Next Article in Journal
Mental Disabilities Increase the Risk of Respiratory Infection-Related Healthcare Utilization
Previous Article in Journal
Can Between-Session Homework Be Delivered Digitally? A Pilot Randomized Clinical Trial of CBT for Adjustment Disorders
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Methods in Experimental Work Break Research: A Scoping Review

1
Department of Management Science, Bonn-Rhein-Sieg University of Applied Sciences, 53757 Sankt Augustin, Germany
2
Federal Institute for Occupational Safety and Health Dresden, 01099 Dresden, Germany
3
Institute of Movement and Neuroscience, German Sport University Cologne, 50933 Cologne, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16(20), 3844; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16203844
Submission received: 12 September 2019 / Revised: 4 October 2019 / Accepted: 6 October 2019 / Published: 11 October 2019

Abstract

:
The number of studies on work breaks and the importance of this subject is growing rapidly, with research showing that work breaks increase employees’ wellbeing and performance and workplace safety. However, comparing the results of work break research is difficult since the study designs and methods are heterogeneous and there is no standard theoretical model for work breaks. Based on a systematic literature search, this scoping review included a total of 93 studies on experimental work break research conducted over the last 30 years. This scoping review provides a first structured evaluation regarding the underlying theoretical framework, the variables investigated, and the measurement methods applied. Studies using a combination of measurement methods from the categories “self-report measures,” “performance measures,” and “physiological measures” are most common and to be preferred in work break research. This overview supplies important information for ergonomics researchers allowing them to design work break studies with a more structured and stronger theory-based approach. A standard theoretical model for work breaks is needed in order to further increase the comparability of studies in the field of experimental work break research in the future.

1. Introduction

From a social and economic perspective, companies are becoming increasingly aware of their responsibility for employees’ health. As a consequence, the corporate field has shown a rising interest in concepts of “corporate health management” and in a variety of offers related to health benefits for employees [1]. In the organizational setting, work breaks are considered to positively affect employees’ physical and cognitive performance, well-being, and health.
The rationale behind work breaks was initially set in working conditions, prevalent decades ago. In times when work was generally associated with strenuous physical activities, employees became tired after a certain time, resulting in more variable or even declining task performance with time on task. They needed a rest to physically recover, often combining this rest with food intake. Although work break scheduling and regulations at that time were subjective and not backed by biological measurements [2], numerous countries passed at least some minimal standards for daily within-shift work breaks, as were passed for Europe [3]. However, in the meantime, despite a significant change in workplaces and their psychophysical demands, work break regulations and behavior have basically remained the same.
Earlier research on work breaks focused on performance and productivity [4] and the relation to physical aspects [5] or on incidents and injuries (e.g., [6]). Over time, especially once computers were introduced into office settings and even more because of the change of computer work from less repetitive to more complex [5], research on work breaks changed and mental/psychological strains became a higher attention in work break research [5,7,8].
Investigations into work break research have been conducted in numerous study settings and with varying focuses. Research questioned or focused on either duration, timing, frequency or activity, or a combination of these aspects. Furthermore, there was diversity among the scientific disciplines interacting on work break research (e.g., ergonomics, psychology, work science, medicine) and the investigated variables (e.g., performance, productivity, stress, fatigue, mood, numerous physiological variables). This variety meant that study designs and measurement methods used in work break research were heterogeneous. As a result, there is currently a large number of measurement methods and variables for investigating physiological, psychological, and performance parameters and, unfortunately, with little standardization [9,10]. This is the natural consequence of variation in job design factors across work contexts, with the corresponding need to vary associated measures accordingly in relation to specific design factors.
Comparing work break studies is also difficult because the term “work break” has not been uniformly defined and there are so many different types. Work breaks are described with terms such as rest break, rest, lunch break, coffee break, pauses or break meaning the same or, sometimes, specific types of a work interruption. Wendsche and Lohmann-Haislah [11] differentiated between mandatory, optional, or supplementary work breaks and their length and reasons and between regulations in different countries. Mandatory work breaks can be binding for companies because of legislation or for employees because of company regulations. Work breaks can be planned by the company or the employees themselves. Furthermore, they can be triggered by operational procedures such as waiting time or production stops. Work breaks and work break behavior are often strongly linked to regulations in the different countries or even within industries, which can cause difficulties when work break behavior is under employee control [12], potentially leading to conflicts with the legislative regulations. Work breaks can also be distinguished by their length, lasting from a few seconds (micro break) to a few hours (siesta), by their content (e.g., coffee break, toilet break, lunch break), or by their intensity divided into active (e.g., stretching, walking, yoga) and passive (e.g., napping, relaxation) breaks. In our understanding, the term “work break” can be used as a general term to describe any intended interruption of work, regardless of length (e.g., micro break) or content (e.g., lunch break).
The effect of work breaks is multidimensional as they have an influence on physiological and psychological fatigue and strain, on mood, productivity, and social aspects, to name but a few. For example, if employees tire, they are generally less productive; a negative performance effect they can compensate for by working harder, which will lead to greater physiological or psychological strain. To capture these multidimensional effects, it makes sense to include methods from different measurement aspects. O’Donnell and Eggemeier [13] named five criteria for method selection that can also be transferred to work break research: sensitivity, diagnosticity, intrusiveness, implementation requirements, and acceptance by the operator. In particular, the aspects of sensitivity and diagnosticity are decisive aspects in the choice of suitable measurement methods. De Waard [14] also developed O’Donnell and Eggemeier’s [13] workload assessment methodology to a three-fold division of variables: self-report measures, performance measures, and physiological measures. The division into three categories is well suited for structuring effects in work break research.
Comparing work break studies is difficult as there is no theoretical model specialized on work breaks. As long as there is no independent theoretical model on the effect of work breaks, it makes sense to use as a basis the general models that take into account aspects of work breaks. Theoretical models that have already been used in the context of work breaks are the effort-recovery model by Meijman and Mulder (1998) (e.g., [15,16]) and the conservation of resources theory by Hobfoll (1989) (e.g., [17,18,19]).
The effort-recovery model [20] describes the positive development of recovery over a period of time when work-related demands no longer continue. Work breaks have an influence on the recovery of load reactions. Work break research can follow this theory by investigating forms of improving the recovery process. The conservation of resources theory [21] describes the stress-related loss of resources and predicts stress outcomes, e.g., in organizational settings. Researchers use this theory as a basis since work breaks influence stress processes and resource gain.
The aims of this review are: (1) To structure previously published intervention studies in work break research and (2) to evaluate them regarding their theoretical and methodical constructs. This review therefore provides a first structured overview of intervention studies on work breaks, their underlying theoretical models, and their measurement methods. This overview is important for ergonomics researchers when designing future work break studies and for extending theory-based knowledge of work breaks. It can help to maximize the positive potential of work breaks. Moreover, our review also intends to uncover possible methodological causes that may have led to inconsistent findings in earlier work break research [8,9,10,11].

2. Materials and Methods

To describe and summarize the variety of applied measurement methods in work break research, we conducted a scoping review based on the five stages described by Arksey and O’Malley [22]: (1) Identifying the research question, (2) identifying relevant studies, (3) study selection, (4) charting the data, and (5) collating, summarizing, and reporting the results.

2.1. Stage 1: Identifying the Research Question

The scoping review was led by the following research questions: (a) Which measurement methods are used in work break research to investigate certain variables? (b) Are there any typical theoretical frameworks or hypotheses for work break research? and (c) Is it possible to standardize the future work break research?
Although we are aware of the problem of defining the term “work break,” we wanted to give an overview of applied measurement methods and so did not restrict the identified studies by the varying definitions of “work breaks,” accepting a wide definition. We did select the studies for further evaluation according to their intention to investigate work breaks in occupational settings or to give advice on occupational work design.

2.2. Stage 2: Identifying Relevant Studies

We started with a systematic review. To identify published studies on work breaks, electronic literature databases that are relevant for potential research in ergonomics, psychology, work science and management, and medicine were searched (PubMed, Web of Science, ScienceDirect), using a publication date from 1989 onward (last 30 years). The period of 30 years was chosen since at that time computers changed workplaces and working structures significantly. The three databases were searched in October 2017, and the search was updated in June 2019. Publications in languages other than English, review articles, book chapters, and conference proceedings were excluded from the methods’ evaluation.
Some work break studies have collected their data exclusively through questionnaires, diaries, or surveys. Those studies (e.g., [19,23,24]), which come close to a field study in the true sense, have a high value in understanding work breaks. However, because of the focus of this scoping review, studies without manipulation of either work break regimens or content were not included in the review.
For the literature search, we created a specific search string with the following main terms: (’work break*’ OR ‘break*’ OR ‘pause*’ OR ‘rest break*’ OR ‘lunch break*’) AND (‘work’ OR ‘job’ OR ‘occupation*’ OR ‘employ*’).
Additionally, a reference list search was conducted, especially for the systematic reviews and literature reviews described above, to ensure the inclusion of all studies (from 1989 onward) that other researchers had identified as relevant.
A hand-search of key ergonomics journals was conducted to find possibly missed articles from the database searches and references list searches. This search is recommended since electronic databases can be incomplete or outdated or because abstract services can vary in scope, indexing, and depth of information [22].
The selection procedure and the outcomes are shown in Figure 1.

2.3. Stage 3: Study Selection

The search strategy identified numerous studies that do not focus on work breaks. Since the line of this scoping review was to identify as many measurement methods as possible, particularly strict exclusion criteria for different study designs were not necessary. Nevertheless, a few criteria had to be defined for a better understanding of the research field and to meet certain quality criteria. To be suitable, a study had to (1) investigate intra-shift work breaks, work break regimens, or work break activities, (2) be an intervention study (manipulating at least one aspect of (1)), (3) be peer-reviewed, (4) be in the English language, and (5) be published in full-text. Further quality criteria were not used.
The database search resulted in 1313 records. By excluding duplicates (k = 396) and adding further records from the reference list and hand searches (k = 30), a total of 947 records were screened by title and abstract. Of those records, 755 were discarded mostly because of not investigating on work breaks, resulting in 159 undergoing a full-text screening for suitability. The full-text screening resulted in 93 potentially eligible studies, which were finally included in the review (see Figure 1 for a flow chart).

2.4. Stage 4: Charting the Data

The key information of the selected studies was charted in line with the description by Arksey and O’Malley [22]. Comparable to a narrative review, the charted data were comprehensive and non-rating. The data (see Table A1) recorded the main information, such as general information (citation), study key data, investigated variables, and applied measurement methods.

2.5. Stage 5: Collating, Summarizing, and Reporting the Results

In the next step, the data were transferred into a structure that could reflect the special features of work break research. For this purpose, a separate search was performed to find a suitable basic structure for the examined variables. The structure was then adapted to work break research (see Structure of Variables in Results). In the next step, the first author developed suitable variable-subcategories by viewing and clustering the variables examined by the studies. Afterward, three tables were created for structuring the variables, the variable-subcategories, and the applied measurement methods. One author performed data extraction. A second author checked this extraction for the listed measurement methods. Articles that did not define the applied methods clearly were excluded.
Unlike a systematic review, a scoping review does not synthesize evidence or summarize findings. Thus, this scoping review did not seek to assess quality of evidence and therefore does not determine the studies’ robustness or generalizability [22]; it describes the applied methods.

3. Results

Table A1 gives an overview of the characteristics of the studies. The following results of the research into measurement methods in work break analysis consists of methods from 93 publications from five continents: America (k = 36), Europe (k = 33), Asia (k = 17), Australia (k = 6), and Africa (k = 1). 55% of the studies were conducted in laboratories and 45% in the field. The review shows an increasing number of studies over the past 30 years, (1989–1998: k = 22; 1999–2008: k = 23; 2009–2019: k = 48). The examined activities can be divided into predominantly mental activities (67%) and predominantly physical activities (33%) including hospital activities (nursing, surgery), whereby many physical activities are of course also mentally demanding (e.g., concentration in surgery) or emotionally demanding (e.g., nursing). The studies were not dominated by specific industries or job titles. They investigated workplaces that can mainly be described as desk-based computer workstations where the participants worked during the studies. About 56% of the studies investigated different break regimens, whereas 44% of the studies focused on activity during work breaks. Two studies [25,26] investigated both break regimen and break activity; they were assigned to the break regimen for further evaluation as they focused on that topic. Almost half the studies (49%) were exploratory, without formulating a hypothesis. A total of 13 studies (14%) used a theoretical model as a study design basis. Of these 13 studies, only one theory (resource theory: [27,28]) occurred more than once. Furthermore, we found that within three decades the focus of work break research seems to be shifting from the United States to Europe and Asia (Table 1) and that the proportion of hypothesis- and theory-based studies has increased in the past decade (Table 2).
As described earlier, measurement methods can be divided into three groups: self-report measures (SRM), performance measures (PER), and physiological measures (PHY) [14]. SRM was used most frequently in 88% of the studies. PER was used in 61% of the studies, and PHY in 69%. Most of the studies (82%) used measurement methods from two or even all the categories. The combination of the applied measurement methods is shown in Figure 2. A combination of methods from all three categories can be found in more than a third of the studies (37%), followed by the combination of SRM and PER (22%) and SRM and PHY (20%). In field studies, the combination SRM and PER dominates (26%), and in laboratory studies, the SMR, PER, and PHY combination dominates (49%). The combination of SMR, PER, and PHY is most frequently used in the study of break regimens (37%) as well as for break activities (37%). Figure 2 gives an overview of the distribution of the method combinations.
De Waard’s [14] division into three methodological categories was further classified depending on the study situation. The structure thus provides an overview of the relevance of individual variables and measurement methods. Important for this review is that many of the studies used standardized questionnaires. However, these questionnaires are not always free of overlaps and the variables were investigated using different methods. The studies often only used certain items of a specific questionnaire to examine a variable.

3.1. Self-Report Measures

Self-report measures (see Figure 3), often indicated as subjective measures [14], were described by means of standardized or self-designed questionnaires (SDQ) and are the most popular assessment form in work break research. Within the self-report measures category, psychological and non-psychological measures can be distinguished. Within the psychological measures, the subdivisions mood/well-being, mental performance, engagement/motivation, workload, and work and break conditions are evident. Within the non-psychological measures, the two subdivisions musculoskeletal discomfort/pain and perceived exertion/fatigue arise.

3.1.1. Psychological Measures

Mood and Well-being. The variable mood/well-being is a central object of investigation in work break research. To measure this state of mind, the Profile of Mood States (POMS; [29]) was most commonly applied. In addition, numerous studies used self-designed questionnaires, often method-based on the Visual Analogue Scale (VAS; first described by Hayes and Paterson [30]), or selecting single scales or items (e.g., rating stress or sleep) from standardized questionnaires.
Mental performance. This variable evaluates cognitive performance in terms of alertness, attention, and concentration. Contrary to this positive state of mind in which personal best possible performance can be achieved, there is sleepiness (usually short term) or fatigue (often a longer lasting state). The Stanford Sleepiness Scale (SSS; [31]) and the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS; [32]) were the main instruments to examine these mental states.
Engagement and Motivation. The engagement and motivation to accomplish a task are also of interest when considering the effects of a work break and have been investigated using various methods.
Workload. This variable is used to determine a subject’s perceived workload. The predominant examination method was the NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX; [33]).
Work and break conditions. The category work and break conditions examines aspects of the workplace design and framework conditions and the associated work break design. Motivation for and continuity in practicing the investigated (and mostly newly introduced) work break or work break design also belong to this category. Usually, self-designed questionnaires or diaries were applied here. In evaluating the work breaks, self-designed questionnaires asked for aspects such as personal preferences and popularity as well as possible barriers. Since these self-designed questionnaires are usually not published and evaluated, they are not discussed in this review.
Others. In addition to these five categories and the related methods, numerous other self-reported methods (e.g., on quality of life or detachment) have been applied in work break studies. These are diverse and have only rarely been used in work break research. They are therefore not discussed in depth in this review.

3.1.2. Non-Psychological Measures

Musculoskeletal discomfort/pain. In the domain of non-psychological assessments, complaints about muscular discomfort or even pain were the most frequently studied variables.
Most of the studies used a Visual Analogue Scale, based on the Borg category ratio scaling (e.g., Borg CR10; [34,35,36]), to evaluate the general physical condition or, more often, individual body parts. The neck and back were the most frequently examined.
Other tools are a Body Part Diagram/Discomfort Scale developed by Corlett and Bishop [37] and a Visual Analogue Discomfort scale [38].
Perceived exertion/fatigue. Perceived exertion was another variable used to assess the physical effects of work breaks. For this purpose, the Borg Rating of Perceived Exertion Scale (Borg RPE scale; [35,39]) has been applied almost universally. It is used to estimate effort and exertion during physical work, among other variables.
Others. Of the other investigated variables, physical fatigue and physical activity should be highlighted. Physical fatigue was measured differently and was surveyed or determined by physical strength exercises. Physical activity, on the other hand, was usually determined using the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ; [40]).

3.2. Performance Measures

According to de Waard [14], performance measures can be grouped into primary-task measures, secondary-task measures, and reference tasks. For work break research, that grouping can be reduced to primary tasks and reference tasks (Figure 4).
Primary tasks were utilized to measure the overall effectiveness. Reference tasks were performed before and after the work break to investigate performance changes through a specific work break regimen or work break activity.

3.2.1. Primary Tasks

In field studies, primary tasks were task specific because of the specificity of the investigated workplace. During quasi-experimental field studies, primary tasks were implemented in the working process and served as a measurable and comparable performance indicator. In laboratory studies, the primary tasks were structured into a few, common tasks. These tasks were often self-designed but aimed at the same measures, such as motor performance, speed of performance, reaction time, accuracy, and number of errors.
Tasks used to measure performance can be divided into typing tasks (keystrokes, error rate, correction rate, number of words typed), reaction tasks (reaction time, accuracy, choice reaction time, auditory reaction time task), and mental arithmetic tasks (mathematical calculation task).

3.2.2. Reference Tasks

Reference tasks mainly serve to check for trend effects and are performed under evaluation before and after the task [14], or for work break research before and after the break. Common tasks were memory tasks, concentration tasks, and reaction tasks. The only standardized tasks that was used more than once as a reference task was the d2 Test of Attention (d2; [41]; d2-R; [42]).

3.3. Physiological Measures

Within the wide range of physiological measurement methods, four methods stand out because of their frequency (Figure 5). The categories cardiac functions, brain activity, and muscle activity are therefore described in more detail below. Other less frequently applied physiological measurement methods are mentioned in the Others category.

3.3.1. Cardiac Functions

The cardiac function can be described as the heart’s ability to fulfill the metabolic demands of the body. As a result of increased metabolic need, e.g., caused by stress or physical exertion, oxygen demand will increase. To fulfill the oxygen demands, the body can increase the respiratory rate or increase heart rate. The electrocardiogram (ECG) can represent the cardiac function by providing information about the electrical activity conduction through the heart [43].
To assess physiological function (and indirect through this, sometimes also mental load, e.g., stress), numerous studies in work break research have measured heart rate and heart rate variability (also described as R-R interval or interbeat interval).

3.3.2. Brain Activity

The activity of the brain is conventionally measured using the encephalogram (EEG), which displays oscillations in several, functionally relevant frequency bands [44]. EEG measurements are conducted using one of two methods: Spontaneous EEG, when the neural oscillations of the brain waves are considered, and the event-related potentials (ERP), when the reaction of the EEG after an evoked stimulus is studied. ERPs are often used to determine the reaction time. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is another tool to measure brain activity applied in work break research. In contrast to the EEG, the fMRI shows regional, time-varying changes in the brain activity but is more complex to use because of the clinical scanner involved.

3.3.3. Muscle Activity

Another physiological measurement of interest is the activity or inactivity of certain muscles. Muscle activity is investigated in particular in connection with physical labor, monotonous or one-sided movements, or sedentary work behavior, with the main focus on neck and shoulder. In particular, the trapezius muscle is used as a standard to measure the shoulder-neck load in occupational or ergonomics studies [45]. The dominant measurement tool is electromyography (EMG) or, more precisely, surface electromyography (sEMG). In work break research, this non-invasive method is usually used.

3.3.4. Others

Further methods in work break research to determine physical functions or the changes of physical parameters because of a certain break regimen or work break activity are eye functions (through electrooculography), spinal shrinkage (through a stadiometer), rest/activity (through actigraphy), sleep, electrodermal activity, temperature, and hormone levels.

4. Discussion

This scoping review examined the work break research conducted over the past 30 years (1989 to 2019). We reviewed (1) the development and domains of research in this area, (2) the theoretical rationale behind this research, and (3) the methodological framework used. Using data from 93 studies, this review therefore goes beyond the existing work and is the first review to focus on the theoretical and methodological approaches.
First, an increasing interest in the effect of work breaks and thus an increasing number of studies on work breaks can be clearly observed for the period investigated. In 2009, Trougakos and Hideg [46] identified ergonomics literature as the primary domain investigating within-day work breaks. In 2019, ergonomics literature is still the main publication channel for work break research but its dominance has changed, and research in the area has now been extended to “medicine,” “neurology,” “physiology,” and others. The reviewed studies come from various countries, with the industrial nations clearly dominating the research activities. The work areas examined in the studies are dominated by computer workstations. However, there are also some studies that investigate activities defined by mainly physical activity.
Second, the difficulty in comparing work break studies and their results could be explained by the heterogeneous approach taken in designing and realizing empirical studies. Nearly half of the studies were exploratory and only 14% of the studies were based on a theoretical model (e.g., resource theory). This is problematic as interpretation of study findings is limited, especially in this case of research where results have been often reported inconsistent across studies. Moreover, it seems that this lack of theory-driven approaches has developed into repeated investigations of rather simple research questions (e.g., Do work breaks reduce mental and physical strain? Which break activities reduce mental and physical strain?). In turn, current knowledge on mechanisms and moderators shaping effects of work breaks is sparse. Therefore, future work break research should be more theoretically guided to explain if, when, and why work breaks have any effects on certain outcomes.
Third, with regard to the issues discussed above, the measurement methods used varied greatly across the studies. The measurement methods were sorted according to three categories of measurement variables: “self-report measures,” “performance measures,” and “physiological measures.” The choice of variables to be investigated depended on the study design and the research question. Ideally, a study design considers variables from two or even all three categories [14].
To examine the multidimensional effects of a work break, using a number of measurement methods is reasonable and a large number of methods for measuring each individual variable are already available, especially in the case of self-reported measures. In most papers, the authors explained their method choice simply by referring to the method being used in previous work. Few studies provide an independent evaluation of the method, especially with regard to the sensitivity and diagnosticity, which are related to the individual study design. The methodological heterogeneity of the research limits a final evaluation of previous results in all relevant aspects of work break research such as performance, motivation, well-being, health, break regimen, break location, and break content [11].
A combination of measurement methods of self-report measures, performance measures, and physiological measures is a promising approach to determine interdependencies of the multidimensional effects [11,14] and has been repeatedly recommended for future research (e.g., [18,23]). The combination of measurement methods from the three measure categories will lead to better results. These methods should be evaluated regarding the five criteria by O’Donnell and Eggemeier [13] especially sensitivity and diagnosticity. Furthermore, the scientific community recommends describing the methods’ objectivity, validity, and reliability. A description of objectivity, validity, and reliability (e.g., [47,48]) was given on 11% of the reviewed studies. Especially in studies with a small number of test persons, effect sizes should be reported (e.g., [49,50]).
The sample selection and sample planning are other critical points. As in many other areas of research, rest break research often uses students as participants in lab studies. In studies that investigate aspects such as stress experience, physical discomfort, and compensatory capacity, this group of young and, in general, healthier people is not necessarily transferable to the work situation and the average employee. Sample planning can only be found sporadically in the studies investigated (e.g., [51]), but could help to improve results in terms of practical relevance in the future.
A central role in evaluating measurement results in work break research is certainly played by the individual level of demands. The individual course of demands and the individual course of recovery from these demands still offer investigative potential and would allow work break effects to be evaluated from the process perspective. In particular, for a detailed assessment of the performance and recovery processes, a distinction must be made between an investigation of the demands and recovery at one point in time or over a certain period of time.
We would like to note that the results of our review are limited by scope of literature search (e.g., selection of electronic databases, search string, inclusion and exclusion criteria). However, we think that the potential of a publication bias is low here as we used databased literature search, inspection of references, and hand-search of journals in combination which resulted in a much higher number of included studies than in other current reviews on work breaks [8,9,10,11].

5. Conclusions

Work breaks are an important measure to offer employees time to immediately recover from work-related consequences of strain after high work demands and, in turn, to improve their mental and physical wellbeing, their performance, and workplace safety.
In our review we set out to structure and evaluate the experimental work break research from 1989 to 2019 (k = 93 studies), since there are manifold ways of analyzing the effects of work breaks regarding study designs and measurement methods.
Most of the examined studies were exploratory, thus, missing a theoretical foundation. Moreover, during the last 30 years most researchers applied previously used measurement methods without evaluating them for their own studies; neither regarding criteria as sensitivity and diagnosticity nor regarding the methods’ objectivity, validity, and reliability.
In sum, in future studies it would be desirable to develop a theoretical model that is focused on the particular and various effects of work breaks and can serve as a standard model for future work break studies [52]. Furthermore, variables measured by methods out of all three categories (“self-report measures,” “performance measures,” and “physiological measures”) should be considered in future study designs.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.S., J.W., A.G. and S.S.; investigation, A.S.; formal analysis, A.S., A.G. and U.S.; visualization, A.S.; writing—original draft preparation, A.S. and U.S.; writing—review and editing, J.W., A.S., U.S., T.P. and S.S.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Acknowledgments

We thank Anne Wegner for proofreading and Nils Günzel for his technical support. André Scholz was supported by a research grant from the Graduate Institute of Bonn-Rhein-Sieg University of Applied Sciences. We would like to thank two unknown reviewer for their valuable comments on a previous version of this manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Characteristics and categories of applied measurement methods of included studies. Abbreviations: SRM = self-reported measures, PER = performance measures, PHY = physiological measures.
Table A1. Characteristics and categories of applied measurement methods of included studies. Abbreviations: SRM = self-reported measures, PER = performance measures, PHY = physiological measures.
Applied Measurement Methods
AuthorYearCountrySiteWork/TaskSample SizeDaysTime-on-TaskObjectHypothesisTheory/ModelSRMPERPHY
Arrabito et al. [53]2011CanadaLabMental98140 minActivitynonoyesyes
Arrabito et al. [54]2015CanadaLabMental150140 minActivityyesyesyesyes
Balci and Aghazadeh [55,56,57]2000 (2003, 2004)USALabMental101120 minRegimennonoyesyesyes
Beynon and Reilly [58]2001UKLabPhysical102240 minActivityyesnoyes yes
Beynon et al. [59]2000UKLabPhysical82full working dayRegimenyesnoyes yes
Boucsein and Thum [5,60]1995 (1997)GermanyFieldMental112full working dayRegimennonoyes yes
Brown et al. [51]2014UKFieldMental7356full working dayActivityyesnoyes yes
Byström et al. [61]1991SwedenLabPhysical813 minActivitynonoyesyesyes
Chaikumarn et al. [62]2018ThailandLabMental30160 minActivityyesno yesyes
Chakrabarty et al. [63]2016IndiaFieldPhysical400120360 minRegimenyesnoyes
Chavaillaz et al. [64]2019SwitzerlandLabMental72160 minRegimenyesyesyesyes
Christensen et al. [65]2000DenmarkFieldPhysical481full working dayRegimenyesno yes
Coburn et al. [66]2006GermanyFieldPhysical302450 minRegimenyesnoyesyes
Constable et al. [67]1994USALabPhysical81180 minActivityyesno yes
Cordoza et al. [68]2018USAFieldPhysical2984full working dayActivitynonoyes
Crenshaw et al. [69]2006SwedenLabMental15260 minActivityyesnoyesyesyes
Dababneh et al. [70]2001USAFieldPhysical3036full working dayRegimennonoyesyes
Davis and Kotowski [71]2014USAFieldMental3740full working dayRegimennonoyesyesyes
Davy and Göbel [25]2013GermanyLabPhysical243full night shiftRegimennonoyesyesyes
de Looze et al. [72]2010The NetherlandsFieldPhysical143440 minRegimennonoyesyes
Dorion and Darveau [73]2013CanadaFieldPhysical163full working dayRegimenyesnoyesyesyes
Engelmann et al. [74]2011GermanyFieldPhysical71full working dayRegimenyesnoyesyesyes
Faucett et al. [75]2007USAFieldPhysical66/322/6480 minRegimennonoyesyes
Frey et al. [76]2002AustriaFieldPhysical112full 24h shiftActivitynonoyesyesyes
Galinsky et al. [77]2000USAFieldPhysical4260full working dayRegimennonoyesyes
Galinsky et al. [78]2007USAFieldMental5156full working dayRegimennonoyesyes
Garcia et al. [79]2018SwitzerlandLabPhysical302300 minRegimenyesnoyes yes
Genaidy and al-Rayes [80]1993USALabPhysical5/82240 minRegimennonoyesyesyes
Genaidy et al. [81]1995USAFieldPhysical2828full working dayRegimennonoyes
Hayashi et al. [82]2004JapanFieldMental102120 minActivitynonoyesyesyes
Helander and Quance [83]1990USALabMental74200 minRegimenyesno yes
Henning et al. [84]1989USALabMental202120 minRegimenyesnoyesyesyes
Henning et al. [85]1993USALabMental38148 minRegimennonoyesyes
Henning et al. [86]1994aUSALabMental31165 minRegimennonoyesyesyes
Henning et al. [87]1994bUSALabMental38148 minRegimennonoyesyesyes
Henning et al. [88]1995USALabMental30155 minRegimennonoyesyesyes
Henning et al. [89]1996USALabMental31/30160 minRegimennonoyesyesyes
Henning et al. [26]1997aUSAFieldMental73/1928/42full working dayRegimenyesnoyesyes
Henning et al. [90]1997bUSALabMental60146 minRegimennonoyesyesyes
Hirose and Nagasaka [91]2003JapanLabMental81 100 minActivitynonoyesyesyes
Hofer-Tinguely et al. [92]2005SwitzerlandLabPhysical50160 minActivitynonoyesyesyes
Howard et al. [93]2010AustraliaLabMental83full night shiftRegimennonoyesyesyes
Irmak et al. [94]2012TurkeyFieldMental3970full working dayRegimennonoyes
Januario et al. [95]2018BrazilLabPhysical18140 minActivityyesnoyes yes
Kakarot et al. [96]2012GermanyLabPhysical292420 minRegimenyesnoyes yes
Karwowski et al. [97]1994USALabMental1212240 minRegimenyesnoyes yes
Kennedy and Ball [98]2007AustraliaFieldMental7528full working dayActivityyesnoyes
Kietrys et al. [99]2007USAFieldMental7228full working dayActivitynonoyes
Kopardekar and Mital [100]1994USALabMental81120 minRegimennonoyesyes
Krajewski et al. [101]2011GermanyFieldMental14168full working dayActivityyesyes yes
Lacaze et al. [102]2010BrazilFieldMental6440full working dayActivitynonoyes
Larsen et al. [103]2009DenmarkFieldMental18130 minActivityyesnoyesyesyes
Lim and Kwok [104]2016SingaporeLabMental71160 minRegimenyesyesyesyes
Lim et al. [105]2013SingaporeLabMental31265 minRegimenyesno yesyes
Lim et al. [106]2016SingaporeLabMental2721 minRegimenyesno yesyes
Mailey et al. [107]2016USAFieldMental4956full working dayRegimennonoyes yes
Mathiassen & Winkel [108]1996SwedenLabPhysical87240–360 minActivitynonoyes yes
Mathiassen et al. [109]2014SwedenLabMental18342 minRegimennoyesyesyesyes
McLean et al. [110]2001CanadaFieldMental1530full working dayRegimenyesnoyesyesyes
Miller and Fathallah [111]2006USAFieldPhysical14155 minRegimennono yes
Mitra et al. [112]2008AustraliaFieldPhysical23328full working dayRegimennonoyesyes
Mohamed and Radwan [113]2017Saudi ArabiaLabMental45160 minRegimennono yes
Nakphet et al. [114]2014ThailandLabMental30160 minActivitynonoyesyesyes
Neri et al. [115]2002USALabMental281360 minRegimenyesnoyes yes
O’Donnell et al. [116]2015AustraliaLabMental60148 minRegimenyesyesyesyesyes
Oriyama et al. [117]2014JapanFieldPhysical151full night shiftRegimennonoyes yes
Rahman et al. [118]2018MalaysiaFieldPhysical151160 minRegimennono yes
Reyner & Horne [119]1997UKLabMental121180 minActivitynonoyesyesyes
Ross et al. [120]2014New ZealandLabMental40140 minActivityyesyesyesyes
Rupp et al. [121]2017USALabMental65115 minActivityyesyesyesyes
Samani et al. [122]2009aDenmarkLabMental12212 + 4 minActivityyesyesyes yes
Samani et al. [123]2009bDenmarkLabMental12140 minActivityyesnoyes yes
Schnieder et al. [124]2013GermanyFieldMental14180full working dayActivityyesnoyesyes
Scholz et al. [50]2018GermanyLabMental124120 minActivityyesnoyesyesyes
Sheahan et al. [125]2016CanadaLabMental20460–70 minRegimenyesnoyesyesyes
Sianoja et al. [15]2017FinlandFieldMental9710full working dayActivityyesyesyes
Sommer et al. [126]2013GermanyLabMental3220Activityyesnoyesyesyes
Steinborn and Hystegge [127]2016GermanyLabMental68150 minActivityyesyesyesyes
St-Onge et al. [128]2017CanadaLabMental27140 minActivityyesnoyesyesyes
Sun et al. [49]2017SingaporeLabMental20222 minRegimenyesyesyesyesyes
Sundelin [129]1993SwedenLabPhysical121120 minActivitynonoyes yes
Sundelin and Hagberg [130]1989SwedenFieldMental12190 minActivitynonoyes yes
Takahashi et al. [131]1998JapanLabMental301full working dayRegimenyesnoyesyesyes
Takahashi et al. [132]2004JapanFieldPhysical821full working dayActivitynonoyesyesyes
Taylor et al. [133]2013USAFieldMental35180/365full working dayActivitynonoyes
Taylor et al. [47]2016USAFieldMental175180full working dayActivityyesnoyes yes
Tiwari and Gite [134]2006IndiaFieldPhysical54360 minRegimennonoyes yes
Torrente et al. [135]2017FinlandFieldMental15330full working dayActivityyesyes yes
van den Heuvel et al. [136]2003The NetherlandsFieldMental26860full working dayActivitynonoyesyes
van Dieen and Oude Vrielink [137]1998The NetherlandsFieldPhysical123full working dayRegimennonoyes yes
Wang and Pei [138]2014ChinaFieldMental331120–240 minRegimennonoyesyes
Watling et al. [48]2014AustraliaLabMental201180 minActivityyesnoyesyesyes
Wollseiffen et al. [139]2015GermanyFieldMental501full working dayActivityyesnoyesyesyes

References

  1. Montano, D.; Hoven, H.; Siegrist, J. Effects of organisational-level interventions at work on employees’ health: A systematic review. BMC Public Health 2014, 14, 135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Bhatia, N.; Murrel, K.F.H. An industrial experiment in organized rest pauses. Hum. Factors 1969, 11, 167–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. European Parliament. Directive 2003/88/EC of the European Parliament and of the Council of 4 November 2003 Concerning Certain Aspects of the Organisation of Working Time; European Parliament: Brussels, Belgium, 2003; Volume Directive 2003/88/EC. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bechtold, S.E.; Janaro, R.E.; Sumners, D.W.L. Maximization of labor productivity through optimal rest-break schedules. Manag. Sci. 1984, 30, 1442–1458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Boucsein, W.; Thum, M. Design of work/rest schedules for computer work based on psychophysiological recovery measures. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 1997, 20, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Pragier, E. Work breaks for keyboard operators. Aust. J. Physiother. 1986, 32, 157–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Ghadiri, A. Arbeitspausen in der betrieblichen Gesundheitsförderung. ZFO 2016, 85, 270–276. [Google Scholar]
  8. Tucker, P. The impact of rest breaks upon accident risk, fatigue and performance: A review. Work Stress 2003, 17, 123–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Barredo, R.D.V.; Mahon, K. The effects of exercise and rest breaks on musculoskeletal discomfort during computer tasks: An evidence-based perspective. J. Phys. Therapy Sci. 2007, 19, 151–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Brewer, S.; Eerd, D.V.; Amick, B.C., III; Irvin, E.; Daum, K.M.; Gerr, F.; Moore, J.S.; Cullen, K.; Rempel, D. Workplace interventions to prevent musculoskeletal and visual symptoms and disorders among computer users: A systematic review. J. Occup. Rehab. 2006, 16, 317–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Wendsche, J.; Lohmann-Haislah, A. Psychische Gesundheit in der Arbeitswelt: Pausen; Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin: Dortmund, Germany; Berlin, Germany; Dresden, Germany, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  12. Zhu, Z.; Kuykendall, L.; Zhang, X. The impact of within-day work breaks on daily recovery processes: An event-based pre-/post-experience sampling study. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2019, 92, 191–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. O’Donnell, R.D.; Eggemeier, F.T. Workload assessment methodology. In Handbook of Perception and Human Performance Vol. 2: Cognitive Processes and Performance; John Wiley & Sons: Oxford, UK, 1986; pp. 1–49. ISBN 0-471-82956-0. [Google Scholar]
  14. De Waard, D. The Measurement of Drivers’ Mental Workload; University of Groningen: Groningen, The Netherlands, 1996; ISBN 978-90-6807-308-9. [Google Scholar]
  15. Sianoja, M.; Syrek, C.J.; de Bloom, J.; Korpela, K.; Kinnunen, U. Enhancing daily well-being at work through lunchtime park walks and relaxation exercises: Recovery experiences as mediators. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2017, 23, 428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Kim, S.; Park, Y.; Niu, Q. Micro-break activities at work to recover from daily work demands. J. Organ. Behav. 2017, 38, 28–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Zacher, H.; Brailsford, H.A.; Parker, S.L. Micro-breaks matter: A diary study on the effects of energy management strategies on occupational well-being. J. Vocat. Behav. 2014, 85, 287–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kuehnel, J.; Zacher, H.; de Bloom, J.; Bledow, R. Take a break! Benefits of sleep and short breaks for daily work engagement. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 2017, 26, 481–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Bosch, C.; Sonnentag, S.; Pinck, A.S. What makes for a good break? A diary study on recovery experiences during lunch break. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2018, 91, 134–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Meijman, T.F.; Mulder, G. Psychological aspects of workload. In A Handbook of Work and Organizational Psychology; Drenth, P.J.D., Thierry, H., de Wolff, C.J., Eds.; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 1998; pp. 5–34. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hobfoll, S.E. Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am. Psychol. 1989, 44, 513–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Arksey, H.; O’Malley, L. Scoping studies: Towards a methodological framework. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 2005, 8, 19–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Kim, S.; Park, Y.; Headrick, L. Daily micro-breaks and job performance: General work engagement as a cross-level moderator. J. Appl. Psychol. 2018, 103, 772–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sianoja, M.; Kinnunen, U.; de Bloom, J.; Korpela, K.; Geurts, S. Recovery during Lunch Breaks: Testing Long-Term Relations with Energy Levels at Work. Scan. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 2016, 1, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Davy, J.; Göbel, M. The effects of a self-selected nap opportunity on the psychophysiological, performance and subjective measures during a simulated industrial night shift regimen. Ergonomics 2013, 56, 220–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Henning, R.A.; Jacques, P.; Kissel, G.V.; Sullivan, A.B.; Alteras-Webb, S.M. Frequent short rest breaks from computer work: Effects on productivity and well-being at two field sites. Ergonomics 1997, 40, 78–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Kahneman, D. Attention and Effort; Prentice-Hall Series in Experimental Psychology; Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1973; ISBN 978-0-13-050518-7. [Google Scholar]
  28. Warm, J.S.; Parasuraman, R.; Matthews, G. Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Hum. Factors J. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. 2008, 50, 433–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. McNair, D.M.; Lorr, M.; Droppleman, L.F. Profile of Mood States; Educational and Industrial Testing Service: San Diego, CA, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hayes, M.; Patterson, D. Experimental development of the graphic rating method. Psychol. Bull. 1921, 98–99. [Google Scholar]
  31. Hoddes, E.; Zarcone, V.; Smythe, H.; Phillips, R.; Dement, W.C. Quantification of sleepiness: A new approach. Psychophysiology 1973, 10, 431–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Akerstedt, T.; Gillberg, M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int. J. Neurosci. 1990, 52, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Hart, S.G.; Staveland, L.E. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. Adv. Psychol. 1988, 52, 139–183. [Google Scholar]
  34. Borg, G. Physical Performance and Perceived Exertion; University Lund: Oxford, UK, 1962. [Google Scholar]
  35. Borg, G. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1982, 14, 377–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Borg, G. Anstrengungsempfinden und körperliche Aktivität. Dtsch. Arztebl. Int. 2004, 101, A-1016. [Google Scholar]
  37. Corlett, E.N.; Bishop, R.P. A technique for assessing postural discomfort. Ergonomics 1976, 19, 175–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Huskisson, E.C. Measurement of pain. Lancet 1974, 2, 1127–1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Borg, G. Perceived exertion as an indicator of somatic stress. Scand. J. Rehabil. Med. 1970, 2, 92–98. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  40. Craig, C.L.; Marshall, A.L.; Sjöström, M.; Bauman, A.E.; Booth, M.L.; Ainsworth, B.E.; Pratt, M.; Ekelund, U.; Yngve, A.; Sallis, J.F.; et al. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2003, 35, 1381–1395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Brickenkamp, R.; Zillmer, E. d2 Test of Attention; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  42. Brickenkamp, R.; Schmidt-Atzert, L.; Liepmann, D. Test d2–Revision Aufmerksamkeits-und Konzentrationstest; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  43. Carreiro, J. The cardiovascular system. In An Osteopathic Approach to Children; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2009; pp. 73–83. ISBN 978-0-443-06738-9. [Google Scholar]
  44. Moffett, S.X.; O’Malley, S.M.; Man, S.; Hong, D.; Martin, J.V. Dynamics of high frequency brain activity. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Mathiassen, S.E.; Winkel, J.; Hägg, G.M. Normalization of surface EMG amplitude from the upper trapezius muscle in ergonomic studies—A review. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 1995, 5, 197–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Trougakos, J.P.; Hideg, I. Momentary work recovery: The role of within-day work breaks. In Current Perspectives on Job-Stress Recovery; Research in Occupational Stress and Well-being; Emerald Group Publishing Limited: Bingley, UK, 2009; Volume 7, pp. 37–84. [Google Scholar]
  47. Taylor, W.C.; Paxton, R.J.; Shegog, R.; Coan, S.P.; Dubin, A.; Page, T.F.; Rempel, D.M. Impact of booster breaks and computer prompts on physical activity and sedentary behavior among desk-based workers: A cluster-randomized controlled trial. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2016, 13, E155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Watling, C.N.; Smith, S.S.; Horswill, M.S. Stop and revive? The effectiveness of nap and active rest breaks for reducing drivers sleepiness. J. Sleep Res. 2014, 23, 261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Sun, Y.; Lim, J.; Dai, Z.; Wong, K.; Taya, F.; Chen, Y.; Li, J.; Thakor, N.; Bezerianos, A. The effects of a mid-task break on the brain connectome in healthy participants: A resting-state functional MRI study. NeuroImage 2017, 152, 19–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Scholz, A.; Ghadiri, A.; Singh, U.; Wendsche, J.; Peters, T.; Schneider, S. Functional work breaks in a high-demanding work environment: An experimental field study. Ergonomics 2018, 61, 255–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Brown, D.K.; Barton, J.L.; Pretty, J.; Gladwell, V.F. Walks4Work: Assessing the role of the natural environment in a workplace physical activity intervention. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2014, 40, 390–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Wendsche, J.; Ghadiri, A.; Bengsch, A.; Wegge, J. Antecedents and outcomes of nurses’ rest break organization: A scoping review. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 2017, 75, 65–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Arrabito, G.; Ho, G.; Aghaei, B.; Burns, C.; Hou, M. Effects of vibrotactile stimulation for sustaining performance in a vigilance task. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 2011, 55, 1160–1164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Arrabito, G.R.; Ho, G.; Aghaei, B.; Burns, C.; Hou, M. Sustained attention in auditory and visual monitoring tasks: Evaluation of the administration of a rest break or exogenous vibrotactile signals. Hum. Factors 2015, 57, 1403–1416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Balci, R.; Aghazadeh, F. A Strategy to design a work-rest schedule for VDT operators performing data entry and mental tasks. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 2000, 44, 6–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Balci, R.; Aghazadeh, F. The effect of work-rest schedules and type of task on the discomfort and performance of VDT users. Ergonomics 2003, 46, 455–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Balci, R.; Aghazadeh, F. Effects of exercise breaks on performance, muscular load, and perceived discomfort in data entry and cognitive tasks. Comput. Ind. Eng. 2004, 46, 399–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Beynon, C.; Reilly, T. Spinal shrinkage during a seated break and standing break during simulated nursing tasks. Appl. Ergon. 2001, 32, 617–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Beynon, C.; Burke, J.; Doran, D.; Nevill, A. Effects of activity-rest schedules on physiological strain and spinal load in hospital-based porters. Ergonomics 2000, 43, 1763–1770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Boucsein, W.; Thum, M. Recovery from strain under different work/rest schedules. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 1995, 39, 785–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Byström, S.E.G.; Mathiassen, S.E.; Fransson-Hall, C. Physiological effects of micropauses in isometric handgrip exercise. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 1991, 63, 405–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Chaikumarn, M.; Nakphet, N.; Janwantanakul, P. Impact of rest-break interventions on the neck and shoulder posture of symptomatic VDU operators during prolonged computer work. Int. J. Occup. Saf. Ergon. 2018, 24, 251–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Chakrabarty, S.; Sarkar, K.; Dev, S.; Das, T.; Mitra, K.; Sahu, S.; Gangopadhyay, S. Impact of rest breaks on musculoskeletal discomfort of Chikan embroiderers of West Bengal, India: A follow up field study. J. Occup. Health 2016, 58, 365–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Chavaillaz, A.; Schwaninger, A.; Michel, S.; Sauer, J. Work design for airport security officers: Effects of rest break schedules and adaptable automation. Appl. Ergon. 2019, 79, 66–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Christensen, H.; Sogaard, K.; Pilegaard, M.; Engineer, H.B.O. The importance of the work/rest pattern as a risk factor in repetitive monotonous work. Int. J. Indl. Ergon. 2000, 25, 367–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Coburn, M. Influence of a 30-min break on divided attention and working memory in resident anaesthetists on daily routine. Br. J. Anaesth. 2006, 97, 315–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Constable, S.H.; Bishop, P.A.; Nunneley, S.A.; Chen, T. Intermittent microclimate cooling during rest increases work capacity and reduces heat stress. Ergonomics 1994, 37, 277–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Cordoza, M.; Ulrich, R.S.; Manulik, B.J.; Gardiner, S.K.; Fitzpatrick, P.S.; Hazen, T.M.; Mirka, A.; Perkins, R.S. Impact of nurses taking daily work breaks in a hospital garden on burnout. Am. J. Crit. Care 2018, 27, 508–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Crenshaw, A.G.; Djupsjobacka, M.; Svedmark, A. Oxygenation, EMG and position sense during computer mouse work. Impact of active versus passive pauses. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2006, 97, 59–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Dababneh, A.J.; Swanson, N.; Shell, R.L. Impact of added rest breaks on the productivity and well being of workers. Ergonomics 2001, 44, 164–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Davis, K.G.; Kotowski, S.E. Postural variability: An effective way to reduce musculoskeletal discomfort in office work. Hum. Factors 2014, 56, 1249–1261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. De Looze, M.P.; Bosch, T.; van Rhijn, J.W. Increasing short-term output in assembly work. Hum. Factors Ergon. Manuf. Serv. Ind. 2010, 20, 470–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Dorion, D.; Darveau, S. Do micropauses prevent surgeon’s fatigue and loss of accuracy associated with prolonged surgery? An experimental prospective study. Ann. Surg. 2013, 257, 256–259. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  74. Engelmann, C.; Schneider, M.; Kirschbaum, C.; Grote, G.; Dingemann, J.; Schoof, S.; Ure, B.M. Effects of intraoperative breaks on mental and somatic operator fatigue: A randomized clinical trial. Surg. Endosc. 2011, 25, 1245–1250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Faucett, J.; Meyers, J.; Miles, J.; Janowitz, I.; Fathallah, F. Rest break interventions in stoop labor tasks. Appl. Ergon. 2007, 38, 219–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Frey, R.; Decker, K.; Reinfried, L.; Klosch, G.; Saletu, B.; Anderer, P.; Semlitsch, H.V.; Seidler, D.; Laggner, A.N. Effect of rest on physicians’ performance in an emergency department, objectified by electroencephalographic analyses and psychometric tests. Crit. Care Med. 2002, 30, 2322–2329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Galinsky, T.L.; Swanson, N.G.; Sauter, S.L.; Hurrell, J.J.; Schleifer, L.M. A field study of supplementary rest breaks for data-entry operators. Ergonomics 2000, 43, 622–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Galinsky, T.L.; Swanson, N.G.; Sauter, S.L.; Dunkin, R.J.; Hurrell, J.; Schleifer, L.M. Supplementary breaks and stretching exercises for data entry operators: A follow-up field study. Am. J. Ind. Med. 2007, 50, 519–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Garcia, M.-G.; Läubli, T.; Martin, B.J. Muscular and vascular issues induced by prolonged standing with different work-rest cycles with active or passive breaks. Hum. Factors 2018, 60, 806–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Genaidy, A.M.; al-Rayes, S. A psychophysical approach to determine the frequency and duration of work-rest schedules for manual handling operations. Ergonomics 1993, 36, 509–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Genaidy, A.M.; Delgado, E.; Bustos, T. Active microbreak effects on musculoskeletal comfort ratings in meatpacking plants. Ergonomics 1995, 38, 326–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Hayashi, M.; Chikazawa, Y.; Hori, T. Short nap versus short rest: Recuperative effects during VDT work. Ergonomics 2004, 47, 1549–1560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Helander, M.G.; Quance, L.A. Effect of work-rest schedules on spinal shrinkage in the sedentary worker. Appl. Ergon. 1990, 21, 279–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Henning, R.A.; Sauter, S.L.; Salvendy, G.; Krieg, E.F. Microbreak length, performance, and stress in a data entry task. Ergonomics 1989, 32, 855–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Henning, R.A.; Kissel, G.V.; Maynard, D.C. A compensatory rest break system for VDT operators. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 1993, 37, 905–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Henning, R.A.; Ortega, A.M.; Callaghan, E.A.; Kissel, G.V. Self management of rest breaks by VDT users. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 1994, 38, 754–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Henning, R.A.; Kissel, G.V.; Maynard, D.C. Compensatory rest breaks for VDT operators. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 1994, 14, 243–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Henning, R.A.; Callaghan, E.A.; Guttman, J.I.; Braun, H.A. Evaluation of two self-managed rest break systems for VDT users. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 1995, 39, 780–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Henning, R.A.; Callaghan, E.A.; Ortega, A.M.; Kissel, G.V.; Guttman, J.I.; Braun, H.A. Continuous feedback to promote self-management of rest breaks during computer use. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 1996, 18, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Henning, R.A.; Bopp, M.I.; Tucker, K.M.; Knoph, R.D.; Ahlgren, J. Team-managed rest breaks during computer-supported cooperative work. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 1997, 20, 19–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Hirose, A.; Nagasaka, A. The effects of the level of alertness during the rest period on subsequent performance. Int. J. Occup. Saf. Ergon. 2003, 9, 479–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Hofer-Tinguely, G.; Achermann, P.; Landolt, H.-P.; Regel, S.J.; Retey, J.V.; Durr, R.; Borbely, A.A.; Gottselig, J.M. Sleep inertia: Performance changes after sleep, rest and active waking. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2005, 22, 323–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Howard, M.E.; Radford, L.; Jackson, M.L.; Swann, P.; Kennedy, G.A. The effects of a 30-minute napping opportunity during an actual night shift on performance and sleepiness in shift workers. Biol. Rhythm Res. 2010, 41, 137–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Irmak, A.; Bumin, G.; Irmak, R. The effects of exercise reminder software program on office workers’ perceived pain level, work performance and quality of life. Work 2012, 41, 5692–5695. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  95. Januario, L.B.; Madeleine, P.; Cid, M.M.; Samani, A.; Oliveira, A.B. Can exposure variation be promoted in the shoulder girdle muscles by modifying work pace and inserting pauses during simulated assembly work? Appl. Ergon. 2018, 66, 151–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Kakarot, N.; Mueller, F.; Bassarak, C. Activity-rest schedules in physically demanding work and the variation of responses with age. Ergonomics 2012, 55, 282–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Karwowski, W.; Eberts, R.; Salvendy, G.; Noland, S. The effects of computer interface design on human postural dynamics. Ergonomics 1994, 37, 703–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Kennedy, G.A.; Ball, H. Power break: A brief hypnorelaxation program to reduce work-related fatigue and improve work satisfaction, productivity, and well-being. Aust. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2007, 35, 169–193. [Google Scholar]
  99. Kietrys, D.M.; Galper, J.S.; Verno, V. Effects of at-work exercises on computer operators. Work 2007, 28, 67–75. [Google Scholar]
  100. Kopardekar, P.; Mital, A. The effect of different work-rest schedules on fatigue and performance of a simulated directory assistance operator’s task. Ergonomics 1994, 37, 1697–1707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Krajewski, J.; Sauerland, M.; Wieland, R. Relaxation-induced cortisol changes within lunch breaks—An experimental longitudinal worksite field study. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2011, 84, 382–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Lacaze, D.H.D.C.; Sacco, I.D.C.N.; Rocha, L.E.; Pereira, C.A.D.B.; Casarotto, R.A. Stretching and joint mobilization exercises reduce call-center operators’ musculoskeletal discomfort and fatigue. Clinics 2010, 65, 657–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Larsen, M.K.; Samani, A.; Madeleine, P.; Olsen, H.B.; Søgaard, K.; Holtermann, A. Short-term effects of implemented high intensity shoulder elevation during computer work. BMC Musculoskelet. Disord. 2009, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Lim, J.; Kwok, K. The Effects of varying break length on attention and time on task. Hum. Factors 2016, 58, 472–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Lim, J.; Quevenco, F.-C.; Kwok, K. EEG alpha activity is associated with individual differences in post-break improvement. NeuroImage 2013, 76, 81–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Lim, J.; Teng, J.; Wong, K.F.; Chee, M.W.L. Modulating rest-break length induces differential recruitment of automatic and controlled attentional processes upon task reengagement. Neuroimage 2016, 134, 64–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Mailey, E.L.; Rosenkranz, S.K.; Casey, K.; Swank, A. Comparing the effects of two different break strategies on occupational sedentary behavior in a real world setting: A randomized trial. Prev. Med. Rep. 2016, 4, 423–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  108. Mathiassen, S.E.; Winkel, J. Physiological comparison of three interventions in light assembly work: Reduced work pace, increased break allowance and shortened working days. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 1996, 68, 94–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Mathiassen, S.E.; Hallman, D.M.; Lyskov, E.; Hygge, S. Can cognitive activities during breaks in repetitive manual work accelerate recovery from fatigue? A controlled experiment. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e112090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. McLean, L.; Tingley, M.; Scott, R.N.; Rickards, J. Computer terminal work and the benefit of microbreaks. Appl. Ergon. 2001, 32, 225–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Miller, B.J.; Fathallah, F.A. The effects of a stooped work task on the muscle activity and kinematics of the lower back. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Ann. Meet. 2006, 50, 1284–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Mitra, B.; Cameron, P.A.; Mele, G.; Archer, P. Rest during shift work in the emergency department. Aust. Health Rev. 2008, 32, 246–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Mohamed, M.A.; Radwan, N.L. Optimal work-rest schedules for computer users. Int. J. Physiother. 2017, 4, 71–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Nakphet, N.; Chaikumarn, M.; Janwantanakul, P. Effect of different types of rest-break interventions on neck and shoulder muscle activity, perceived discomfort and productivity in symptomatic VDU operators: A randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Occup. Saf. Ergon. 2014, 20, 339–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Neri, D.F.; Oyung, R.L.; Colletti, L.M.; Mallis, M.M.; Tam, P.Y.; Dinges, D.F. Controlled breaks as a fatigue countermeasure on the flight deck. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2002, 73, 654–664. [Google Scholar]
  116. O’Donnell, E.; Landolt, K.; Hazi, A.; Dragano, N.; Wright, B.J. An experimental study of the job demand-control model with measures of heart rate variability and salivary alpha-amylase: Evidence of increased stress responses to increased break autonomy. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2015, 51, 24–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Oriyama, S.; Miyakoshi, Y.; Kobayashi, T. Effects of Two 15-min naps on the subjective sleepiness, fatigue and heart rate variability of night shift nurses. Ind. Health 2014, 52, 25–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Rahman, I.A.; Mohamad, N.; Rohani, J.M.; Zein, R.M. The impact of work rest scheduling for prolonged standing activity. Ind. Health 2018, 56, 492–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  119. Reyner, L.A.; Horne, J.A. Suppression of sleepiness in drivers: Combination of caffeine with a short nap. Psychophysiology 1997, 34, 721–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Ross, H.; Russell, P.; Helton, W. Effects of breaks and goal switches on the vigilance decrement. Exp. Brain Res. 2014, 232, 1729–1737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Rupp, M.A.; Sweetman, R.; Sosa, A.E.; Smither, J.A.; McConnell, D.S. Searching for affective and cognitive restoration: Examining the restorative effects of casual video game play. Hum. Factors 2017, 59, 1096–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Samani, A.; Holtermann, A.; Sogaard, K.; Madeleine, P. Effects of eccentric exercise on trapezius electromyography during computer work with active and passive pauses. Clin. Biomech. 2009, 24, 619–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Samani, A.; Holtermann, A.; Sogaard, K.; Madeleine, P. Active pauses induce more variable electromyographic pattern of the trapezius muscle activity during computer work. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 2009, 19, e430–e437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Schnieder, S.; Stappert, S.; Takahashi, M.; Fricchione, G.L.; Esch, T.; Krajewski, J. Sustainable reduction of sleepiness through salutogenic self-care procedure in lunch breaks: A pilot study. Evid. Based Complement. Altern. Med. 2013, 2013, 387356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Sheahan, P.J.; Diesbourg, T.L.; Fischer, S.L. The effect of rest break schedule on acute low back pain development in pain and non-pain developers during seated work. Appl. Ergon. 2016, 53, 64–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Sommer, W.; Stürmer, B.; Shmuilovich, O.; Martin-Loeches, M.; Schacht, A. How about lunch? Consequences of the meal context on cognition and emotion. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e70314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Steinborn, M.B.; Huestegge, L. A walk down the lane gives wings to your brain. Restorative benefits of rest breaks on cognition and self-control. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2016, 30, 795–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. St-Onge, N.; Samani, A.; Madeleine, P. Integration of active pauses and pattern of muscular activity during computer work. Ergonomics 2017, 60, 1228–1239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Sundelin, G. Patterns of electromyographic shoulder muscle fatigue during MTM-paced repetitive arm work with and without pauses. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 1993, 64, 485–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Sundelin, G.; Hagberg, M. The effects of different pause types on neck and shoulder EMG activity during VDU work. Ergonomics 1989, 32, 527–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Takahashi, M.; Fukuda, H.; Arito, H. Brief naps during post-lunch rest: Effects on alertness, performance, and autonomic balance. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 1998, 78, 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Takahashi, M.; Nakata, A.; Haratani, T.; Ogawa, Y.; Arito, H. Post-lunch nap as a worksite intervention to promote alertness on the job. Ergonomics 2004, 47, 1003–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Taylor, W.C.; King, K.E.; Shegog, R.; Paxton, R.J.; Evans-Hudnall, G.L.; Rempel, D.M.; Chen, V.; Yancey, A.K. Booster Breaks in the workplace: Participants’ perspectives on health-promoting work breaks. Health Educ. Res. 2013, 28, 414–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  134. Tiwari, P.S.; Gite, L.P. Evaluation of work-rest schedules during operation of a rotary power tiller. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 2006, 36, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Torrente, P.; Kinnunen, U.; Sianoja, M.; de Bloom, J.; Korpela, K.; Tuomisto, M.T.; Lindfors, P. The effects of relaxation exercises and park walks during workplace lunch breaks on physiological recovery. Scan. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 2017, 2, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Van den Heuvel, S.G.; de Looze, M.P.; Hildebrandt, V.H.; Thé, K.H. Effects of software programs stimulating regular breaks and exercises on work-related neck and upper-limb disorders. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2003, 29, 106–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  137. Van Dieen, J.H.; Oude Vrielink, H.H. Evaluation of work-rest schedules with respect to the effects of postural workload in standing work. Ergonomics 1998, 41, 1832–1844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  138. Wang, L.; Pei, Y. The impact of continuous driving time and rest time on commercial drivers’ driving performance and recovery. J. Saf. Res. 2014, 50, 11–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. Wollseiffen, P.; Ghadiri, A.; Scholz, A.; Strüder, H.K.; Herpers, R.; Peters, T.; Schneider, S. Short bouts of intensive exercise during the workday have a positive effect on neuro-cognitive performance: Neuroexercise. Stress Health 2015, 32, 514–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flow chart on study selection.
Figure 1. Flow chart on study selection.
Ijerph 16 03844 g001
Figure 2. Method combinations in different study settings. Abbreviations: SRM = self-reported measures, PER = performance measures, PHY = physiological measures.
Figure 2. Method combinations in different study settings. Abbreviations: SRM = self-reported measures, PER = performance measures, PHY = physiological measures.
Ijerph 16 03844 g002
Figure 3. Self-reported measures and the subcategories psychological measures and non-psychological measures. Note: Number of studies investigating on certain variables by using at least one of psychological or non-psychological measures.
Figure 3. Self-reported measures and the subcategories psychological measures and non-psychological measures. Note: Number of studies investigating on certain variables by using at least one of psychological or non-psychological measures.
Ijerph 16 03844 g003
Figure 4. Performance measures and the subcategories primary-task measures and reference tasks. Note: Number of studies investigating different task categories.
Figure 4. Performance measures and the subcategories primary-task measures and reference tasks. Note: Number of studies investigating different task categories.
Ijerph 16 03844 g004
Figure 5. Physiological measures. Note: Number of studies investigating different physiological functions.
Figure 5. Physiological measures. Note: Number of studies investigating different physiological functions.
Ijerph 16 03844 g005
Table 1. Change in countries of origin in the last three decades.
Table 1. Change in countries of origin in the last three decades.
Continentk1989–19981999–20082009–2019
America3614913
Europe337818
Asia171412
Australia6024
Africa1001
Total93222348
Table 2. Distribution of selected study contents over the last three decades.
Table 2. Distribution of selected study contents over the last three decades.
Study Designk1989–19981999–20082009–2019
Lab study5117727
Field study4251621
Mental work task62151037
Physical work task3171311
Hypothesis-based466832
Theory-based130013

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Scholz, A.; Wendsche, J.; Ghadiri, A.; Singh, U.; Peters, T.; Schneider, S. Methods in Experimental Work Break Research: A Scoping Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3844. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16203844

AMA Style

Scholz A, Wendsche J, Ghadiri A, Singh U, Peters T, Schneider S. Methods in Experimental Work Break Research: A Scoping Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019; 16(20):3844. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16203844

Chicago/Turabian Style

Scholz, André, Johannes Wendsche, Argang Ghadiri, Usha Singh, Theo Peters, and Stefan Schneider. 2019. "Methods in Experimental Work Break Research: A Scoping Review" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16, no. 20: 3844. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16203844

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop