 2020, 157, 103986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gavin, M. Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods, 2nd edition. Organ. Res. Methods 2004, 7, 228–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Eribaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  32. Knapp, T.R. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral-sciences, 2nd edition—Cohen. J. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1990, 50, 225–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Trezza, B.M.; Apolinario, D.; de Oliveira, R.S.; Busse, A.L.; Gonçalves, F.L.; Saldiva, P.H.; Jacob-Filho, W. Environmental heat exposure and cognitive performance in older adults: A controlled trial. Age 2015, 37, 9783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Vasmatzidis, I.; Schlegel, R.E.; Hancock, P.A. An investigation of heat stress effects on time-sharing performance. Ergonomics 2002, 45, 218–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Tian, X.; Fang, Z.; Liu, W. Decreased humidity improves cognitive performance at extreme high indoor temperature. Indoor Air 2020, 31, 608–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Zhu, M.; Liu, W.; Wargocki, P. Changes in EEG signals during the cognitive activity at varying air temperature and relative humidity. J. Expo. Sci. Environ. Epidemiol. 2020, 30, 285–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Moyen, N.E.; Ellis, C.L.V.; Ciccone, A.B.; Thurston, T.S.; Cochrane, K.C.; Brown, L.E.; Judelson, D.A. Increasing Relative Humidity Impacts Low-Intensity Exercise in the Heat. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2014, 85, 112–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Tillett, T. Climate Change and Elderly Americans Examining Adaptability in an Aging Population. Environ. Health Perspect. 2013, 121, A33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Loughnan, M.E.; Carroll, M.; Tapper, N. Learning from our older people: Pilot study findings on responding to heat. Australas. J. Ageing 2014, 33, 271–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Mattay, V.S.; Fera, F.; Tessitore, A.; Hariri, A.R.; Berman, K.F.; Das, S.; Weinberger, D.R. Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory capacity. Neurosci. Lett. 2006, 392, 32–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Reuter-Lorenz, P.A.; Cappell, K.A. Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 17, 177–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Gaoua, N.; Herrera, C.P.; Racinais, S.; El Massioui, F. Heat Induces An Overload During Complex Cognitive Performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2012, 44, 367. [Google Scholar]
  43. Liu, K.; Sun, G.; Li, B.; Jiang, Q.; Yang, X.; Li, M.; Liu, Y. The impact of passive hyperthermia on human attention networks: An fMRI study. Behav. Brain Res. 2013, 243, 220–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Pauls, F.; Petermann, F.; Lepach, A.C. Gender differences in episodic memory and visual working memory including the effects of age. Memory 2013, 21, 857–874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Avellini, B.A.; Kamon, E.; Krajewski, J.T. Physiological-responses of physically fit men and women to acclimation to humid heat. J. Appl. Physiol. 1980, 49, 254–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Altuğ, H.; Fuks, K.B.; Hüls, A.; Mayer, A.K.; Tham, R.; Krutmann, J.; Schikowski, T. Air pollution is associated with depressive symptoms in elderly women with cognitive impairment. Environ. Int. 2020, 136, 105448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Schikowski, T.; Vossoughi, M.; Vierkötter, A.; Schulte, T.; Teichert, T.; Sugiri, D.; .Luckhaus, C. Association of air pollution with cognitive functions and its modification by APOE gene variants in elderly women. Environ. Res. 2015, 142, 10–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Wang, J.N.; Wang, Q.; Li, T.T.; Shi, X.M. Association between air pollution and cognitive function in the elderly. Zhonghua Yu Fang Yi Xue Za Zhi 2017, 51, 364–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Ding, P.H.; Wang, G.S.; Guo, Y.L.; Chang, S.C.; Wan, G.H. Urban air pollution and meteorological factors affect emergency department visits of elderly patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease in Taiwan. Environ. Pollut. 2017, 224, 751–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Peters, T.M. Use of the aerodynamic particle sizer to measure ambient PM10-2.5: The coarse fraction of PM10. J. Air Waste Manag. Assoc. 2006, 56, 411–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Luo, Y.; Yang, M.; Guo, M.; Zhong, X.; Hu, Y. Huang Qin Hua Shi decoction for high-temperature- and high-humidity-induced cognitive-behavioral disorder in rats is associated with deactivation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. J. Int. Med. Res. 2019, 47, 5752–5766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Hancock, P.A.; Ross, J.M.; Szalma, J.L. A meta-analysis of performance response under thermal stressors. Hum. Factors 2007, 49, 851–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Pilcher, J.J.; Nadler, E.; Busch, C. Effects of hot and cold temperature exposure on performance: A meta-analytic review. Ergonomics 2002, 45, 682–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Yeganeh, A.J.; Reichard, G.; McCoy, A.P.; Bulbul, T.; Jazizadeh, F. Correlation of ambient air temperature and cognitive performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Build. Environ. 2018, 143, 701–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The proposed multi-level model.
Figure 1. The proposed multi-level model.
Ijerph 18 05981 g001
Figure 2. The memory scores between urban and rural areas under different humidity levels.
Figure 2. The memory scores between urban and rural areas under different humidity levels.
Ijerph 18 05981 g002
Table 1. The demographic information of all the participants.
Table 1. The demographic information of all the participants.
VariablesAll Population
Continuous VariablesnMean ± SD (min–max)
Age344867.5 ± 6.0 (60–94)
Memory test score34486.89 ± 3.02 (0–14)
Sequence test score34484.59 ± 3.82 (0–14)
Categorical variablesn%
Gender
Male218763.4
Female126136.6
Education level
Low (≤6)158445.9
High (>6)186454.1
Place of residence
Urban190355.1
Rural154544.9
Chronic disease
Yes108131.3
No236768.7
Table 2. The gap value, variance, ICC, and design effect value of models.
Table 2. The gap value, variance, ICC, and design effect value of models.
ModelGap (−2)VarianceICCDesign Effect
Within-Province Between-Province
Empty model17,314.5018.7400.6790.07210.86
Level 1 model17,108.2438.2100.5670.0659.90
Level 1 and 2 model17,134.7798.2030.5030.0588.94
Full model17,180.7338.2000.5140.0599.08
Table 3. Descriptive statistics of memory scores of categorical variables.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics of memory scores of categorical variables.
Categorical VariablesnMean ± SD
Gender
male21876.70 ± 2.955
female12617.21 ± 3.104
Residence
urban19037.16 ± 3.089
rural15456.55 ± 2.899
Chronic disease
Yes10816.79 ± 3.064
No23676.93 ± 3.000
Table 4. The main effects of the empty, individual level, and provincial level model.
Table 4. The main effects of the empty, individual level, and provincial level model.
ParameterThe Empty ModelThe Individual Level ModelThe Individual Level and Provincial Level Model
Estimatetp95% CIEstimatetp95% CIEstimateTp95% CI
Intercept6.73837.758<0.0016.366–7.11014.72223.199<0.00113.477–15.96617.88911.935<0.00114.940–20.839
Individual level variable
Age −0.112−13.464<0.001−0.128–−0.095−0.112−13.488<0.001−0.128–−0.096
Gender 0.3112.9990.0030.108–0.5150.3173.0530.0020.113–0.520
Residence −0.582−5.450<0.001−0.791–−0.372−0.598−5.602<0.001−0.808–−0.389
Chronic disease −0.078−0.7320.464−0.287–0.131−0.083−0.7810.435−0.292–0.126
Provincial level variable
Temperature 0.0191.1230.262−0.014–0.053
Humidity −0.048−2.6850.008−0.084–−0.013
Control variable
Traffic 9.49210 × 10 −70.1450.886−1.28 × 10 −5–1.47 × 10 −5
Table 5. The effects of individual level and provincial level variables in the full model.
Table 5. The effects of individual level and provincial level variables in the full model.
ParameterThe Full Model
Estimatedftp95% CI
Intercept22.2733431.9892.5220.0124.957–39.588
Individual level variable
Age−0.1763419.325−1.4810.139−0.409–0.057
Gender2.4823417.0221.7050.088−0.373–5.337
Residence−2.9953312.824−2.0830.037−5.815–−0.175
Chronic disease1.1053418.0570.7760.438−1.687–3.898
Provincial level variable
Temperature0.0073417.2970.0400.968−0.326–0.340
Humidity−0.1013431.016−0.828−0.408−0.341–0.139
Control variable
Traffic7.1501 × 10 −719.0590.1080.915−1.31 × 10 −5–1.46 × 10 −5
Provincial level moderating effects
Age × Temperature0.0013422.1190.4370.662−0.003–0.005
Age × Humidity0.0013419.4330.3100.757−0.003–0.004
Gender × Temperature−0.0033423.568−0.1000.921−0.058–0.052
Gender × Humidity−0.0283420.440−1.3810.167−0.067–0.012
Residence × Temperature−0.0393309.418−1.3010.193−0.099–0.020
Residence × Humidity0.0453258.0902.1970.0280.005–0.085
Chronic disease × Temperature−0.0023421.325−0.0570.955−0.060–0.056
Chronic disease × Humidity−0.0153416.835−0.7660.444−0.054–0.024
Table 6. The gap value, variance, ICC, and design effect value of models.
Table 6. The gap value, variance, ICC, and design effect value of models.
ModelGap (−2)VarianceICCDesign Effect
Within-Province Between-Province
Empty model18,947.05914.0490.9160.0619.35
Level 1 model18,806.27413.4520.8350.0588.94
Level 1 and 2 model18,837.13513.4600.7770.0558.53
Full model18,877.73513.4560.6980.0497.71
Table 7. Descriptive statistics of logical sequence test scores of categorical variables.
Table 7. Descriptive statistics of logical sequence test scores of categorical variables.
Categorical VariablesnMean ± SD
Gender
Male21874.86 ± 3.87
Female12614.11 ± 3.70
Residence
Urban19034.96 ± 3.97
Rural15454.12 ± 3.59
Chronic disease
Yes10814.73 ± 3.88
No23674.52 ± 3.80
Table 8. The main effects of individual level variables.
Table 8. The main effects of individual level variables.
ParameterEstimatedftp95% CI
Individuallevel variable
Intercept13.6322218.28216.831<0.00112.04415.221
Age−0.0983431.986−9.269<0.001−0.119−0.078
Gender−1.0373431.832−7.805<0.001−1.297−0.776
Residence−0.7363404.812−5.391<0.001−1.003−0.468
Chronic disease0.2703431.2261.9810.0480.0030.538
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Qiu, Y.; Wei, K.; Zhu, L.; Wu, D.; Jiao, C. The Association of Meteorological Factors with Cognitive Function in Older Adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5981. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115981

AMA Style

Qiu Y, Wei K, Zhu L, Wu D, Jiao C. The Association of Meteorological Factors with Cognitive Function in Older Adults. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(11):5981. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115981

Chicago/Turabian Style

Qiu, Yuehong, Kaigong Wei, Lijun Zhu, Dan Wu, and Can Jiao. 2021. "The Association of Meteorological Factors with Cognitive Function in Older Adults" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 11: 5981. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115981

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop