Next Article in Journal
SlimDeblurGAN-Based Motion Deblurring and Marker Detection for Autonomous Drone Landing
Next Article in Special Issue
Badminton Activity Recognition Using Accelerometer Data
Previous Article in Journal
A Tool to Calculate the Level of Occupancy in Indoor and Outdoor Spaces Using BLE and Open Data to Be Published in Real-Time
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Dynamics of the Prefrontal Cortex during Chess-Based Problem-Solving Tasks in Competition-Experienced Chess Players: An fNIR Study

by
Telmo Pereira
1,
Maria António Castro
1,2,
Santos Villafaina
3,*,
António Carvalho Santos
1 and
Juan Pedro Fuentes-García
3
1
Polytechnic Institute of Coimbra, Coimbra Health School, 3046-854 Coimbra, Portugal
2
Centre for Mechanical and Engineering Materials and Processes, University of Coimbra, 3030-788 Coimbra, Portugal
3
Faculty of Sport Science, University of Extremadura. Avda: Universidad S/N, 10003 Cáceres, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sensors 2020, 20(14), 3917; https://doi.org/10.3390/s20143917
Submission received: 7 June 2020 / Revised: 1 July 2020 / Accepted: 12 July 2020 / Published: 14 July 2020
(This article belongs to the Collection Sensor Technology for Sports Science)

Abstract

:
This study aimed to compare the dynamics of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), between adult and adolescent chess players, during chess-based problem-solving tasks of increasing level of difficulty, relying on the identification of changes in oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) and hemoglobin (HHb) through the functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) method. Thirty male federated chess players (mean age: 24.15 ± 12.84 years), divided into adults and adolescents, participated in this cross-sectional study. Participants were asked to solve three chess problems with different difficulties (low, medium, and high) while changes in HbO2 and HHb were measured over the PFC in real-time with an fNIRS system. Results indicated that the left prefrontal cortex (L-PFC) increased its activation with the difficulty of the task in both adolescents and adults. Interestingly, differences in the PFC dynamics but not in the overall performance were found between adults and adolescents. Our findings contributed to a better understanding of the PFC resources mobilized during complex tasks in both adults and adolescents.

1. Introduction

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) has been thoroughly described as the center of cognitive function, being involved in executive functions that include decision-making and problem-solving, also taking part in attention, memory, planning, motor control, and cognitive flexibility [1,2,3]. The PFC, also called the frontal associative cortex and the Magister of the mind [4], is heavily interconnected with other brain regions, receiving quite diverse sensory and cognitive inputs based on which overall coordination of behavior is implemented. Thus, this brain region, particularly the dorsolateral part, is responsible for the temporal organization of behavior, language, and reasoning [5], and the definition and coordination of plans for action [6] entailing its conceptualization and flexibility to the environmental demands [7]. Furthermore, emotional and inhibitory control processes have been associated with the orbitofrontal region of the PFC, while the medial region has implications in motivation and behavioral drive [5].
Several studies have demonstrated the importance of the frontal lobe, and particularly the PFC, in problem-solving tasks [8,9] including playing chess games [10]. Playing Chess is a particular and challenging activity that requires the orchestration of diverse cognitive resources such as memory, attention, and perceptual grouping [11]. It also involves the recognition of complex spatial relationships as determined by the game rules, and thus, the need to simultaneously handle multiple objects under such rule constraints [12]. In addition, motor timing, movement selection, and gait control are also enrolled in the multi-componential processes involved in chess-playing, all facets under PFC control [13]. Other studies have looked into the overall pattern of brain activation as a function of the level of expertise, identifying differences in the cortical resources engaged during chess-playing activities, with the experts manifesting significant activation of areas related to object perception or expertise-related pattern recognition [14,15], as well as recruitment in brain areas involved in knowledge storage and retrieval and memory, whilst the novice players activate predominantly brain areas involved in learning and retrieving of new information [10]. Higher activation of brain regions involved in attention and problem-solving was also demonstrated in expert chess players engaging a chess-based problem-solving task [16], highlighting the existence of significant differences in brain dynamics, and underlying cognitive operations in chess players.
The PFC is also of great interest in adolescence due to its relation to cognitive control and emotion processing [17]. Differences between adult and adolescence PFC have been reported, identifying a reduction in the gray matter between adolescence and adulthood [18]. This indicates that during adolescence, the prefrontal regions are still developing [19]. In this regard, the lateral regions of the PFC are the latest developing areas involved in executive regions [20]. A recent study examined the brain electrical pattern of adolescent chess players during problem-solving tasks [21]. However, this study did not compare the brain processing of adolescent and adult chess players.
Much of the available evidence concerning brain activation during chess playing tasks have been based on fMRI and electrophysiological methods, but to the best of our knowledge, no studies have previously addressed the PFC activation associated with chess playing tasks with a functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) method. This method provides information on hemodynamic changes associated with cortical activation by noninvasively measuring changes in the relative ratios of deoxygenated hemoglobin (HHb) and oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) [22]. Comparatively to other non-invasive neuroimaging methodologies, fNIR is more tolerant to motion artifacts and provides a balance between spatial and temporal resolution, thus being a good method for tasks involving motor components [23,24].
Previous studies have compared the dynamics of the PFC between adults and adolescents using emotional tasks [25,26,27]. However, to the best of our knowledge, these comparisons have not been performed using high cognitive demand tasks such as chess. This would be relevant as it would allow reporting whether the differences in the PFC between adolescents and adults [18] have any impact on the dynamics of the PFC or in the task performance during high demand cognitive tasks. Hence, we sought to compare the dynamics of the PFC activation during three chess-based problem-solving tasks of increasing level of difficulty in both competitive adult and adolescent chess players, relying on the identification of changes in HbO2 and HHb. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first experimental approach of the PFC activation in such particular challenging chess tasks monitored with fNIR, and, therefore, the results could contribute to a better understanding of the PFC resources mobilized during the handling of complex problems associated with chess playing in both adults and adolescents.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Participants

Federated players from official Portuguese chess clubs were invited to participate in a cross-sectional study. The playing level for chess was determined by the ELO rating system, developed by Arpad Elo and introduced by the World Chess Federation (FIDE) as a ranking system [28]. It is a method for calculating the relative skill levels of players in competitor-versus-competitor games [29]. All the participants were classified according to the ranking system of the FIDE. Exclusion criteria included (1) inability to perform the tasks with the computer, (2) diseases that affect the autonomic and central nervous system, (3) not being on medication, and (4) not being classified by the International Chess Federation with ELO. A total of 31 players was selected to participate (30 males; 1 female) and screened for suitability based on clinical history, behavioral profile, and chess practice characterization. The female player was excluded to avoid gender bias. All the remaining volunteers met the research requirements and were included in the study, thus, a total of 30 male chess players (24.15 ± 12.84 years) were enrolled, with more than 4 years of continuous competitive chess playing experience (participation in chess competition on average: 10.79 ± 7.73 years). Half of the study population were adults (age > 19 years) and half were adolescents. The participants all had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. After a detailed description of the objective and research methodology, all participants signed an informed consent. The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. Anonymity and confidentiality of the collected data were assured, and the study was developed for scientific purposes only, free of any financial or economic interests. All procedures were approved by the University Research Ethics committee (approval number: 85/2015).

2.2. Procedure

Data collection was made in an appropriate room, with adequate temperature and humidity, in a dimmed environment where light would not contaminate the collected information, and silent, so the participant’s concentration was not disturbed during the tests. Before the tests, a structured questionnaire was filled with sociodemographic information and details regarding the chess playing history, including years of practice, years of competitive playing, hours of practice per day and days of practice per week, and habit of playing in digital chess platforms and solving problems. The individual ELO score was calculated [28,29]. The participants were questioned about their baseline level of motivation towards the task and the degree of tiredness, providing such information on a 10-point scale.
The participants were then instructed on how to perform the tasks and all the requirements to ensure the quality of the physiological information and the ecological approach of the chess-based problem-solving tasks. The participants were seated in a comfortable chair in front of a computer screen that ran the chess problems and were monitored with a 16-channel fNIRS stand-alone functional brain imaging system (fNIR100A-2, Biopac System Inc., CA, USA), adjusted on the forehead and insulated using a dark light-proof tape. The fNIRS acquisition was performed with a dedicated computer running the COBI Studio program [30]. Real-time monitoring of HbO2 and HHb in the prefrontal cortex were performed while the participant solved each one of the three chess problems randomly presented. After each problem-solving task, the participants were inquired how they perceived the task in terms of complexity, difficulty, and level of engagement stress.

2.3. Chess Problems

Before starting the experimental task, procedures and protocol requirements were explained to the participants. Moreover, all participants underwent a familiarization period with the computer and the equipment required for testing. Participants conducted a total of three chess-based problem-solving tasks. The problem-solving tasks were selected from Total Chess Training CT-ART 3.0 (Convekta, Moscow, Russia) by a FIDE master (ELO rating of 2300 or more). These chess problems consisted of three levels of difficulty intended for chess players with an ELO rating of 1600–2400 raised by Blokh [31], with 1 being the lowest and 10 the highest level of difficulty: low-level (1), medium-level (5), and high-level (10) chess problems. Participants had two and a half minutes to solve each problem. Two moves for each problem were required (see Figure 1).
The Fritz 15 software, using Stockfish 6, 64 BIT, for Windows was used as chess engine [32]. It is one of the strongest chess engines in the world and it is open source (GPL license). In addition, chess engines are a useful tool for chess training, being similar to the tactical responses given by humans [33]. A laptop was employed (Intel Core i7-6500U, (Intel, Santa Clara, USA) 1 TB, 8 GB memory DDR3L-SDRAM, (Dell, Round Rock, USA)). In order to simulate a real chess environment, the Fritz software automatically responded to moves with the best move previously computed by the research group, simulating a real chess environment. The research technician selected the Fritz level according to the ELO level of each player. This methodology was used in previous studies [34,35,36].

2.4. Functional Brain Imaging—fNIRS

The measurement of prefrontal cortex activity was performed with an fNIRS system (fNIR100A-2, Biopac System Inc., CA, USA), as previously stated, which detects changes in HbO2 and HHb (both in μmol/L) resulting from brain activation [22,23,24]. Signal acquisition was performed using a sensor pad containing 4 light sources (LED) and 10 light detectors with a fixed source-detector distance of 2.5 cm and a depth of light penetration of approximately 1.5 cm beneath the scalp, generating an array of 16 measurement sites (voxels or channels) per wavelength [22,23,24]. The sensor array is embedded in a flexible pad and is placed over the forehead during signal acquisition. The light sources emit two infrared light wavelengths (730 nm and 850 nm) for every 16 channels. As the light penetrates the scalp, part of it is absorbed by the hemoglobin and the remaining light reaches the detectors in a banana-shaped path. Thus, concentrations of hemoglobin are calculated by the ratio of light absorbed at different wavelengths, considering that HbO2 and HHb have different absorption coefficients. The sampling rate of the system was 2 Hz and LED current and detector gain was adjusted prior to the acquisition to prevent signal saturation. The fNIRS acquisition was made for each problem-solving task, resulting in one signal file for each problem. The acquisition started with an initial 10-s baseline recording, after which the task was initiated, and was managed with a dedicated laptop running the Cognitive Optical Brain Imaging (COBI) Studio program [23] (Biopac system Inc., CA, USA). Figure 2 represents an example of the mean changes in HbO2 and HHb recorded for one participant during the time course of the three experimental tasks.

2.5. Data Processing

After visual inspection and elimination of low-quality channels and motion artifacts, the raw files were filtered with a 20-order low-pass finite impulse response (FIR) filter (0.02–0.40 Hz) and a cutoff frequency set at 0.1 Hz to remove long-term drift [30], high-frequency noise, and cardiac and respiratory cycle effects [23,24]. After this process, a sliding-window motion artifact rejection algorithm was used to filter out spikes and to improve signal quality [23]. Relative changes in the concentration of HbO2 (ΔHbO2) and HHb (ΔHHb) were calculated based on the modified Beer–Lambert law [23,24,37], with a 10-s baseline recorded at the beginning of each task. Blood oxygenation (Δoxy) was calculated as the difference of ΔHbO2–ΔHHb. Blood volume changes (ΔHbT) were calculated as ΔHbO2 + ΔHHb.
All aspects of data processing were managed with the fNIR software [23,24,30], version 4.5 (Biopac system Inc., USA). The absolute values obtained after data processing were Z-scored and outliers were removed [37]. Data per channel was averaged for each condition and four regions of interest (ROI) for the prefrontal cortex were created, as previously proposed [13], by grouping anatomically congruent channels. The generated ROIs were the left prefrontal cortex (L-PFC), the right prefrontal cortex (R-PFC), the left medial prefrontal cortex (LM-PFC), and the right medial prefrontal cortex (RM-PFC). For studying laterality effects, we further considered the mean left hemisphere prefrontal cortex (LH-PFC) and the mean right hemisphere prefrontal cortex (RH-PFC) by grouping channels accordingly.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

Data was gathered in Excel 2016 (Microsoft Office, Redmond, WA, USA), and then imported to SPSS Statistics version 24 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) for statistical analysis.
Categorical variables were reported as frequencies and percentages, and χ2 or Fisher exact tests were used when appropriate. The Shapiro–Wilks test was used to confirm the normal distribution of all continuous variables, expressed as mean and standard deviation. As stated before, the fNIRS variables were Z-scored. Other continuous variables with a non-normal distribution were log-transformed. Student’s t-test was applied for group comparisons of descriptive variables only. Individual variables were checked for homogeneity of variance via Levene’s test. A repeated-measures ANOVA was used to evaluate modifications of the variables between the three problem-solving tasks, in the whole population, in each, and between groups. Factors included in the ANOVA were task (three increasing levels of difficulty) and ROI (four prefrontal cortex locations, as described previously). Group was also entered (two levels: adults and adolescents) to test for interactions. The Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used when sphericity was violated, and the Bonferroni adjustment was adopted for multiple comparisons designed to locate the significant effects of a factor. A correlational analysis, using Pearson correlation coefficient, was performed to test for age or years of practice effects on the fNIRS parameters. Significant correlations would be included as covariates in an ANCOVA analysis for the functional biomarkers. A two-tailed p < 0.05 was considered significant. The magnitude of the effects was also checked with the ηp2 value.

3. Results

The main characterization of the study population is summarized in Table 1. The 30 male chess players enrolled in the study and had a mean age of 24.15 ± 12.84 years, ranging from 13 to 55 years. All participants were clinically healthy, with three participants reporting the use of medication (mainly anti-histaminic drugs). Mean chess practice was 14.00 ± 10.30 years (range: 6–43 years) and mean chess competition participation time was 10.92 ± 7.85 years (range: 4–40 years). The mean ELO was 1677 ± 332, and all participants referred to regular chess practicing habits as depicted in Table 1. The majority of the participants attributed the beginning of their chess playing either to school activities (46.7%) or family influence (36.6%). The population was divided according to age into a group of adults (age above 19 years) and adolescents (age between 13 and 19 years). As expected, the adults had a significantly longer chess playing background, although no significant differences were observed concerning chess practicing habits. Similar patterns of playing with digital chess platforms and problem-solving training routines were observed in adults and adolescents.
Regarding the initial levels of motivation towards the tasks of the study, a mean score of 7.67 ± 2.26 was obtained for the entire population, with similar results in the adults (mean score: 7.80 ± 2.54) and the adolescents (mean score: 7.53 ± 2.03; p = 0.753). The degree of initial tiredness showed a similar trend, with a mean initial score of 2.30 ± 2.07 in the whole population, and no differences comparing adults and adolescents (p = 0.467).
The majority of the players (n = 22; 73.3%) were able to solve the low difficulty problem, independently of their age (adults: n = 12; adolescents: n = 10; p = 0.409). Regarding the medium difficulty problem, half of the participants were able to solve them, with no significant differences between the adults and the adolescents (p = 0.715). None of the participants were able to find the solution for the high difficulty problem.
Table 2 summarizes the main findings for the functional biomarkers in the whole study population. An overall increase in PFC oxygenation with task difficulty was observed, with a mean Δoxy of 0.78 ± 0.21 μmol/L in the low difficulty task, increasing to 0.91 ± 0.26 μmol/L in the medium difficulty task and 0.98 ± 0.25 μmol/L in the high difficulty task (F = 8.782; p < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.232). Considering the four defined ROIs, significant changes in all four biomarkers were observed only over the L-PFC, showing an increase in ∆HbO2, Δoxy, and ΔHbT with increasing task complexity. These changes were followed by a significant decrease in ∆HHb, indicating a higher activation in the L-PFC as a function of the difficulty of the problem-solving tasks. The aforementioned results explain the significant changes observed when comparing hemispheric contributions according to the task difficulty, with significant changes observed for the left hemisphere region of the PFC (L-PFC and LM-PFC; Δoxy F = 10.896; p < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.273) but not for the right hemisphere region (R-PFC and RM-PFC; Δoxy F = 1.637; p = 0.203; ηp2 = 0.053).
Considering the main findings for the functional biomarkers in the adolescents and the adults, different patterns of oxygenation were depicted as demonstrated in Figure 3. A significant Task difficulty*ROI*Group interaction was observed regarding the ∆HbO2 (Finteraction = 2.580; p = 0.020; ηp2 = 0.084) and the ∆oxy (Finteraction = 2.345; p = 0.034; ηp2 = 0.077), but not for the ∆HHb and the ∆HbT. In both groups, significant changes were observed in the L-PFC, with an increase in ∆HbO2, Δoxy, and ΔHbT and a decrease in ∆HHb with increasing task complexity. A significant effect was also observed for the ∆HbO2 in the medium level chess problem, with the adolescents presenting significant greater relative change over the R-PFC when compared with the adults (F = 4.808; p = 0.004; ηp2 = 0.147). No significant changes were observed in the low-level chess problem, but differences emerged with increasing complexity of the task, mainly located in the R-PFC and the dorsolateral regions (both LM- and RM-PFC) for the medium level chess problem, and in the L-PFC for the high-level chess problem. In the medium level chess problem, the adolescents presented higher relative changes in Δoxy over the R-PFC and smaller relative changes in Δoxy over the dorsolateral regions of the PFC. In the higher complexity task, the L-PFC responded more in the adults, leading to greater relative change in the Δoxy as compared with the adolescents. No significant differences were observed between groups in what concerns the overall PFC oxygenation levels in the three experimental conditions. To test for age or years of practice effects, we performed a correlational analysis of the fNIRS parameters and these two covariates. A significant (weak to moderate) correlation was found only between age and the LM-PFC changes in HbO2 (r = 0.371; p = 0.044), HHb (r = −0.378; p = 0.039), and Oxy (r = 0.449; p = 0.013). Consequently, we performed an ANCOVA analysis for the functional biomarkers, with age as covariate, and no substantive changes were observed in the factorial results regarding the ∆HbO2 (Finteraction = 2.286; p = 0.038; ηp2 = 0.078) and the ∆oxy (Finteraction = 3.421; p = 0.003; ηp2 = 0.112), although a significant task difficulty*ROI*Group interaction was depicted for the ∆HHb (Finteraction = 2.590; p = 0.020; ηp2 = 0.088) when age was included as covariate in the model.

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to evaluate and compare the dynamics of the PFC activation during three chess-based problem-solving tasks of increasing level of difficulty in competitive adult and adolescent chess players. Several main findings emerged. First, the activation of the PFC, measured by fNIR spectroscopy, was increased with more demanding chess problems in both adolescent and adult chess players. Second, differences in the patterns of PFC activation were found between adult and adolescent chess players. Adult chess players showed greater relative changes in blood oxygenation over the dorsolateral regions of the PFC and greater activation of the L-PFC during the high difficulty problem, whereas the adolescent chess players showed a greater relative change in the ∆oxy in the R-PFC in the medium level task. However, an alternative explanation could emerge. In this regard, since the performance is the same in the two groups, it could be that younger participants exhibited lower mental effort in the high level condition. Moreover, adults could be more engaged in the high level problem-solving task than young chess players (less motivated than adult chess players). In this line, a lower PFC activation when an individual drops the task has been found in fNIRs studies [38,39].
Considering the factorial profiles of changes in oxyhemoglobin and deoxyhemoglobin, a more efficient management of cognitive resources is apparent in adult chess players, regardless of the overall level of PFC activation. Interestingly, these differences in PFC activation had no consequences in the overall performance. These results have a significant relevance in the field of chess since fNIR spectroscopy could be used to determine the cognitive load of a task as well as to test if a training could change the efficiency of the PFC. In addition, future studies should use this technology in older adults or special populations to evaluate the effects of therapies in the PFC activation.
In order to study the PFC activation, we employed fNIR spectroscopy to measure the relative changes in HbO2 and HHb during the chess tasks. Changes in HbO2 have been considered as the most sensitive parameter to measure activity-dependent changes in regional cerebral blood flow [40,41,42] and is also particularly sensitive to mental workload variations [37,42]. Our main findings are consistent with the involvement of the PFC during the experimental tasks, increasing its activation following the increase in the complexity of the experimental tasks. Furthermore, the defined ROIs in the PFC showed different contributions as a function of the difficulty of the chess problems, particularly in the L-PFC in which a progressively higher activation was identified through the four considered biomarkers with increasing level of difficulty. The increased activation of the PFC for increasingly more demanding chess problems was observed both in adults and adolescents, although the level of activation in the considered ROIs was different particularly in the medium and high difficulty level. The adult chess players showed greater relative changes in blood oxygenation over the dorsolateral regions of the PFC and greater activation of the L-PFC during the high difficulty problem as compared with the adolescents, which showed a greater relative change in the ∆oxy in the R-PFC in the medium level task. Notwithstanding, these between-group differences in the PFC activation had no consequence in the overall performance during the tasks, since no differences were observed between the groups when comparing the level of success solving either experimental scenario.
The enrollment of the PFC in complex tasks has been widely demonstrated [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,9] and was also observed in our study. Furthermore, the dorsolateral region of the PFC has been shown to play a crucial role in the overall coordination of several cognitive resources which are necessary during problem-solving tasks, such as the temporal organization of behavior, language, and reasoning [5], the definition and coordination of plans for action [6], and the flexibility to the environmental demands [7]. This PFC region was mostly active in the chess players during the experimental tasks, and particularly the L-PFC was quite sensitive to changes in the cognitive load as expressed by the difficulty of the problem, with increasing activation following the increase in the complexity of the tasks.
The differences in the patterns of PFC activation in adult and adolescent players could express the interaction of both brain maturation and level of expertise or experience. In fact, previous research identified the recruitment of different psychological functions and the activation of different brain areas or different magnitudes of activation of the same brain areas during chess-related activities in expertise versus novice players [10,14,15,16]. This is in line with our findings which highlight the existence of significant differences in the PFC dynamics during chess-based problem-solving tasks comparing the adult with adolescent chess players. This could confirm that the differences in the PFC detected by previous studies [18] also affect the functioning of the PFC. Curiously, the different patterns of PFC activation were not followed by differences in terms of the overall performance during the task, and, therefore, the differences in brain dynamics over the PFC could merely translate into greater underlying efficiency in the adult players. This could be connected with the chunking theory and its augmented theory, the template theory [43]. Chase and Simon [44] proposed that Masters access information in long-term memory rapidly by recognizing familiar constellations of pieces on the board, the patterns acting as cues that trigger access to the chunks. This could be the reason why cognitive processes are more efficient in adults than in adolescent chess players. However, future studies should investigate this hypothesis.
This study has several limitations that should be considered. The small number of participants is a significant aspect, particularly on the between-group comparison, even though the results were consistent in the most relevant outcomes considered in the study. We did not analyze the temporal changes in PFC oxygenation during the tasks, and, therefore, the time course of hemodynamic changes during the problem-solving tasks was not considered for the analysis. Considering that the fNIRS system used in this research does not integrate short channels-based technology, we cannot exclude the possibility of picking extra-cerebral oxygenation signals. Such limitation could have been obviated through the use of task replications, and, therefore, further studies should include replications in the experimental design. The difference in the ELO score and overall chess experience between groups could also explain some of the observed differences at the group level and should be addressed in future research. Due to the heterogeneity of our sample, future studies should explore the PFC activation during problem-solving tasks in different levels of expertise (novice vs. expert paradigm) in age-matched groups.

5. Conclusions

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study addressing the dynamics of the PFC activation during chess-based problem-solving tasks with fNIR spectroscopy. The PFC dynamics differ in adults and adolescents, corresponding to a more efficient cortical organization in the adult players for the same overall level of performance. Furthermore, we demonstrated the participation of the PFC during complex chess problem tasks, with the L-PFC responding with increasing activation to the increasing level of difficulty of the tasks and corresponding cognitive load in both adults and adolescents.
Our findings contributed to a better understanding of the PFC resources mobilized during the handling of complex problems associated with chess-playing, also adding evidence to the understanding of the neural substrate underlying overall human problem-solving mechanisms. Moreover, revealed differences in the PFC functioning between adults and adolescents during high cognitive demand tasks should be investigated in future studies. Further studies should also include the continuous measurement of fNIRS-based biomarkers to allow for the examination of spontaneous hemodynamic fluctuations (as in Verdière et al.’s [45] study), as well as the adoption of more sophisticated NIRS technologies, such as high-definition near-infrared spectroscopy or diffuse optical tomography. Studying the functional correlates of unsuccessful moves during chess playing could also add novel insights, and connectivity metrics could also support the study of hemispheric interplay during complex chess problem tasks, contributing to a better understanding of cortical dynamics in chess playing.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.P. and S.V.; data curation, T.P., M.A.C., S.V., and A.C.S.; formal analysis, T.P.; funding acquisition, J.P.F.-G.; investigation, T.P., M.A.C., and S.V.; methodology, M.A.C. and S.V.; project administration, J.P.F.-G.; resources, A.C.S. and J.P.F.-G.; software, T.P. and A.C.S.; validation, M.A.C. and J.P.F.-G.; visualization, A.C.S. and J.P.F.-G.; writing—original draft, T.P.; writing—review & editing, M.A.C., S.V., A.C.S., and J.P.F.-G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study has been conducted thanks to the contribution of the Ministry of Economy and Infrastructure of the Junta de Extremadura through the European Regional Development Fund. A way to make Europe. (GR18129). Furthermore, S.V. was supported by a grant from the regional department of economy and infrastructure of the Government of Extremadura and the European Social Fund (PD16008) and by a research mobility grant of the AUIP—Asociación Universitaria Iberoamericana de Postgrado. The funders played no role in the study design, the data collection and analysis, the decision to publish, or the preparation of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Chayer, C.; Freedman, M. Frontal lobe functions. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. 2001, 1, 547–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Goel, V.; Grafman, J. Are the frontal lobes implicated in “planning” functions? Interpreting data from the Tower of Hanoi. Neuropsychologia 1995, 33, 623–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Koenraadt, K.L.M.; Roelofsen, E.G.J.; Duysens, J.; Keijsers, N.L.W. Cortical control of normal gait and precision stepping: An fNIRS study. NeuroImage 2014, 85, 415–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Goldberg, E. The Executive Brain: Frontal Lobes and the Civilized Mind; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  5. Fuster, J.n.M. The prefrontal cortex—An update: Time is of the essence. Neuron 2001, 30, 319–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Robbins, T.W. Dissociating executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos. T. R. Soc. B. 1996, 351, 1463–1471. [Google Scholar]
  7. Damasio, A.R. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos. T. R. Soc. B. 1996, 351, 1413–1420. [Google Scholar]
  8. Newman, S.D.; Carpenter, P.A.; Varma, S.; Just, M.A. Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia 2003, 41, 1668–1682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Delis, D.C.; Squire, L.R.; Bihrle, A.; Massman, P. Componential analysis of problem-solving ability: Performance of patients with frontal lobe damage and amnesic patients on a new sorting test. Neuropsychologia 1992, 30, 683–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Amidzic, O.; Riehle, H.J.; Fehr, T.; Wienbruch, C.; Elbert, T. Pattern of focal γ-bursts in chess players. Nature 2001, 412, 603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Bilalić, M.; McLeod, P.; Gobet, F. Specialization effect and its influence on memory and problem solving in expert chess players. Cogn. Sci. 2009, 33, 1117–1143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Gobet, F.; Waters, A.J. The role of constraints in expert memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. 2003, 29, 1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Liang, L.-Y.; Shewokis, P.A.; Getchell, N. Brain activation in the prefrontal cortex during motor and cognitive tasks in adults. J. Behav. Brain. Sci. 2016, 6, 463–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Bilalić, M.; Langner, R.; Erb, M.; Grodd, W. Mechanisms and neural basis of object and pattern recognition: A study with chess experts. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2010, 139, 728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Bilalić, M.; Langner, R.; Ulrich, R.; Grodd, W. Many faces of expertise: Fusiform face area in chess experts and novices. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 10206–10214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Duan, X.; Liao, W.; Liang, D.; Qiu, L.; Gao, Q.; Liu, C.; Gong, Q.; Chen, H. Large-scale brain networks in board game experts: Insights from a domain-related task and task-free resting state. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e32532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Caballero, A.; Granberg, R.; Tseng, K.Y. Mechanisms contributing to prefrontal cortex maturation during adolescence. Neurosci. Biobehav. R. 2016, 70, 4–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Sowell, E.R.; Thompson, P.M.; Holmes, C.J.; Jernigan, T.L.; Toga, A.W. In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nat. Neurosci. 1999, 2, 859–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Kanwal, J.; Jung, Y.; Zhang, M. Brain plasticity during adolescence: Effects of stress, sleep, sex and sounds on decision making. Anat. Physiol. Curr. Res. 2016, 6, e135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Fuster, J.M. Frontal lobe and cognitive development. J. Neurocytol. 2002, 31, 373–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fuentes-Garcia, J.P.; Pereira, T.; Castro, M.A.; Carvalho Santos, A.; Villafaina, S. Psychophysiological stress response of adolescent chess players during problem-solving tasks. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 209, 112609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ayaz, H.; Shewokis, P.A.; Curtin, A.; Izzetoglu, M.; Izzetoglu, K.; Onaral, B. Using MazeSuite and functional near infrared spectroscopy to study learning in spatial navigation. J. Vis. Exp. 2011, 56, 3443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Ayaz, H.; Izzetoglu, M.; Shewokis, P.A.; Onaral, B. Sliding-window motion artifact rejection for functional near-infrared spectroscopy. In Proceedings of the 2010 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology, Buenos Aires, Argentina, 31 August–4 September 2010; IEEE: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  24. Izzetoglu, M.; Chitrapu, P.; Bunce, S.; Onaral, B. Motion artifact cancellation in NIR spectroscopy using discrete Kalman filtering. Biomed. Eng. Online 2010, 9, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Mueller, S.C.; Cromheeke, S.; Siugzdaite, R.; Nicolas Boehler, C. Evidence for the triadic model of adolescent brain development: Cognitive load and task-relevance of emotion differentially affect adolescents and adults. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 26, 91–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Silvers, J.A.; Shu, J.; Hubbard, A.D.; Weber, J.; Ochsner, K.N. Concurrent and lasting effects of emotion regulation on amygdala response in adolescence and young adulthood. Dev. Sci. 2015, 18, 771–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Hare, T.A.; Tottenham, N.; Galvan, A.; Voss, H.U.; Glover, G.H.; Casey, B.J. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 63, 927–934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Elo, A.E. The Rating of Chessplayers, Past and Present; Ishi Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  29. Di Fatta, G.; Haworth, G.M.; Regan, K.W.; IEEE. Skill Rating by Bayesian Inference. In Proceedings of the 2009 IEEE Symposium on Computational Intelligence and Data Mining, Nashville, TN, USA, 30 March–2 April 2009; IEEE: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  30. Ayaz, H.; Onaral, B. Analytical Software and Stimulus-Presentation Platform to Utilize, Visualize and Analyze Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Measures. Master Thesis, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  31. Blokh, M. Combinational Motifs, 1st ed.; Hannaco Enterprises: Moscow, Russia, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  32. Regan, K.W.; Biswas, T.; Zhou, J. Human and computer preferences at chess. In Proceedings of the Workshops at the Twenty-Eighth AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Québec City, QC, Canada, 27–31 July 2014; AAAI Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  33. Laureano-Cruces, A.L.; Hernández-González, D.E.; Mora-Torres, M.; Ramírez-Rodríguez, J. Application of a cognitive model of emotional appraisal to the board evaluation function of a program that plays chess. Rev. de Matemática Teoría y Apl. 2012, 19, 211–237. [Google Scholar]
  34. Fuentes, J.P.; Villafaina, S.; Collado-Mateo, D.; de la Vega, R.; Gusi, N.; Clemente-Suarez, V.J. Use of Biotechnological Devices in the Quantification of Psychophysiological Workload of Professional Chess Players. J. Med. Syst. 2018, 42, 40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Fuentes, J.P.; Villafaina, S.; Collado-Mateo, D.; de la Vega, R.; Olivares, P.R.; Clemente-Suárez, V.J. Differences between high vs low performance chess players in heart rate variability during chess problems. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Villafaina, S.; Collado-Mateo, D.; Cano-Plasencia, R.; Gusi, N.; Fuentes, J.P. Electroencephalographic response of chess players in decision-making processes under time pressure. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 198, 140–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Gateau, T.; Durantin, G.; Lancelot, F.; Scannella, S.; Dehais, F. Real-time state estimation in a flight simulator using fNIRS. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0121279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Durantin, G.; Gagnon, J.F.; Tremblay, S.; Dehais, F. Using near infrared spectroscopy and heart rate variability to detect mental overload. Behav. Brain Res. 2014, 259, 16–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Izzetoglu, M.; Bunce, S.C.; Izzetoglu, K.; Onaral, B.; Pourrezaei, K. Functional brain imaging using near-infrared technology. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. 2007, 26, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Maidan, I.; Bernad-Elazari, H.; Gazit, E.; Giladi, N.; Hausdorff, J.M.; Mirelman, A. Changes in oxygenated hemoglobin link freezing of gait to frontal activation in patients with Parkinson disease: An fNIRS study of transient motor-cognitive failures. J. Neurol. 2015, 262, 899–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Miyai, I.; Tanabe, H.C.; Sase, I.; Eda, H.; Oda, I.; Konishi, I.; Tsunazawa, Y.; Suzuki, T.; Yanagida, T.; Kubota, K. Cortical mapping of gait in humans: A near-infrared spectroscopic topography study. Neuroimage 2001, 14, 1186–1192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Hoshi, Y.; Kobayashi, N.; Tamura, M. Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: A study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. J. Appl. Physiol. 2001, 90, 1657–1662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Gobet, F.; Simon, H.A. Expert chess memory: Revisiting the chunking hypothesis. Memory 1998, 6, 225–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Chase, W.G.; Simon, H.A. Perception in chess. Cogn. Psychol. 1973, 4, 55–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Verdière, K.J.; Roy, R.N.; Dehais, F. Detecting pilot’s engagement using fNIRS connectivity features in an automated vs. manual landing scenario. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 6. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Representation of the three chess-based problem-solving tasks by level of complexity: (a) panel—low level problem (L); (b) panel—medium level problem (M); (c) panel—high level problem (H).
Figure 1. Representation of the three chess-based problem-solving tasks by level of complexity: (a) panel—low level problem (L); (b) panel—medium level problem (M); (c) panel—high level problem (H).
Sensors 20 03917 g001
Figure 2. Example of the relative changes in oxygenated hemoglobin and deoxygenated hemoglobin in one participant during the three experimental tasks. Bin 0 marks the baseline and Bin 11 the end of each task. The relative changes were computed as the mean change in the overall optodes. (a) panel—low level problem (L); (b) panel—medium level problem (M); (c) panel—high level problem (H).
Figure 2. Example of the relative changes in oxygenated hemoglobin and deoxygenated hemoglobin in one participant during the three experimental tasks. Bin 0 marks the baseline and Bin 11 the end of each task. The relative changes were computed as the mean change in the overall optodes. (a) panel—low level problem (L); (b) panel—medium level problem (M); (c) panel—high level problem (H).
Sensors 20 03917 g002
Figure 3. Relative changes in oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (HHb) in the adolescents group ((a) panel) and in the adults group ((b) panel), according to the level of difficulty of the chess-based problem-solving tasks. L-PFC—left prefrontal cortex; R-PFC—right prefrontal cortex; LM-PFC—left medial prefrontal cortex; RM-PFC—right medial prefrontal cortex.
Figure 3. Relative changes in oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO2) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (HHb) in the adolescents group ((a) panel) and in the adults group ((b) panel), according to the level of difficulty of the chess-based problem-solving tasks. L-PFC—left prefrontal cortex; R-PFC—right prefrontal cortex; LM-PFC—left medial prefrontal cortex; RM-PFC—right medial prefrontal cortex.
Sensors 20 03917 g003
Table 1. Characterization of the participants according to age, chess practicing habits, and individual ELO.
Table 1. Characterization of the participants according to age, chess practicing habits, and individual ELO.
Total
(n = 30)
Adults
(n = 15)
Adolescents
(n = 15)
Ƥ
Age (years)24.2 ± 12.832.7 ± 13.515.6 ± 1.7<0.001
Chess playing (years)14.0 ± 10.319.6 ± 12.208.4 ± 2.00.003
Chess competition (years)10.9 ± 7.814.7 ± 9.607.2 ± 2.20.010
Chess practicing habits
Days/week2.5 ± 2.22.5 ± 2.32.5 ± 2.20.968
Hours/Day1.7 ± 1.31.8 ± 1.31.6 ± 1.40.689
Hours/week5.2 ± 6.36.1 ± 7.34.4 ± 5.10.470
ELO1677 ± 3321825 ± 2491529 ± 3470.012
Number of problem-solving chess tasks solved
Low difficulty22 (73.3%)12 (80%)10 (66.7%)0.409
Medium difficulty15 (50%)7 (46.7%)8 (53.3%)0.715
High difficulty0 (0%)0 (0%)0 (0%)1.000
Table 2. Prefrontal cortex dynamics according to the variation of the functional biomarkers in the three problem-solving chess tasks.
Table 2. Prefrontal cortex dynamics according to the variation of the functional biomarkers in the three problem-solving chess tasks.
Low DifficultyMedium DifficultyHigh DifficultyFPηp2
∆HbO2
(μmol/L)
L-PFC0.33 ± 0.190.50 ± 0.220.77 ± 0.2871.656<0.0010.712
R-PFC0.36 ± 0.260.50 ± 0.340.41 ± 0.341.5470.2210.051
LM-PFC0.34 ± 0.220.38 ± 0.290.41 ± 0.290.7530.4760.025
RM-PFC0.41 ± 0.270.43 ± 0.340.49 ± 0.370.5630.5720.019
∆HHb
(μmol/L)
L-PFC−0.51 ± 0.35−0.48 ± 0.25−0.69 ± 0.323.9010.0260.119
R-PFC−0.39 ± 0.25−0.39 ± 0.24−0.33 ± 0.241.0490.3570.035
LM-PFC−0.43 ± 0.25−0.48 ± 0.290.34 ± 0.272.4090.0990.077
RM-PFC−0.37 ± 0.33−0.45 ± 0.34−0.38 ± 0.3137230.4900.024
∆HbT
(μmol/L)
L-PFC−0.19 ± 0.420.01 ± 0.260.08 ± 0.355.8650.0050.168
R-PFC−0.3 ± 0.350.11 ± 0.270.09 ± 0391.3250.2740.044
LM-PFC−0.10 ± 0.29−0.10 ± 0.320.07 ± 0.362.7270.0740.086
RM-PFC0.04 ± 0.42−0.02 ± 0.400.11 ± 0.390.9390.3970.031
∆oxy
(μmol/L)
L-PFC0.84 ± 0.380.98 ± 0.391.50 ± 0.4823.777<0.0010.451
R-PFC0.74 ± 0.380.90 ± 0.530.74 ± 0.451.1500.2290.049
LM-PFC0.77 ± 0.390.87 ± 0.490.75 ± 0.430.8980.4130.030
RM-PFC0.78 ± 0.430.87 ± 0.560.87 ± 0.560.4380.6480.015
∆HbO2—variation in oxyhemoglobin; ∆HHb—variation in deoxyhemoglobin; ∆HbT—variation in total hemoglobin; ∆oxy—difference between oxyhemoglobin and deoxyhemoglobin; L-PFC—left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; R-PFC—right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; LM-PFC—left medial prefrontal cortex; RM-PFC—right medial prefrontal cortex.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pereira, T.; Castro, M.A.; Villafaina, S.; Carvalho Santos, A.; Fuentes-García, J.P. Dynamics of the Prefrontal Cortex during Chess-Based Problem-Solving Tasks in Competition-Experienced Chess Players: An fNIR Study. Sensors 2020, 20, 3917. https://doi.org/10.3390/s20143917

AMA Style

Pereira T, Castro MA, Villafaina S, Carvalho Santos A, Fuentes-García JP. Dynamics of the Prefrontal Cortex during Chess-Based Problem-Solving Tasks in Competition-Experienced Chess Players: An fNIR Study. Sensors. 2020; 20(14):3917. https://doi.org/10.3390/s20143917

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pereira, Telmo, Maria António Castro, Santos Villafaina, António Carvalho Santos, and Juan Pedro Fuentes-García. 2020. "Dynamics of the Prefrontal Cortex during Chess-Based Problem-Solving Tasks in Competition-Experienced Chess Players: An fNIR Study" Sensors 20, no. 14: 3917. https://doi.org/10.3390/s20143917

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop