Next Article in Journal
Implementation Models of Compassionate Communities and Compassionate Cities at the End of Life: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Journal
Changes in Markers of Oxidative Stress and α-Amylase in Saliva of Children Associated with a Tennis Competition
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Association between Longest-Held Lifetime Occupation and Late-Life Cognitive Impairment: Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (2006–2016)

1
Department of Preventive Medicine, College of Medicine, Seoul National University, 103 Daehak-ro, Jongno-gu, Seoul 08826, Korea
2
Institute of Health and Environment, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(17), 6270; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17176270
Submission received: 10 July 2020 / Revised: 25 August 2020 / Accepted: 25 August 2020 / Published: 28 August 2020
(This article belongs to the Section Health Care Sciences & Services)

Abstract

:
The association between longest-held lifetime occupation and late-life cognitive impairment: Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (2006–2016). Backgrounds: Our study hypothesized that occupation in adulthood may be one of the modifiable factors in cognitive performance. This follow-up study aimed to examine whether there was an association between the longest-held occupation in a lifetime and cognitive impairment. Methods: This study used data from the 2006, 2012, and 2016 waves of the Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging, and a total of 1733 subjects aged over 65 were included. Longest-held occupation in a lifetime was classified into blue-collar, pink-collar, and white-collar. Cognitive function was evaluated using the Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination. Results: In males, no significant associations were observed. In females, on the contrary, risk of cognitive impairment in the blue-collar occupation was consistently higher than in the white-collar occupation over the 10-year follow-up (2006, OR = 2.49, 95% CI 1.05–5.88; 2016, OR = 2.17, 95% CI 1.02−4.65). Conclusions: Lifetime occupation should be taken into consideration in the process of screening for cognitive decline in the elderly, especially females. This study needs to be interpreted cautiously in view of inherent data and methodological limitations.

1. Introduction

With the rapid population aging, cognitive health is of critical importance for maintaining independence and quality of life in old age [1]. Cognitive function tends to decline after peaking at approximately age 40 and leads to more significant declines after the age of 80 [2,3,4,5]. It is known that 30–40% of cases with mild cognitive impairment consequently progress to more severe conditions such as dementia [6]. As cognitive impairment and dementia undermine overall daily functioning and cause disability, in older adults, even delaying the onset of cognitive decline is vital [7]. There is a consensus that a substantial proportion of cases of cognitive impairment is preventable [8]. The early onset of cognitive deficits could be attributable to the following potentially modifiable factors: Low educational attainment, housing environment, smoking, dietary intake (e.g., Mediterranean diet, vegetable, and ω-3 fatty acid intake), cognitive inactivity, and clinical condition (e.g., diabetes, midlife hypertension and obesity, and depression) [9,10]. However, few studies demonstrated the effect of occupation on cognitive decline. Therefore, it is necessary to identify job-related modifiable risk factors for cognitive impairment.
Occupation is highly dependent on educational attainment in early life, determines income and economic resources, and affects healthy lifestyles [11,12,13]. Occupation is considered as an important predictor of cognitive decline in later life [12,14], because of the disparities in working environment and variations in cognitive demands across occupations [15]. Employees in manual (or blue-collar) occupations than non-manual occupations are more likely to experience a variety of deleterious work factors such as greater job-strain, lack of social support, occupational mobility, job insecurity, physical/chemical/ergonomic hazards at work, and low wage income, which may cause occupational health disparities [16,17,18]. Several studies have shown that manual workers are vulnerable to poorer physical (e.g., cardiovascular disease, musculoskeletal pain, sickness absence) and mental health condition (e.g., stress, depression, suicide ideation) than non-manual workers [19,20]. There is evidence indicating the role of occupation in subsequent cognitive deterioration. Individuals holding an inferior position in their job have higher cognitive impairment risks [21], and occupation-related cognitive requirement and occupational complexity are associated with late-life cognitive decline [22]. Prior research also demonstrated the relation between mental work demands and cognitive change over age [23]. As most people spend a substantial portion of their lives at work [24]. The workplace environment has a greater effect on workers’ cognitive health [25]. Manual workers are frequently exposed to industrial toxicants and neurotoxic materials in the work environment [8,26]. While those in white-collar occupations tend to constantly exert themselves intellectually [27,28]. Moreover, the longest-held lifetime occupation is a significant discriminator of socioeconomic differentials and reflects the individual’s accumulated lifetime employment, working conditions, and other factors related to occupation [21]. Lifelong working in their principal job, individuals would be consistently exposed to intellectually stimulating activities, psychosocial workplace conditions, income-related healthy lifestyles, and environmental neurotoxicants in their workplace, which may trigger cognitive decline in later life [26]. For older people, health status is not the result of events occurring over a short period but the accumulation of health behaviors throughout the life course. Thus, the cumulative effect of job experience would produce health disparities in old age [29,30].
From employment to retirement, males and females experience various social positions and social roles throughout their lives [31]. There is a gender differential in labor compensation, occupied industrial sectors, wages, and household roles [32]. Females have concentrated in female-dominated occupations, and these jobs pay less than male-dominated occupations [33]. Moreover, female workers are highly employed in jobs requiring nurturant social skills such as nursing, teaching, child-care work, and counseling. They have limited internal mobility in their job and low capital intensity compared to male workers [34]. Thus, there may be a different association between lifetime occupation on later health status between genders.
Based on this background, this study aimed to investigate whether there is an association between the longest-held lifetime occupation and cognitive impairment in the Korean elderly. Specifically, considering gender differences in the occupational experience, we estimated the risk of cognitive impairment across the type of longest-held lifetime occupation for both genders, respectively.

2. Methods

2.1. Data Source and Study Population

This study used the secondary data source of the 1st, 4th, and 6th waves of the Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (KLoSA; 2006, 2012, and 2016). We included information regarding demographic factors, socioeconomic status, health behaviors, occupation, cognitive function, and the job history dataset. The KLoSA, a nationally representative longitudinal study, is a biennial undertaking of the Korean Labor Institute of the Ministry of Labor. It uses multistage, stratified cluster sampling to obtain representative estimates for non-institutionalized residents in Korea [35]. The initial survey in 2006 covered 10,245 adults aged 45 years and older. In the following ten years (2006–2016), the follow-up rate was 78% (N = 7015). Only individuals who were enrolled in all the follow-up cohorts were included in the current study (N = 6453; male 2718, female 3735), and we excluded the participants with no response in the job history survey (N = 494; male 87, female 407). Since our research focused on the elderly, 3986 subjects aged 64 or below were excluded (N = 3986; male 1795, female 2191). In addition, 240 subjects with missing values in MMSE scores, income, education, and smoking status were excluded (N = 240; male 100, female 140). Therefore, the final sample comprised 1733 subjects: 736 males and 997 females (Figure 1).

2.2. Longest-Held Job in a Lifetime

The occupational history dataset included profession-related details over the lifetime, right from the participants’ first job in life: Calendar period at hire, calendar period at the exit for each job, and calendar time for unemployment. Occupation generally presents workplace exposure [4,36,37,38] and the longest-held occupation reflects the worker’s accumulated lifetime working conditions and work-related risk exposures [21]. Longest-held occupation in a lifetime was determined by the occupation in which an individual was employed for the longest duration in adulthood. Individuals who had never been employed were excluded. Longest-held occupation in a lifetime was categorized into three types: White-collar, blue-collar, and pink-collar. White-collar refers to clerical and kindred work and included assistant to executives, officials, business, and professional workers [39]. Pink-collar was generally defined as retail and service industry work [38], and blue-collar refers to the industry with manual labor [40]. We also categorized managers, professionals, and clerks as white-collar workers; service and sales workers as pink-collar workers; and skilled agricultural, forestry and fishery workers, craft and trades workers, equipment and machine operation and assembly workers, elementary workers, and armed forces as blue-collar workers [41].

2.3. Cognitive Impairment

Cognitive function was determined by the Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (K-MMSE). The K-MMSE consists of 19 items, including orientation to time and space (five items), memory registration and recall (two items), concentration and calculation (five items), language (six items), and visuospatial construction (one item). The scores range from 0 to 30, with lower scores indicating lower cognitive function. Individuals with less than 24 points for K-MMSE scores were classified into cognitive impairment, and equal or more than 24 points were classified into a high cognitive function [20,42]. The cognitive impairment (MMSE) was assessed approximately 4.7 years after the retirement in male (blue-collar, 3.9 years; pink-collar, 3.5 years; and white-collar, 7.3 years), and approximately 12.9 years in female (blue-collar, 12.5 years; pink-collar, 12.5 years; and white-collar 20.9 years).

2.4. Other Variables of Interests

Covariates included sociodemographic occupational factors, and health-related factors, which can potentially confound the relationship between longest-held occupation and cognitive impairment in later life. Sociodemographic factors included age (65–74 years or 75 years and older), marital status (married, divorced/widowed, or never married), living alone (yes or no), educational level (elementary or lower, middle/high school, or college or higher), yearly income ((10,000,000 KRW (approximately <9000 USD), 10,000,000−25,000,000 KRW (approximately 9000–22,500 USD), or >25,000,000 KRW (approximately 22,500 USD)). Occupational factors included work duration (mean = 31.65, SD = 21.60 in female; mean = 40.30, SD = 15.35 in female) (gender—specific median values; high (male > 39 years and female > 29 years) or low (male ≤ 39 years and female ≤29 years)), and time spent from retirement to examination of cognitive function (gender-specific median values; high (male >1 year and female >8 years) or low (male ≤1 year and female ≤8 years)). Health-related factors included smoking status (never, former, or current), alcohol consumption (never, former, or current), muscle strength (gender-specific median values; high (male > 28.75 kg and female > 17.0 kg) or low (male ≤ 28.75 kg and female ≤17.0 kg)), physical activity (yes or no), well-being (yes (general life satisfaction score ≥50) or no (general life satisfaction score <50)), and chronic diseases (yes or no) such as history of heart disease, diabetes, psychiatric diseases, and stroke.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

All the analyses were gender stratified because of the potential gender disparities in the association between occupation and cognitive deficits. Differences in subjects’ general characteristics by the longest-held lifetime occupation were tested using ANOVA and chi-square tests. Moreover, if there are more than 20% of the expected cell frequencies <5, we conducted Fisher’s exact test (differences in marital status by longest-held occupation in male; differences in marital status, education, smoking status, and alcohol consumption). Trajectories of K-MMSE scores from 2006 to 2016 by the longest-held lifetime occupation were presented and the Cochran-Armitage trend test was used to determine whether the K-MMSE scores demonstrated any wave-related trends. Furthermore, differences in the proportions of subjects with cognitive impairment across the longest-held lifetime occupation in each wave were examined using chi-square tests. Unadjusted and adjusted logistic regression models were used to examine the association between the longest-held lifetime occupation and cognitive impairment in all three waves (2006, 2012, and 2016). The regression models were adjusted for age, marital status, education, income, cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking, physical activity, muscle strength, the presence of chronic diseases, work duration, time spent after retirement, well-being, and living alone. All analyses were conducted using the SAS 9.4 software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA), and p ≤ 0.05 was considered to indicate statistical significance.

2.6. Ethical Approval

The study protocol of this cohort study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Seoul National University/Seoul National University Hospital (IRB No. E-1905-076-1033).

3. Results

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the study population according to the longest-held lifetime occupation by gender. In males, a higher proportion of blue and pink-collar workers than white-collar workers were divorced/widowed, had a low educational level, were current smokers, and did not engage in daily physical activity. Blue and pink-collar workers were more likely to have low muscle strength, to live no well-being life, and to spend a longer time after retirement. In females, a higher proportion of blue and pink-collar workers than white-collar workers were older and current drinkers, blue and pink-collar workers were more likely to have a low educational level, no physical activity, and low muscle strength and to spend a longer time after retirement. For both males and females blue-collar workers were more likely to work longer than white and pink-collar workers.
Figure 2 depicts the gender-specific proportion of high cognitive function (K-MMSE ≥ 24) and mean K-MMSE scores according to the occupational types over ten years. In males, the prevalence of high cognitive function in all occupations was 77.04% in 2006, decreasing over the following ten years. The percentage of high cognitive function was the lowest in blue-collar workers (55.6–70.4%). Moreover, K-MMSE scores decreased significantly over the years in all occupational types (from 25.48 to 23.68), (p < 0.0001). The gap in K-MMSE scores between blue-collar and white-collar workers increased over 10 years (from 1.62 to 2.2). Similarly, in females, the proportion of subjects with high cognitive function decreased over the years (from 43.63% to 35.41%). The lowest percentage of high cognitive function was observed in blue-collar workers (31.49–38.09%). Further, K-MMSE scores significantly decreased over the years in blue (from 21.1 to 19.47) (p < 0.0001) and pink-collar workers (from 23.15 to 21.22) (p = 0.0019). The gap in K-MMSE scores between blue and white-collar workers increased slightly over the years (from 5.03 to 6.05).
Table 2 shows the prevalence of cognitive impairment and unadjusted and adjusted odds ratios (ORs) of cognitive impairment by the longest-held lifetime occupation in males over ten years (2006, 2012, and 2016). In 2006, the proportion of subjects with cognitive impairment was highest in blue-collar workers (29.60%) and lowest in pink-collar workers; this proportion increased over the years.
For females in Table 3, the proportion of subjects with cognitive impairment was the highest in blue-collar workers and the lowest in white-collar workers in 2006. The prevalence of cognitive impairment increased gradually over the years for each occupation type. In the full adjusted model, the association between the longest-held occupation and cognitive impairment was significant in 2006 (OR = 2.49, 95% CI 1.05–5.88) and maintained after 10 years (2016) (OR = 2.17, 95% CI 1.02–4.65).

