1. Introduction
Fecal glucocorticoid metabolites (FGMs) are an established physiological measurement that can provide insight into individual animal welfare [
1,
2], although it is recognized that glucocorticoids in general do not show a straightforward relationship with physiological stress [
3]. While FGMs can offer insight into the physiological experience of a particular animal, these measurements must be interpreted in relation to other contextual data, such as health measures, behavior, or environmental conditions. Animal welfare is defined by the Association of Zoos and Aquariums (AZA) as “an animal’s collective physical, mental, and emotional states over a period of time, and is measured on a continuum from good to poor” [
4]. Animal welfare data are often complicated by individual life history, making cross-individual comparisons and population-level generalizations difficult. Habitat transitions and alterations, changes in available social partners, and involvement in breeding programs can all potentially affect an animal’s FGMs [
5,
6,
7]. There may even be significant variation in hormonal and behavioral stress responses between individual animals of the same species experiencing the same or similar social and environmental conditions [
8], due to variations in genetics, temperament, or life history.
Cortisol enzyme immunoassays (EIAs) have been biologically validated for measuring FGMs in captive and wild adult polar (
Ursus maritimus) and grizzly bears
(Ursus arctos horribilis) and can be used to evaluate their responses to medium to long-term stressors [
9,
10,
11,
12,
13]. While higher FGMs have been observed in concert with increased pacing and low interest in novel stimuli in adult polar bears, environmental variables, such as dry land availability, may also influence FGMs [
14].
Despite the established methodology, the published literature exploring relationships between FGMs and other contextualizing variables in bears is relatively limited. Though age is often a variable of interest in welfare and captive husbandry, studies that include FGM measurements tend to focus on adult individuals [
10,
11,
13,
14,
15]. While significant trends in relation to age have been recorded, such as differential responses to temperature based on age [
16], the lack of published data detailing polar and grizzly bear physiological development leaves gaps in our current understanding of these animals [
17]. As such, establishing physiological descriptions and trends for polar and grizzly bears during key developmental stages would be greatly beneficial to the existing body of work surrounding these ursids.
Here, we investigate FGM changes over time during development, as well as behavioral and environmental relationships with FGMs, in three juvenile bears. Our analysis explores physiological data in relation to differential rearing experiences of polar bear cubs, the use of steroid and anti-seizure medications on a polar bear cub, and the cohabitation of one polar and one grizzly bear cub. Given the challenges that can accompany captive polar and grizzly bear development and the scarcity of available information, the following physiological and behavioral data will help to progress the current understanding of bear development and could be a first step in establishing species-level trends in juvenile polar and grizzly bears.
3. Results
As previously discussed, there is an established relationship between medications containing synthetic glucocorticoids and FGMs. As such, the table detailing results of the Wilcoxon and Dwass–Steel–Critchlow–Fligner tests comparing FGMs between individuals based on Laerke’s medication dosages is provided as a supplement in
Appendix A Table A1. Based on the Kruskal–Wallis results (
Table 5), we can broadly separate Laerke’s medication conditions into two larger categories: conditions a and b (hereafter referred to as Full Dosages) and conditions c-f (hereafter referred to as Decreasing Dosages). These two categories differed notably from each other, with Laerke displaying an average concentration of 68.49 ng/g ± 5.58 SE (average CV of 99%) in the combined Full Dosage conditions and an average concentration of 22.63 ng/g ± 1.09 SE (average CV of 79%) across the combined Decreasing Dosages conditions (
Figure 4).
We compared mean FGM parameters between the three cubs using the Decreasing Dosages condition of Laerke’s samples (
Table 5). Laerke and Astra had similar FGM baselines, mean levels, and variability despite being hand- and mother-reared, respectively. Jeb had significantly lower overall FGM measurements compared to the two polar bears (
Table 5). Additionally, there was a high amount of variability in the FGM results for all three bear cubs, with CVs ranging from 78–99%.
Laerke’s GLMMs indicated the random intercept (age in weeks) as the most significant predictor (as measured by
p-value) of log-transformed FGM concentrations according to the environmental, all-occurrence behavior, and interval models (
Table 6). In the environmental model, the intercept was the only significant predictor with log-transformed FGM concentrations increasing as Laerke aged, without an additional effect of temperature or crowd size. While the all-occurrence behavior model also suggests that Laerke’s log-transformed FGM concentrations increased as her rate of social negative interactions increased and decreased as her rate of social positive interactions increased, the effect (as indicated by the estimate) is relatively negligible (
Table 6). The interval model demonstrates that Laerke’s time spent in abnormal behaviors had a greater effect on FGM concentrations, but at a lower significance value than age. Generally, Laerke’s log-transformed FGM concentrations decreased as her time spent in abnormal behaviors and swimming increased (
Table 6). Intriguingly, the proximity model was the only model in which the random intercept was not a significant predictor of log-transformed FGM concentrations. Rather, this model suggests that Laerke’s concentrations increased as her time spent at distances of both greater than and less than five meters from Jeb increased. Her concentrations also trended towards increasing as her time spent in contact with Jeb increased. This is supported by the test of concentrations between housing conditions which demonstrated that Laerke’s FGMs were significantly higher when she was housed with Jeb compared to when she was housed alone (F
1,272 = 4.63,
p = 0.03) (
Figure 5). Across all models, we see a pattern in which Laerke’s log-transformed FGM concentrations increased with age, some measures of social proximity, and hourly rates of social negative interactions, but decreased with hourly rate of social positive behaviors and time spent in abnormal and swimming behaviors (
Table 6).
The mean FGM concentration for Jeb was 15.14 ng/g ± 0.86 SE (
Figure 6) with a CV of 86%. According to the GLMMs, the random intercept (age in weeks) was the best predictor of Jeb’s log-transformed FGM concentrations according both to significance (
p-value) and effect size (as measured by the estimate) (
Table 6). Jeb’s FGM concentrations demonstrated no relationship with predictors in the environmental model (temperature and crowd size) and only a trending relationship with vocalizations in the interval behavior model such that his long-transformed FGM concentrations trended towards increasing as his time spent in vocalizations increased (
Table 6). We saw significance in the all-occurrence model such that FGM concentrations decreased as the hourly rate of swimming increased. However, the estimate for this is almost negligible. The proximity model demonstrated that Jeb’s FGM concentrations decreased as his time spent within one meter of Laerke increased. This supports results comparing Jeb’s housing conditions, as following the separation from Laerke Jeb’s FGMs were significantly higher (F
1,229 = 10.72,
p = 0.001) (
Figure 5).
The mean FGM concentration for Astra was 22.71 ng/g ± 1.61 SE (
Figure 7) with a CV of 79%. The strongest predictor for log-transformed FGM concentrations for Astra in each model was the random intercept (age in weeks) such that Astra’s concentrations increased as she aged (
Table 6). While the proximity and all-occurrence behavior models demonstrated no other trending or significant predictor variables outside of the intercept, we did see other significant predictors in the environmental and interval behavior models. According to the environmental model, Astra’s log-transformed FGM concentrations increased as the temperature increased, but the effect size (as measured by estimates) was almost negligible (
Table 6). We saw more significant/trending results, along with more meaningful effect sizes, in the interval behavior predictors. Specifically, the model indicated that Astra’s log-transformed FGM concentrations increased as her time spent in social positive, investigation, and locomotory behaviors increased. Her concentrations also trended towards increasing as her time spent in eating, object manipulation, and swimming behaviors increased (
Table 6).
4. Discussion
Although the circumstances surrounding the individual subjects of this study were rather unique, there are many valuable insights that can be drawn from the available combined physiological, behavioral, and environmental data. The similarity in the FGM baselines between Astra and Laerke, after accounting for the effects of the latter’s medication, suggests that the two polar bear cubs likely experienced similar levels of adrenocortical activity regardless of their distinctive rearing experiences, potentially due to canalization [
33,
34]. Although this is certainly a significant finding, this inference should be interpreted with caution due to the small sample size (both the number of bears and the dearth of fecal samples from Astra in the latter months of the study), as well as several external factors such as Laerke’s shifting social conditions. We expected an increased variability in Laerke’s FGMs while receiving prednisone, given the potential for cross-reaction within the EIA and the established endocrine side effects of synthetic glucocorticoids in humans [
29]. However, the influence of lower doses of phenobarbital and any epileptic episodes on FGM variability, or whether they had any detectable impact at all, is unknown.
The high amount of FGM variability was consistent across species and individuals. Jeb showed higher CVs than the three adult male grizzly bears in our previous long-term study [
10]. The variability in glucocorticoids for Astra and Laerke follow a demonstrated trend of increased variability in the FGMs of juvenile polar bears as compared to adults [
16]. The variability of FGMs can provide information on how frequently the HPA axis is activated [
35,
36]. Multiple studies have found higher FGMs in juveniles compared to adults in different species, evidence of a difference in adrenal activity, perhaps due to a higher sensitivity to external stressors [
37,
38]. Juvenile polar and grizzly bears may perhaps have higher HPA activation, due to increased activity compared to adults, or different physiological responses to external stressors and conditions. This is consistent with our GLMM results, which demonstrated a significant relationship between log-transformed FGM concentrations and individual age in weeks (as our random effect) across multiple models in all three bears. Given that this study spanned a formative developmental period, we expect that the increased activity and independence of the bears influenced this result. Continued long-term monitoring into adulthood may see this effect lessen, as has been observed in the previously mentioned studies [
37,
38].
Broadly, FGMs have been demonstrated to be influenced by variables such as sex, species, and diet [
35,
39,
40]. Our sample demographics restrict us from drawing any conclusions regarding potential sex-based differences in FGM trends. Instead, we can cautiously make some inferences about species differences, recognizing that individual differences could also underlie any apparent trends detected in our small sample size. While significantly different than the two polar bear cubs, Jeb’s FGM baselines were similar to those of three other adult male grizzly bears housed at the Detroit Zoo, individuals who were also found orphaned in the wild, when measured using the same EIA [
10], supporting a potential species-specific trend. Though no literature directly comparing the two bear species was found, previous work on FGMs in wild grizzly bears found significant differences in FGMs based on season and dietary differences, but not sex or age [
13]. In other mammals, increased dietary fiber was associated with greater fecal cortisol metabolite concentrations [
41]. However, in the aforementioned von der Ohe et al. [
13] study, this relationship was not so clear, as wild grizzlies that consumed relatively high-fiber diets had the lowest FGMs measured when the greatest number of samples were analyzed. While the relationship between dietary fiber and FGMs in ursids is unclear, the dissimilar diets of these two species, and how their diets changed over time, may nevertheless be influential factors in their respective overall FGM measurements. Therefore, we cautiously suspect that this finding reflects a difference between the two species of bear, resulting either from innate biological differences, their divergent diets, or both.
Shepherdson, Carlstead, and Wielebnowski [
24] evaluated variability in polar bear FGMs, finding that bears with higher FGM CVs demonstrated less stereotypic behavior, suggesting that healthy adrenal reactivity could be associated with a more positive behavioral profile. Our results for Laerke partially support this observation, as she did demonstrate decreased FGMs with higher rates of social positive behaviors. However, we also observed decreased FGMs with increased time spent in abnormal or stereotypic behaviors. While this may appear to be in direct conflict with results from Shepherdson and colleagues [
24], Laerke’s abnormal behavior was primarily a suckling behavior rather than other typical stereotypies in bears such as pacing or head-swinging. We suspect this behavior served a self-soothing purpose, resulting in the observed decreases in FGM concentrations. Laerke also demonstrated a negative relationship between FGMs and social positive behaviors, suggesting that positive social interactions may have mitigated adverse responses to other stressors, a trend observed in adult grizzly bears housed at the Detroit Zoo [
10].
Broom and Johnson [
42] observed positive correlations between FGMs and active behaviors such as swimming and locomotion. Astra demonstrated the same relationship between FGMs and locomotion, as well as a trending relationship with swimming. However, our results for Laerke and Jeb showed that their FGMs decreased when they engaged in swimming behaviors. This may be due to limitations in our behavioral dataset or obstruction of results by the shifting medical and social conditions experienced by Laerke and Jeb. In addition, a previous study found a positive correlation between FGMs and increasing temperatures in adult polar bears but not in juveniles [
16]. They hypothesized that juveniles have additional physiological challenges compared to adults that impact adrenal cortisol production. We found similar results in that there was no relationship between FGMs and temperature for Laerke and Jeb, and Astra’s positive relationship between FGMs and temperature had a relatively negligible effect size. In addition to small sample sizes, there was limited variation in FGMs for Astra, and shifting conditions for Laerke could have masked temperature results, making direct comparisons between studies difficult. However, continuing to explore potential differential responses to temperatures between adult and juvenile polar bears could be useful.
Laerke and Jeb showed different physiological reactions to their social separation, in that Jeb demonstrated significantly higher FGMs following the separation, while Laerke’s FGMs were significantly lower. This is supported by results from both bears’ proximity models. Jeb demonstrated decreased FGM concentrations with increased time spent within one meter of Laerke. The proximity results for Laerke appear to be more conflicting, in that she had higher FGM concentrations while both less than five meters and greater than five meters from Jeb. However, this could be a result of how their relationship changed over time. When they were introduced, it appeared to be beneficial for her to have a social companion. After several months, Jeb grew larger than Laerke, resulting in more social negative interactions and thus potentially higher FGMs while they were closer together. For both grizzly and polar bears, dispersal typically occurs around two and half years of age, demonstrating that bear cubs of both species would not typically be solitary at under two years of age [
43,
44]. While Jeb and Laerke showed opposite FGM trends between social conditions, it is difficult to attribute their responses solely to the separation, as there could also be influences of seasonality or activity level. In addition, Laerke was given more opportunities to interact with conspecifics than Jeb. Although these were inconsistent and opportunistic at this stage of Laerke’s development, perhaps she experienced less social isolation than Jeb. Social isolation has been found to impact FGMs [
45]. Maybe the different responses to their separation were also influenced by the ability to have even limited social interaction. Due to the small sample size and number of confounding variables in the dataset (age, temperature, crowd size), we acknowledge these findings are preliminary and warrant further investigation. Still, the physiological results of Jeb and Laerke for social proximity and behavior may suggest that access to a social partner was beneficial to their development, regardless of species.
Overall, the information presented here contributes to the growing body of knowledge on polar and grizzly bear development. In particular, the FGM measurements, in conjunction with other contextual data, provide novel insights into the physiological state of these ursids during key developmental stages. We found physiological evidence of the value of social companionship in early development for both polar and grizzly bears. Despite the difference in rearing, Astra and Laerke (once she was weaned off medication) demonstrated similar FGM baselines and ranges, which could be the results of a species-typical FGM range or could be due to canalization. We also found evidence that certain behaviors, such as social positive interactions, demonstrated by polar bears could be coping behaviors for stressors. With such a scarcity of available data, these findings may be greatly beneficial in aiding animal care professionals with challenging management decisions, while simultaneously helping to inform future research efforts.