1. Introduction
Sedentary behavior has become a public health problem worldwide. It is defined as a behavior/s where energy expenditure is ≤1.5 metabolic equivalents and is distinguished from time spent sleeping [
1]. Independent of physical activity, sedentary behaviors have been associated with negative health outcomes, including overweight/obesity status, elevated blood pressure and total cholesterol, lower levels of self-esteem, physical fitness and academic achievement [
2]. Studies have shown that sedentary behavior has many sub-domains, such as watching television, playing computer games, browsing the Internet, reading, and engaging in passive transport [
3].
Recently, great attention has been given to exploring the associations between sedentary behaviors and sleep duration [
4]. Sleeping <7 h or >8 h is associated with higher rates of mortality [
5], as well as with cardiovascular and metabolic diseases [
6,
7]. Previous findings have reported that inadequate sleep quality (sleep deprivation) is associated with both short- and long-term consequences [
8]. Specifically, short term consequences of sleep deprivation are increase stress levels, reduced quality of life, psychological distress and performance deficits, yet long-term consequences are higher incidence of cardiovascular, metabolic and mental diseases and all-cause mortality [
8]. Although sedentary behavior consists of many sub-domains, only screen-time exposure (assessed as a proxy of sedentary behavior) has been associated with decreased sleep in many countries among children and adolescents [
9]. In this population specifically, the link between sedentary behaviors and sleep duration shows that (1) the time spent in front of the screen can be replaced with sleep [
10], (2) the use of media can potentially increase alertness, leading to disturbed sleep [
10] and (3) emitted light from the screen may affect sleep by modifying melatonin production [
10]. However, previous evidence in adults has shown no association between sedentary behaviors and sleep duration [
11,
12,
13]. Most recently, Lakerveld et al. [
14] conducted the study among 6037 European adults which showed similar results to the previous one, where no significant associations between sleep duration and total or other domains of sedentary behaviors were found.
To the best of authors’ knowledge, no study has yet explored the associations between different domains of sedentary behaviors and sleep duration in young adults. A recent study showed that young adults spent approximately 64 h/week (9.1 h/day) in sedentary behaviors [
15], yet 18.1% and 17.1% of them were ‘short’ and ‘long’ sleepers [
16]. Also, in a majority of countries, adolescents/young adults ≥19 years of age start university, move out of their parents’ home or start work, which leads to higher amount of psychological distress, extensive electronic media use and disturbed sleep [
17]. Therefore, the population of young adults is at extreme risk engaging in different domains of sedentary behavior and having irregular sleep hygiene.
Thus, the main purpose of the present study was to explore the associations between different domains of sedentary behaviors (screen-time, at the faculty, leisure-time and total) and sleep duration.
4. Discussion
The main purpose of the present study was to examine whether sedentary behavior domains and total sedentary behavior were associated with sleep duration. Our study shows that more time spent in front of the screen, in leisure-time sedentary behavior and in total sedentary behavior is associated with higher likelihood of being a ‘short’ sleeper, yet more time spent in front of the screen and in total sedentary behavior is associated with higher likelihood of being a ‘long’ sleeper.
Our results which show the associations between screen-time and ‘short’ sleep duration are in line with previous studies [
10,
14,
22]. Lakerveld et al. [
14] showed that shorter sleep was significantly associated with increased screen-time in a large sample of European adults (
N = 6037), irrespective of urban region, gender, age, educational level and weight status. As mentioned in the ‘Introduction’ section, the link between screen-time and ‘short’ sleep duration is described by three potential mechanisms as follows: (1) the time spent in front of the screen can be replaced by sleep [
10], (2) the use of media can potentially increase alertness, leading to disturbed sleep [
10] and (3) emitted light from the screen may affect sleep by modifying melatonin production [
10]. Replacing screen-time with sleep may be an effective way to reduce the risk of all-cause mortality [
23]. In adolescents, studies have shown similar associations where screen-time is a significant predictor of shorter sleep and daytime sleepiness [
24]. From the physiological perspective, the hypothalamic pituitary has a role of maintaining both alertness and sleep functioning. Thus, by engaging in screen-time (especially playing computer games), the level of alertness increases and modifies sleep and circadian rhythm [
25].
Our study also shows that screen-time sedentary behaviors are associated with ‘long’ sleep duration, which is not in line with previous studies [
10,
14,
22]. However, although non-significant, Lakerveld et al. [
14] showed positive association between screen-time and ‘long’ sleep duration. The mechanism underlying such association is based on the interconnection between screen-time and sleep disturbances. One previous study showed that more time spent in front of the screen is associated with higher levels of sleep disturbances [
26]. Moreover, Patel et al. [
27] showed that participants reported having restless legs syndrome and those who were snorers were more likely to be categorized as ‘long’ sleepers. Therefore, sleep disturbances may be a potential mediator between screen-time and ‘long’ sleep duration according to our study. However, we did not adjust for sleep disturbances and can only speculate that they might have a significant mediation role between sedentary behavior and sleep duration.
Next, leisure-time sedentary behavior (reading, listening to music) was significantly associated with ‘short’, but not ‘long’ sleep duration. To the best of our knowledge, this was the first study examining the association between leisure-time sedentary behavior and sleep duration in a large sample of university students. We speculate that academic performance, which includes doing homework and studying, was being performed by students—during the evening and before bedtime. To justify, our study was conducted between October and November of 2017 and at this specific period first examinations (first tests) take place at Croatian universities. Also, classes are often scheduled between 8 am and 4 pm and students attend to their academic obligations in their leisure-time. Thus, it is possible that during that period students were more engaged in pursuing their academic obligations, along with their hobbies (playing an instrument) late at night, which could have potentially led to ‘short’ sleep duration.
Finally, our results show that being in the highest quartile of total sedentary behavior is associated with both ‘short’ and ‘long’ sleep duration, after adjusting for numerous covariates. Previous studies have shown no association between total sedentary behaviors and sleep duration [
14,
28]. Specifically, Lakerveld et al. [
14] showed that, although non-significant, total sedentary behavior was positively associated with ‘short’ sleep duration, yet negatively associated with ‘long’ sleep duration. Similar findings were observed in the study by Loprinzi et al. [
28]. However, the same study showed that for every 60 min increase in sedentary behavior, participants were 16.0% more likely to almost feel not rested during the day and were 22.0% more likely to feel overly sleepy. The existing association in our study could be explained by the fact that the study was conducted among a relatively homogenous sample of university students from one city, whereas previous studies had been conducted among adult populations. Also, our classification of ‘short’ (<7 h), ‘normal’ (7–9 h) and ‘long’ (>9 h) sleep duration was not the same as in the aforementioned studies. Finally, a relatively high proportion of our participants reported being ‘short’ sleepers (18.1%) and ‘long’ sleepers (17.1%), which might have led to more stable confident interval parameters.
Our study has a few strong points. Firstly, we conducted a study among a representative and large sample of university students (N = 2100). Secondly, we adjusted for numerous socio-demographic and health-related covariates.
However, our study also has several limitations. Firstly, a cross-sectional design of the study does not let us conclude the causality of the associations, that is, if ‘short’ and ‘long’ sleep duration lead to more time spent in sedentary behavior. Secondly, we used subjective measures to assess sedentary behaviors and sleep duration, which might have caused possible measurement error and bias. Thirdly, we conducted the study among university students, who do not represent young adults in general and included young adults who do not attend faculties and are currently employed, which could also have led to different results. Finally, the study did not take into account measure of physical activity that could interact with the effects on sleeping (a sporty young person could sleep differently from a non-sporty one with the same hour spent in front a screen).