4. Discussion

Our study demonstrated the 10-year association between the longest-held lifetime occupation on cognitive impairment in elderly Koreans. In males, no significant risks of cognitive impairment were observed in all three waves. In females, there was a 2.49-fold increased risk of cognitive impairment in blue-collar workers compared to white-collar workers. This risk was maintained in the following wave, at 2.17-fold higher in 2016. As per these results, in individuals aged 65 and older, working in blue-collar during lifetime was associated with decreased cognitive function, and the longitudinal effect was significant only in females.
To our knowledge, few studies have conducted follow-ups to determine whether the longest-held lifetime occupation consistently affects cognitive functioning in later life. However, several scholars have attempted to clarify the relationship between longest-held occupation in a lifetime and cognitive decline. Although it is difficult to summarize previous results because of classification-related variations, they commonly concluded that, in comparison with former white-collar workers, blue-collar workers are vulnerable to cognitive declines in old age [12,21,43]. For instance, Chung-Yi Li et al. (2002), Cat Tuong Nguyen et al. (2008), and Jean-Francois Dartigues et al. (1992) reported that manual workers (e.g., plant and machine operators, assemblers, and elementary workers), workers in the agriculture, forestry, and fishery sectors, and housewives had an elevated risk of cognitive impairment as compared to non-manual workers (e.g., business executives, managers or government administrators, and workers employed in intellectual occupations) [12,21,43]. Additional studies have demonstrated that the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease, the most severe expressions of cognitive impairment, were greater among manual workers involved in goods production, farmers, and domestic service employees than among professionals and managers [14,26].
Our results support and expand these prior findings. Several hypotheses would be useful in understanding the association between the longest-held lifetime occupation and risk of cognitive impairment. First, occupations that involve frequent and intense intellectual challenges and cognitive requirements produce greater intellectual stimulation in the workplace [24,28]. Further, these occupational activities can increase neural growth and synaptic density and compensate for cognitive decline. [44] Thus, blue-collar workers, whose jobs place lower intellectual demands on them, are at a disadvantage with regard to cognitive function as compared to white-collar workers. Second, blue-collar work is related to low income, which is associated with poor housing conditions, nutrition, and social environment, potentially giving rise to cognitive impairment [4,36,45]. Third, differences in health-risk behaviors between occupational categories may partly explain the cognitive deficits observed. Individuals who belong to a manual occupational background are more likely to engage in unhealthy lifestyle practices such as smoking, heavy drinking, and physical inactivity [26]. They also have fewer opportunities to engage in psychosocial interaction and receive medical care [46]. In addition to this, blue-collar workers could be exposed to neurotoxic materials at workplace including pesticides, heavy metals, organic solvents, and defoliants [8,37]. Individuals exposed to multi-toxicants are at a higher risk of developing memory impairment and cognitive aging than unexposed workers [8].
Our findings indicate that there is a gender-related gap in the degree of association between the longest-held lifetime occupation and cognitive impairment. No significant differences in the risk of cognitive impairment across the occupation were found in men during the 10-year follow-up. This result might be influenced by the fact that men have shorter life expectancy than women [47]. Accordingly, elderly men with poor cognitive function die early and were more likely to dropout. Better and healthier male elderly have high probability of being study participants. Further studies are required to clarify the association between occupation and cognitive impairment.
On the other hand, female blue-collar workers were at an increased risk of cognitive impairment, and this was sustained even in the ten years of follow-up. In the age group studied, females were disadvantaged in both educational and occupational attainment compared with their male counterparts [48]. Women were given a very low status, and the majority of the oldest women were exposed to a simple environment [49]. Taken together, female blue-collar workers may have been more likely to be exposed to disadvantaged work conditions than male blue-collar workers, and employment in manual work may have played a significant role in the consistent declines in their cognitive abilities [47,49].
This study is focused on cognitive impairment; the cut-point MMSE score of 24 was used. MMSE is the most frequently administered brief cognitive screening method used for identifying dementia [50]. This test is internationally recognized and used not only in research, but also in clinical practice [50]. For persons with MMSE score of 24 or lower, the GPs, with the help of patient’s family members, collected data about the medical history and family history of dementia [51]. However, the use of MMSE as a sole screening modality has been criticized [50]; the score is less sensitive to detect mild impairments (ceiling effect), especially in the highly educated individuals [50]. Furthermore, in those with little formal education and those with severe language problems, the test is insensitive to detect dementia (floor effect) [50]. Other screening tests, such as “Montreal Cognitive Assessment”, “Cambridge Cognitive Examination”, and “Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-cognitive part” are commonly used in research, clinical, and community setting [52,53,54]. However, there is no agreement about which test should be used as the best approach for early diagnosis of dementia. To estimate strengths and weaknesses, all the above mentioned methods should be comprehensively and systematically assessed in diverse populations and settings [55].
Although previous studies have demonstrated the association between occupation and development of cognitive function impairment, gender-specific analysis has not been conducted despite established gender-based differences in occupational status. Additionally, many previous studies have assessed cognitive function only once [12,21,43]; however, how long the occupational effect lasts remains unclear. The novelty of this study is that blue-collar women were identified to be vulnerable to cognitive impairment in later life, and in these women cognitive impairment persisted for 10 years. Based on our study results, we can suggest several conceptual and practical implications of the analysis. First, our findings indicate the importance of adopting a life-course perspective in designing interventions for preventing cognitive impairment [56]. Accordingly, interventions aiming at mid-life periods might be beneficial especially in the occupational field. Second, although occupational status might not be easily modified, policies reducing exposure to poor residential and work environments (e.g., improve regulations for chemical exposure), building proper social and social cohesion (e.g., investment in community infrastructure), and promoting positive health behaviors (e.g., antismoking programs) may enhance cognitive health of blue-collar workers [57]. Additionally, educational support by the government for improving cognitive health in later life could reduce cognitive disparities [58]. Moreover, if blue-collar workers continue with their education and lifelong learning program elsewhere outside the workplace, it may be expected to promote intellectual stimulation [58].
However, results need cautious interpretation in view of inherent data and methodological limitations and it should be considered. First, they may raise a misclassification bias because the information on the occupation could not fully obtain specific workplace exposure. However, as there are typical workplace exposures for each job type (e.g., non-intellectual demand, low income, and neurotoxic materials at work in blue-collar [4,36,37]; bullying from aggressive customers and emotional fatigue, and mental illness in pink-collar [38]), this study defined the occupational type as approximate workplace exposure. Further studies need to demonstrate the specific occupational exposures that affect cognitive deficits in later life. Second, selection bias may occur when study populations are not representative of target populations. Consequently, the measure of risks would not accurately represent the target population [59]. In this study, selection bias could occur because demographic characteristics were different for the analyzed population and the dropouts. A large proportion of dropouts are composed of attrition during 1st, 4th, and 6th waves and participants without responses in the job history survey. Moreover, subjects with missing values in MMSE scores, income, education, and smoking status were excluded. Thus, the better and healthier elderly have a high probability of study participants and, the prevalence and ORs may be lower than true prevalence and risks in elderly Koreans. In our study, the estimated prevalence of cognitive impairment may be lower than true prevalence, especially in elderly men. The data might be biased because men with an overall better cognitive health condition were most likely included. However, there were no significant differences of follow-ups across the three occupational types for both male (p = 0.475) and female (p = 0.432) (e.g., the proportion of the dropout across occupation was evenly distributed). Thus, the errors caused by selection bias may be reduced. Third, this study followed the ORs for cognitive impairment for 10 years rather than performing analysis using the comprehensive longitudinal model because there was no information about the exact time-point of cognitive impairment development. This study has a cross-sectional design which cannot be used to test causality. Further studies should clarify the longitudinal changes of cognitive function. Fourth, our study used simplified job classification which cannot reflect specific occupational aspects. Although detailed classifications (e.g., physical demand or complexity of job) have been used in this field, available job information cannot detect all sophisticated occupational characteristics. Fifth, to obtain data related to the longest-held lifetime occupation, participants in the KLoSA were asked to think back over their lives, potentially giving rise to recall bias. However, the method of data collection relied upon the filling calendar to help boost participants’ memory. Thus, the probability of misclassification bias associated with lifetime occupation was minimized. Sixth, the results of our study cannot be generalized to the entire Korean population aged over 65 because the survey excluded institutionalized elderly. In our data, 20% of female participants were housewives who had never been employed, and were accordingly excluded from our analysis. Therefore, further research on cognitive decline in the unemployed population is required. Seventh, the duration of the longest-held lifetime occupation in males was approximately 10 years longer than in females (40.05 years versus 31.78 years). Thus, attempts to compare the impact of the longest-held lifetime occupation on cognitive impairment between the genders should be undertaken with caution. Eighth, although the KLoSA surveys are biennial and data for all relevant years in the period from 2006 to 2016 were available, we only included data from 2006, 2012, and 2016. Had we used all the data from 2006 to 2016, more than half of the baseline sample would have been ruled out; including only those followed up in 2006, 2012, and 2016 led to the lowest possible attrition rate (51.68%). Ninth, there may be other unmeasured confounders, such as dietary intake, digestive health problems, and sleep quality and duration, which could not be adjusted for in our analysis. Further studies are needed to consider these confounding factors for clarifying the association in the analysis. Finally, although the K-MMSE is an easy measure of cognitive functioning and is widely used for screening early dementia, it is insensitive to detecting early dementia because it causes false negative rates and has negative predictive power [60,61]. Thus, further studies are needed using a more accurate tool for assessing cognitive function in the elderly.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the longest-held lifetime occupation was a significant discriminator with regard to cognitive impairment in later life. No significant association between cognitive impairment and the longest-held occupation in a lifetime was observed in males. In female older adults, however, the disparities in cognitive impairment remained significant even after 10 years. This study suggests that the longest-held lifetime occupation should be taken into consideration when screening cognitive decline in the elderly, especially females.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.-J.K., J.-Y.M., and K.-B.M.; methodology, J.-Y.M. and K.-B.M.; formal analysis, H.-J.K., J.-Y.M., and K.-B.M.; writing—original draft preparation, H.-J.K.; writing—review and editing, J.-Y.M. and K.-B.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Ministry of Education, Science and Technology (grant number, 2017R1E1A1A01078235).

Acknowledgments

This work was supported partly by a research grant from the Seoul National University Hospital.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. Data are available in a public, open access repository (https://survey.keis.or.kr/klosa/klosa01.jsp).

References

  1. Reas, E.T.; Laughlin, G.A.; Bergstrom, J.; Kritz-Silverstein, D.; Richard, E.L.; Barrett-Connor, E.; McEvoy, L.K. Lifetime physical activity and late-life cognitive function: The Rancho Bernardo study. Age Ageing 2019, 48, 241–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Caskie, G.I.; Schaie, K.W.; Willis, S.L. Individual differences in the rate of change in cognitive abilities during adulthood. In Proceedings of the Gerontological Society of America Conference, San Francisco, CA, USA, 19–23 November 1999. [Google Scholar]
  3. Singh-Manoux, A.; Kivimaki, M.; Glymour, M.M.; Elbaz, A.; Berr, C.; Ebmeier, K.P.; Ferrie, J.E.; Dugravot, A. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: Results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ 2012, 344, d7622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Zhang, M.; Gale, S.D.; Erickson, L.D.; Brown, B.L.; Woody, P.; Hedges, D.W. Cognitive function in older adults according to current socioeconomic status. Aging Neuropsychol. Cognit. 2015, 22, 534–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Yaffe, K.; Peltz, C.B.; Ewing, S.K.; McCulloch, C.E.; Cummings, S.R.; Cauley, J.A.; Hillier, T.A.; Ensrud, K.E. Long-term cognitive trajectories and mortality in older women. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biomed. Sci. Med Sci. 2016, 71, 1074–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Na, K.-S. Prediction of future cognitive impairment among the community elderly: A machine-learning based approach. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 3335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Baldivia, B.; Andrade, V.M.; Bueno, O.F.A. Contribution of education, occupation and cognitively stimulating activities to the formation of cognitive reserve. Dement. Neuropsychol. 2008, 2, 173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Killin, L.O.; Starr, J.M.; Shiue, I.J.; Russ, T.C. Environmental risk factors for dementia: A systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2016, 16, 175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Jang, A.R.; Yoon, J.Y. Factors affecting reversion from mild cognitive impairment to normal cognition in midlife to later life in Korea: A national population study. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 2019, 19, 1129–1135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Plassman, B.L.; Williams, J.W.; Burke, J.R.; Holsinger, T.; Benjamin, S. Systematic review: Factors associated with risk for and possible prevention of cognitive decline in later life. Annu. Intern. Med. 2010, 153, 182–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Corna, L.M. A life course perspective on socioeconomic inequalities in health: A critical review of conceptual frameworks. Adv. Life Course Res. 2013, 18, 150–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Nguyen, C.T.; Couture, M.-C.; Alvarado, B.E.; Zunzunegui, M.-V. Life course socioeconomic disadvantage and cognitive function among the elderly population of seven capitals in Latin America and the Caribbean. J. Aging Health 2008, 20, 347–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Lee, H.; Jung, E. Socioeconomic status and elderly health in life course perspective: Testing of multi-mediational effects of socioeconomic status in early adult and middle aged. Health Soc. Welf. Rev. 2016, 36, 53–84. [Google Scholar]
  14. Helmer, C.; Letenneur, L.; Rouch, I.; Richard-Harston, S.; Barberger-Gateau, P.; Fabrigoule, C.; Orgogozo, J.; Dartigues, J. Occupation during life and risk of dementia in French elderly community residents. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2001, 71, 303–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Lips-Wiersma, M.; Wright, S.; Dik, B. Meaningful work: Differences among blue-, pink-, and white-collar occupations. Career Dev. Int. 2016, 21, 534–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Melchior, M.; Krieger, N.; Kawachi, I.; Berkman, L.F.; Niedhammer, I.; Goldberg, M. Work factors and occupational class disparities in sickness absence: Findings from the GAZEL cohort study. Am. J. Public Health 2005, 95, 1206–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hemström, Ö. Health inequalities by wage income in Sweden: The role of work environment. Soc. Sci. Med. 2005, 61, 637–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Schrijvers, C.T.; van de Mheen, H.D.; Stronks, K.; Mackenbach, J.P. Socioeconomic inequalities in health in the working population: The contribution of working conditions. Int. J. Epidemiol. 1998, 27, 1011–1018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Ahn, J.; Kim, N.-S.; Lee, B.-K.; Park, J.; Kim, Y. Relationship of occupational category with risk of physical and mental health problems. Saf. Health Work 2019, 10, 504–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Min, J.Y.; Park, J.B.; Lee, K.J.; Min, K.B. The impact of occupational experience on cognitive and physical functional status among older adults in a representative sample of Korean subjects. Annu. Occup. Environ. Med. 2015, 27, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Li, C.-Y.; Wu, S.C.; Sung, F.-C. Lifetime principal occupation and risk of cognitive impairment among the elderly. Ind. Health 2002, 40, 7–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Finkel, D.; Andel, R.; Gatz, M.; Pedersen, N.L. The role of occupational complexity in trajectories of cognitive aging before and after retirement. Psychol. Aging 2009, 24, 563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Fisher, G.G.; Stachowski, A.; Infurna, F.J.; Faul, J.D.; Grosch, J.; Tetrick, L.E. Mental work demands, retirement, and longitudinal trajectories of cognitive functioning. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2014, 19, 231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Andel, R.; Crowe, M.; Pedersen, N.L.; Mortimer, J.; Crimmins, E.; Johansson, B.; Gatz, M. Complexity of work and risk of Alzheimer’s disease: A population-based study of Swedish twins. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2005, 60, P251–P258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Then, F.S.; Luck, T.; Luppa, M.; Thinschmidt, M.; Deckert, S.; Nieuwenhuijsen, K.; Seidler, A.; Riedel-Heller, S.G. Systematic review of the effect of the psychosocial working environment on cognition and dementia. Occup. Environ. Med. 2014, 71, 358–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Qiu, C.; Karp, A.; von Strauss, E.; Winblad, B.; Fratiglioni, L.; Bellander, T. Lifetime principal occupation and risk of Alzheimer’s disease in the Kungsholmen project. Am. J. Ind. Med. 2003, 43, 204–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Andel, R.; Kåreholt, I.; Parker, M.G.; Thorslund, M.; Gatz, M. Complexity of primary lifetime occupation and cognition in advanced old age. J. Aging Health 2007, 19, 397–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Smyth, K.; Fritsch, T.; Cook, T.; McClendon, M.; Santillan, C.; Friedland, R. Worker functions and traits associated with occupations and the development of AD. Neurology 2004, 63, 498–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Russo, A.; Onder, G.; Cesari, M.; Zamboni, V.; Barillaro, C.; Capoluongo, E.; Pahor, M.; Bernabei, R.; Landi, F. Lifetime occupation and physical function: A prospective cohort study on persons aged 80 years and older living in a community. Occup. Environ. Med. 2006, 63, 438–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ross, C.E.; Wu, C.-L. Education, age, and the cumulative advantage in health. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1996, 37, 104–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Oaxaca, R. Male-female wage differentials in urban labor markets. Int. Econ. Rev. 1973, 14, 693–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. England, P. Households, Employment, and Gender: A Social, Economic, and Demographic View; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  33. England, P. Comparable Worth: Theories and Evidence; Aldine De Gruyter: New York, NY, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  34. Kilbourne, B.; England, P.; Beron, K. Effects of individual, occupational, and industrial characteristics on earnings: Intersections of race and gender. Soc. Forces 1994, 72, 1149–1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Jang, S.-N. Korean Longitudinal Study of Ageing (KLoSA): Overview of research design and contents. Encycl. Geropsychol. 2015, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Lee, Y.; Back, J.H.; Kim, J.; Byeon, H. Multiple socioeconomic risks and cognitive impairment in older adults. Dement. Geriatr. Cognit. Disord. 2010, 29, 523–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Jaishankar, M.; Tseten, T.; Anbalagan, N.; Mathew, B.B.; Beeregowda, K.N. Toxicity, mechanism and health effects of some heavy metals. Interdiscip. Toxicol. 2014, 7, 60–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Chun, H.R.; Cho, I.; Choi, Y.; Cho, S.I. Effects of emotional labor factors and working environment on the risk of depression in pink-collar workers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Edwards, A.M. The" White-Collar Workers". Mon. Labor Rev. 1934, 38, 501–505. [Google Scholar]
  40. Marandi, E.H.; Moghaddas, E.J. Motivation factors of Blue collar workers verses White collar workers in Herzberg’s Two Factors theory. 2013; (unpublished). [Google Scholar]
  41. Yang, S.; Kim, W.; Choi, K.H.; Yi, Y.G. Influence of occupation on lumbar spine degeneration in men: The Korean national health and nutrition examination survey 2010–2013. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2016, 89, 1321–1328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Kim, H.-J.; Min, J.-Y.; Min, K.-B. Successful aging and mortality risk: The Korean longitudinal study of aging (2006–2014). J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 2019, 20, 1013–1020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Dartigues, J.-F.; Gagnon, M.; Letenneur, L.; Barberger-Gateau, P.; Commenges, D.; Evaldre, M.; Salamon, R. Principal lifetime occupation and cognitive impairment in a French elderly cohort (Paquid). Am. J. Epidemiol. 1992, 135, 981–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Alvarado, B.E.; Zunzunegui, M.-V.; del Ser, T.; Béland, F. Cognitive decline is related to education and occupation in a Spanish elderly cohort. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2002, 14, 132–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. James, R.N., III; Sweaney, A.L. Housing dissatisfaction and cognitive decline in older adults. J. Hous. Elder. 2010, 24, 93–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Shankar, A.; McMunn, A.; Steptoe, A. Health-related behaviors in older adults: Relationships with socioeconomic status. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2010, 38, 39–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Martikainen, P.; Valkonen, T.; Martelin, T. Change in male and female life expectancy by social class: Decomposition by age and cause of death in Finland 1971-95. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 2001, 55, 494–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Park, H. Inequality of educational opportunity in Korea by gender, socio-economic background, and family structure. In Equality in Asia-Pacific; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2014; pp. 182–200. [Google Scholar]
  49. Zhang, Z. Gender differentials in cognitive impairment and decline of the oldest old in China. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2006, 61, S107–S115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Nieuwenhuis-Mark, R.E. The death knoll for the MMSE: Has it outlived its purpose? J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 2010, 23, 151–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pezzotti, P.; Scalmana, S.; Mastromattei, A.; di Lallo, D. The accuracy of the MMSE in detecting cognitive impairment when administered by general practitioners: A prospective observational study. BMC Fam. Pract. 2008, 9, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Seo, E.H. Neuropsychological assessment of dementia and cognitive disorders. J. Korean Neuropsychiatr. Assoc. 2018, 57, 2–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Kueper, J.K.; Speechley, M.; Montero-Odasso, M. The Alzheimer’s disease assessment scale–cognitive subscale (ADAS-Cog): Modifications and responsiveness in pre-dementia populations. Narrat. Rev. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2018, 63, 423–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Wouters, H.; van Gool, W.A.; Schmand, B.; Zwinderman, A.H.; Lindeboom, R. Three sides of the same coin: Measuring global cognitive impairment with the MMSE, ADAS-cog and CAMCOG. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2010, 25, 770–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Arevalo-Rodriguez, I.; Smailagic, N.; i Figuls, M.R.; Ciapponi, A.; Sanchez-Perez, E.; Giannakou, A.; Pedraza, O.L.; Cosp, X.B.; Cullum, S. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the detection of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias in people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2015, 2015, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Wang, H.-X.; MacDonald, S.W.; Dekhtyar, S.; Fratiglioni, L. Association of lifelong exposure to cognitive reserve-enhancing factors with dementia risk: A community-based cohort study. PLoS Med. 2017, 14, e1002251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Lyu, J.; Burr, J.A. Socioeconomic status across the life course and cognitive function among older adults: An examination of the latency, pathways, and accumulation hypotheses. J. Aging Health 2016, 28, 40–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Le Carret, N.; Lafont, S.; Letenneur, L.; Dartigues, J.-F.; Mayo, W.; Fabrigoule, C. The effect of education on cognitive performances and its implication for the constitution of the cognitive reserve. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2003, 23, 317–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Heckman, J.J. Selection Bias and Self-Selection; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 1990; pp. 201–224. [Google Scholar]
  60. Kang, Y.; Na, D.L.; Hahn, S. A validity study on the Korean Mini-Mental State Examination (K-MMSE) in dementia patients. J. Korean Neurol. Assoc. 1997, 15, 300–308. [Google Scholar]
  61. Oh, E.; Kang, Y.; Shin, J.; Yeon, B. A validity study of K-MMSE as a screening test for dementia: Comparison against a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation. Dement Neurocognitive Disord 2010, 9, 8–12. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. A flow chart of study population (N = 1733; male 736, female 997).
Figure 1. A flow chart of study population (N = 1733; male 736, female 997).
Ijerph 17 06270 g001
Figure 2. Gender-specific proportion of high cognitive function (K-MMSE ≥ 24) and mean scores of K-MMSE according to the occupational types over 10 years (2006, 2012, and 2016). (In male, the prevalence of high cognitive function and K-MMSE scores in all occupations decreased over the following ten years with the lowest cognitive function in blue-collar workers; in female, the proportion of subjects with high cognitive function decreased over the years with the lowest percentage of high cognitive function in blue-collar workers. K-MMSE scores significantly decreased over the years in blue and pink-collar workers.).
Figure 2. Gender-specific proportion of high cognitive function (K-MMSE ≥ 24) and mean scores of K-MMSE according to the occupational types over 10 years (2006, 2012, and 2016). (In male, the prevalence of high cognitive function and K-MMSE scores in all occupations decreased over the following ten years with the lowest cognitive function in blue-collar workers; in female, the proportion of subjects with high cognitive function decreased over the years with the lowest percentage of high cognitive function in blue-collar workers. K-MMSE scores significantly decreased over the years in blue and pink-collar workers.).
Ijerph 17 06270 g002
Table 1. Characteristics of the study population according to the longest-held lifetime occupation by gender.
Table 1. Characteristics of the study population according to the longest-held lifetime occupation by gender.
MaleFemale
Blue-CollarPink-CollarWhite-Collarp-ValueBlue-CollarPink-CollarWhite-Collar p-Value
n = 473
(64.27%)
n = 75
(10.19%)
n = 188
(25.54%)
n = 743
(74.52%)
n = 200
(20.06%)
n = 54
(5.42%)
Age
Mean (SD)70.23 (4.33)69.41 (4.56)70.92 (4.51)0.032571.16 (4.98)70.64 (5.09)69.04 (3.37)0.0059
Marital Status
Married442 (93.45)66 (88)178 (94.68)0.2898376 (50.61)98 (49)34 (62.96)0.3743
Divorced/Widowed30 (6.34)9 (12)10 (5.32) 364 (48.99)102 (51)20 (37.04)
Never Married1 (0.21)0 (0)0 (0) 3 (0.4)0 (0)0 (0)
Education
Elementary or Less 306 (64.69)28 (37.33)23 (12.23)<0.0001703 (94.62)166 (83)12 (22.22)<0.0001
Middle/High School 160 (33.83)42 (56)95 (50.53) 40 (5.38)32 (16)36 (66.67)
College or Higher7 (1.48)5 (6.67)70 (37.23) 0 (0)2 (1)6 (11.11)
Income (Yearly, 10,000 KRW) *
<1000304 (64.27)43 (57.33)103 (54.79)0.051519 (69.85)138 (69)34 (62.96)0.7943
1000–2500117 (24.74)26 (34.67)54 (28.72) 159 (21.4)41 (20.5)14 (25.93)
>250052 (10.99)6 (8)31 (16.49) 65 (8.75)21 (10.5)6 (11.11)
Cigarette Smoking
Never Smoker206 (43.55)33 (44)87 (46.28)0.0003716 (96.37)190 (95)53 (98.15)0.8463
Former Smoker108 (22.83)19 (25.33)68 (36.17) 6 (0.81)2 (1)0 (0)
Current Smoker159 (33.62)23 (30.67)33 (17.55) 21 (2.83)8 (4)1 (1.85)
Alcohol Drinking
Current Drinker275 (58.14)42 (56)105 (55.85)0.862591 (12.25)27 (13.5)5 (9.26)0.0371
Former Drinker70 (14.8)9 (12)30 (15.96) 20 (2.69)0 (0)3 (5.56)
Nondrinker128 (27.06)24 (32)53 (28.19) 632 (85.06)173 (86.5)46 (85.19)
Physical Activity
Yes152 (32.14)33 (44)138 (73.4)<0.0001158 (21.27)74 (37)29 (53.7)<0.0001
No321 (67.86)42 (56)50 (26.6) 585 (78.73)126 (63)25 (46.3)
Muscle Strength **
Low226 (47.78)29 (38.67)68 (36.17)0.0159363 (48.86)80 (40)18 (33.33)0.0123
High247 (52.22)46 (61.33)120 (63.83) 380 (51.14)120 (60)36 (66.67)
Chronic Disease
Yes105 (22.2)13 (17.33)40 (21.28)0.633169 (22.75)54 (27)9 (16.67)0.2241
No368 (77.8)62 (82.67)148 (78.72) 574 (77.25)146 (73)45 (83.33)
Living Alone
Yes311 (65.75)50 (66.67)118 (62.77)0.7336453 (60.97)105 (52.5)38 (70.37)0.0252
No162 (34.25)25 (33.33)70 (37.23) 290 (39.03)95 (47.5)16 (29.63)
Well-Being
Yes382 (80.76)63 (84)174 (92.55)0.0009564 (75.91)152 (76)45 (83.33)0.4605
No91 (19.24)12 (16)14 (7.45) 179 (24.09)48 (24)9 (16.67)
Work Duration †
Low168 (35.52)66 (88)146 (77.66)<0.0001305 (41.05)160 (80)40 (74.07)<0.0001
High305 (64.48)9 (12)42 (22.34) 438 (58.95)40 (20)14 (25.93)
Time Spent after Retirement ‡
Low286 (60.47)47 (62.67)48 (25.53)<0.0001399 (53.7)93 (46.5)24 (44.44)0.1057
High187 (39.53)28 (37.33)140 (74.47) 344 (46.3)107 (53.5)30 (55.56)
K-MMSE Score
Mean (SD)24.9026.4326.53<0.000121.1023.1526.13<0.0001
* <10,000,000 KRW (approximately <9000 USD), 10,000,000e 25,000,000 KRW (approximately 9000–22,500 USD), or >25,000,000 KRW (approximately 22,500 USD). ** Muscle strength was categorized based on gender-specific handgrip strength median values; high (male >28.75 kg and female >17.0 kg) or low (male ≤28.75 kg and female ≤17.0 kg). † Work duration was categorized based on gender-specific median values; high (male >39 years and female >29 years) or low (male ≤39 years and female ≤29 years). ‡ Time spent from retirement to examination of MMSE was categorized based on gender-specific median values; high (male >1 year and female >8 years) or low (male ≤1 year and female ≤8 years).
Table 2. The odds ratios (ORs) of cognitive impairment (MMSE < 24) in male elderly over 10 years (2006, 2012, and 2016).
Table 2. The odds ratios (ORs) of cognitive impairment (MMSE < 24) in male elderly over 10 years (2006, 2012, and 2016).
200620122016
Unadjusted ModelOR95% CIOR95% CIOR95% CI
White-Collar JobReferenceReferenceReference
Blue-Collar Job3.34(2.04–5.48)2.02(1.36–3.00)2.16(1.49–3.12)
Pink-Collar Job0.95(0.40–2.25)1.1(0.58–2.09)1.46(0.82–2.60)
Adjusted Model †OR95% CIOR95% CIOR95% CI
White-Collar JobReferenceReferenceReference
Blue-Collar Job1.69(0.88–3.24)1.13(0.65–1.95)1.34(0.80–2.26)
Pink-Collar Job0.68(0.26–1.80)0.94(0.44–2.00)1.24(0.62–2.48)
† The model was adjusted for age, marital status, education, income, cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking, physical activity, muscle strength, chronic diseases, job duration, time passed from retirement to assessing cognitive function, well-being, and living alone.
Table 3. The ORs of cognitive impairment (MMSE < 24) in female elderly over 10 years (2006, 2012, and 2016).
Table 3. The ORs of cognitive impairment (MMSE < 24) in female elderly over 10 years (2006, 2012, and 2016).
200620122016
Unadjusted ModelOR95% CIOR95% CIOR95% CI
White-Collar JobReferenceReferenceReference
Blue-Collar Job8.13(3.91–16.88)3.56(1.99–6.38)4.82(2.66–8.73)
Pink-Collar Job4.35(2.02–9.36)1.86(1.00–3.48)3.11(1.64–5.89)
Adjusted model †OR95% CIOR95% CIOR95% CI
White-Collar JobReferenceReferenceReference
Blue-Collar Job2.49(1.05–5.88)1.57(0.74–3.31)2.17(1.02–4.65)
Pink-Collar Job1.96(0.81–4.78)0.93(0.43–2.02)1.89(0.86–4.16)
† The model was adjusted for age, marital status, education, income, cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking, physical activity, muscle strength, chronic diseases, job duration, time passed from retirement to assessing cognitive function, well-being, and living alone.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kim, H.-J.; Min, J.-Y.; Min, K.-B. The Association between Longest-Held Lifetime Occupation and Late-Life Cognitive Impairment: Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (2006–2016). Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6270. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17176270

AMA Style

Kim H-J, Min J-Y, Min K-B. The Association between Longest-Held Lifetime Occupation and Late-Life Cognitive Impairment: Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (2006–2016). International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(17):6270. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17176270

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kim, Hye-Jin, Jin-Young Min, and Kyoung-Bok Min. 2020. "The Association between Longest-Held Lifetime Occupation and Late-Life Cognitive Impairment: Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (2006–2016)" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 17: 6270. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17176270

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop