Next Article in Journal
Evaluating Land Surface Temperature Trends and Explanatory Variables in the Miami Metropolitan Area from 2002–2021
Previous Article in Journal
Use of Smartphone Lidar Technology for Low-Cost 3D Building Documentation with iPhone 13 Pro: A Comparative Analysis of Mobile Scanning Applications
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

“How Far Is the Closest Bus Stop?” An Evaluation of Self-Reported versus GIS-Computed Distance to the Bus among Older People and Factors Influencing Their Perception of Distance

1
Tech4Care SRL, Via Primo Maggio, 20, 60131 Ancona, Italy
2
Transport and Roads, Lund University, 22002 Lund, Sweden
3
Lunds Kommun, 22223 Lund, Sweden
4
MobistikUtveckling, 41469 Göteborg, Sweden
5
Department of Biomedical Sciences and Public Health, Section of Hygiene Preventive Medicine, and Public Health, Università Politecnica delle Marche, 60131 Ancona, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Geomatics 2023, 3(4), 580-596; https://doi.org/10.3390/geomatics3040031
Submission received: 15 September 2023 / Revised: 30 November 2023 / Accepted: 9 December 2023 / Published: 13 December 2023

Abstract

:
Previous research showed that living closer to bus stops could be a factor in promoting a healthy and active lifestyle. However, most of the studies relied on self-reported measures of distance, which might be affected by several confounders. In this study, self-reported distances among study participants were compared to actual ones, computed by the use of GIS (Geographic Information System) technology and routing algorithms. We tested whether distance to the bus stop is associated with health and socioeconomic conditions of the respondents, using data among 2398 older people (75–90 years) in three cities in Sweden. We found that several variables including older age, female gender, living alone, and worse health status are associated with an over-estimation of bus stop distance. People who use public transport daily or several times a week and are satisfied with the walking environment in the neighbourhood tend to underestimate bus stop distances. Evidence based on self-reported measures only should be treated cautiously. Considering the limitations still present in open-data-based routing algorithms, the best indication is to combine the subjective with the objective measure of distance. Having the possibility to combine the two measures appears as a sound strategy to overcome the limitations associated with each single measure.

1. Introduction

Mobility has been defined as one person’s ability to move to a desired destination independently and safely [1]. It is a fundamental ability, but also a “personal right” for the individual. Mobility skill is acquired in early infancy and consolidated during childhood and adulthood, whereas it can deteriorate in later age, due to functional and cognitive impairments [2].
Impairment of mobility is not, however, a deterministic consequence of ageing, which applies to all older individuals. Many people in modern societies are today ageing actively and in good health, as a consequence of the overall improvement of living conditions [3]. Nonetheless, the risk of reduced mobility affects significant shares of the older population, especially those living in the most deprived cultural and social contexts [4].
Mobility is a multidimensional and cross-disciplinary concept. It may be seen as a single element mirroring a specific subject’s health status: lower walking speed (most commonly assessed using usual walking speed achieved during a timed walk test over a short distance) and other multisystem performance measures of physical function (such as the time taken to rise from a chair and sit down several times, or tests of balance) are consistently associated with poorer well-being and quality of life in old age, track overall health status, and predict adverse health outcomes, including rising multimorbidity, health care resource utilisation, disability in activities of daily living, nursing home admission, and earlier mortality [2]. At the same time, mobility can be seen as a much more complex concept that goes beyond individual health and physiological characteristics and may refer to mobility capabilities within social and urban contexts. The idea of having the right to mobility has been fundamental to modern Western citizenship and is expressed in many legal and government documents, including the Universal Declaration of Human Rights. Still, the right to travel and move freely has been highly uneven across countries, regions, and groups of people within countries. Thus, mobility as a resource is always achievable in different ways for different groups of people. As a right and a resource, mobility can also be understood as a cultural and social value that is pursued for various reasons. The discussion of mobility in old age is closely linked to issues of physical (in)capability and physical movements from the medical point of view, without giving profound consideration to the social dimension in tactile–kinaesthetic feelings that phenomenologists have seen as vital for meaningful human life [5].
Given the ongoing demographic shift, the importance of mobility should be considered by policy makers and professionals in the broader context of active and healthy ageing frameworks. Evidence strongly supports that the capacity of an ageing individual to maintain an adequate and active mobility is associated with their independence and good health [6]. A Swedish study correlated data on health and distance to the bus network among people over 75 years, and found out that mental health scores are significantly lower among those living far from the closest bus stop and never or seldom using public transport [7]. Mobility is associated with social participation, stronger social networks, and independence of older people [8,9], which in turn are factors leading to lower morbidity and mortality rates and a higher quality of life [10]. In this respect, stronger social networks supported by effective, efficient, and accessible mobility services might ensure that older peoples’ wished mobility is realised [11,12].
There is a common concern, however, that the current characteristics of the public transport sector might not meet the needs of older people. The traditional concept of efficiency within the public transport framework might collide with the special needs of older travelers, who for instance could request short distances by a more spread bus stop network and/or slower travelling paces [13,14]. Therefore, in order to adapt public transport characteristics to the changed mobility needs of older travelers, evidence on their preferences and on factors influencing their use and opinion of public transport is of interest for policy makers and professionals responsible for development of the new strategies of the sector.
In this respect, access to public transport services is the first prerequisite for their actual use [15]. Therefore, investigation of the multiple factors associated with access to and use of public transport services among the older population is of paramount importance to inform policy [13].
In general terms, accessibility can be defined as “the extent to which transport systems enable individuals to reach activities or destinations by means of a combination of transport modes” (Geurs and van Wee 2004). However, as there are different definitions of accessibility, there are also several computation methods which could possibly be employed to take it into account. Essentially, the main factors involved are distance, time, and cost. Isodistance (same-distance) maps represent areas that are equidistant from a specific point, typically visualised using buffers or radiuses around points. Isochrones (same-time), instead, represent areas that can be reached within the same amount of time, rather than distance, and focus on factors such as road networks congestion, traffic conditions, and transportation modes. Lastly, economic distances take into account not only physical distance or travel time but also economic factors, such as the cost of travel. It is interesting to notice how the cost of travel might include elements outside the conventional costs (e.g., tolls, fuel costs, or other expenses associated with travel), such as the effort required for mobility for older, disadvantaged or disabled people.
It is important to notice, moreover, that the personal characteristics of specific groups of people (e.g., elderly, vulnerable, school pupils, etc.), or of the area (walking surface, green spaces, etc.) are playing a growingly important role in the literature, in addition to the conventional elements of accessibility discussed above.
Methodology-wise, the distance to a bus stop from home can be considered a good proxy measure for the level of accessibility to public transport of older people. Despite the relevance of this measure, most of the studies on public transport mobility in older age collected data on this dimension using only self-reported measures. Self-reported measures, however, might be biased by several confounding factors, such as age and health conditions of the respondents.
Accessibility data can be obtained from a variety of sources, starting from government and institutional open data portals, transportation authorities, who often release data related to public transit schedules, routes, and infrastructure, non-profit organisations, and research institutions. In the case of Sweden, a strategy for Spatial Data Infrastructure (SDI) has been developed since 2006; a national web portal (www.geodata.se (accessed on 25 November 2023)) works as a node containing a large number of datasets being stored and maintained by the responsible organisations [16]. The University of Lund also has a portal dedicated to geographical data (https://www.gis.lu.se/gis-centre/geographical-data, last accessed on 25 November 2023).
During the last decade, the emergence of Volunteered Geographic Information (VGI), geographic data provided voluntarily by individuals (Goodchild, 2007), has made it possible to create new datasets and to enrich or complement official sources of data. OpenStreetMap (OSM) is an open-source mapping platform that allows users to contribute and access geographical data, including information related to roads, points of interest (POIs), and more. Operationally, the availability of GIS software, VGI, and optimised routing algorithms have made the computation of walking distance a growingly accessible operation.
User-generated geographic data typically have advantages in terms of free use, timely creation, high detail in local areas, and site-specific crisis management [17]. A large amount of research has focused on VGI quality assessment, classifying methods into two major strands: comparisons with authoritative/official reference datasets and internal quality assessment based on indicators of VGI quality [18]. When health studies are concerned, objectively measured distances to target destinations are a major advantage in complementing self-reported distances.
In order to contribute to the debate in this area, the SEBEM-GIS (Socio-Economic Benefits of Elderly Mobility-GIS) study aimed to investigate whether health and socioeconomic characteristics of older individuals are associated with the perceived distance to the bus stop in urban public mainstream services. SEBEM-GIS tested whether specific subjects’ characteristics might be associated either with overestimated or underestimated distance perception. Answering this question could be very useful for policy decision makers in terms of the perspective of optimizing public transport services, and focusing on possible education and active participation of the general population.

2. Data and Methods

2.1. The SEBEM-GIS Study

This paper draws on data from the SEBEM (Socio-Economic Benefits of Elderly Mobility) project, funded by the Centre for Ageing and Supportive Environment (CASE) at Lund University (Sweden) [7]. SEBEM aimed to investigate several dimensions related to public transport use in old age. Its specific aims included the evaluation of mobility patterns among people above 75 years old and related socioeconomic correlates and the analysis of the costs for mobility services provision among three Swedish municipalities (Göteborg, Lund, and Helsingborg).
A large cross-sectional survey among older citizens (75 to 90 years old) was performed in those cities. The three cities were selected as they represent urban contexts homogeneous in terms of share of older population, but heterogeneous in terms of availability of mobility options. In particular, Göteborg has invested considerably in the last 20 years in the development of Flexlinjen, an advanced flexible (demand-responsive) mobility service for older people [19], which is absent in Lund and Helsingborg. Lund, instead, is characterised by a relevant share of educated older population, as a consequence of its status of “university town”. Göteborg is also the biggest of the cities considered. Even if an in-depth analysis of the morphological characteristics of the cities is out of the scope of this study, the three urban areas are rather flat and similar in terms of elevation (see elevation maps in Supplementary Materials).
The structured questionnaire used in the survey included both instruments already validated in the Swedish context for addressing complex constructs (e.g., health and wellbeing) and study-specific items developed by the study team to retrieve information on specific aspects of social activities (e.g., visiting friends and relatives) and mobility (e.g., use of a private car) [20]. This study obtained ethical clearance by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Lund, Sweden (DNR 2015/30). An extension of the previous ethical clearance was applied for an extension of the original protocol. Ethical clearance was granted by the Regional Ethical Board of Skåne (EPN DNR 2018/368).
A list with names of addresses (and national id-number) of the population aged between 75 and 90 years in the three study locations was retrieved from the municipalities using general population registries. The persons were randomly selected based on a population list (obtained from the municipalities under signing a confidentiality agreement) of all residents aged 75–90 in the respective study area, after all those living in special nursing homes were discarded with the help of staff from the respective municipality. Age and residential location were the only inclusion criteria. Participants were then randomly selected using a routine created using the software Microsoft Excel (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, DC, USA, 2015). Excel was used in the following manner: a random number up to 12 digits was created and assigned to each citizen by the municipalities. The citizen list was then ordered by this random number. The first names of each city list were then selected for participation in this study, according to the planned quotas. A final list of 3600 participants was extracted from the three cities (1800 from Göteborg, 900 from Lund and Helsingborg, respectively) and participants received the survey questionnaires, together with the informed consent, directly at their home addresses.
The first mailing of the postal survey to the subjects was conducted on 6–8 May 2014, with a cover letter inviting them to participate in the current research project. Participants who did not respond within 3–4 weeks received a reminder with a new questionnaire. Out of the 3600 questionnaires sent to the study sample, 2411 were returned. The overall response rate was thus 67%, ranging from 68.3% among those aged 75–80 to 66.8% and 63.7% among those aged 81–85 and 86–90, respectively. Response rates were higher in Lund (73.7%) compared to Helsingborg (65.4%) and Göteborg (64.4%). All data referred to individuals have been stored in a physical memory device inside a secure room in the basement of the Department of Transport Studies of Lund University.

2.2. Computation of Walking Distance Variable and GIS

Originally, distance to the closest bus stop was retrieved directly from the participants in the SEBEM project using a self-reported categorical item (classified in “less than 200 m”, “between 200 and 500 m”, and “more than 500 m”). In order to overcome the limitation by this way of measuring, GIS technology and geocoding and routing algorithms based on OpenStreetMap data (www.openstreetmap.org, last accessed on 25 November 2023) were used to compute walking distances between bus stops and study participants’ home addresses. The three municipalities and the transport authorities of the area (Skånetrafiken) provided data regarding the bus stops available, including latitude-longitude coordinates, and respondents’ home addresses have been geocoded. For each pair of coordinates, the shortest walking distance was computed using the Graphhopper Directions API (https://www.graphhopper.com/, last accessed on 25 November 2023) to perform routings. In all, a total of 2398 individuals answered the SEBEM questionnaire. However, the algorithm could not compute the walking distance for some address pairs, generating 334 missing values. Moreover, the algorithm generated some unrealistically long walking distances, e.g., when pedestrian routes could not be found across gardens or parking lots, or under patios, etc. (See example figure in Supplementary Material S3). Nevertheless, the pattern of missing values was unbiased among the areas of study, reflecting proportionally the total number of respondents in the three cities (48% Göteborg, 27% Lund, and 25% Helsingborg) (cf. Table 1).
To enrich the analysis, and to double-check the accuracy of the estimates, we also computed straight-line distance using the OpenStreetMap base of data and STATA. These Euclidean (straight-line) distances have been compared with Graphhopper walking distances. Since walking paths tend to mimic—whenever possible—shortest straight-line paths, we treated overly high discrepancies between walking and straight-line distances as an indication of bias. Overall, the discrepancy between the two measures of distance was 350 m or more for 67 subjects (3.25% of the total), while 29 respondents have a bias of 500 m or more (1.41%). In order to replace unrealistically high routes, straight-line distances have been used for the subjects with a bias above the 350 m threshold, being aware that this is just a rough approximation (see example map in Supplementary Materials).

2.3. Creation of Dependent Variable

The dependent variable of this study was created by comparing estimated vs. GIS-measured distance. We classified study participants in three categories, according to the fact that they “underestimated” (e.g., live farther than estimated), “correctly estimated”, or “overestimated” (e.g., live actually closer than reported) the distance of the closest bus stop to their home. The criterion adopted to define people who wrongly estimate their distance to bus stops was when they actually lived outside the distance band they estimated they lived in (“less than 200 m”, “between 200 and 500 m”, and “more than 500 m”). Seventy respondents who reported not being able to classify their distance to bus stops were excluded from the regression analysis (resulting in N = 1994).

2.4. Statistical and Spatial Analysis

As the data used came from different sources, a preliminary process of cleaning and harmonisation was performed, together with outliers and distributional inspection. In particular, the data from the health survey were integrated with those containing the distances calculated by the algorithm. In this phase, several distance variables and perception variables were also constructed, and recoded in categorical form. Outcome variables’ distribution was inspected with boxplots highlighting outliers, and with Q-Q plots to assess normality. Outlier points in terms of actual distance were not omitted since their effects were attenuated after recoding to categorical variables.
Then, the relation between perception of distance to bus stops and explicative variables was analysed with bivariate tests. Specifically, all the categorical variables were tested using chi-square tests across the three groups identified as correct/under-/over-estimate (See Table 2 and Table 3). For continuous variables (PCS12 and MCS12), the probabilities are referred to a non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis test.
Regarding variables, we assessed the Physical Composite Score (PCS) and the Mental Component Score (MCS), both ranging from 0 to 100, using the SF12 Health Survey. Other variables collected in order to characterise the study population included gender; place of birth; level of education; and living arrangements.
The questions to assess the engagement of participants in social activities have been retrieved and partially modified from the Omnibus Survey of the British Office for National Statistics (Banister and Bowling, 2004). Study participants have been asked the following question: “How often have you participated in the following social activities during the last four weeks?” The listed social activities were (slightly grouped) the following: “Senior get-togethers“, “Associations/clubs”, “Classes, libraries, and lectures”, “Church/religious activities”, “Cinema, theater, concerts”, “Voluntary work”, “Sports, gym, dance”, “Meet relatives/friends”. The answers were arranged on a 5-grade scale: “Daily”; “Several times a week”; “Once a week”; “Once a month”; “More rarely/never”. A study-specific social score index was created to obtain a single, easy-to-read measure indicating the intensity of engagement of the older person in a range of possible social activities. The score (ranging from 0 to 8) simply counts the number of social activities in which the person is engaged at least weekly. This means that a person meeting weekly his relatives and attending daily the city library was categorised as having a social score equal to 2.
We also assessed the frequency of walking more than 500 m, using the same 5-graded scale. This threshold has been chosen as it corresponds to approximately 10 min of walking activity per day, a meaningful time span for achieving health benefits suggested by the WHO (2010).
Several study-specific questions addressed the domain of mobility and access to transport options. Respondents were asked about their possibility to have access to a private car (response categories were “Yes, always”, “Yes, sometimes”, and “Never, or almost never”); their use of private car and public transport (also using the 5-graded scale used for social activities); and the distance between the nearest bus stop from their home (“Less than 200 m”, “About 200–500 m”, “More than 500 m”, “Do not know”).
Finally, a multinomial logistic regression model was used to investigate factors associated with an underestimated or overestimated perception of distance, controlling for the influence of potential confounders. The exclusion of respondents who could not estimate their distance and the interactions among independent variables caused a decrease in the final number of valid observations of the statistical model (N = 1794). Post-estimation diagnostics were conducted by checking normality of residuals and measuring the information criteria. All analyses were performed using STATA, version 14 (Stata Corp., College Station, TX, USA, 2010).
In order to investigate the spatial structuring of the data, and to validate the statistical analysis, some specific spatial analyses have been carried out (see results in Supplementary Materials). First, the location of respondents in the three towns have been mapped by means of kernel density estimations (KDE) [21], superimposed to point pattern of bus stops. Then, specific tests for spatial autocorrelations have been carried out, namely global Moran’s I [22] and Local Indicators of Spatial Association (LISA, Anselin, 1995).
Spatial autocorrelation measures how close objects are in comparison with other close objects. Moran’s I can be classified as positive or negative; a Moran’s I close to zero indicates the absence of spatial autocorrelation, so a relative spatial independence of geographical units. Global statistics, referring to the entire study area, only indicate if the spatial configuration is overall autocorrelated, but does not allow us to understand which units are autocorrelated to the others, or in what areas they are located. However, LISA statistics allow us to calculate spatial correlation for each area of the study region and to assess whether a statistically significant difference exists, comparing the results with a random distribution. For a detailed description of spatial methodologies, see O’Sullivan and Unwin [21] and Anselin [23].

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

As a result of the sampling procedure of the SEBEM project, the majority (48%) of the respondents were located in Göteborg (N = 993) (Table 1). Overall, women represented the majority of the sample (59.4%), reflecting their prevalence in the general population. Almost one out of five respondents were born outside Sweden (19.4%). However, the share of those born abroad was higher in Göteborg (reaching 23.7% of the total there). The study population in Lund had a higher formal education status, where the share of those with tertiary education (i.e., university/college) reaches 56.1%, compared to Helsingborg (21.7%) and Göteborg (25.7%). In spite of their old age, the respondents showed active physical mobility patterns: 76% of them used to walk 500 m or more at least several times a week, and 40% of them drove a car with the same frequency. In addition, more than 50% of the respondents declared to be very satisfied with the walking environment in their neighbourhood, while only 4% were not. Overall, 25% of the respondents did not use the public transport services. Among those, 5% argued that they did not use the transport services because the closest bus stop was too far away from their homes.

3.2. Walking Distance to Bus Stops and Characteristics of Respondents

According to the computed/real walking distances, the overall mean distance to bus stops was 269 m. It was higher than the average in Göteborg (282 m; +14 m), probably due to the urban dimension, and lower in Lund, the smallest city, where the minimum distance was recorded (17.7 m). An amount of 95% of the respondents lived no farther than 550 m from the closest bus stop, and less than 10% of them lived within a 110 m distance range. While the majority of the respondents had their closest bus stop in a 180 m–380 m distance band, only 44 people (2.1%) lived more than 600 m away from the bus stop.
In spatial terms, although there is a certain clustering in the spatial distribution of the respondents, their location is rather scattered across the study areas (Figure 1). The spatial structure is especially sparse for Göteborg, which is the largest of the cities considered. When looking at the outcome variable—the distance from respondents’ homes to bus stops—the global spatial autocorrelation is quite low (Moran’s I is around 0.3 on average for the three cities). Interestingly, however, some local clustering appears when computing LISA’s indicators. The results are easily interpretable for Göteborg, in light of its larger dimension. The “blue” clusters of low distance to bus stops are evident in the central part of the city, while the red cluster is dispersed across peripheral areas. Similar evidence arises for Lund and Helsingborg (see autocorrelation maps in Supplementary Materials).
When comparing the real walking distance to bus stops with background data from the SEBEM project, the distances were significantly higher for people who were born outside Sweden (309 m vs. 281 m, p = 0.0064), less educated (271 m vs. 298 m, p = 0.0127), and living with other people (303 m vs. 269 m, p < 0.0001). Gender, age, and frequency of social activities coefficients were instead not significant (cf. Table 2 and Table 3 for variables list). The distance to bus stops was also associated with mobility-related variables. Thus, people who had access to private cars lived farther from bus stops (297 m vs. 268 m, p = 0.0017), in particular those who used the car daily or frequently (327 m vs. 277 m, p = 0.0002). Likewise, respondents who never or rarely used public transport lived farther away from the bus stops (319 m vs. 231 m, p < 0.0001). In particular, the difference was high for individuals who reported not using public transport because stops are too far to reach. Those 115 subjects, in fact, lived as an average 402 m away from bus stops, against 267 m as an average for the rest of the sample (p < 0.0001). The respondents reporting that bus stops were too far away generally had less access to a private car (45% against 65%), were older (82 against 80 mean age), and with poorer health conditions (−5.6 points against PCS12 = 38.1 and −4.6 in MCS12 = 51.4). At the same time, they reported more often to be unhappy to move and walk around in their neighbourhoods (Pearson chi2(4) = 42.3, p < 0.0001).

3.3. Perception of Distance

Looking at the distribution of respondents in terms of self-reported and real walking distance to bus stops, there was not a strong discrepancy for most of the subjects included in the 200–500 m and less-than-200 m bands (Figure 1). Instead, the mean distance for people who said they lived farther than 500 m was 436 m (425 m median), more than 50 m below the class lower limit. The majority of the respondents in the highest distance class live in fact nearer than 500 m to a bus stop. Moreover, for larger perceived distances, the estimates were more variable (e.g., their standard deviation was higher). People who could not say how far they live from a bus stop actually were in between the 200–500 m band (362 m), with a high standard deviation (Figure 2).
The text continues here (Figure 2 and Table 2).
Overall, 1242 (60%) respondents provided a correct estimation of their walking distance to the closest bus stop and 822 respondents (40%) estimated their distance wrongly or could not estimate it. The percentage of biased estimation was highest for the 500 m+ class (53.5%), and lowest for the middle 200–500 m class. The mean error (in absolute value with respect to central values of classes) was 201 m, with 75 m standard deviation.
The respondents who provided a biased estimation had identifiable characteristics (see Table 2 and Table 3). According to bivariate chi-square tests, the oldest age group was more likely to overestimate the real distance. The PCS12 of people who overestimated their distance was low (36.7) in comparison to those who underestimated (39.3, p < 0.0001) (Table 2). However, it should be noted that all three groups are relatively close to the reference values, and in light of population variability (SD = 11 for both PCS12 and MCS12), the observed effects are small in terms of standardised effect size.
When mobility behaviours are concerned, there was a significant association between use of public transport and distance perception. People who rarely used public transport or did not use it because they considered the bus stop too far tended to overestimate the real distance (Table 3). But respondents who said they were very satisfied with the walking environment of their neighbourhood tended to underestimate their distance to the bus stop; in other terms, they perceived it as closer than it actually is.
When analysing the above associations simultaneously in a regression model, the fact of being born outside Sweden increased the probability of overestimating the real distance (Table 4). In the same way, those who never used public transport because they estimated that bus stops were too far from home also tended to overestimate the real distance (2.9 in terms of Relative Risk Ratio) (consistently, the sign is reversed for underestimate). Predictably, people in good physical health (i.e., with a high PCS) were less likely to overestimate distance and so were respondents who lived farther (200 m or more) from bus stops (with respect to those who lived closer).
Conversely, respondents who were satisfied with the walking environment of the neighbourhood more likely underestimated the distance (1.2 RRR). Analogously, people who use public transport daily or several times a week tended to underestimate distance (2.06 RRR). The coefficients of those who lived in Göteborg, were aged 86–90, frequently used the car or walked 500 m daily or several times a week or more were negative, meaning that those people were more prone to correctly estimate distances (i.e., less likely to underestimate with respect to correct estimate).

4. Discussion

This study investigates the difference between perceived (survey-reported) and real walking distance (GIS-computed) and also analyses what socioeconomic characteristics of older individuals are associated with the perception of the distance to the closest bus stop to their home. First of all, our findings are important for the planning sector of society in that they show a discrepancy between real and self-reported distances in this study focusing on distances to the closest bus stop. Another strength of this study worth mentioning is that the response rate of the SEBEM study was high, which increases its reliability [20].
An amount of 60% of respondents estimated the distances correctly. Among the others, 16.4% (327) overestimated the distance meaning that they estimated the distance to be longer than in reality. Individuals in this group are associated with higher age and poor health. Furthermore, there is an overrepresentation of people born abroad in this group. From a societal point of view, these findings are important since they highlight a vulnerable group which have a risk of no longer finding public transport as a choice for travelling. This puts a pressure on society to seriously take the importance of shorter distances into account in urban transport planning in order to integrate all citizens and to maintain mobility and an active life for an ageing population. A positive finding in this respect from this study is that the group who underestimated the distances to the closest bus stop are associated not only with frequent use of public transport but also with higher satisfaction with the walking environment. Therefore, planning for an accessible public transport system must also put the highest priority to the walking environment on the neighbourhood level in order to keep this vulnerable group active.
According to scientific literature, the main factors that influence the correct perception of distance are different. Our study showed results in accordance with previous studies since an underestimation was influenced by the walking environment in the neighbourhood like it has been found by Cepolina et al. regarding the comfort of pedestrian facilities (width, surface, crossings, obstructions, traffic density, etc.) [24]. Moreover, physical condition can also influence the correct estimation of waking distance since the physical composite score showed a statistically significant association with overestimation and this result recalls the importance of physical abilities as found by previous studies [25,26].
Living far from the bus stop is associated with socioeconomic characteristics. Annual income, living alone or in a family nucleus, job situation, and other demographic characteristics of individuals are associated with their residential choices. As a result, some neighbourhoods of cities may be occupied by subjects with homogeneous characteristics [27]. As a consequence, personal characteristics of the respondents were reflected in the distance between their homes and the stops of public transport. In this regard, it is an interesting finding that people with other backgrounds than Swedish tended to overestimate the distances to the closest bus stop. Possibly, the subjects with less knowledge of the local environment might have a biased image of the surrounding space and, consequently, of real distances. Likewise, those reporting they did not use public transport due to long distances to the bus stops also had a tendency of overestimating the real distances. Evidently, those subjects gave a strong weight to the walking activity and tended to weight distances more.
Interestingly, respondents who reported being very satisfied with the walking opportunities of their neighbourhood tend to underestimate their distance to the bus stop. The perception of distance is in fact strongly influenced by the mobility habits of the subjects. In particular, those who are satisfied with the walking environment in their neighbourhood (such as the presence of parks, etc.) tend to underestimate the distance, i.e., they do not perceive much effort in walking for longer distances. That is a relevant indication in terms of urban planning, showing that a comfortable setting may be used as a factor in promoting active lifestyles and enhancing sustainable transport. This result also confirms that the nature of the area in terms of comfort and adequacy of features plays an important role.
This study is conducted in three cities with similar socioeconomic backgrounds but with some differences in size and in the way mobility services for older people are treated and conducted in the respective municipality. Göteborg, situated along the Göta River, is characterised by a mix of urban and waterfront areas with a series of canals, contributing to its distinct geography. It has pedestrian-friendly central areas, with a well-maintained and extensive cycling infrastructure. Public transportation includes trams, buses, and ferries, providing efficient connectivity throughout the city. The city centre is compact and easy to navigate on foot. Lund has a pedestrian-friendly city centre with narrow streets and historic buildings: due to its smaller size, many residents and students prefer walking or cycling to move around the city. Situated in the southern part of Sweden, Helsingborg is located on the Oresund Strait; it is a key transportation hub, offering ferry connections to and from Danmark. The city has a mix of historical and modern architecture. Its waterfront areas, including the marina, are often explored on foot by residents and visitors.
There are also differences between the three cities concerning background variables. For example, Lund is characterised by a relevant share of educated older population, as a consequence of its status of “university town”, and Göteborg has a high proportion of people born abroad—Göteborg is also different in terms of population size, being the second biggest city in Sweden.
Nonetheless, this study has some limitations, mostly dependent on the algorithm based on OSM data, which can sometimes be imperfect when routing and distance measurements are concerned. First, OSM data extent and quality are dependent on the overall availability of contributors and features in an area [17]. Secondly, OSM routing can be inconsistent for some cases, thus not computing walking distances for a subset of the cases (see Section 2.2 and Figure S10 in the Supplementary Materials). It should be acknowledged that mapping guidelines exists in OSM to take into account the roads dedicated to pedestrians (tag: highway = pedestrian) or, on the contrary, where walking might not be safe or recommended: for example, if the road has sidewalks (tag: sidewalk = none), or if it has been classified as foot = yes. In spite of that, the completeness tags to this respect are not perfect, and actual walking behaviours cannot be determined in many parts of the map, leaving space for inconsistent cases to arise.
At present, the computed walking distances are not weighted in terms of walking barriers, tunnels, surface conditions, traffic, and other road attributes that influence the perception of distance [24,28]. Taking into account those occurrences and topography, however, may represent a promising technical extension of the current study, as well as producing a more sophisticated algorithm to take other morphological features into account. In terms of topography, the general differences among cities are only considered in the statistical model using dummy variables. This is a simplistic approach. In this sense, the dummy variables are a basic way to control for the diverse characteristics of the cities, and especially to take the largest demographic dimension of Göteborg into account, with respect to Lund and Helsingborg. Other differences, such as the general topography of the areas (steepness, etc.) remain untreated (see Supplementary Materials for descriptive elevation maps).
It should be noted, also, that both the objective and subjective measures used in this study are relative to the nearest bus stop to peoples’ homes, even if those are not necessarily the ones used by the respondents, who may use other stops because of personal or context-related features (i.e., bigger hubs, safer neighbourhoods, stops including shops and other services or routes with destinations of interest, etc.). Respondents’ choices may be influenced also by their perception of time needed to walk to the bus stops, which, even if related, may be different from the perception of distance [29]
From a technical point of view, the results of this study allow us to overcome an issue common to spatial analyses, i.e., the Modifiable Area Unit Problem (MAUP) [30]. When the centroid of area units (such as neighbourhoods) are used to compute population’s walking distances to bus stops, the underlying assumption is that all the residents from the same neighbourhood have the same distance to the bus stops [31]. That measure of distance is less accurate than the point-to-point distance used in this study, which from a methodological perspective allows us to test the level of MAUP in a similar way as the perceived distance has been validated. In addition to the spatial perspective, the existence of possible spatial autocorrelation has been tested (see results in Supplementary Materials). Without going deeply into this topic, when looking at the distance from respondents’ homes to bus stops, the global autocorrelation seemed to be quite low (Moran’s I is around 0.3 on average for the three cities). Interestingly, however, some clustering appeared at the local level when computing LISA’s indicators. The results for Göteborg are easily interpretable: the “blue” clusters of low distance to bus stops are evident in the central part of the city, while the “red” cluster is dispersed across peripheral areas. Similar evidence arose for Lund and Helsingborg.
The results of this methodological contribution suggest that evidence based on self-reported measures only should be treated cautiously. Nevertheless, considering the limitations still present in open-data-based routing algorithms, the best indication is to combine the subjective with the objective measure of distance. Having the possibility to combine the two measures appears as a sound strategy to overcome the limitations associated with each single measure.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the policy implications of our studies are not negligible. Policy making in the context of urban planning and accessibility promotion is often informed by data and evidence coming from surveys and pools. When considering the generalised ageing phenomenon, one might acknowledge the paramount importance of bus use for the older population. Ageing is often associated with the reduction in modal options for transport [32], which makes access and actual use of bus key for avoiding unwanted reduction in, e.g., social engagement and use of public services. The concept of distance perception is thus relevant when analyzing accessibility. As our study suggested that individual factors might be associated with perception of bus stop distance, others have shown that accessibility too is a subjective concept [33]. It appears to be a notion intertwined with individual and subjective perceptions of one’s own capabilities. Consequently, accessibility analyses to inform policy changes should not be based on methods which neglect the intertwining of individuals’ characteristics, self-reported and objective measurements. More specifically, our study points to the importance of considering variables related to health and functional status and actual patterns of use of transport modes by the respondents.
The perception of accessibility might ultimately influence the overall satisfaction of people when using public transport services to the same extent of other quality improvements [34,35]. In this respect, it is interesting to note that recent mobility management programs have started leveraging modern marketing strategies for perception improvements [36]. Cities in northern Europe have invested to communicate the advantages of their public transport system to users: high frequency services, high quality vehicles, good off-peak provision, and smart and simple multi-modal ticketing (with multi-modal fares delivering excellent value for money in comparison to incomes) (For examples related to Scandinavia, see the following: “The Scandinavian Way To Better Public Transport”, Urban Transport Group, available at: http://www.urbantransportgroup.org/system/files/general-docs/UTG%20Scandinavian%20Transport%20Report_Final.pdf, last accessed on 25 November 2023). With our findings, we aim at informing future strategies to promote an increasing use of public transport among the older population by reducing the walking distance, e.g., by the introduction (or wider use) of demand-responsive modes. Moreover, a more comprehensive engagement of the general population about public transport is needed, with educating and showing already existing possibilities of correct use and approach. Our results show possible variables associated with an underestimation or overestimation of perceived distances, indicating possible areas of knowledge improvement for specific population groups.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/geomatics3040031/s1. The attached Supplementary Materials contain maps, figures and a scheme of methods.

Author Contributions

F.B.: Conceptualisation; data curation; formal analysis; methodology; software; supervision; validation; visualisation; writing, review and editing of the article’s text. A.S.: Conceptualisation; investigation, supervision, validation; writing, review and editing. O.S.: Data curation; software. B.J.: Data curation; software. Y.W.: Conceptualisation; investigation, supervision, validation; writing, review and editing. J.D.: Revision, health literature integration. C.C.: Conceptualisation; funding acquisition; project administration; resources; investigation, supervision, validation; writing, review and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding. The original SEBEM (Socio-Economic Benefits of Elderly Mobility) project was funded by the Centre for Ageing and Supportive Environment (CASE) at Lund University (Sweden).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of the Regional Ethical Review Board in Lund, Sweden (DNR 2015/30). An extension of the previous ethical clearance was applied for an extension of the original protocol. Ethical clearance was granted by the Regional Ethical Board of Skåne (EPN DNR 2018/368).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The clinical data that support the findings of this study are confidential. Cartographic data coming from OpenStreetMap are freely downloadable upon OpenDBLicence ((C) OpenStreetMap contributors).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Rantakokko, M.; Mänty, M.; Rantanen, T. Mobility Decline in Old Age. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 2013, 41, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Ferrucci, L.; Cooper, R.; Shardell, M.; Simonsick, E.M.; Schrack, J.A.; Kuh, D. Age-Related Change in Mobility: Perspectives From Life Course Epidemiology and Geroscience. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2016, 71, 1184–1194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Jagger, C.; Matthews, F.; Wohland, P.; Fouweather, T.; Stephan, B.C.; Robinson, L.; Arthur, A.; Brayne, C. A comparison of health expectancies over two decades in England: Results of the Cognitive Function and Ageing Study I and II. Lancet 2016, 387, 779–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. OECD. Preventing Ageing Unequally; OECD Publishing: Paris, France, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Parviainen, J. Kinetic Values, Mobility (in)equalities, and Ageing in Smart Urban Environments. Ethic Theory Moral Pract. 2021, 24, 1139–1153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Shergold, I.; Lyons, G.; Hubers, C. Future mobility in an ageing society—Where are we heading? J. Transp. Health 2015, 2, 86–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Chiatti, C.; Westerlund, Y.; Ståhl, A. Access to public mobility services and health in old age: A cross-sectional study in three Swedish cities. J. Transp. Health 2017, 7, 218–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Stjernborg, V. The Meaning of Social Participation for Daily Mobility in Later Life: An Ethnographic Case Study of a Senior Project in a Swedish Urban Neighbourhood. Ageing Int. 2017, 42, 374–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Corran, P.; Steinbach, R.; Saunders, L.; Green, J. Age, disability and everyday mobility in London: An analysis of the correlates of ‘non-travel’ in travel diary data. J. Transp. Health 2018, 8, 129–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Wretstrand, A.; Svensson, H.; Fristedt, S.; Falkmer, T. Older people and local public transit: Mobility effects of accessibility improvements in Sweden. J. Transp. Land Use 2009, 2, 49–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hallgrimsdottir, B.; Svensson, H.; Stahl, A. Long term effects of an intervention in the outdoor environment—A comparison of older people’s perception in two residential areas, in one of which accessibility improvements were introduced. J. Transp. Geogr. 2015, 42, 90–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Sze, N.N.; Christensen, K.M. Access to urban transportation system for individuals with disabilities. IATSS Res. 2017, 41, 66–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ståhl, A. Planning for a Community Responsive Public Transport System—The Swedish Model; Anchor Research: London, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  14. Wretstrand, A.; Ståhl, A. User Needs and Expectations Relative to Accessible Transport. Framework for Mobility Planning; EuroAccess WP2-Lund-Deliverable2-v.1.0. EU:s Sixth Framework Programme; EURO ACCESS Consortium: London, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  15. Murray, A.T.; Davis, R.; Stimson, R.J.; Ferreira, L. Public Transportation Access. Transp. Res. Part D Transp. Environ. 1998, 3, 319–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Kjellson, B. Country Report of Sweden Swedish Spatial Data Infrastructure. Submitted to United Nations Committee of Experts on Global Geospatial Information Management Fifth Session New York, August 2015. Available online: https://ggim.un.org/country-reports/documents/Sweden-2015-country-report.pdf (accessed on 25 November 2023).
  17. Haklay, M. How good is volunteered geographical information? A comparative study of OpenStreetMap and ordnance survey datasets. Environ. Plan. B Plan. Des. 2010, 37, 682–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Antoniou, V.; Skopeliti, A. Measures and indicators of VGI quality: An overview. ISPRS Ann. Photogramm. Remote Sens. Spat. Inf. Sci. 2015, 2, 345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Westerlund, Y.; Ståhl, A. Kollektivtrafikens betydelse för de äldres mobilitet och välbefinnande. Del B—Analys av resor och kostnader för mobilitetstjänster för äldre. Lund, Lunds Universitet, Tekniska högskolan i Lund, Institutionen för teknik och samhälle. Bulletin 2017, 303. [Google Scholar]
  20. Chiatti, C.; Ståhl, A.; Westerlund, Y.; Franzén, L. Kollektivtrafikens betydelse för de äldres mobilitet och välbefinnande. Del A—Samband mellan mobilitet, aktivitetsnivå och hälsa. Lund, Lunds Universitet, Tekniska högskolan i Lund, Institutionen för teknik och samhälle. Bulletin 2017, 304. [Google Scholar]
  21. O’Sullivan, D.; Unwin, D. Geographic Information Analysis; John Wiley and Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  22. Moran, P. The interpretation of statistical maps. J. R. Stat. Soc. 1948, 10, 243–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Anselin, L. Local indicators of spatial association—LISA. Geogr. Anal. 1995, 27, 93–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Cepolina, E.M.; Menichini, F.; Gonzalez Rojas, P. Level of service of pedestrian facilities: Modelling human comfort perception in the evaluation of pedestrian behaviour patterns. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2018, 58, 365–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Bhalla, M.; Proffitt, D.R. Visual–motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1999, 25, 1076–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Proffitt, D.R.; Bhalla, M.; Gossweiler, R.; Midgett, J. Perceiving geographical slant. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1995, 2, 409–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Diez Roux, A.V. Investigating neighborhood and area effects on health. Am. J. Public Health 2001, 91, 1783–1789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Stefanucci, J.K.; Proffitt, D.R.; Banton, T.; Epstein, W. Distances appear different on hills. Percept. Psychophys. 2005, 67, 1052–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Watson, K.B.; Carlson, S.A.; Humbert-Rico, T.; Carroll, D.D.; Fulton, J.E. Walking for Transportation: What do U.S. Adults Think is a Reasonable Distance and Time? J. Phys. Act. Health 2015, 12, S53–S61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Openshaw, S. The Modifiable Areal Unit Problem; GeoBooks: Norwick, Virginia, 1983; ISBN 0860941345. [Google Scholar]
  31. Viegas, J.; Silva, E.A.; Martinez, L. Effects of the Modifiable Areal Unit Problem on the Delineation of Traffic Analysis Zones. Environ. Plan. B Plan. Des. 2009, 36, 625–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Haustein, S.; Siren, A. Older People’s Mobility: Segments, Factors, Trends. Transp. Rev. 2015, 35, 466–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ryan, J.; Pereira, R.H.M. What are we missing when we measure accessibility? Comparing calculated and self-reported accounts among older people. J. Transp. Geogr. 2021, 93, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Andreassen, T.W. (Dis)satisfaction with public services: The case of public transportation. J. Serv. Mark. 1995, 9, 30–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Margareta, F. Implementing Quality Improvements in Public Transport. J. Public Transp. 2004, 7, 49–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Taniguchi, A.; Fujii, S. Promoting Public Transport Using Marketing Techniques in Mobility Management and Verifying their Quantitative Effects. Transportation 2007, 34, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Bus stop locations and density maps of respondents: Lund (panel A), Helsingborg (B) and Göteborg (C). Note: Black crosses indicate bus stops. Kernel Density Estimation (KDE) heatmap colored in orange, Radius: 350 m; Source of geodata: OSM, STAMEN.
Figure 1. Bus stop locations and density maps of respondents: Lund (panel A), Helsingborg (B) and Göteborg (C). Note: Black crosses indicate bus stops. Kernel Density Estimation (KDE) heatmap colored in orange, Radius: 350 m; Source of geodata: OSM, STAMEN.
Geomatics 03 00031 g001
Figure 2. Distribution of actual distance, by respondents’ perceived distance band. Outlier points are indicated.
Figure 2. Distribution of actual distance, by respondents’ perceived distance band. Outlier points are indicated.
Geomatics 03 00031 g002
Table 1. Sample in the study areas: full sample and respondents with computed walking distance.
Table 1. Sample in the study areas: full sample and respondents with computed walking distance.
Full SampleRespondents with
Computed Walking Distance
Missing Values
Age GroupN%N%N
Lund 75–8036455.431855.9946
81–8519129.0716328.728
86–9010215.538715.3215
Total65710056810089
Helsingborg 75–8029650.625851.2938
81–8518331.2815831.4125
86–9010618.128717.319
Total58510050310082
Göteborg 75–8057149.3949549.8576
81–8536431.4931231.4252
86–9022119.1218618.7335
Total1156100993100163
Total 75–80123151.33107151.89160
81–8573830.7863330.67105
86–9042917.8936017.4469
Total23981002064100334
Table 2. Perception of distance according to respondents’ characteristics. Column percentages for each indicator, by perception of distance (N = 1994).
Table 2. Perception of distance according to respondents’ characteristics. Column percentages for each indicator, by perception of distance (N = 1994).
Perception of Real Distance
Underestimated
(N = 425)
Correct
(N = 1242)
Overestimated
(N = 327)
p-Value
Gender 0.106
Men
45.641.5637.89
Females
54.458.4462.11
Age Group 0.011
75–80
59.5350.8947.71
81–85
27.0631.3232.42
86–90
13.4117.7919.88
Place of birth 0.322
Sweden
82.7483.2879.75
Abroad
17.2616.7220.25
Education 0.666
Primary
38.4237.3238.01
Secondary
26.0127.5830.22
Tertiary
35.5635.1131.78
Living arrangements 0.302
Home, alone
46.147.7651.54
Home, with others
53.952.2448.46
Areas of living 0.776
Lund
28.2427.7825.69
Helsingborg
24.9423.5126.91
Göteborg
46.8248.7147.4
Frequency of social activities 0.388
0 activities
40.8145.0545.28
1 activity
34.8435.2234.28
2–3 activities
21.2417.219.18
4–6 activities
3.12.541.26
Self-reported health (mean)
PCS1239.3338.1536.710.001
MCS1253.9853.1351.990.0561
Note: significant coefficients (95%) are highlighted in bold.
Table 3. Perception of distance according to mobility behaviours. Column percentages for each indicator, by perception of distance (N = 1994).
Table 3. Perception of distance according to mobility behaviours. Column percentages for each indicator, by perception of distance (N = 1994).
Perception of Real Distance
Underestimated
(N = 425)
Correct
(N = 1242)
Overestimated
(N = 327)
p-Value
Walking 500 m or more 0.301
Seldom/never
5.678.319.51
Once a month
3.553.995.21
Once a week
9.698.729.2
Several times a week
33.3336.6335.89
Daily
47.7542.3640.18
Satisfied with the walking environment in the neighbourhood <0.001
Very dissatisfied
0.241.341.86
Pretty unhappy
1.672.434.35
Neither satisfied nor dissatisfied
7.1410.1315.53
Pretty satisfied
29.2932.8332.61
Very satisfied
61.6753.2745.65
Access to private car 0.747
Never, or almost never
57.0557.6553.33
Yes, sometimes
18.6618.820.33
Yes, always
24.323.5426.33
Use of private car 0.538
Seldom/never
23.0822.6423.53
Once a month
21.6318.4621.67
Once a week
13.4615.1217.03
Several times a week
34.3835.2529.41
Daily
7.458.528.36
Use of public transport 0.001
No, for other reasons
16.7518.2621.1
No, because bus stop is too far
0.474.465.81
Yes, once a week or once a month
34.233.832.11
Yes, daily or several times a week
48.5843.4740.98
Note: significant coefficients (95%) are highlighted in bold.
Table 4. Multinomial Logistic Regression Model. Factor associated with perception of distance (baseline = correct estimate).
Table 4. Multinomial Logistic Regression Model. Factor associated with perception of distance (baseline = correct estimate).
Underestimated
(Living Farther than Perceived)
Overestimated
(Living Closer than Perceived)
Coef.Std. E.p-ValueCoef.Std. E.p-Value
Gender (=Female)−0.2130.1860.2510.2000.2090.339
Age group:
81–85
−0.3290.2270.1480.3050.2610.242
86–90
−0.719 *0.3170.0240.1010.3250.756
Gender # Age group (interaction):
Female#81–85
−0.0900.3060.768−0.2990.3390.378
Female#86–90
0.1010.4310.8140.0740.4060.855
Born abroad (=Yes)−0.0610.1830.7390.421 *0.1850.023
Area of living (ref. = Lund)
Helsingborg
−0.2450.1880.1930.3610.2000.071
Göteborg
−0.582 ***0.1740.0010.1740.1810.337
Freq. of social activities0.0150.0870.8660.1350.0940.150
Physical Composite Score (PCS12)0.0020.0120.852−0.023 *0.0110.042
Mental Composite Score (MCS12)0.0040.0080.666−0.0080.0090.34
Walk 500 m (daily or several times week)−0.414 *0.2000.0390.2890.2130.176
Car use (daily or several times week)−0.331 *0.1510.0280.0730.1620.651
Satisfied with the walking environment in the neighbourhood 0.243 *0.1040.02−0.1050.0970.277
Public transport use: (ref. = never, for other reasons)
Never, because bus stop is too far
−3.520 **1.1280.0021.072 **0.4010.007
Yes, once a week or once a month
0.1600.1950.412−0.0500.2080.81
Yes, daily or several times a week
0.728 ***0.202<0.001−0.3420.2140.11
Walking distance to bus stop (ref. = less than 200 m)
200 m–500 m
18.7600.7190.979−1.542 ***0.161<0.001
More than 500 m
20.496 ***0.124<0.001−19.270 ***0.293<0.001
Constant−19.779 ***0.59600.2850.6030.636
Note: N = 1742; Log likelihood = −1247.9; Pseudo R2 = 0.22; AIC = 2569.99; BIC = 2772.11. Significance levels: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. # indicates the interaction between regressor variables.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Balducci, F.; Ståhl, A.; Svensson, O.; Jonsson, B.; Westerlund, Y.; Dolcini, J.; Chiatti, C. “How Far Is the Closest Bus Stop?” An Evaluation of Self-Reported versus GIS-Computed Distance to the Bus among Older People and Factors Influencing Their Perception of Distance. Geomatics 2023, 3, 580-596. https://doi.org/10.3390/geomatics3040031

AMA Style

Balducci F, Ståhl A, Svensson O, Jonsson B, Westerlund Y, Dolcini J, Chiatti C. “How Far Is the Closest Bus Stop?” An Evaluation of Self-Reported versus GIS-Computed Distance to the Bus among Older People and Factors Influencing Their Perception of Distance. Geomatics. 2023; 3(4):580-596. https://doi.org/10.3390/geomatics3040031

Chicago/Turabian Style

Balducci, Francesco, Agneta Ståhl, Ola Svensson, Benny Jonsson, Yngve Westerlund, Jacopo Dolcini, and Carlos Chiatti. 2023. "“How Far Is the Closest Bus Stop?” An Evaluation of Self-Reported versus GIS-Computed Distance to the Bus among Older People and Factors Influencing Their Perception of Distance" Geomatics 3, no. 4: 580-596. https://doi.org/10.3390/geomatics3040031

APA Style

Balducci, F., Ståhl, A., Svensson, O., Jonsson, B., Westerlund, Y., Dolcini, J., & Chiatti, C. (2023). “How Far Is the Closest Bus Stop?” An Evaluation of Self-Reported versus GIS-Computed Distance to the Bus among Older People and Factors Influencing Their Perception of Distance. Geomatics, 3(4), 580-596. https://doi.org/10.3390/geomatics3040031

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop