Next Article in Journal
Effects of a 12-Week Change-of-Direction Sprints Training Program on Selected Physical and Physiological Parameters in Professional Basketball Male Players
Previous Article in Journal
Mindfulness and Psychological Distress in Kindergarten Teachers: The Mediating Role of Emotional Intelligence
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Consumers’ Implicit and Explicit Recall, Understanding and Perceptions of Products with Nutrition-Related Messages: An Online Survey

1
Department of Nutritional Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Medical Sciences Building, 1 King’s College Circle Rm 5368, Toronto, ON M5S 1A8, Canada
2
École de Nutrition, Université L and Al, Pavillon des Services, Bureau 2729-E, 2440 Boul. Hochelaga, Quebec City, QC G1V 0A6, Canada
3
Faculty of Law, University of Ottawa, 57 Louis Pasteur St, Ottawa, ON K1N 6N5, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(21), 8213; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17218213
Submission received: 9 October 2020 / Revised: 29 October 2020 / Accepted: 3 November 2020 / Published: 6 November 2020

Abstract

:
This study aimed to assess consumers’ implicit and explicit recall, understanding and perceptions of products with a nutrition claim and a symbol depicting ‘health,’ and to determine whether these perceptions differed among Nutrition Facts table (NFt) users vs. nonusers. In an online survey, participants (n = 1997) were randomized to one of eight conditions in a 2 × 2 × 2 factorial design, consisting of a label with a claim (present/absent) a heart-shaped symbol depicting ‘health’ (present/absent) for a healthier or less healthy soup. Participants were shown a label for 10 s and asked whether they recalled seeing a claim. If participants answered yes, they were then asked to describe their response using open-ended questions. Participants also rated the product’s perceived nutritional quality and purchase intentions using seven-point Likert scales. In the claim condition, most participants (75%) were able to recall the presence of a claim, while 12% incorrectly mentioned the presence of a claim when there was none. Claims likely attracted consumers’ attention and increased perceived nutritional quality, although with limited influence among NFt users (23%). The symbol depicting ‘health’ did not enhance perceived nutritional quality or purchase intentions. Although most participants (77%) made their decisions implicitly using the front of labels, those who used the NFt had a better understanding of the nutritional quality of products.

1. Introduction

In past decades, psychologists and consumer behavior scientists have investigated consumers’ evaluation of products and decision-making [1]. Psychologists have classified how people make decisions in two ways: One that is fast, automatic, and effortless, which relies on preconceived beliefs, intentions, patterns, perceptions, intuition, and/or memory, commonly known as “System 1” [1]; and a second, which is slow, effortful, and conscious, which relies on cognitive reflection of options when decisions are being made, often called “System 2” [1]. Thus, it is not surprising that people use the former system more often than the latter to make decisions.
When consumers retrieve information automatically and without reasoning (i.e., using System 1), this process is called an “implicit” evaluation [2,3]. An “explicit” evaluation occurs when consumers judge products through a thorough assessment of the available information on the product (i.e., using System 2) [2,4]. Implicit evaluations can be measured with response time-based methods and open-ended questions, while explicit evaluations can be measured through direct questioning using self-report methods, such as Likert scales [5].
There are many factors affecting food choices, such as the impact of brand and sponsorship [6,7,8], price [9,10,11,12], and health motivation [13,14,15]. However, time, nutrition education and knowledge, familiarity with foods, and visual attention to labels have been found to be among the most important aspects of decision-making during grocery shopping [9,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24]. These factors can greatly determine which path (automatic/fast vs. conscious/slow) consumers may use to evaluate and purchase products [1]. For example, when consumers have less time to make a food choice, only certain product characteristics (e.g., price, content or absence of a particular nutrient, expiration date) might be evaluated by consumers before purchase [25,26]. Nutrition knowledge can also influence how information on a food label is processed by consumers [27]. Moreover, greater nutrition knowledge has been associated with label use [20,28]. Attention to labels can also predict food choice [17,18]. The more time consumers spend viewing a label, the more likely it is that product will be purchased [16,17,29,30]. Therefore, visual attention becomes critical for product selection when consumers are challenged with many options at supermarkets [16].
In recent years, consumers’ attention and interest in the health aspects of food products have risen [24,30,31,32,33,34,35,36]. Health-related messages, such as nutrition claims, images, and symbols, are frequently used on food labels, likely due to their ability to attract consumers’ attention [29]. Nutrition claims, which include nutrient content claims and health claims, are a voluntary component of nutrition labelling [37]. They can be found on a considerable proportion on food labels [38,39,40,41]. Whereas these claims are largely regulated by governments, some products that display nutrition claims can be of poor nutritional quality [39,42,43]. In Canada, regulations for nutrition labelling are outlined in the Food and Drug Regulations, which are governed by Health Canada [44]. Some nutrition labelling features, such as the Nutrition Facts table, Ingredients List, and other information (e.g., weight, allergen statements), are mandatory [44]. Although nutrition claims are a voluntary component of nutrition labelling in Canada, there are defined criteria that must be met if nutrition claims are displayed on food labels [44,45,46]. Moreover, nutrition claims not only can create a ‘halo’ effect on products and increase perceived product nutritional quality among consumers [47,48,49], but they also are chosen more often compared to products without claims [50]. In addition, qualitative research has shown consumers may not be able to distinguish between different claims types (i.e., nutrient content claims vs. health claims) [51,52].
Health-oriented images and symbols are also used to infer health-related benefits on products, which are known to alter people’s beliefs and perceptions [47,48,53,54,55,56,57,58]. Health-oriented images can increase trust [53,55], create a ‘positive’ attitude toward the general qualities of a product [56], be used to indicate a ‘natural’ product [57], or can be falsely used by consumers to recall nonexistent product attributes [58]. For example, an earlier Canadian study found that, when foods were presented with unregulated “positive” front-of-pack (FOP) symbols, products were perceived as healthier [47]. Another study found that even simple symbols, such as an image of a plant leaf, may lead consumers to overrate a product’s “healthiness” [48]. A recent study also found that health-related images increase perceived benefits and decreased perceived risk of consuming dietary supplements [56]. In contrast, nutrient declarations (called the Nutrition Facts table (NFt) in Canada), which are part of the mandatory nutrition labelling requirement in many countries, are often overshadowed by nutrition claims and health-related symbols due their position on the back of labels and overwhelming numerical content [59].
In Canada, nutrition claims are often displayed on food labels [60]. Yet, few studies have assessed how simple cues, such as health-related symbols rather than text, perhaps aided by intuition or prior nutrition knowledge, impact consumers’ understanding of claims. Thus, the primary objective of this study was to assess consumers’ implicit and explicit recall, understanding and perceptions of products, with a nutrition claim and a symbol depicting ‘health.’ It was hypothesized that labels with a claim and/or a symbol depicting ‘health’ would be perceived as healthier than comparable labels without a claim or symbol. As label use has been associated with greater nutrition knowledge [20,28], secondary objectives assessed whether consumers’ perceptions of products with claims differed among those who used the NFt (i.e., those who likely used an explicit evaluation) compared to nonusers (i.e., those who likely used an implicit evaluation), as well as the characteristics of NFt users. We hypothesized that most participants would not use the NFt.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Survey Design, Participants, and Initial Randomization

This study was a randomized controlled trial and part of a larger online consumer survey, which had individual tasks and research questions [61,62,63]. This study was registered at Clinicaltrials.gov (#NCT03290118), and the Health Sciences Research Ethics Board at the University of Toronto (Protocol ID#34393) reviewed and approved the study prior to it being conducted between September–October 2017, as described elsewhere [61,62,63]. Anonymized data were provided to the research team. Briefly, recruitment was conducted via email by a marketing company. The eligibility criteria consisted of age ≥18 years, English as primary spoken language, residency in Canada (territories excluded), some responsibility for household grocery shopping, ownership of a smartphone version iPhone 3 or later or Android, and the ability to able to complete the survey on a device with a minimum screen size of 9.7 in. The sampling criteria were established to be nationally representative as much as possible in terms of gender, age, and location, based on 2011 census data. However, other inclusion criteria (e.g., shopping habits, smartphone ownership) resulted in a final sample that was no longer representative (i.e., a greater proportion of participants had college/university education and were two years younger than the median Canadian population).
A link to the consent form and survey was emailed to each participant, who viewed and provided informed consent prior to the start of the survey. Participants were compensated with $10 or the equivalent in Air Miles® if the survey was completed. Participants’ self-reported sociodemographic information (e.g., gender, age, education, income, ethnicity) was also collected. As part of the baseline data collection, participants answered a Canadian-adapted health literacy test (Newest Vital Sign) [64,65], in which participants were asked to interpret textual and numerical information from an NFt [64]. Participants were initially randomized to be in one front-of-pack (FOP) labelling condition: Control (no front-of-pack), warning labels (WL), health star rating (HSR), or traffic light labels (TLL). After randomization, participants were asked to test the FoodFlip©, which is a smartphone application that provides nutrition information using different front-of-pack symbols (WL, HSR, or TLL, and a control with an NFt). The results of the FoodFlip© trial have been described elsewhere [63]. For the purpose of this phase of the survey, no front-of-pack labelling was presented on any label, as it was not part of the objectives. The experimental design and stimuli for this particular study are described in the following section. The CONSORT diagram and checklist are provided in Supplementary Figure S1 and Table S1.

2.2. Experimental Design and Stimuli

Participants were re-randomized in a 1:1 ratio to one of eight conditions consisting of a label with a nutrient content claim (present, absent) and a symbol representing ‘health’ (present, absent) for soups of two different levels of nutritional quality (healthier, less healthy) (Figure 1a), in a 2 × 2 × 2 factorial design (8 groups, n = 250/group). The sample size was estimated to detect an effect size of 0.5 in the Likert scale, with at least 239 participants per group (power = 0.80, two-sided α = 0.05, and SD = 1.95). Randomization was conducted by the marketing company using an online computerized system. Mock labels of chicken noodle soups (same brand and design with four different variations) were created based on comparable products found in the Canadian packaged food supply [66]. A “low in saturated and trans fats” was selected as the nutrient content claim, since this type of product could qualify for such a claim and it can also be found on many similar products in grocery stores in Canada [60]. We also avoided a sodium-related claim, which is a claim often used on soups [60]. A heart-shaped bowl was selected as the symbol depicting ‘health,’ since images implying health functions are often (incorrectly) linked to health-related claims [58] and the heart symbol was identified as one used on Canadian food labels. The nutritional quality of soups was determined using the Food Standards Australia New Zealand Nutrient Profiling Scoring Criterion (FSANZ-NPSC), which is a nutrient profiling (NP) model used to determine the eligibility of products to carry health claims [67]. Briefly, in this NP model, a product gains ‘baseline points’ for its content of nutrients to limit (calories, sodium, saturated fat, and sugars), and points are deducted if a food contains nutrients or ingredients to encourage (protein, fiber, and fruits, vegetables, nuts and legumes) [67]. A final score is calculated and assessed against established cutoffs that, if exceeded, categorizes products as “less healthy.” Nutrition information for the “healthier” and “less healthy” soups are presented in Figure 1b.
The experimental design was structured into two tasks, modelled after a method used earlier to detect implicit evaluations over nutrition and health claims, proposed by Klepacz and colleagues [58], and methods used to detect explicit evaluations by Wills et al. [24] and Wong et al. [53,68].
Task one. This task had two phases: An encoding phase and a recall phase. In the encoding phase [58], participants were shown a label on the screen for 10 s (Figure 2a). In the recall or recognition phase [58], the label was removed from the screen, and participants were asked if they recalled seeing a claim on the label. Participants were also asked to describe their understanding of the claim using open-ended questions (Figure 2b).
Task two. Participants were shown the same label again, which was left on the screen, and were asked to rate product’s nutritional quality and purchase intentions using seven-point Likert scales, where 1 was lowest nutritional quality/purchase intention and 7 was the highest nutritional quality/purchase intention. Participants had the option to look at the NFt by clicking at the hyperlink provided at the bottom of the screen, with no time restriction (Figure 2c).

2.3. Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis tests examined proportions of participants who recalled seeing a claim on the label and differences in those proportions, both overall and by condition. Responses to the open-ended questions were analyzed to identify key themes, which were later coded thematically and quantified. Generalized linear models with Bonferroni correction (adjusted for gender, education, income, ethnicity, and health literacy) were conducted to assess perceived nutritional quality and purchase intentions by condition and stratified by NFt use. We also conducted sensitivity analyses to determine whether results differed by gender and healthy literacy.

3. Results

Participants’ characteristics (n = 1997) are presented in Table 1. Participants were between 18 and 85 years old, with an average age of 39 years. Of the respondents, 52% were female, half had university education, were mostly white, and had adequate health literacy. The initial results showed that the nutritional quality of the soups did not have significant effects on consumers’ recall (p = 0.63, as per the Kruskal–Wallis test) or consumers’ perceptions (p = 0.26, as per the generalized linear model), meaning that the nutritional quality of the soups did not influence participants’ ability to better recall or influence their perceptions. Therefore, participants were combined into four groups based on the label they were shown: (1) No Claim No Symbol, (2) Nutrient Content Claim, (3) Symbol depicting ‘health,’ and (4) Claim + Symbol (Figure 1).

3.1. Claim Recall and Understanding of the Claim

The results showed that most participants who saw the soup with the claim displayed on the label were able to recall it (Table 2), with 73% (n = 340/463) of participants in group 2 and 76% of participants in group 4 (n = 352/463) correctly recalling the claim. We also found that 12% (n = 55/457) and 11% (n = 50/455) of participants in groups 1 and 3, respectively, thought they had read a claim, even though no claim was displayed on those labels. Of the 1997 participants, only 33% (n = 658) provided a description of the claim, and 30% (n = 595) of participants provided a statement about their understanding of the claim, mostly given by those in groups 2 and 4 (Table 2). When responses were analyzed from those who provided feedback, we found that most participants identified the claim as being related to fats (68%). Interestingly, 16% mentioned a “sodium-related” claim alongside a fat claim, although no mention of sodium was made on package. Likewise, when asked about the meaning of the claim, 50% of participants interpreted the claim as being related to fat, 27% thought the product was healthy, and 11% of participants thought the product had a sodium-related claim in addition to fat claims.

3.2. Perceived Nutritional Quality and Purchase Intentions

Figure 3 shows consumers perceptions (details in Supplementary Table S2). As expected, products displaying the claim (Figure 3A, “All,” groups 2, 4) were perceived as healthier than those without (i.e., ‘halo’ effect). However, the health-related symbol did not magnify the effect of a claim as hypothesized, with no significant differences between participants in groups 2 and 4 (pairwise comparison p = 1).
Only 23% (n = 401/1755) of participants clicked on the NFt link (Figure 3A). Among participants who did not click the NFt link (“Non-NFt users”), soups with a claim were perceived as healthier compared to those without the claim (p < 0.001). For “NFt users” (Figure 3A), we did not find significant differences. Whereas we did not find significant differences between healthier and less healthy soups among all participants, we found that NFt users were able to discriminate products with varying nutritional composition. For example, NFt users perceived the soup with better nutritional quality as healthier (mean = 3.2, Confidence Intervals (CI) = 2.9–3.4) compared to the less healthy soup (mean = 2.5, CI = 2.3–2.7, p < 0.001).
Regarding purchase intentions, we found similar results on perceived nutritional quality (Figure 3B). Participants had significantly higher purchase intentions for soups with a claim (group 2, mean = 3.3, CI = 3.0–3.5) and soups with a claim and symbol (group 4, mean = 3.2, CI = 3.0–3.5) than for soups without a claim (group 1, mean = 2.9, CI = 2.7–3.1) and soups with a symbol only (group 3, mean = 2.6, CI = 2.6–3.0). Whereas only 7% (n = 124/1823) of participants clicked on the NFt link when asked about their purchase intentions, we did not find any significant differences in purchase intentions between groups (p = 0.61). Detailed information is provided in Supplementary Table S2.
We did not collect information on the time that participants may have looked at/evaluated the NFt.

3.3. Sensitivity Analyses

An interesting finding was that male participants tended to rate products “healthier” than female participants. However, overall, both genders perceived products with claims as “healthier” than products without claims (Table 3). A similar result was found when products were assessed by level of health literacy. Participants with likely/possible low level of health literacy rated products as “healthier” compared to those with adequate health literacy (Table 3). Yet, our main finding was consistent with what we have identified: Products with claims were perceived as “healthier” than those without claims.

4. Discussion

This study assessed consumers’ recall and understanding of nutrition claims and health-related symbols (through an assessment of their implicit evaluation of labels) and their impact on perceived nutritional quality and purchase intentions (through an assessment of their implicit/explicit evaluation of labels).
Overall, our results support earlier observations that nutrition claims likely attract consumers’ attention [29]. For instance, more than 70% of participants that had a claim displayed on the label (groups 2 and 4) were able to recall it, despite labels being shown for only 10 s. Our results support previous research that has identified, using eye-tracking technology, that nutrition information is one of the most viewed elements on a food label [13,18,30].
Importantly, we also found that 16% of participants mentioned a sodium-related message (in conjunction with fat claims), despite no claim or symbol showing that information. This finding could be related to participants’ preconceptions about levels of sodium in soups. Perhaps for some participants, product assessment was based on recall of information ‘previously learned’ (i.e., through System 1 [1]), which could have triggered their memory to associate that soups often carry sodium claims, as shown in previous Canadian research [60].
This study also showed that nutrition claims were more likely to create a ‘halo’ effect, regardless of product’s nutritional quality, especially among the majority who did not click on the NFt. This finding is in line with a growing body of evidence [47,49,62,68,69,70,71,72,73], suggesting that products with nutrition claims are perceived “healthier,” and therefore, nutrition claims are more likely to be used as a marketing tool rather than to promote healthier food choices among consumers. These results are worrying, as claims are often used on ultra-processed foods [38,74,75]. We also found that, without an accompanying written claim, an image implying a health function [58] (i.e., heart-shaped bowl) did not enhance the ‘halo’ effect already created by the claim, as we did not find significant differences on the ratings of perceived nutritional quality between products with a claim and products with both a claim and symbol, even among Non-NFt users. Similar results were also found when assessing purchase intentions.
Results demonstrated that consumers were more likely to assess products quickly using the information provided on the front of the label (likely using System 1, fast decision-making), as only one-quarter of participants in this sample clicked on the NFt link. This proportion, which is much lower than what has been self-reported [76], is probably a more accurate reflection of the actual purchase situation at the point of sale. Importantly, previous research has shown that attention to labels can be significantly disrupted when there is a time constraint [18]. It could be hypothesized that, because the information presented on the NFt requires time and a cognitive process by consumers (i.e., requires consumers to transition from System 1 to System 2 thinking), the NFt therefore is less preferable than short-text claims and images that are already “interpreting” nutrition information for consumers. This assumption concurs with published psychology research concluding that consumers more often rely on System 1 than on System 2 to make choices [1].
As consumers may not often use the NFt or have enough time to thoroughly assess product labels when shopping, FOP labelling that displays nutrition information in a prominent, easy, and simple way [77,78,79] might be ideally suited for “System 1 fast decision-making.” FOP labelling can attract consumers’ attention more quickly compared to nutrition claims [79]. FOP labelling, which highlights nutrients of public health concern, could be very useful for consumers, particularly when trying to quickly differentiate between healthier and less healthy food options, and might counterbalance the ‘halo’ effect created by claims, especially for less healthy foods [49,79].
As an online survey, this study might not represent in-store consumer behaviors. However, as technology is expanding toward a more digital world, the prevalence of online grocery shopping will likely increase. It is important to note that this study was conducted in 2017, more than two years prior the global pandemic of COVID-19. Thus, many of our participants may not have experienced online grocery shopping before engaging in this study. Given the growth of online grocery shopping in the current environment, it might be worth repeating this study to determine whether the same results are found. Another limitation is that participants were required to have a smartphone, and therefore, the sample is not representative of the population in Canada. However, in 2017, over 90% of the population had access to Long-Term Evolution Advance network services [80]. We only evaluated one type of food (soup), and therefore, extrapolation to other foods and beverages is limited. Another limitation is that the ‘heart-shaped’ symbol depicting ‘health’ may not have been visible enough to attract consumers’ attention. We also were not able to identify what the first element of the label viewed by participants was or how long they observed a certain element or characteristic. Eye-tracking technology can overcome this shortcoming [18,30,79]. This study was strengthened by the use of a randomized experimental design and a large sample size calculated to have statistical power to detect differences among all groups, and by including healthier and less healthy versions of the same product.

5. Conclusions

This study raised important issues about the use of nutrition claims on food labels. As discussed, claims likely attract consumers’ attention. However, not all consumers may identify or understand the message of claims correctly. Thus, claims can mislead consumers. The presence of the claim increased perceived nutritional quality (i.e., the ‘halo’ effect) on consumers, regardless of the product’s nutritional composition, which is particularly concerning given the number of less healthy products that might display nutrition claims on their labels. Those few participants who clicked on and likely used the NFt had a better understanding of the nutritional quality of the product. Therefore, the influence of the claim was reduced. The influence of the health-related heart-shaped symbol was limited. As consumers are less likely to use nutrient declarations when assessing and/or purchasing foods, results from this study stress the importance of providing consumers with nutrition labels that also support fast decision-making.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/17/21/8213/s1, Figure S1: CONSORT Diagram, Table S1: CONSORT checklist of information to include when reporting a randomized trial, Table S2. Means ratings of perceived nutritional quality (a) and purchase intentions (b) by condition overall and by Nutrition Facts table use.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.F.-A., L.V., M.A., A.O. and M.R.L.; Data curation, B.F.-A., L.V., M.A. and A.O.; Formal analysis, B.F.-A.; Funding acquisition, M.R.L.; Methodology, B.F.-A., L.V., M.A. and A.O.; Project administration, L.V.; Supervision, M.R.L.; Validation, B.F.-A., L.V., M.A. and A.O.; Writing—original draft, B.F.-A.; Writing—review & editing, B.F.-A., L.V., M.A., A.O. and M.R.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Support for this research was obtained from the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada #PG140611 (MRL), E.W. McHenry Research Chair Grant (MRL), Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Strategic Operating Grants 201103SOK-118150 and 2016PJT-152979 (MRL), Burroughs Wellcome Fund, Innovation in Regulatory Science Grant #1014187 (MRL); CIHR Frederick Banting and Charles Best Canada Graduate Scholarship GSD-152299 (BFA); Department of Nutritional Sciences Graduate Student Fellowship (BFA), CIHR Banting Postdoctoral Fellowship (LV), Mitacs-Elevate Postdoctoral Fellowship (MA), Ontario Graduate Scholarship (MA).

Acknowledgments

Authors would also like to thank JoAnne Arcand for her critical review of an earlier draft.

Conflicts of Interest

Prior coming to the University of Toronto Beatriz Franco-Arellano was a PepsiCo Mexico employee. The company had no connection or funding to the research. Lana Vanderlee declares no conflict of interests. Mavra Ahmed was a Mitacs Elevate Postdoctoral Fellow at the University of Toronto from September 2017 to August 2019, jointly funded by the Government of Canada Mitacs Program and the Nestlé Research Center. None of the previous companies/organizations were involved in any way in the present research. Angela Oh declares no conflict of interests. Mary L’Abbé declares that she has received research grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Canadian Stroke Network, International Development Research Centre, University of Toronto, McHenry Chair (unrestricted research funds). The funding sponsors had no role in the design of the study, data collection and analyses, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  1. Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow; Anchor Canada: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bekker, G.A.; Fischer, A.R.H.; Tobi, H.; van Trijp, H.C.M. Explicit and implicit attitude toward an emerging food technology: The case of cultured meat. Appetite 2017, 108, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Fazio, R.H. Attitudes as Object-Evaluation Associations of Varying Strength. Soc. Cogn. 2007, 25, 603–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Gawronski, B.; Bodenhausen, G.V. Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychol. Bull. 2006, 132, 692–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Hendrick, T.A.M.; Fischer, A.R.H.; Tobi, H.; Frewer, L.J. Self-reported attitude scales: Current practice in adequate assessment of reliability, validity, and dimensionality. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 43, 1538–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Kelly, B.; Baur, L.A.; Bauman, A.E.; King, L.; Chapman, K.; Smith, B.J. “Food company sponsors are kind, generous and cool”: (mis)conceptions of junior sports players. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2011, 8, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Polonsky, M.J.; Speed, R. Linking sponsorship and cause related marketing: Complementarities and conflicts. Eur. J. Mark. 2001, 35, 1361–1389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Cavanagh, K.V.; Forestell, C.A. The effect of brand names on flavor perception and consumption in restrained and unrestrained eaters. Food Qual. Prefer. 2013, 28, 505–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Chou, S.Y.; Grossman, M.; Saffer, H. An economic analysis of adult obesity: Results from the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. J. Health Econ. 2004, 23, 565–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Block, J.P.; Chandra, A.; McManus, K.D.; Willett, W.C. Point-of-purchase price and education intervention to reduce consumption of sugary soft drinks. Am. J. Public Health 2010, 100, 1427–1433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Powell, L.M.; Jones, K.; Duran, A.C.; Tarlov, E.; Zenk, S.N. The price of ultra-processed foods and beverages and adult body weight: Evidence from U.S. veterans. Econ. Hum. Biol. 2019, 34, 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Kern, D.M.; Auchincloss, A.H.; Stehr, M.F.; Diez Roux, A.V.; Moore, K.A.; Kanter, G.P.; Robinson, L.F. Neighborhood price of healthier food relative to unhealthy food and its association with type 2 diabetes and insulin resistance: The multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Prev. Med. 2018, 106, 122–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Tanner, S.A.; McCarthy, M.B.; O’Reilly, S.J. Exploring the roles of motivation and cognition in label-usage using a combined eye-tracking and retrospective think aloud approach. Appetite 2019, 135, 146–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Hung, Y.; Grunert, K.G.; Hoefkens, C.; Hieke, S.; Verbeke, W. Motivation outweighs ability in explaining European consumers’ use of health claims. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 58, 34–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Visschers, V.H.; Hess, R.; Siegrist, M. Health motivation and product design determine consumers’ visual attention to nutrition information on food products. Public Health Nutr. 2010, 13, 1099–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Gidlöf, K.; Anikin, A.; Lingonblad, M.; Wallin, A. Looking is buying. How visual attention and choice are affected by consumer preferences and properties of the supermarket shelf. Appetite 2017, 116, 29–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Pieters, R.; Warlop, L. Visual attention during brand choice: The impact of time pressure and task motivation. Int. J. Res. Mark. 1999, 16, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Fenko, A.; Nicolaas, I.; Galetzka, M. Does attention to health labels predict a healthy food choice? An eye-tracking study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 69, 57–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. O’Brien, M.C.; McConnon, A.; Hollywood, L.E.; Cuskelly, G.J.; Barnett, J.; Raats, M.; Dean, M. Let’s talk about health: Shoppers’ discourse regarding health while food shopping. Public Health Nutr. 2015, 18, 1001–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Soederberg Miller, L.M.; Cassady, D.L. The effects of nutrition knowledge on food label use. A review of the literature. Appetite 2015, 92, 207–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Malloy-Weir, L.; Cooper, M. Health literacy, literacy, numeracy and nutrition label understanding and use: A scoping review of the literature. J. Hum. Nutr. Diet. 2017, 30, 309–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. McGowan, L.; Pot, G.K.; Stephen, A.M.; Lavelle, F.; Spence, M.; Raats, M.; Hollywood, L.; McDowell, D.; McCloat, A.; Mooney, E.; et al. The influence of socio-demographic, psychological and knowledge-related variables alongside perceived cooking and food skills abilities in the prediction of diet quality in adults: A nationally representative cross-sectional study. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2016, 13, 111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Soederberg Miller, L.M.; Cassady, D.L.; Applegate, E.A.; Beckett, L.A.; Wilson, M.D.; Gibson, T.N.; Ellwood, K. Relationships among food label use, motivation, and dietary quality. Nutrients 2015, 7, 1068–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Wills, J.M.; Storcksdieck genannt Bonsmann, S.; Kolka, M.; Grunert, K.G. European consumers and health claims: Attitudes, understanding and purchasing behaviour. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2012, 71, 229–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. van Herpen, E.; Trijp, H.C. Front-of-pack nutrition labels. Their effect on attention and choices when consumers have varying goals and time constraints. Appetite 2011, 57, 148–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Mandle, J.; Tugendhaft, A.; Michalow, J.; Hofman, K. Nutrition labelling: A review of research on consumer and industry response in the global South. Glob. Health Action 2015, 8, 25912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Walters, A.; Long, M. The effect of food label cues on perceptions of quality and purchase intentions among high-involvement consumers with varying levels of nutrition knowledge. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2012, 44, 350–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Grunert, K.G.; Wills, J.M.; Fernández-Celemín, L. Nutrition knowledge, and use and understanding of nutrition information on food labels among consumers in the UK. Appetite 2010, 55, 177–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Steinhauser, J.; Janssen, M.; Hamm, U. Consumers’ purchase decisions for products with nutrition and health claims: What role do product category and gaze duration on claims play? Appetite 2019, 141, 104337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Steinhauser, J.; Janssen, M.; Hamm, U. Who Buys Products with Nutrition and Health Claims? A Purchase Simulation with Eye Tracking on the Influence of Consumers’ Nutrition Knowledge and Health Motivation. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Hoefkens, C.; Verbeke, W. Consumers’ health-related motive orientations and reactions to claims about dietary calcium. Nutrients 2013, 5, 82–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Hieke, S.; Kuljanic, N.; Wills, J.M.; Pravst, I.; Kaur, A.; Raats, M.M.; van-Trijp, H.C.; Verbeke, W.; Grunert, K.G. The role of health-related claims and health-related symbols in consumer behaviour: Design and conceptual framework of the CLYMBOL project and initial results. Nutr. Bull. 2015, 40, 66–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Czarniecka-Skubina, E.; Górska-Warsewicz, H.; Laskowski, W.; Jeznach, M. Consumer Choices and Service Quality in the University Canteens in Warsaw, Poland. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Tugault-Lafleur, C.N.; Black, J.L. Differences in the Quantity and Types of Foods and Beverages Consumed by Canadians between 2004 and 2015. Nutrients 2019, 11, 526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Sajdakowska, M.; Gębski, J.; Żakowska-Biemans, S.; Jeżewska-Zychowicz, M. Willingness to eat bread with health benefits: Habits, taste and health in bread choice. Public Health 2019, 167, 78–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sajdakowska, M.; Gębski, J.; Gutkowska, K.; Żakowska-Biemans, S. Importance of Health Aspects in Polish Consumer Choices of Dairy Products. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. CODEX Alimentarius. Guidelines for Use of Nutrition and Health Claims (CAC/GL 23-1997). Available online: http://www.fao.org/ag/humannutrition/32444-09f5545b8abe9a0c3baf01a4502ac36e4.pdf (accessed on 4 November 2020).
  38. Pulker, C.E.; Scott, J.A.; Pollard, C.M. Ultra-processed family foods in Australia: Nutrition claims, health claims and marketing techniques. Public Health Nutr. 2017, 21, 38–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Al-Ani, H.H.; Devi, A.; Eyles, H.; Swinburn, B.; Vandevijvere, S. Nutrition and health claims on healthy and less-healthy packaged food products in New Zealand. Br. J. Nutr. 2016, 116, 1087–1094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Kaur, A.; Scarborough, P.; Matthews, A.; Payne, S.; Mizdrak, A.; Rayner, M. How many foods in the UK carry health and nutrition claims, and are they healthier than those that do not? Public Health Nutr. 2016, 19, 988–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Lalor, F.; Kennedy, J.; Flynn, M.A.; Wall, P.G. A study of nutrition and health claims--a snapshot of what’s on the Irish market. Public Health Nutr. 2010, 13, 704–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Kaur, A.; Scarborough, P.; Hieke, S.; Kusar, A.; Pravst, I.; Raats, M.; Rayner, M. The nutritional quality of foods carrying health-related claims in Germany, The Netherlands, Spain, Slovenia and the United Kingdom. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2016, 70, 1462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Franco-Arellano, B.; Labonté, M.; Bernstein, J.T.; L’Abbé, M.R. Examining the Nutritional Quality of Canadian Packaged Foods and Beverages with and without Nutrition Claims. Nutrients 2018, 10, 832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Government of Canada. Regulations amending the Food and Drug Regulations (Nutrition Labelling, Nutrient Content Claims and Health Claims). Available online: http://publications.gc.ca/gazette/archives/p2/2003/2003-01-01/pdf/g2-13701.pdf (accessed on 19 June 2018).
  45. Health Canada. Nutrition Claims. Bureau of Nutritional Sciences Food Directorate Health Products and Food Branch. Available online: http://www.hc-sc.gc.ca/fn-an/label-etiquet/nutrition/cons/claims-reclam/index-eng.php2003 (accessed on 4 November 2020).
  46. Government of Canada. Regulations Amending the Food and Drug Regulations (Nutrition Labelling, Other Labelling Provisions and Food Colours). Canada Gazette Part II 2016, 150, 4351–4479. Available online: http://gazette.gc.ca/rp-pr/p2/2016/2016-12-14/html/sor-dors305-eng.php (accessed on 4 November 2020).
  47. Emrich, T.E.; Qi, Y.; Mendoza, J.E.; Lou, W.; Cohen, J.E.; L’abbé, M.R. Consumer perceptions of the Nutrition Facts table and front-of-pack nutrition rating systems. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 2014, 39, 417–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Saba, A.; Vassallo, M.; Shepherd, R.; Lampila, P.; Arvola, A.; Dean, M.; Winkelmann, M.; Claupein, E.; Lähteenmäki, L. Country-wise differences in perception of health-related messages in cereal-based food products. Food Qual. Prefer. 2010, 21, 385–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Talati, Z.; Pettigrew, S.; Dixon, H.; Neal, B.; Ball, K.; Hughes, C. Do Health Claims and Front-of-Pack Labels Lead to a Positivity Bias in Unhealthy Foods? Nutrients 2016, 8, 787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Kaur, A.; Scarborough, P.; Rayner, M. A systematic review, and meta-analyses, of the impact of health-related claims on dietary choices. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2017, 14, 93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Benson, T.; Lavelle, F.; McCloat, A.; Mooney, E.; Bucher, T.; Egan, B.; Dean, M. Are the Claims to Blame? A Qualitative Study to Understand the Effects of Nutrition and Health Claims on Perceptions and Consumption of Food. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Hodgkins, C.E.; Egan, B.; Peacock, M.; Klepacz, N.; Miklavec, K.; Pravst, I.; Pohar, J.; Gracia, A.; Groeppel-Klein, A.; Rayner, M.; et al. Understanding How Consumers Categorise Health Related Claims on Foods: A Consumer-Derived Typology of Health-Related Claims. Nutrients 2019, 11, 539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Wong, C.L.; Mendoza, J.; Henson, S.J.; Qi, Y.; Lou, W.; L’Abbé, M.R. Consumer attitudes and understanding of cholesterol-lowering claims on food: Randomize mock-package experiments with plant sterol and oat fibre claims. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 68, 946–952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hall, M.G.; Lazard, A.J.; Grummon, A.H.; Mendel, J.R.; Taillie, L.S. The impact of front-of-package claims, fruit images, and health warnings on consumers’ perceptions of sugar-sweetened fruit drinks: Three randomized experiments. Prev. Med. 2020, 132, 105998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Carrillo, E.; Fiszmana, S.; Lähteenmäkib, L.; Varela, P. Consumers’ perception of symbols and health claims as health-related label messages. A cross-cultural study. Food Res. Int. 2014, 62, 653–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Delivett, C.P.; Klepacz, N.A.; Farrow, C.V.; Thomas, J.M.; Raats, M.M.; Nash, R.A. Front-of-pack images can boost the perceived health benefits of dietary products. Appetite 2020, 155, 104831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Smith, V.; Barratt, D.; Sørensen, H.S. Do natural pictures mean natural tastes? Assessing visual semantics experimentally. Cogn. Semiot. 2015, 8, 53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Klepacz, N.A.; Nash, R.A.; Egan, M.B.; Hodgkins, C.E.; Raats, M.M. When is an image a health claim? A false-recollection method to detect implicit inferences about products’ health benefits. Health Psychol. 2016, 35, 898–907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Campos, S.; Doxey, J.; Hammond, D. Nutrition labels on pre-packaged foods: A systematic review. Public Health Nutr. 2011, 14, 1496–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Franco-Arellano, B.; Bernstein, J.T.; Norsen, S.; Schermel, A.; L’Abbé, M.R. Assessing nutrition and other claims on food labels: A repeated cross-sectional analysis of the Canadian food supply. BMC Nutr. 2017, 3, 74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Vanderlee, L.; Franco-Arellano, B.; Ahmed, M.; Oh, A.; Lou, W.; L’Abbé, M.R. The efficacy of ‘high in’ warning labels, health star and traffic light front-of-package labelling: An online randomised control trial. Public Health Nutr. 2020, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Franco-Arellano, B.; Vanderlee, L.; Ahmed, M.; Oh, A.; L’Abbé, M. Influence of front-of-pack labelling and regulated nutrition claims on consumers’ perceptions of product healthfulness and purchase intentions: A randomized controlled trial. Appetite 2020, 149, 104629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Ahmed, M.; Oh, A.; Vanderlee, L.; Franco-Arellano, B.; Schermel, A.; Lou, W.; L’Abbé, M.R. A randomized controlled trial examining consumers’ perceptions and opinions on using different versions of a FoodFlip© smartphone application for delivery of nutrition information. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2020, 17, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Weiss, B.D.; Mays, M.Z.; Martz, W.; Castro, K.M.; DeWalt, D.A.; Pignone, M.P.; Mockbee, J.; Hale, F.A. Quick assessment of literacy in primary care: The newest vital sign. Ann. Fam. Med. 2005, 3, 514–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Mansfield, E.D.; Wahba, R.; Gillis, D.E.; Weiss, B.D.; L’Abbé, M. Canadian adaptation of the Newest Vital Sign©, a health literacy assessment tool. Public Health Nutr. 2018, 21, 2038–2045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Bernstein, J.T.; Schermel, A.; Mills, C.M.; L’Abbe, M.R. Total and Free Sugar Content of Canadian Prepackaged Foods and Beverages. Nutrients 2016, 8, 582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Implementation Subcommittee for Food Regulation. Getting Your Claims Right-A Guide to Complying with the Nutrition, Health and Related Claims Standard of the Australia New Zealand Food Standards Code. Available online: http://www.foodstandards.gov.au/publications/Documents/FINAL%20-%20ISFR%20Health%20Claims.pdf (accessed on 10 May 2018).
  68. Wong, C.L.; Arcand, J.; Mendoza, J.; Henson, S.J.; Qi, Y.; Lou, W.; L’Abbé, M.R. Consumer attitudes and understanding of low-sodium claims on food: An analysis of healthy and hypertensive individuals. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2013, 97, 1288–1298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Benson, T.; Lavelle, F.; Bucher, T.; McCloat, A.; Mooney, E.; Egan, B.; Collins, C.E.; Dean, M. The Impact of Nutrition and Health Claims on Consumer Perceptions and Portion Size Selection: Results from a Nationally Representative Survey. Nutrients 2018, 10, 656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Arrúa, A.; Curutchet, M.R.; Rey, N.; Barreto, P.; Golovchenko, N.; Sellanes, A.; Velazco, G.; Winokur, M.; Giménez, A.; Ares, G. Impact of front-of-pack nutrition information and label design on children’s choice of two snack foods: Comparison of warnings and the traffic-light system. Appetite 2017, 116, 139–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Maubach, N.; Hoek, J.; Mather, D. Interpretive front-of-pack nutrition labels. Comparing competing recommendations. Appetite 2014, 82, 67–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Menozzi, D.; Nguyen, T.T.; Sogari, G.; Taskov, D.; Lucas, S.; Castro-Rial, J.L.S.; Mora, C. Consumers’ Preferences and Willingness to Pay for Fish Products with Health and Environmental Labels: Evidence from Five European Countries. Nutrients 2020, 12, 2650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Bialkova, S.; Sasse, L.; Fenko, A. The role of nutrition labels and advertising claims in altering consumers’ evaluation and choice. Appetite 2016, 96, 38–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Moubarac, J.C.; Martins, A.P.; Claro, R.M.; Levy, R.B.; Cannon, G.; Monteiro, C.A. Consumption of ultra-processed foods and likely impact on human health. Evidence from Canada. Public Health Nutr. 2013, 16, 2240–2248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Nardocci, M.; Leclerc, B.S.; Louzada, M.L.; Monteiro, C.A.; Batal, M.; Moubarac, J.C. Consumption of ultra-processed foods and obesity in Canada. Can. J. Public Health 2018, 110, 4–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Christoph, M.J.; Larson, N.; Laska, M.N.; Neumark-Sztainer, D. Nutrition Facts Panels: Who Uses Them, What Do They Use, and How Does Use Relate to Dietary Intake? J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2018, 118, 217–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Egnell, M.; Talati, Z.; Hercberg, S.; Pettigrew, S.; Julia, C. Objective Understanding of Front-of-Package Nutrition Labels: An International Comparative Experimental Study across 12 Countries. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Graham, D.J.; Jeffery, R.W. Location, location, location: Eye-tracking evidence that consumers preferentially view prominently positioned nutrition information. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2011, 111, 1704–1711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Tórtora, G.; Machín, L.; Ares, G. Influence of nutritional warnings and other label features on consumers’ choice: Results from an eye-tracking study. Food Res. Int. 2019, 119, 605–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. The Canadian Radio-television and Telecommunications Commission (CRTC). Communications Monitoring Report 2018. Available online: https://crtc.gc.ca/pubs/cmr2018-en.pdf (accessed on 4 November 2020).
Figure 1. Images of mock labels used in the survey. (a) Summary of the eight conditions (2 × 2 × 2 factorial design) used in the survey and the four different labels created. (b) Nutrition Facts tables (NFts) for the healthier and less healthy soups. The nutritional quality was determined using the Food Standards Australia New Zealand Nutrient Profiling Scoring Criterion [67] and based on similar products found in the Canadian food supply.
Figure 1. Images of mock labels used in the survey. (a) Summary of the eight conditions (2 × 2 × 2 factorial design) used in the survey and the four different labels created. (b) Nutrition Facts tables (NFts) for the healthier and less healthy soups. The nutritional quality was determined using the Food Standards Australia New Zealand Nutrient Profiling Scoring Criterion [67] and based on similar products found in the Canadian food supply.
Ijerph 17 08213 g001
Figure 2. Study design and questions used in the survey. (a) Encoding phase: Participants were shown a label for 10 s. (b) Recall and understanding: Once the label was removed from the screen, participants were asked, ‘To the best of your recollection, was there a claim regarding the nutritional quality of the product that you were shown?’ If yes (open-ended questions): ‘What did the claim describe?’; ‘What do you think this claim means?’ (c) Perceptions: Participants were shown the same label again, which was left on the screen, and asked to rate product’s nutritional quality (‘How healthy do you think this soup is?’) and purchase intentions (‘How likely would you be to buy this product?’) using a seven-point Likert scale, where 1 was lowest nutritional quality/purchase intention and 7 was the highest nutritional quality/purchase intention. Participants had the option to click on the Nutrition Facts table.
Figure 2. Study design and questions used in the survey. (a) Encoding phase: Participants were shown a label for 10 s. (b) Recall and understanding: Once the label was removed from the screen, participants were asked, ‘To the best of your recollection, was there a claim regarding the nutritional quality of the product that you were shown?’ If yes (open-ended questions): ‘What did the claim describe?’; ‘What do you think this claim means?’ (c) Perceptions: Participants were shown the same label again, which was left on the screen, and asked to rate product’s nutritional quality (‘How healthy do you think this soup is?’) and purchase intentions (‘How likely would you be to buy this product?’) using a seven-point Likert scale, where 1 was lowest nutritional quality/purchase intention and 7 was the highest nutritional quality/purchase intention. Participants had the option to click on the Nutrition Facts table.
Ijerph 17 08213 g002
Figure 3. Means ratings of perceived nutritional quality (A) and purchase intentions (B) by condition overall and by Nutrition Facts table use (n = 1997)1,2,3. 1 Adjusted for gender, education, income, ethnicity, and health literacy. 2 Error bars show the standard error of the mean (SEM). 3 Because the nutritional quality of soups had no effect, participants were combined into four groups based on the label they were shown: (1) No Claim No Symbol, (2) Nutrient Content Claim, (3) Symbol depicting ‘health,’ and (4) Nutrient Content Claim + Symbol depicting ‘health.’ a,b Dark gray bars (All Participants), (means ± SEM ratings by all participants) with different superscript letters indicated statistically significant differences at p < 0.001 by the generalized linear model with Bonferroni correction. j,k Light grey bars (Non-NFt users), (means ± SEM ratings by Non-NFt users) with different superscript letters indicated statistically significant differences at p < 0.001 by the generalized linear model with Bonferroni correction. z Medium grey bars (NFt users), (means ± SEM ratings by NFt users) with different superscript letters indicated statistically significant differences at p < 0.001 by the generalized linear model with Bonferroni correction. Separate generalized linear models were conducted for each of the three.
Figure 3. Means ratings of perceived nutritional quality (A) and purchase intentions (B) by condition overall and by Nutrition Facts table use (n = 1997)1,2,3. 1 Adjusted for gender, education, income, ethnicity, and health literacy. 2 Error bars show the standard error of the mean (SEM). 3 Because the nutritional quality of soups had no effect, participants were combined into four groups based on the label they were shown: (1) No Claim No Symbol, (2) Nutrient Content Claim, (3) Symbol depicting ‘health,’ and (4) Nutrient Content Claim + Symbol depicting ‘health.’ a,b Dark gray bars (All Participants), (means ± SEM ratings by all participants) with different superscript letters indicated statistically significant differences at p < 0.001 by the generalized linear model with Bonferroni correction. j,k Light grey bars (Non-NFt users), (means ± SEM ratings by Non-NFt users) with different superscript letters indicated statistically significant differences at p < 0.001 by the generalized linear model with Bonferroni correction. z Medium grey bars (NFt users), (means ± SEM ratings by NFt users) with different superscript letters indicated statistically significant differences at p < 0.001 by the generalized linear model with Bonferroni correction. Separate generalized linear models were conducted for each of the three.
Ijerph 17 08213 g003
Table 1. Participants’ characteristics (n = 1997).
Table 1. Participants’ characteristics (n = 1997).
Demographics Clicked at the NFt when Assessing Nutritional QualityClicked at the NFt when Assessing Purchase Intentions
AllNon-NFt Users NFt UsersNon-NFt Users NFt Users
(n = 1997)(n = 1554)(n = 443)(n = 1860)(n = 137)
Age (years)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
18-25256 (12.8)225 (14.5)43 (9.7)242 (13.0)14 (10.2)
26-35652 (32.6)528 (34.0)112 (25.3)621 (33.4)31 (22.6)
36-45493 (24.7)384 (24.7)109 (24.6)459 (24.7)34 (24.8)
46-55359 (18.0)251 (16.2)108 (24.4)328 (17.6)31 (22.6)
56-65176 (8.8)121 (7.8)55 (12.4)157 (8.4)19 (13.9)
66+61 (3.1)45 (2.9)16 (3.6)53 (2.8)8 (5.8)
Refused0 (0.0)0 (0.0)0 (0.0)0 (0.0)0 (0.0)
Gendern (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
Male957 (47.9)764 (49.2)193 (43.6)892 (48.0)65 (47.4)
Female1037 (51.9)789 (50.8)248 (56.0)965 (51.9)72 (52.6)
Another3 (0.2)1 (0.1)2 (0.5)3 (0.2)0 (0.0)
Educationn (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
Did not graduate high school36 (1.8)27 (1.7)9 (2.0)32 (1.7)4 (2.9)
High school certificate or equivalent324 (16.2)255 (16.4)69 (15.6)305 (16.4)19 (13.9)
Trades certificate or diploma99 (4.9)73 (4.7)26 (5.9)91 (4.9)8 (5.8)
Community college, technical college, or CEGEP511 (25.6)382 (24.6)129 (29.1)475 (25.5)36 (26.3)
University (undergraduate degree)762 (38.2)617 (39.7)145 (32.7)715 (38.4)47 (34.3)
Post-graduate degree (Masters, PhD)259 (13)194 (12.5)65 (14.7)236 (12.7)23 (16.8)
Not stated6 (0.3)6 (0.4)0 (0.0)6 (0.3)0 (0.0)
Ethnicityn (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
White1375 (68.9)1038 (66.8)337 (76.1)1264 (68.0)111 (81.0)
Nonwhite589 (29.5)487 (31.3)102 (23.0)566 (30.4)23 (16.8)
Not stated33 (1.7)29 (1.9)4 (0.9)30 (1.6)3 (2.2)
Household incomen (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
$25,000 or less169 (8.5)134 (8.6)35 (7.9)164 (8.8)5 (3.6)
$25,000–$49,999373 (18.7)290 (18.7)83 (18.7)356 (19.1)17 (12.4)
$50,000–$74,999409 (20.5)322 (20.7)87 (19.6)384 (20.6)25 (18.2)
$75,000–$99,999338 (16.9)269 (17.3)69 (15.6)304 (16.3)34 (24.8)
$100,000–$124,999274 (13.7)218 (14.0)56 (12.6)253 (13.6)21 (15.3)
$125,000 or more288 (14.4)213 (13.7)75 (16.9)265 (14.2)23 (16.8)
Not stated146 (7.3)108 (6.9)38 (8.6)134 (7.2)12 (8.8)
Language primarily spoken at homen (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
English1830 (91.6)1427 (91.8)403 (91.0)1701 (91.5)129 (94.2)
French44 (2.2)29 (1.9)15 (3.4)41 (2.2)3 (2.2)
Other118 (5.9)93 (6.0)25 (5.6)113 (6.1)5 (3.6)
Not stated5 (0.3)5 (0.3)0 (0.0)5 (0.3)0 (0.0)
Dependent children (<18 years)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
Yes758 (38)591 (38.0)167 (37.7)710 (38.2)48 (35.0)
No1229 (61.5)954 (61.4)275 (62.1)1140 (61.3)89 (65.0)
Not stated10 (0.5)9 (0.6)1 (0.2)10 (0.5)0 (0.0)
Health literacy *n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
Likely low health literacy202 (10.1)197 (12.7)5 (1.1)200 (10.8)2 (1.5)
Possible low health literacy263 (13.2)226 (14.5)37 (8.4)251 (13.5)12 (8.8)
Adequate health literacy1528 (76.5)1127 (72.5)401 (90.5)1405 (75.5)123 (98.8)
Missing4 (0.2)4 (0.3)0 (0.0)4 (0.2)0 (0.0)
* Assessed with the Newest Vital Sign questionnaire [64,65].
Table 2. Responses to open-ended questions assessing consumers’ recall and understanding of the claim 1,2.
Table 2. Responses to open-ended questions assessing consumers’ recall and understanding of the claim 1,2.
All (1) No Claim No Symbol 2(2) Claim 2(3) Symbol 2(4) Claim + Symbol 2
n = 1997 n = 496 n = 500n = 503 n = 498
n%responsesn%responsesn%responsesn%responsesn% responses
In response to “To the best of your recollection, was there a claim regarding the nutritional quality of the product that you were shown?”
Yes79743.4%5512.0%34073.4%5011.0%35276.0%
No104156.6%40288.0%12326.6%40589.0%11124.0%
Total responses1838100%457100%463100%455100%455100%
In response to “What did the claim describe?”
Low sat fat and trans fat, low fat44968.2%14.5%21071.7%00.0%23873.2%
Low in sodium, fat, sat fat, trans fat10415.8%14.5%5318.1%211.1%4814.8%
Healthy, hearty, OK, good for you335.0%313.6%134.4%633.3%113.4%
Number of calories, Net Weight, servings243.6%1045.5%20.7%316.7%92.8%
Chicken noodle soup, great soup, convenience, reliability, taste, quality182.7%627.3%41.4%422.2%41.2%
Low calories, cholesterol, sugars, any fat172.6%00.0%93.1%00.0%82.5%
Can’t remember, not sure101.5%00.0%10.3%211.1%72.2%
Not healthy, high in sodium30.5%14.5%10.3%15.6%00.0%
Total responses658100.0%22100.0%293100.0%18100.0%325100.0%
In response to “What do you think this claim means?”
Low fat (sat, trans, total)29649.7%00.0%14453.9%00.0%15252.8%
Healthy, healthier choice, better choice, good for you, nutrition value, OK15926.7%1354.2%6424.0%956.3%7325.3%
Low in salt/sodium, low in fats (sat, trans, total)6611.1%00.0%3613.5%318.8%279.4%
Nothing, not much, not sure, neutral, marketing366.1%416.7%114.1%212.5%196.6%
Less calories, sugars, cholesterol, low in any fat162.7%00.0%41.5%00.0%124.2%
Homestyle, homemade, minimal processed, tasty, great soup, noodle soup91.5%312.5%31.1%16.3%20.7%
Number of calories, Nt Wt, servings, portions71.2%416.7%10.4%00.0%20.7%
Contains ± chemicals, flavors, fiber61.0%00.0%41.5%16.3%10.3%
Total responses595100.0%24100.0%267100.0%16100.0%288100.0%
1 The open-ended responses from participants contained multiple but similar key themes. 2 Because the nutritional quality did not have significant effects in either consumers’ recall or consumers’ perceptions, therefore participants were combined into four groups based on the label they were shown: (1) No Claim No Symbol, (2) Nutrient Content Claim, (3) Symbol depicting ‘health,’ and (4) Nutrient Content Claim + Symbol depicting ‘health’.
Table 3. Means ratings of perceived nutritional quality and purchase intentions by (a) gender and (b) health literacy (n = 1997) 1.
Table 3. Means ratings of perceived nutritional quality and purchase intentions by (a) gender and (b) health literacy (n = 1997) 1.
Perceived Nutritional Quality
(a) Gender 2Males (n = 843)Females (n = 909)
Mean95% CIp-value 4Mean95% CIp-value 4
Group 1No Claim, No Symbol3.23.0–3.5p < 0.0012.92.7–3.2p < 0.001
Group 2Claim3.93.6–4.33.63.3–4.0
Group 3Symbol3.12.9–3.43.02.7–3.3
Group 4Claim + Symbol3.93.6–4.33.63.3–3.9
(b) Health literacy 3Likely/possible low health literacy (n = 416)Adequate health literacy (n = 1339)
Mean95% CIp-value 4Mean95% CIp-value 4
Group 1No Claim, No Symbol4.13.6–4.7p = 0.0012.82.6–3.0p < 0.001
Group 2Claim4.54.0–5.03.63.3–3.8
Group 3Symbol3.53.1–4.02.92.7–3.1
Group 4Claim + Symbol4.53.9–5.03.63.3–3.8
Purchase Intentions
(a) Gender 2Males (n = 890) Females (n = 930)
Mean95% CIp-value 4Mean95% CIp-value 4
Group 1No Claim, No Symbol3.02.7–3.3p = 0.0012.82.5–3.1p = 0.076
Group 2Claim3.43.1–3.83.12.8–3.4
Group 3Symbol2.92.6–3.22.72.5–3.0
Group 4Claim + Symbol3.43.1–3.83.12.8–3.4
(b) Health literacy 3Likely/possible low health literacy (n = 424)Adequate health literacy (n = 1399)
Mean95% CIp-value 4Mean95% CIp-value 4
Group 1No Claim, No Symbol3.93.4–4.5p = 0.0122.62.4–2.9p = 0.001
Group 2Claim4.43.8–5.02.92.7–3.1
Group 3Symbol3.43.0–3.92.62.4–2.8
Group 4Claim + Symbol4.13.5–4.73.02.8–3.3
1 Because the nutritional quality of soups had no effect, participants were combined into four groups based on the label they were shown: (1) No Claim No Symbol, (2) Nutrient Content Claim, (3) Symbol depicting ‘health,’ and (4) Nutrient Content Claim + Symbol depicting ‘health.’2 Adjusted for education, income, ethnicity, and health literacy. 3 Adjusted for education, income, ethnicity, and gender. 4 As determined by generalized linear models. CI—Confidence Intervals.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Franco-Arellano, B.; Vanderlee, L.; Ahmed, M.; Oh, A.; L’Abbé, M.R. Consumers’ Implicit and Explicit Recall, Understanding and Perceptions of Products with Nutrition-Related Messages: An Online Survey. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 8213. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17218213

AMA Style

Franco-Arellano B, Vanderlee L, Ahmed M, Oh A, L’Abbé MR. Consumers’ Implicit and Explicit Recall, Understanding and Perceptions of Products with Nutrition-Related Messages: An Online Survey. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(21):8213. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17218213

Chicago/Turabian Style

Franco-Arellano, Beatriz, Lana Vanderlee, Mavra Ahmed, Angela Oh, and Mary R. L’Abbé. 2020. "Consumers’ Implicit and Explicit Recall, Understanding and Perceptions of Products with Nutrition-Related Messages: An Online Survey" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 21: 8213. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17218213

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop