Next Article in Journal
Comparative Toxic Effects of Manufactured Nanoparticles and Atmospheric Particulate Matter in Human Lung Epithelial Cells
Next Article in Special Issue
Can Individual Attitudes toward Aging Predict Subsequent Physical Disabilities in Older Taiwanese Individuals? A Four-Year Retrospective Cohort Study
Previous Article in Journal
Socioeconomic Inequality in the Use of Long-Term Care among European Older Adults: An Empirical Approach Using the SHARE Survey
Previous Article in Special Issue
Perspectives of the Elderly with Mild Cognitive Impairment Living Alone on Participating in a Dementia Prevention Program: A Q Methodology Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Intergenerational Ties in Context: Association between Caring for Grandchildren and Cognitive Function in Middle-Aged and Older Chinese

1
School of Public Health, Fudan University, Shanghai 200032, China
2
School of Health Science and Nursing, Wuhan Polytechnic University, Wuhan 430023, China
3
ESSCA School of Management, 1 Rue Joseph Lakanal-BP 40348, CEDEX 01, 49003 Angers, France
4
Grenoble Ecole de Management, 12 Rue Pierre Semard, 38000 Grenoble, France
5
School of Medicine and Health Management, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan 430030, China
6
Research Center for Rural Health Services, Hubei Province Key Research Institute of Humanities and Social Sciences, Wuhan 430030, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(1), 21; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18010021
Submission received: 7 December 2020 / Accepted: 16 December 2020 / Published: 22 December 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Health Promotion for the Elderly)

Abstract

:
Grandchild caregiving is suggested to improve the elderly’s cognitive function, but the specific relationship remains under-investigated. Considering gender disparity, this study aimed to understand the relationship between grandchild caregiving and cognition. In total, 7236 Chinese residents (≥45 years old) were selected from the 2015 China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). The China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocol (CHARLS-HCAP) was used to measure cognition. Grandparenting was measured from three dimensions: caregiving frequency, intensity, and the number of grandchildren cared for. The relationship was examined by multivariate linear regression, with age as a moderator. The results showed that the majority of respondents provided care to their grandchildren, especially grandmothers. Grandchild caregiving was positively associated with cognition (β = 0.686, 95% CI = 0.334–1.038), especially in the older-aged group. Moderate, not regular grandparenting, or caring for one grandchild was more positively associated with cognitive function. However, intensive and regular grandchild care was significantly associated with cognition only in men. No moderating effects of age were found in women. The study confirmed that moderate intensity and frequency of caregiving was related to better cognitive function in middle-aged and older Chinese population, whereas cultural context and gender differences could be considered when designing targeted policies.

1. Introduction

With the global population aging [1], the age-related cognitive decline [2,3] has become a worldwide public health issue. It not only damages physical and mental health, increasing the economic, physical, and psychological burden of family caregivers [4,5], but also increases the cost to family, community, and government [6]. Moreover, dementia is diagnosed when cognitive impairment develops seriously enough to affect daily activities [7,8], and most of the dementia will occur in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) [9]. In China, as one of the LMICs [10] and the most populous country in the world [11], cognitive impairment has become a severe health problem for the elderly population. However, like other LMICs, China also lacks sufficient professional mental health resources [12,13] due to the high demand for professional resources [14,15], along with a relatively low public awareness rate of cognitive impairment [16]. Therefore, China urgently needs a more widespread and cost-effective way to address this issue.
Caring for grandchildren has both broad applicability and potential effectiveness. Numerous studies have suggested that social connections and productive activities are beneficial not only to physical health, e.g., helping treat chronic diseases [17] but also to mental health, e.g., improving cognitive function in the elderly [18,19,20]. As one of the productive activities [21,22], grandparent caregiving is more common for the Chinese elderly due to China’s special social [23] and cultural [24] background. Moreover, considering the fact that grandparent caregiving has a gender-specific nature that women are seen as primary caregivers [25], it is customary for grandmothers to provide care to grandchildren, especially in intensive grandchild care [26].
Some studies have proposed that grandparents may bear different role-taking mechanisms during their caregiving activities, namely “role enhancement theory” [27] or “role strain theory” [28]. The relationship between grandparent caregiving and cognition in the middle-aged and older adults has not been determined. Studies have shown that caring for grandchildren may maintain the cognition of the elderly due to its positive nature [29]. Grandparent caregiving may be a huge demand on caregivers, both socially and emotionally, and may require relatively large cognitive resources [30], which could delay cognitive function decline [31]. Similarly, caring for grandchildren could increase grandparents’ daily activities [32], such as having fun with grandchildren to encourage physical activities and help them feel more energetic and young, thereby increasing their social participation and improving cognition [33,34].
Nevertheless, a negative association between grandparent caregiving and cognition was also observed. Some studies found that grandparent caregiving may cause stress, limit other forms of social participation, and reduce self-care in the elderly, thus negatively affecting cognitive function and mental health [35,36]. Moreover, a handful of research further investigated the frequency [36], intensity [37], number of grandchildren [38], and cohabitation of grandparent caregiving [39], but also failed to reach an agreement on the optimal patterns of grandparenting that can contribute to cognition [40,41,42]. In other words, caring for grandchildren and its relationship with cognition is a multidimensional concept that needs further study. Meanwhile, most research is conducted in Western countries, leaving China and other LMICs uninvestigated.
Given this, we hypothesized that a positive association between grandparent caregiving and cognitive function among the middle-aged and elderly Chinese existed. Thus, our study aimed to investigate the patterns of grandparent caregiving and explore its relationship with cognitive function from three dimensions, including intensity, frequency, and the number of grandchildren they cared for. Furthermore, considering the gendered nature of grandparent caregiving that compared with men, women play a more important role in the care of grandchildren [43,44,45], and the potential interactions between age and grandparenting, this study was carried out with gender differences and the moderating effect of age taken into consideration.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Sample

The data were extracted from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS) Wave 4 (2015) survey [46], which is a nationally representative longitudinal survey. The CHALRS collects the demographic and socioeconomic data of Chinese residents aged 45 years and above, as well as health behaviors and health-related outcomes. The baseline survey was conducted in 2011 and followed up in 2013, 2014, and 2015. Using a multistage probability-proportional-to-size (PPS) sampling technique [46,47], CHARLS surveyed with 28 provinces, 450 villages or communities in 150 districts or counties covered, and 21,097 individuals aged 45 years and older in 12,221 households were investigated in 2015 [48]. Considering that grandparent caregiving may have contemporaneous impacts on mental health and some short-term effects may diminish over time [49], we only employed Wave 4 data and selected 7236 participants according to the following criteria: (1) aged 45 and older; (2) provided information on both grandparent caregiving and cognitive function.

2.2. Variables

2.2.1. Outcome Variables

Cognitive function was measured by the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocol (CHARLS-HCAP) [50], which was similar to the U.S. Health and Retirement Study (HRS) [51] and has been proven feasible and valid to be used in the CHARLS sample and hospital samples [50]. Similar to the HRS, the CHARLS-HCAP combined items from several scales, including Mini-mental State Examination (MMSE) and Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). The CHALRS adopts Date Naming, Serial 7′s test, and drawing of overlapping pentagons from MMSE [50], and adopts the CERAD version’s key components of immediate and delayed word recall to measure memory [52]. As a result, considering that the measures in CHARLS included components of different scales and referring to previous studies using the CHARLS data [53,54,55], no cutoffs have been employed in our study.
Based upon previous studies using the data from HRS [51] and CHARLS [53,54,55], respectively, we assessed the two dimensions of cognitive function separately: episodic memory and mental status (Table 1). Specifically speaking, the tests in CHARLS measured four aspects of cognitive function, ranging from 0 to 31 points, representing the overall cognitive status of the respondents: episodic memory, orientation, visuoconstruction, and mathematical performance [56]. The higher the scores, the better the cognitive function [55]. In CHARLS, episodic memory was assessed by immediate and delayed word recall, which was to test respondents’ capacity to repeat after reading ten Chinese nouns in any order immediately and four minutes later, respectively. Both of them ranged from 0 to 10 points. By summing the immediate and delayed recall scores, the total score of episodic memory was 20 points. Mental status consisted of orientation, visuoconstruction, and mathematical performance, ranging from 0 to 11 points. Orientation was measured using a five-item scale that asks participants to name the day, month, year, season, and correct day of the week. Visuoconstruction was measured by asking participants to re-draw a previously shown picture accurately (0–1 point). The mathematical performance was assessed by asking participants to subtract 7 from 100 up to 5 times (0–5 points).

2.2.2. Explanatory Variables

In the CHARLS 2015 questionnaire, grandparent caregiving was measured by the following questions: (1) Did you spend time caring for your grandchildren last year; (2) if yes, which child’s children did you provide care for; (3) how many weeks and how many hours per week did you spend caring for this child’s children approximately.
Intensity: according to the respondents’ answers on how many hours they spent on caring for grandchildren per week, the average number of hours the grandparents looked after their grandchildren was 33.4 h per week, roughly equivalent to holding a full-time job. By referring to the previous study that used a standard working time of 5 days a week and 8 h a day as a cutoff [57], and based on the principle of averaging the sample size of each category, this continuous variable was divided into three categories: Never/Moderate grandchild care (1–39 h)/ Intensive grandchild care (≥40 h).
Frequency: according to the respondents’ answer about how many weeks a year they spent caring for their grandchildren, the average number of weeks the grandparents cared for their grandchildren was 19 weeks a year, roughly equivalent to half a year. Considering that some participants had no caregiving activities, this continuous variable was divided into three categories according to the principle of averaging the sample size of each category: Never/Not regularly (≤half a year: 1–26 weeks)/Regularly (≥half a year: ≥26 weeks).
Number of grandchildren: according to the respondents’ answers on which grandchildren they took care of and considering that some participants had no caregiving activities, the continuous variable was divided into three categories based upon the principle of averaging the sample size of each category: 0/1/≥2.

2.2.3. Control Variables

Table 2 presents the codes and (or) definitions of all control variables referred from prior studies [58,59,60].

2.3. Data Analysis

Percentages and frequencies were calculated for descriptive data. Chi-square test for categorical variables and Wilcoxon rank sum test and independent sample T-test for continuous variables were performed to test gender differences. One-way ANOVA test was performed to analyze the cognition performance in different grandchild caregiving patterns. After controlling for potentially confounding variables, multivariate linear regression analysis was employed to assess the association between grandparent caregiving and cognition in the whole sample and each gender group. In order to avoid the potential multicollinearity, different caregiver types were entered separately as a single explanatory variable. Additionally, whether relations were moderated by age was examined. A moderating effect was indicated by a significant interaction of grandchildren caregiving patterns × moderator age. In all regression analyses, the predictor variables were mean-centered. Coefficients (β1) and their 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) were employed to depict the associated effect. The data were analyzed by R Version 3.5.1 (Bell Laboratories, Wien, Austria).

3. Results

3.1. Basic Characteristics of the Participants

Table 3 shows the basic characteristics of the whole participants, as well as male/female subgroups. The median age was 62 years old. Of the 7236 participants, greater proportions were rural residents (62.10%), with a primary school or lower level of education (44.03%), having a partner (79.91%), and not retired (65.10%). Moreover, most participants did not drink alcohol (63.90%) or smoke (70.10%); suffered from at least one type of NCD (82.90%), experienced no difficulty in activities of daily living (78.50%); lived near children (54.20%), had weekly contact with children (78.30%), and received economic support (93.00%) from them.
As shown in Table 3, gender differences in sociodemographic characteristics and health status were also significant. Compared with women, men had a higher education level, a greater proportion of alcohol and tobacco consumption, but a lower retirement rate, and suffered less from chronic disease and activities of daily living (ADLs). Furthermore, men scored significantly higher than women in cognition (15 vs. 13). Moreover, a lower proportion of men lived with their children, received economic support, and contacted with them than women.

3.2. Patterns of Grandparent Caregiving

Table 4 presents the frequency, intensity, and the number of grandchildren respondents cared for. In general, over half of respondents cared for their grandchildren (52.90%). Amongst those who provided care, 26.19% provided care not regularly, with the intensity of 40 h and above a week (29.53%) to one grandchild (42.03%). Significant differences between men and women were also observed. Compared with male counterparts, women were the primary caregivers for their grandchildren regarding frequency (24.65% vs. 21.30% care regularly) and intensity (32.88% vs. 26.46% intensive grandchild care).

3.3. Cognitive Function in Different Patterns of Grandparent Caregiving

Table 5 presents the mean and standard deviation (SD) of cognitive function in different grandparent caregiving patterns, as well as the cognitive differences in specific caregiving patterns. In general, significant differences in cognitive function existed among different patterns of grandchildren caregiving. The respondents who cared for their grandchildren had higher cognitive scores than their counterparts with no caregiving activities, and the trend was the same in the two gender subgroups.

3.4. Relationship between Grandparent Caregiving and Cognitive Function

Results from the multivariate linear regressions of the relationship between grandparent caregiving and cognition for all participants and each gender group are reported in Table 6. Broadly speaking, caring for grandchildren was significantly associated with cognition. Providing moderate or not regular grandchild care was more strongly associated with cognition than intensive or regular grandchild care. The trend was similar to the trend for the number of grandchildren cared for, in that, compared with taking care of two or more grandchildren, caring for only one grandchild was more positively associated with cognitive function.
Similar relationships were found between grandchild care and cognitive function in different gender subgroups. A positive association between grandparent caregiving and cognition was significant in both men and women, with lower intensity, lower frequency, and one grandchild to care for. However, providing intensive and regular grandchild care was significantly associated with cognition only for male respondents.
Interaction analysis revealed that in the whole sample, age moderated the positive associations between grandchildren care and cognition (Table 6). These associations were more pronounced in the older aged group, especially among those who provided moderate care to one grandchild. Similar relationships were also found in the male subgroup. However, we did not find such a moderating effect in the female subgroup.
Furthermore, by referring to previous studies [62,63,64], we employed R2adjusted and variance inflation factor (VIF) to measure the effect size and multicollinearity of the multivariable linear regression model, respectively. In all three models, the value of R2adjusted > 0.26 and VIF < 10, which suggested that the effect size was relatively large [65] and the strength of association between dependent and predictable variables was moderate [63], as well as that the multicollinearity was weak in our three models [64].

4. Discussion

This study advances the knowledge on the relationship between grandparent caregiving and cognition function in the middle-aged and older Chinese population, based on which, we further conducted a detailed analysis of the association between grandparent caregiving and cognition within each gender group, as well as considering three dimensions of care: frequency, intensity, and the number of grandchildren.

4.1. Prevalence of Grandparent Caregiving

The finding outlined a high prevalence of grandparent caregiving in the middle-aged and older Chinese population (≥50%), which is similar to the results from research conducted in China [42] and other countries [66,67]. The underlying reason might be that, with more mothers working in the labor market and rising separation and divorce levels, grandparents are now playing an increasingly significant role in child caregiving [68,69]. Additionally, rooted in the filial piety of Confucian norm [70], it is more common for older adults to live with their children and grandchildren in China [71], whereas the majority of grandparents in Western countries conform to a norm of non-interference in intergenerational relationships and do not play a central role in caring their grandchildren [72]. Moreover, although dramatically changing as a result of China’s socioeconomic development, the historically strong tie between grandparents and grandchildren has persisted [73,74]; as more young adults migrate to search for better economic opportunities, a large number of children have been left behind and are often cared for by their grandparents [75]. This suggests that caring for grandchildren is common and plays an active role in China’s childcare system.

4.2. The Relationship between Grandparent Caregiving and Cognition

The present study found a significantly positive association between grandparent caregiving and cognitive function. Similar findings can be retrieved from previous studies [37,76]. We explain our findings based on three potential cognitive mechanisms: first, enhance cognition by improving physiological functionality [77]. To be more specific, the underlying reasons might be as follows: First, it increases mental stimulation and therefore enhances brain function [78,79]. During the process of caretaking activities, such as living arrangements for grandchildren and the direct or indirect transmission of individual experience and knowledge, they might strengthen their mental stimulation and cognitive function by practicing their cognitive skills in learning, thinking, and reasoning [80]. Second, grandparents perform various grandparental roles—prior studies have categorized them into four major functions: fun seeker, daily life helper, the reservoir of family wisdom, and surrogate parent [81,82,83]. In this case, grandparent caregiving might have different impacts on cognitive function through various caretaking activities. For instance, through daily life activities such as cleaning the house, washing clothes, feeding, dressing, and bathing, grandparents’ daily activity levels could be improved [84], as well as their mobility and physiological functionality [85]. Third, it mitigates the stress-related impact on cognition [2]. For older adults, grandparenthood can be a meaningful source of leisure [86], which provides them with enjoyable experiences and chances to develop relationships with family members, gives a sense of purpose, and is a source of personal satisfaction or growth [87,88], and even reduces the risk of depression [89]. This could also be supported by the role enhancement theory that grandparents who help to care for their grandchildren could promote an active lifestyle and gain emotional and social support by interacting with others [90], as well as attain social gratification and integration from their various social roles [91]. The demand for multiple roles could help increase social support and offset the risks of role strain [92], which might mitigate the stress-related impact, and as a result, be helpful to maintain cognitive function status.
On the other hand, some previous studies have suggested that dementia or other cognitive impairment could cause disability and affect individuals’ capacity to accomplish daily routines. This may result in the inability to live independently [78,93] and provide care for their grandchildren. Some studies also suggested that the prevalence of depression in older adults with cognitive impairment was higher than that in the general population, regardless of age, race, or gender [94,95]. Consequently, the opposite direction of causality could also be considered, and there is a need for further research.
We also investigated the relationship between patterns of grandparent caregiving and cognition. The results showed that, although intensive and regular grandchild care was positively associated with cognition, providing lower-intensive and frequent grandchild care to fewer grandchildren were more strongly related to cognitive function, which is similar to the findings in prior studies [36,38,96,97]. The reasons might be that intensive and regular caregiving requires grandparents to invest more time and energy, which could cause physical burden and take away resources from them, and consequently, brings more pressure and responsibilities to grandparent caregivers [32,35], compromises physical and mental health [98], and to some extent, offsets the good effects characterized by the grandparental role [99].

4.3. Gender Differences

To sum up, significant gender differences existed in cognitive status, grandparent caregiving patterns, and its association with cognition.
Significant differences in cognitive function status in men/women subgroups were observed. Compared with female respondents, male respondents scored significantly higher, which can be supported by prior research conducted in China [53,100]. Inspired by previous studies that a positive correlation between socioeconomic status (SES) and mental health existed [101], this may be because levels of education [102] and income [103] are positively related to cognitive function, and in China’s context, Chinese females have historically lower levels of education and income [53], and retire from jobs earlier than men [45], which could result in female disparity in cognition. This finding reveals that women’s cognitive function is relatively poor and more urgent may be needed help to enhance female cognitive function.
In addition, we also revealed different patterns of differences by gender that grandmothers provided more intensive and regular care to grandchildren than grandfathers, which is consistent with a previous study [104]. This could be explained partly by the grandmother hypothesis [44,105] that in the case of older females whose fertility declines, especially post-menopausal women who are nearly infertile, they tend to invest resources to raise their grandchildren rather than continue to reproduce themselves. This care could have not only endowed their daughters with greater fertility to resume reproduction sooner but also increased their own fitness and benefited their more prolonged survival [106]. Moreover, in China, women often have lower education and income levels, retire earlier than men, and often bear the major responsibility for nurturing family members by social norms [45]. As a result, women are in relatively lower socioeconomic groups and are less able to say no to grandparent caregiving [107]. Therefore, they may invest more time and effort to look after their grandchildren. This echoes our proposition that gender differences should be considered when investigating the patterns of grandparent caregiving and their relationship with cognition in the middle-aged and older Chinese population.
Our study further examined the correlation between patterns of grandparent caregiving and cognitive function in middle-aged and older Chinese residents, and no significant differences were observed in gender groups. Caring for grandchildren was positively statistically related to cognition in men and women, mainly manifested in moderate and irregular care for one grandchild. The reason might be the same with the discussion in the whole sample. Interestingly, providing intensive and regular care to grandchildren was significantly related to cognition only in male respondents. We speculate that, on the one hand, this may be due to different activities involved in grandparent caregiving. Regarding intensive grandparent caregiving, grandfathers usually play the role of a playmate, fun-seeker, and companion, while grandmothers more possibly undertake more intensive responsibilities, such as feeding, dressing, and bathing [108]. Hence, grandmothers may feel more stressed and powerless in the face of intensive caregiving. On the other hand, differences in personality characteristics between men and women are also significant, in that, women are emotion-oriented or interpersonally oriented regarding coping strategies, revolving around expressiveness and sensitivity [109], while men are usually task-oriented [110]. Grandmothers generally contact more frequently and report greater closeness with their grandchildren than grandfathers [111]. They tend to care for grandchildren more directly and spend more time with them, which may bring them more burden and pressure from grandparent caregiving. Moreover, compared with men, women are more susceptible to stress than men, and their stress response is stronger [112,113]. Meanwhile, a physiological stress response could be caused by stress, which is thought to be the biological basis for the link between stress and cognitive function. The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis could be activated, and the stress hormone cortisol is secreted in the stress response. Cortisol can cross the blood–brain barrier into the brain and bind to receptors in different brain areas, such as the frontal lobe, hippocampus, and amygdala, which are all related to cognitive functions, such as learning and memory [114,115,116]. Additionally, stress is closely related to depression [117], and depression is one of the risk factors of cognitive decline [118]. To some extent, intensive care for grandchildren may counteract the beneficial effect on women’s cognitive function, and intensive care of grandchildren was only related to cognition in male grandparents.
The present study also revealed that women had worse cognitive function and were more likely to provide intensive grandchild care. However, during the process of intensive grandchild care, this cognitive vulnerability may be underscored by grandmothers’ tendency to ignore their own health in favor of their grandchildren [119], and to some extent, counteracts the positive effects of caring for grandchildren on cognitive function. The study, therefore, reminds policymakers to consider gender differences when making policies regarding improving and maintaining cognitive function.

4.4. Moderating Effect of Age

In general, the association between grandparenting and cognitive performance was closer in older adults than their middle-aged counterparts. This was especially the case for those who cared for fewer grandchildren with lower intensity. We further explored gender differences and found out that the moderating effects of age held in men, whereas no moderating role of age was observed in women. The possible reason might be that their social interaction became relatively less and limited as they aged, especially after retirement. As one of the very few types of social interactions, grandchild caregiving becomes a more important activity that could help improve their mobility and increase positive health behaviors [119], as well as expand social participation and develop an active lifestyle [120]. This may have positive effects on the health and cognitive function of older adults [41,42]. Compared with male counterparts, women may have more opportunities to take part in different types of social activities after retirement, such as square dance [121] or mahjong [122], and taking care of their grandchildren is one of the ways to socialize, thus it may counteract the positive effects of grandparenting on cognition.

4.5. Strength and Limitations

To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first one to investigate the relationship between caregiving for grandchildren and cognition in middle-aged and older Chinese people from various dimensions, including intensity, frequency, and the number of grandchildren cared for. Meanwhile, it is also one of the very few studies that examined gender disparities, which is particularly important considering the gendered nature of cognition and grandparenting patterns. Our findings add further insights into the correlation between the specific patterns of grandparent caregiving and older adults’ cognitive function in each gender group. They not only inform that lower intensity, frequency of care, and fewer number of grandchildren cared for may be more beneficial to cognition, but also remind that social programs and interventions for preventing cognitive aging should be designed with gender differences taken into consideration.
The study also has the following limitations. First, causal relationships could not be inferred, although considering the contemporaneous impacts of grandparent caregiving on mental health [49]. Second, grandparent caregiving variables were self-reported and measured in the last year, which may have some recall bias. Third, whether grandparents co-habited with their grandchildren, what kind of activities grandparents participated in, or what type of role the grandparents fulfilled was not studied due to the data availability, which may also affect cognitive function. Fourth, due to the limited number of older participants caring for their grandchildren, the relationship between grandparent caregiving and cognitive function in the older-aged population in China might not have been fully represented. Lastly, the CHARLS-HCAP is a self-report screening tool rather than a clinical diagnostic measure. The extent of cognitive impairment or the clinical diagnosis of the cognitive impairment could be not obtained.

5. Conclusions

This study has yielded three main findings: (1) Caregiving for grandchildren was related to better cognitive function, whereas the magnitude depended largely on the frequency and intensity of care. (2) Cognitive function and patterns of caregiving varied by gender subgroups, where women were primary grandchildren caregivers and suffered from worse cognitive function than men. The positive association between caregiving and cognitive function generally held similar in two subgroups, except that a statistically significant association between intensive and regular grandchildren care and cognitive function was only found in men. (3) A moderating effect of age on the association between grandchild caregiving and cognition was found, especially in the older-aged group who provided moderate care to one grandchild. Similar relationships were found in men, whereas no such moderating effect was observed in women.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.W.; methodology, S.L., L.Q., and R.W.; software, S.L.; formal analysis, S.L. and R.W.; resources, L.Q., J.X., J.Y., and R.W.; data curation, S.L. and L.Q.; writing—original draft preparation, S.L.; writing—review and editing, J.X., J.Y., and R.W.; supervision, R.W.; project administration, S.L. and R.W.; funding acquisition, R.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (no. 71704059) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (no. 2017KFYXJJ156).

Data Availability Statement

Datasets are distributable only by CHARLS team. They are available in the public domain through 395 registrations on CHARLS website: http://charls.pku.edu.cn/zh-CN and are also available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the CHARLS research team and all respondents for their contribution.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Lutz, W.; Sanderson, W.; Scherbov, S. The coming acceleration of global population ageing. Nature 2008, 451, 716–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Kremen, W.S.; Lachman, M.E.; Pruessner, J.C.; Sliwinski, M.; Wilson, R.S. Mechanisms of Age-Related Cognitive Change and Targets for Intervention: Social Interactions and Stress. J Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2012, 67, 760–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Costa, A. Charting age-associated cognitive decline. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 2017, 189, E1470–E1471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Cheng, S.-T. Dementia Caregiver Burden: A Research Update and Critical Analysis. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2017, 19, 64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Pinquart, M.; Sorensen, S. Differences between caregivers and noncaregivers in psychological health and physical health: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Aging 2003, 18, 250–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. World Health Organization. Global Action Plan on the Public Health Response to Dementia (2017–2025); World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  7. McKhann, G.; Drachman, D.; Folstein, M.; Katzman, R.; Price, D.; Stadlan, E.M. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984, 34, 939–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  9. Prince, M.; Wimo, A.; Guerchet, M.; Ali, G.-C.; Wu, Y.-T.; Prina, M. World Alzheimer Report 2015—The Global Impact of Dementia: An Analysis of Prevalence, Incidence, Cost and Trends; Alzheimer’s Disease International: London, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  10. World Bank. World Bank List of Global Economics; World Bank: Washington, DC, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  11. United Nations. World Population Prospects: The 2015 Revision, Key Findings and Advance Tables; United Nations: New York, NY, USA, 2015; p. 66. [Google Scholar]
  12. Medlive. CPA2019: Current Situation and Prospect of Mental Health Work in China. Available online: http://news.medlive.cn/psy/info-progress/show-156410_60.html (accessed on 12 April 2020).
  13. World Health Organization. Mental Health Atlas 2017; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  14. Milner, A.J.; Carter, G.; Pirkis, J.; Robinson, J.; Spittal, M.J. Letters, green cards, telephone calls and postcards: Systematic and meta-analytic review of brief contact interventions for reducing self-harm, suicide attempts and suicide. Br. J. Psychiatry 2015, 206, 184–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Peck, K.R.; Smitherman, T.A.; Baskin, S.M. Traditional and Alternative Treatments for Depression: Implications for Migraine Management. Headache 2015, 55, 351–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Liu, D.; Yang, J.; Liu, G.; Huang, E.; Jiang, S.; Huang, Y.; Zeng, Y.; Xu, L.; Ou, Y.; Cheng, G. Survey of cognitive dysfunction awareness status quo among chinese residents. Med. Soc. 2019, 32, 80–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gu, L.; Wu, S.; Zhao, S.; Zhou, H.; Zhang, S.; Gao, M.; Qu, Z.; Zhang, W.; Tian, D. Association of Social Support and Medication Adherence in Chinese Patients with Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Hughes, T.F.; Flatt, J.D.; Fu, B.; Chang, C.-C.H.; Ganguli, M. Engagement in social activities and progression from mild to severe cognitive impairment: The MYHAT study. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2013, 25, 587–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Thomas, P.A. Trajectories of Social Engagement and Limitations in Late Life. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2011, 52, 430–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Peng, D.; Fei, W. Productive Ageing in China: Development of Concepts and Policy Practice. Ageing Int. 2013, 38, 4–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Liu, H.; Lou, V.W.Q. Patterns of productive activity engagement as a longitudinal predictor of depressive symptoms among older adults in urban China. Aging Ment. Health 2017, 21, 1147–1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Li, Y.; Xu, L.; Chi, I.; Guo, P. Participation in Productive Activities and Health Outcomes among Older Adults in Urban China. Gerontologist 2014, 54, 784–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Short, S.E.; Zhai, F.Y.; Xu, S.Y.; Yang, M.L. China’s one-child policy and the care of children: An analysis of qualitative and quantitative data. Soc. Forces 2001, 79, 913–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Yasuda, T.; Iwai, N.; Yi, C.-C.; Xie, G. Intergenerational Coresidence in China, Japan, South Korea and Taiwan: Comparative Analyses Based on the East Asian Social Survey 2006. J. Comp. Fam. Stud. 2011, 42, 703–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Prokos, A.H.; Keene, J.R. The Life Course and Cumulative Disadvantage: Poverty among Grandmother-Headed Families. Res. Aging 2012, 34, 592–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Lee, J.; Bauer, J.W. Motivations for Providing and Utilizing Child Care by Grandmothers in South Korea. J. Marriage Fam. 2013, 75, 381–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Sieber, S.D. Toward a Theory of Role Accumulation. Am. Sociolog. Rev. 1974, 39, 567–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Goode, W.J. A Theory of Role Strain. Am. Sociolog. Rev. 1960, 25, 483–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Sneed, R.S.; Schulz, R. Grandparent Caregiving, Race, and Cognitive Functioning in a Population-Based Sample of Older Adults. J. Aging Health 2019, 31, 415–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Allen, J.S.; Bruss, J.; Damasio, H. The aging brain: The cognitive reserve hypothesis and hominid evolution. Am. J. Hum. Biolog. 2005, 17, 673–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Fried, L.P.; Carlson, M.C.; Freedman, M.; Frick, K.D.; Glass, T.A.; Hill, J.; McGill, S.; Rebok, G.W.; Seeman, T.; Tielsch, J.; et al. A social model for health promotion for an aging population: Initial evidence on the experience corps model. J. Urban Health 2004, 81, 64–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Jendrek, M.P. Grandparents Who Parent Their Grandchildren: Effects on Lifestyle. J. Marriage Fam. 1993, 55, 609–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Villar, F.; Celdran, M.; Triado, C. Grandmothers Offering Regular Auxiliary Care for Their Grandchildren: An Expression of Generativity in Later Life? J. Women Aging 2012, 24, 292–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Fuller-Thomson, E.; Serbinski, S.; McCormack, L. The rewards of caring for grandchildren: Black Canadian grandmothers who are custodial parents, co-parents, and extensive babysitters. Contemporary J. Res. Prac. Policy 2014, 1, 4–31. [Google Scholar]
  35. Minkler, M. Intergenerational Households Headed by Grandparents: Contexts, Realities, and Implications for Policy. J. Aging Stud. 1999, 13, 199–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Burn, K.F.; Henderson, V.W.; Ames, D.; Dennerstein, L.; Szoeke, C. Role of grandparenting in postmenopausal women’s cognitive health: Results from the Women’s Healthy Aging Project. Menopause 2014, 21, 1069–1074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Arpino, B.; Bordone, V. Does Grandparenting Pay Off? The Effect of Child Care on Grandparents’ Cognitive Functioning. J. Marriage Fam. 2014, 76, 337–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Guo-Gui, H.; Peng, D.U.; Gong, C. The Impacts of the Children Care on Grandparent’s Health among the Chinese Old People. Popul. Develop. 2016, 22, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Chen, F.; Liu, G. The Health Implications of Grandparents Caring for Grandchildren in China. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2012, 67, 99–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Song, L.; Li, L. Use It or Lose It? The Impact of Grandchild-Caring on Grandparents’ Cognitive Functioning in Rural China; XVIII ISA World Congress of Sociology: Yokohama, Japan, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  41. Grundy, E.M.; Albala, C.; Allen, E.; Dangour, A.D.; Elbourne, D.; Uauy, R. Grandparenting and psychosocial health among older Chileans: A longitudinal analysis. Aging Ment. Health 2012, 16, 1047–1057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Burn, K.; Szoeke, C. Is grandparenting a form of social engagement that benefits cognition in ageing? Maturitas 2015, 80, 122–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. van Bodegom, D.; Rozing, M.; May, L.; Kuningas, M.; Thomese, F.; Meij, H.; Westendorp, R. When Grandmothers Matter. Gerontology 2010, 56, 214–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hawkes, K.; O’Connell, J.F.; Jones, N.G.B.; Alvarez, H.; Charnov, E.L. Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1998, 95, 1336–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Zhang, W.; Feng, Q.; Liu, L.; Zhen, Z. Social Engagement and Health: Findings From the 2013 Survey of the Shanghai Elderly Life and Opinion. Int. J. Aging Hum. Develop. 2015, 80, 332–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Zhao, Y.; Hu, Y.; Smith, J.P.; Strauss, J.; Yang, G. Cohort Profile: The China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). Int. J. Epidemiol. 2014, 43, 61–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Feng, X.L.; Pang, M.F.; Beard, J. Health system strengthening and hypertension awareness, treatment and control: Data from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study. Bull. World Health Organ. 2014, 92, 29–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Fu, X.; Sun, N.; Xu, F.; Li, J.; Tang, Q.; He, J.; Wang, D.; Sun, C. Influencing factors of inequity in health services utilization among the elderly in China. Int. J. Equity Health 2018, 17, 144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Croezen, S.; Avendano, M.; Burdorf, A.; van Lenthe, F.J. Social Participation and Depression in Old Age: A Fixed-Effects Analysis in 10 European Countries. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2015, 182, 168–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Meng, Q.; Wang, H.; Strauss, J.; Langa, K.M.; Chen, X.; Wang, M.; Qu, Q.; Chen, W.; Kuang, W.; Zhang, N.; et al. Validation of neuropsychological tests for the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocol. Int. Psychoger. 2019, 31, 1709–1719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. McArdle, J.J.; Fisher, G.G.; Kadlec, K.M. Latent variable analyses of age trends of cognition in the health and retirement study, 1992–2004. Psychol. Aging 2007, 22, 525–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Fillenbaum, G.G.; van Belle, G.; Morris, J.C.; Mohs, R.C.; Mirra, S.S.; Davis, P.C.; Tariot, P.N.; Silverman, J.M.; Clark, C.M.; Welsh-Bohmer, K.A.; et al. Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD): The first twenty years. Alzheimers Dement. 2008, 4, 96–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Lei, X.; Smith, J.P.; Sun, X.; Zhao, Y. Gender Differences in Cognition in China and Reasons for Change over Time: Evidence from CHARLS. J. Econom. Ageing 2014, 4, 46–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Zhu, H.L.; Zeng, X.Q. Social activities and the cognition of rural older adults—Evidence from 2011–2015 CHARLS Data. China Labor 2019, 9, 25–39. [Google Scholar]
  55. Luo, Y.; Pan, X.; Zhang, Z. Productive activities and cognitive decline among older adults in China: Evidence from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study. Soc. Sci. Med. 2019, 229, 96–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Xiang, Y.; Zare, H.; Guan, C.; Gaskin, D. The impact of rural-urban community settings on cognitive decline: Results from a nationally-representative sample of seniors in China. BMC Geriatr. 2018, 18, 323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kim, J.; Park, E.-C.; Choi, Y.; Lee, H.; Lee, S.G. The impact of intensive grandchild care on depressive symptoms among older Koreans. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2017, 32, 1381–1391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Zelezna, L. Care-giving to grandchildren and elderly parents: Role conflict or family solidarity? Ageing Soc. 2018, 38, 974–994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Di Gessa, G.; Glaser, K.; Tinker, A. The impact of caring for grandchildren on the health of grandparents in Europe: A lifecourse approach. Soc. Sci. Med. 2016, 152, 166–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Ates, M. Does grandchild care influence grandparents’ self-rated health? Evidence from a fixed effects approach. Soc. Sci. Med. 2017, 190, 67–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Sidney, B.; Sandy, C.; Samuel, L.; Ashley, L.; Drystan, P.; Jenny, W.; Lee, J. Harmonized CHARLS Documentation; USC Program on Global Aging, Health, and Policy: Beijing, China, 2018; pp. 1–428. [Google Scholar]
  62. Vacha-Haase, T.; Thompson, B. How to estimate and interpret various effect sizes. J. Couns. Psychol. 2004, 51, 473–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Ferguson, C.J. An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pr. 2009, 40, 532–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Montgomery, D.C.; Peck, E.A.; Vining, G.G. Introduction to Linear Regression Analysis; John Wiley & Sons Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
  65. Cohen, J.A. Power primer. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 112, 155–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Glaser, K.F.; Di Gessa, G.; Ribe, E.; Tinker, A.; Price, D. Grandparenting in Europe—The role of family policy environments, cultures and structures on grandparent childcare. Gerontologist 2012, 52, 698. [Google Scholar]
  67. Hank, K.; Buber, I. Grandparents Caring for Their Grandchildren Findings From the 2004 Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe. J. Fam. Issues 2009, 30, 53–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Aassve, A.; Arpino, B.; Goisis, A. Grandparenting and mothers’ labour force participation: A comparative analysis using the generations and gender survey. Demogr. Res. 2012, 27, 53–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. King, V. The legacy of a grandparent’s divorce: Consequences for ties between grandparents and grandchildren. J.Marriage Fam. 2003, 65, 170–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Chen, F.; Liu, G.; Mair, C.A. Intergenerational Ties in Context: Grandparents Caring for Grandchildren in China. Soc. Forces 2011, 90, 571–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Unger, J. Urban Families in the Eighties: An Analysis of Chinese Surveys; University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  72. Cherlin, A.J.; Furstenberg, F.F. The new American Grandparent: A Place in the Family, a Life Apart; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  73. Chen, F.; Short, S.E.; Entwisle, B. The impact of grandparental proximity on maternal childcare in China. Pop. Res. Policy Rev. 2000, 19, 571–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Logan, J.R.; Bian, F.Q.; Bian, Y.J. Tradition and change in the urban Chinese family: The case of living arrangements. Soc. Forces 1998, 76, 851–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Zhou, F.; Duan, C. An overview on left-behind children in China. Popul. J. 2006, 3, 61–62. [Google Scholar]
  76. Burn, K.; Szoeke, C. Grandparenting predicts late-life cognition: Results from the Women’s Healthy Ageing Project. Maturitas 2015, 81, 317–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Skoog, I.; Lernfelt, B.; Landahl, S.; Palmertz, B.; Andreasson, L.-A.; Nilsson, L.; Persson, G.; Oden, A.; Svanborg, A. 15-year longitudinal study of blood pressure and dementia. Lancet 1996, 347, 1141–1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Livingston, G.; Sommerlad, A.; Orgeta, V.; Costafreda, S.G.; Huntley, J.; Ames, D.; Ballard, C.; Banerjee, S.; Burns, A.; Cohen-Mansfield, J.; et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet 2017, 390, 2673–2734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Kuiper, J.S.; Zuidersma, M.; Voshaar, R.C.O.; Zuidema, S.U.; van den Heuvel, E.R.; Stolk, R.P.; Smidt, N. Social relationships and risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Res. Rev. 2015, 22, 39–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2012, 11, 1006–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Neugarten, B.L.; Weinstein, K.K.J.J.o.M. The Changing American Grandparent. J. Marriage Fam. 1964, 26, 199–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Kornhaber, A. Contemporary Grandparenting; Sage: London, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  83. Sheehan, N.W.; Petrovic, K. Grandparents and their adult grandchildren: Recurring themes from the literature. Marriage Fam. Rev. 2008, 44, 99–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Cong, Z.; Silverstein, M. Caring for grandchildren and intergenerational support in rural China: A gendered extended family perspective. Ageing Soc. 2012, 32, 425–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Ku, L.-J.E.; Stearns, S.C.; Van Houtven, C.H.; Lee, S.-Y.D.; Dilworth-Anderson, P.; Konrad, T.R. Impact of Caring for Grandchildren on the Health of Grandparents in Taiwan. J.Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2013, 68, 1009–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Liechty, T.; Genoe, M.R. Older Men’s Perceptions of Leisure and Aging. Leis. Sci. 2013, 35, 438–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Campbell, A.; Yang, J. Leisure innovation among post-retirement women in north-eastern China. Ann. Leis. Res. 2011, 14, 325–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Betsy, W. Grandmotherhood as leisure? World Leis. Recr. 1996, 38, 15–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Tsai, F.-J.; Motamed, S.; Rougemont, A. The protective effect of taking care of grandchildren on elders’ mental health? Associations between changing patterns of intergenerational exchanges and the reduction of elders’ loneliness and depression between 1993 and 2007 in Taiwan. BMC Public Health 2013, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Holtzman, R.E.; Rebok, G.W.; Saczynski, J.S.; Kouzis, A.C.; Doyle, K.W.; Eaton, W.W. Social network characteristics and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2004, 59, P278–P284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Moen, P.; Robison, J.; Dempstermcclain, D. Caregiving and womens well-being—A life-course approach. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1995, 36, 259–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Szinovacz, M.E.; Davey, A. Effects of retirement and grandchild care on depressive symptoms. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 2006, 62, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Frankish, H.; Horton, R. Prevention and management of dementia: A priority for public health. Lancet 2017, 390, 2614–2615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Lee, Y.; Shinkai, S. Correlates of cognitive impairment and depressive symptoms among older adults in Korea and Japan. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2005, 20, 576–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Copeland, J.R.M.; Beekman, A.T.F.; Braam, A.W.; Dewey, M.E.; Delespaul, P.; Fuhrer, R.; Hooijer, C.; Lawlor, B.A.; Kivela, S.-L.; Lobo, A.; et al. Depression among older people in Europe: The EURODEP studies. World Psychiatry 2004, 3, 45–49. [Google Scholar]
  96. Musil, C.M.; Gordon, N.L.; Warner, C.B.; Zauszniewski, J.A.; Standing, T.; Wykle, M. Grandmothers and Caregiving to Grandchildren: Continuity, Change, and Outcomes over 24 Months. Gerontologist 2011, 51, 86–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  97. Jun, H.J. Educational Differences in the Cognitive Functioning of Grandmothers Caring for Grandchildren in South Korea. Res. Aging 2015, 37, 500–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Taylor, M.F.; Marquis, R.; Batten, R.; Coall, D. Understanding the Mental Health Travails of Custodial Grandparents. Occup. Ther. Ment. Health 2016, 32, 259–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Landry-Meyer, L.; Gerard, J.M.; Guzell, J.R. Caregiver Stress Among Grandparents Raising Grandchildren: The Functional Role of Social Support. Marriage Fam. Rev. 2005, 37, 171–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Yen, Y.C.; Yang, M.J.; Shih, C.H.; Lung, F.W. Cognitive impairment and associated risk factors among aged community members. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2004, 19, 564–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Xu, H.; Vorderstrasse, A.A.; McConnell, E.S.; Dupre, M.E.; Ostbye, T.; Bei, W. Migration and cognitive function: A conceptual framework for Global Health Research. Global Health Res. Policy 2018, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Alley, D.; Suthers, K.; Crimmins, E. Education and cognitive decline in older Americans—Results from the AHEAD sample. Res. Aging 2007, 29, 73–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Mukadam, N.; Sommerlad, A.; Huntley, J.; Livingston, G. Population attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia in low-income and middle-income countries: An analysis using cross-sectional survey data. Lancet Global Health 2019, 7, E596–E603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Zamberletti, J.; Cavrini, G.; Tomassini, C. Grandparents providing childcare in Italy. Eur. J. Ageing 2018, 15, 265–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Williams, G.C. Pleiotropy, natural selection and the evolution of senescence. Evolution 1957, 11, 398–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Hawkes, K. Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 2003, 15, 380–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. McGarrigle, C.A.; Timonen, V.; Layte, R. Choice and Constraint in the Negotiation of the Grandparent Role: A Mixed-Methods Study. Gerontol. Geriatr. Med. 2018, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Xu, H. Physical and mental health of Chinese grandparents caring for grandchildren and great-grandparents. Soc. Sci. Med. 2019, 229, 106–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Emilia, F.; Cabral-Cardoso, C. Gender asymmetries and the manager stereotype among management students. Women Manag. Rev. 2003, 18, 77–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Forsyth, D.R.; Schlenker, B.R.; Leary, M.R.; McCown, N.E. Self-presentational determinants of sex differences in leadership behavior. Small Group Behav. 1985, 16, 197–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  111. Silverstein, M.; Marenco, A. How Americans enact the grandparent role across the family life course. J. Fam. Issues 2001, 22, 493–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Glei, D.A.; Goldman, N.; Liu, I.W.; Weinstein, M. Sex differences in trajectories of depressive symptoms among older Taiwanese: The contribution of selected stressors and social factors. Aging Ment. Health 2013, 17, 773–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  113. Katsuyama, H.; Tomita, M.; Okuyama, T.; Hidaka, K.; Watanabe, Y.; Tamechika, Y.; Fushimi, S.; Saijoh, K. 5HTT polymorphisms are associated with job stress in Japanese workers. Leg. Med. (Tokyo) 2009, 11, S473–S476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Van Praag, H.M.; De Kloet, R.; Van Os, J. Stress, the Brain and Depression; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  115. Lupien, S.J.; Maheu, F.; Tu, M.; Fiocco, A.; Schramek, T.E. The effects of stress and stress hormones on human cognition: Implications for the field of brain and cognition. Brain Cogn. 2007, 65, 209–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  116. McEwen, B.S. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: Central role of the brain. Physiol. Rev. 2007, 87, 873–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  117. Hammen, C. Stress and depression. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 1, 293–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  118. Wilson, R.S.; Arnold, S.E.; Schneider, J.A.; Li, Y.; Bennett, D.A. Chronic distress, age-related neuropathology, and late-life dementia. Psychosom. Med. 2007, 69, 47–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Baker, L.A.; Silverstein, M. Preventive Health Behaviors among Grandmothers Raising Grandchildren. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2008, 63, S304–S311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  120. Agate, J.R.; Agate, S.T.; Liechty, T.; Cochran, L.J. ‘Roots and wings’: An exploration of intergenerational play. J. Intergener. Relationsh. 2018, 16, 395–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Ying, S.J. An Analysis on the Social Development of Square Dancing. In 2015 International Conference on Psychology, Information Science & Library Science, Lee, G., Ed.; Information Engineering Research Inst: Newark, NJ, USA, 2015; Volume 84, pp. 97–101. [Google Scholar]
  122. Lu, C.-M.; Chang, M.-Y.; Chu, M.-C. Effects of mahjong on the cognitive function of middle-aged and older people. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2015, 30, 995–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Items adopted for the neuropsychological tests for China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocol (CHARLS-HCAP).
Table 1. Items adopted for the neuropsychological tests for China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocol (CHARLS-HCAP).
Cognitive FunctionItemsTotal Scores
Episodic MemoryImmediate word recall: by asking participants to repeat after reading ten Chinese nouns in any order immediately10
Delayed word recall: by asking participants to repeat after reading ten Chinese nouns in any order four minutes later10
Mental StatusOrientation: by asking participants to name the day, month, year, season, and correct day of the week5
Visuoconstruction: by asking participants to re-draw a previously shown picture accurately1
Mathematical performance: by asking participants to subtract 7 from 100 up to 5 times5
Total 31
Table 2. Codes/definition of the control variables.
Table 2. Codes/definition of the control variables.
VariableCodes/Definition
Gender1 = Male; 2 = Female
Age≥45 years, calculated by the respondent’s birth year and month minus the interview year and month
Residence *0 = Urban; 1 = Rural/Residence is defined by household living region. Whether the region was rural or urban was defined by the National Bureau of Statistics of the People’s Republic of China
Education1 = illiterate; 2 = ≤primary school; 3 = middle school; 4 = ≥high school
Marital Status1 = divorced/widowed/single; 2 = married/cohabitating
Retired0 = No; 1 = Yes
Personal IncomeYuan, the sum of all personal income of respondents
Drinking0 = No; 1 = Yes, if the respondent has had an alcoholic beverage in the last 12 months
Smoking0 = No; 1 = Yes, if the respondent is still smoking currently
Number of Types of Chronic Diseases (NCDs)0 = none; 1 = one type of chronic disease; 2 = two types of chronic disease; 3 = three types of chronic disease and above/chronic diseases included whether the respondent reported having hypertension, diabetes or high blood sugar, cancer, chronic lung disease, heart problem, stroke, psychiatric problem, and arthritis
Activities of Daily Living (ADLs)0 = No; 1 = Yes, if the respondent experiences any difficulty dressing, bathing and showering, eating, getting in and out of bed, using the toilet, and controlling urination and defecation
Living Near Children0 = No; 1 = Yes, if any child is co-residing with the respondent and his/her spouse or partner
Economic Support0 = No; 1 = Yes, if the respondent or spouse has received economic support from any of their children in the past year
Weekly Contact0 = No; 1 = Yes, if a respondent or spouse has any weekly contact with any of their children in person
* The definition of residence was quoted from the Harmonized CHARLS Documentation [61].
Table 3. Sample characteristics of the selected participants.
Table 3. Sample characteristics of the selected participants.
VariableTotalMenWomenp
(n = 7236)(n = 3879)(n = 3357)
n (%)n (%)n (%)
Age <0.001 b
Median (Min, Max)62 (45, 101)63 (45, 92)60 (45, 101)
Residence <0.001 a
Urban2743 (37.9)1376 (35.5)1367 (40.7)
Rural4493 (62.1)2503 (64.5)1990 (59.3)
Education <0.001 a
Illiterate1107 (21.8)275 (10.1)832 (35.5)
≤Primary school2237 (44.0)1297 (47.4)940 (40.1)
Middle school1117 (22.0)721 (26.4)396 (16.9)
≥High school620 (11.2)442 (16.2)178 (7.6)
Missing215511441011
Marital Status <0.001 a
Divorced/widowed/single1454 (20.1)479 (12.3)975 (29.0)
Married/cohabitating5782 (79.9)3400 (87.7)2382 (71.0)
Retired <0.001 a
No4622 (65.1)2694 (70.7)1928 (58.6)
Yes2476 (34.9)1114 (29.3)1362 (41.4)
Missing1387167
Personal Income 0.155 b
Median (Min, Max)2640 (−65,775, 650,150)2605 (−65,775, 302,562)2700 (−8450, 650,150)
Missing361119171694
Drinking <0.001 a
No4624 (63.9)1766 (45.6)2858 (85.2)
Yes2608 (36.1)2111 (54.4)497 (14.8)
Missing422
Smoking <0.001 a
No5072 (70.1)1904 (49.1)3168 (94.4)
Yes2159 (29.9)1971 (50.9)188 (5.6)
Missing541
Number of Types of NCDs <0.001 a
01015 (17.1)608 (18.9)407 (14.8)
11449 (24.3)820 (25.5)629 (22.9)
21324 (22.2)694 (21.6)630 (23.0)
≥32164 (36.4)1088 (33.9)1076 (39.2)
Missing1284669615
ADLs <0.001 a
No5630 (78.5)3152 (82.0)2478 (74.4)
Yes1544 (21.5)693 (18.0)851 (25.6)
Missing623428
Living Near Children 0.001 a
No3313 (45.8)1849 (47.7)1464 (43.6)
Yes3923 (54.2)2030 (52.3)1893 (56.4)
Economic Support 0.010 a
No504 (7.0)298 (7.7)206 (43.6)
Yes6732 (93.0)3581 (92.3)3151 (93.9)
Weekly Contact <0.001 a
No1570 (21.7)930 (24.0)640 (19.1)
Yes5663 (78.3)2946 (76.0)2717 (80.9)
Missing330
Cognitive function <0.001 c
Mean (SD)13.97 (5.50)14.63 (5.09)13.20 (5.84)
N.B. Total percentages of some variables are not equal to 100 due to rounding. a—Outcomes of χ2, b—outcomes of Wilcoxon rank sum test, c—outcomes of independent sample t-test.
Table 4. Patterns of grandparent caregiving in male vs. female respondents.
Table 4. Patterns of grandparent caregiving in male vs. female respondents.
VariableTotalMenWomenp
(n = 7236)(n = 3879)(n = 3357)
n (%)n (%)n (%)
Care Grandchildren 0.477 a
No3408 (47.1)1842 (47.5)1566 (46.6)
Yes3828 (52.9)2037 (52.5)1791 (53.4)
Frequency <0.001 a
Never3408 (50.9)1842 (52.7)1566 (49.0)
Not regularly1753 (26.2)910 (26.0)843 (26.4)
Regularly1533 (22.9)745 (21.3)788 (24.7)
Missing294151143
Intensity <0.001 a
Never3408 (51.0)1842 (52.8)1566 (49.0)
Moderate grandchild care1300 (19.5)723 (20.7)577 (18.0)
Intensive grandchild care1973 (29.5)923 (26.5)1050 (32.9)
Missing274140134
Number of Grandchildren 0.135 a
None3408 (47.7)1842 (48.1)1566 (47.2)
13004 (42.0)1576 (41.1)1428 (43.1)
≥2735 (10.3)414 (10.8)321 (9.7)
N.B. Total percentages of some variables are not equal to 100 due to rounding. a—Outcomes of χ2.
Table 5. Cognitive function in different patterns of grandparent caregiving.
Table 5. Cognitive function in different patterns of grandparent caregiving.
VariableTotal (n = 7236)pMen (n = 3879)pWomen (n = 3357)p
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
Care Grandchildren
No13.01 (5.66)<0.001 c13.84 (5.24)<0.001 c12.04 (5.96)<0.001 c
Yes14.82 (5.21)15.35 (4.85)14.22 (5.54)
Frequency
Never13.16 (5.64)<0.001 d13.98 (5.23)<0.001 d12.07 (5.98)<0.001 d
Not regularly15.12 (5.02)15.58 (4.67)14.56 (5.35)
Regularly14.81 (5.23)15.45 (4.78)14.18 (5.56)
Intensity
Never13.16 (5.64)<0.001 d13.99 (5.22)<0.001 d12.09 (5.97)<0.001 d
Moderate grandchild care14.69 (5.27)15.44 (4.72)13.74 (5.76)
Intensive grandchild care14.98 (5.12)15.50 (4.81)14.52 (5.35)
Number of Grandchildren
None13.05 (5.65)<0.001 d13.87 (5.23)<0.001 d12.10 (5.97)<0.001 d
114.99 (5.14)15.52 (4.76)14.41 (5.48)
≥214.13 (5.42)14.74 (5.16)13.35 (5.65)
N.B. Total percentages of some variables are not equal to 100 due to rounding. c—Outcomes of independent sample t-test, d—outcomes of one-way ANOVA test.
Table 6. Associations between cognition and patterns of grandparent caregiving.
Table 6. Associations between cognition and patterns of grandparent caregiving.
VariableModel 1: TotalModel 2: MenModel 3: Women
β (SE)β (SE)β (SE)
(95% CI)(95% CI)(95% CI)
Care Grandchildren (reference: No)0.692 *** (0.179)0.703 ** (0.233)0.744 ** (0.275)
(0.340, 1.043)(0.245, 1.160)(0.203, 1.284)
Care Grandchildren × Age0.068 ** (0.021)0.099 *** (0.029)0.045 (0.032)
(0.026, 0.110)(0.043, 0.155)(−0.017, 0.108)
Age−0.140 *** (0.015)−0.154 *** (0.020)−0.136 *** (0.023)
(−0.170, −0.110)(−0.194, −0.114)(−0.181, −0.090)
Intensity (reference: Never)
Moderate grandchild care0.965 *** (0.239)0.797 ** (0.309)1.346 *** (0.371)
(0.496, 1.434)(0.191, 1.403)(0.618, 2.074)
Moderate grandchild care × Age0.102 *** (0.031)0.138 *** (0.039)0.066 (0.049)
(0.042, 0.162)(0.061, 0.215)(−0.030, 0.162)
Intensive grandchild care0.456 * (0.214)0.619 * (0.286)0.334 (0.318)
(0.037, 0.875)(0.057, 1.181)(−0.290, 0.958)
Intensive grandchild care × Age0.040 (0.027)0.058 (0.038)0.027 (0.038)
(−0.012, 0.092)(−0.016, 0.132)(−0.047, 0.102)
Age−0.142 *** (0.015)−0.154 *** (0.020)−0.139 *** (0.023)
(−0.171, −0.113)(−0.193, −0.116)(−0.184, −0.094)
Frequency (reference: Never)
Not Regularly0.807 *** (0.222)0.633 * (0.292)1.051 ** (0.334)
(0.372, 1.241)(0.061, 1.205)(0.395, 1.707)
Not Regularly × Age0.049 + (0.027)0.096 * (0.038)0.025 (0.041)
(−0.005, 0.103)(0.022, 0.170)(−0.054, 0.105)
Regularly0.578 * (0.227)0.880 ** (0.300)0.376 (0.344)
(0.132, 1.023)(0.292, 1.468)(−0.299, 1.050)
Regularly × Age0.093 ** (0.029)0.111 **(0.039)0.067 (0.044)
(0.036, 0.150)(0.034, 0.188)(−0.018, 0.153)
Age−0.143 *** (0.015)−0.158 *** (0.020)−0.139 *** (0.023)
(−0.173, −0.113)(−0.197, −0.119)(−0.184, −0.094)
Number of Grandchildren (reference: No)
1 child0.677 *** (0.190)0.620 * (0.247)0.788 ** (0.291)
(0.305, 1.049)(0.135, 1.105)(0.218, 1.359)
1 child × Age0.058 * (0.022)0.089 ** (0.031)0.041 (0.035)
(0.013, 0.103)(0.028, 0.149)(−0.026, 0.109)
≥2 children0.556 + (0.302)0.755 + (0.403)0.626 (0.461)
(−0.035, 1.148)(−0.035, 1.545)(−0.278, 1.529)
≥2 children × Age0.068 (0.042)0.115 * (0.058)0.019 (0.060)
(−0.013, 0.150)(0.002, 0.229)(−0.097, 0.136)
Age−0.130 *** (0.015)−0.150 *** (0.020)−0.132 *** (0.023)
(−0.165, −0.106)(−0.189, −0.111)(−0.177, −0.087)
R2adjusted0.33~0.350.26~0.280.40~0.41
VIF1~31~31~3
N.B. All models were adjusted for age, education, marital status, residence, retirement, personal income, drinking, smoking, number of types of chronic diseases, ADLs, living near children, economic support, and weekly contact; +: p < 0.1; *: p < 0.05; **: p < 0.01; ***: p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Liao, S.; Qi, L.; Xiong, J.; Yan, J.; Wang, R. Intergenerational Ties in Context: Association between Caring for Grandchildren and Cognitive Function in Middle-Aged and Older Chinese. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18010021

AMA Style

Liao S, Qi L, Xiong J, Yan J, Wang R. Intergenerational Ties in Context: Association between Caring for Grandchildren and Cognitive Function in Middle-Aged and Older Chinese. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(1):21. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18010021

Chicago/Turabian Style

Liao, Shiming, Ling Qi, Jie Xiong, Jie Yan, and Ruoxi Wang. 2021. "Intergenerational Ties in Context: Association between Caring for Grandchildren and Cognitive Function in Middle-Aged and Older Chinese" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 1: 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18010021

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop