Next Article in Journal
Epidemiology of Traumatic Tusk Fractures of Managed Elephants in North America, South America, Europe, Asia and Australia
Previous Article in Journal
Acknowledgment to Reviewers of Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens in 2021
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Do Birds of a Feather Always Flock Together? Assessing Differences in Group and Individual Zoo Enclosure Usage by Comparing Commonly Available Methods

1
University Centre Sparsholt, Sparsholt, Westley Lane, Winchester SO21 2NF, UK
2
College of Social Sciences & International Studies, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4 4QG, UK
3
Centre for Research in Animal Behaviour, Psychology, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4 4QG, UK
4
Wildfowl & Wetlands Trust (WWT), Slimbridge Wetland Centre, Slimbridge GL2 7BT, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2022, 3(1), 71-88; https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg3010007
Submission received: 10 January 2022 / Revised: 11 February 2022 / Accepted: 18 February 2022 / Published: 23 February 2022

Abstract

:
Data on zoo enclosure usage provide meaningful evaluation of husbandry and welfare but for social species, group-level data may not capture individual occupancy preferences. Determining zone occupancy using group data may give an inaccurate assessment of enclosure suitability for each individual. We compared three formulae (traditional and modified Spread of Participation Index and Electivity Index) to determine how estimations of space usage compare between individuals and their group overall. Two flamingo flocks at two zoos were sampled with enclosures separated into discrete zones. Counts of where each bird in the flock was located at each minute of sampling were compared against the number of minutes that randomly selected individual flamingos spent in each zone. Overall, there was little difference in preferred zone occupancy when flock data were compared to individual data. Group data suggested that flamingos were more consistent in their enclosure usage, with individual data showing wider overall usage of enclosures. Individual Electivity Index (EI) values suggested zone underuse whereas group EI suggested zone overuse. As a proxy for welfare measurement, we recommend both group-level and individual monitoring of space use to provide a complete picture of how individuals within a social group occupy their space and choose where to be.

1. Introduction

Zoo enclosures do not fully meet the needs of wild animals [1] meaning that animals cannot experience the habitat structures or features that they would in the wild. Appropriate territory or home range areas and specific biotic conditions that are required for the performance of adaptive behaviour patterns can be hard to replicate in captivity [2,3], limiting activity patterns or behavioural outputs [4,5]. Limits on activity patterns often correspond to the amount of biologically relevant space provided in the enclosure since a limited amount of useable space can reduce the range of behaviours an animal can perform [6,7]. Understanding how environmental conditions specifically influence an animal’s responses can be achieved through the monitoring of behaviour and occupancy of space [8,9]. Assessment of daily activity patterns within the different spatial zones provided is an important tool that can (in part) infer welfare states [3,9,10]. Such data collection enables detailed behavioural profiles to be created, which in turn enable our understanding of what an animal gains and needs from its immediate environment [11]. This eventually allows for the creation of enclosures tailored to the behavioural and biological priorities of species or individual animals [12]. Identification of positive welfare outputs can be inferred from enclosure use studies that observe an animal occupying multiple zones throughout the enclosure and that document specific zones being actively avoided [13].
Different research approaches can be applied to understand enclosure use and inform future improvements, including multi-zoo comparison studies [14] and post-occupancy evaluation of behaviour and space use across conditions [15,16]. To further improve the helpfulness of these empirical studies it is important to expand the researcher’s understanding of whether certain methods of measuring space usage (especially those that consider the biological relevance of enclosure zones) are more appropriate under certain conditions, such as for social species where usage of an enclosure will be influenced by ties to the group overall. Distribution of flamingos across their enclosure for example, be they standing as a tight group in one location or loosely associating in smaller cohorts across numerous enclosure areas, provides information on flock level welfare [17,18], as the more enclosure space used, the more comfortable the flock is within its environment.
Three examples of empirical methods for assessing an animal’s spatial use of an enclosure are the unmodified Spread of Participation Index, SPI [19], the modified SPI, mSPI [20] and the Electivity Index, EI [21,22]. Each of these methods vary in how they calculate enclosure usage, but all ultimately provide a score that can be used to interpret how evenly enclosure zones are being used. SPI involves an enclosure being zoned into areas of equal size and the time an animal spends in each zone is recorded. Use of the unmodified SPI is preferable when an animal’s entire available space needs investigating and either resources are distributed evenly throughout each zone or enclosure complexity means assigning zones on a resource level is challenging. mSPI was developed to study enclosure use from a resource-based context, which is helpful for assessing space usage when an enclosure contains multiple zones of differing sizes and value to the animal.
Like mSPI, EI allows the assessment of use of biologically relevant zones that are uneven in size, but in this case each zone can be assessed in terms of utilisation [23]. Identification of species enclosure areas that are under-utilised and that are over-utilised allows for preferred enclosure features to be recognised (and potentially increased in availability) and provides keepers a clear idea of areas in need of improvement. These different methods can provide incredibly useful quantifiable evidence to inform animal care and enclosure design. However, despite some studies [24,25,26,27,28] there is minimal literature available on the use of such enclosure usage tools on social or colonial species, such as flock-dwelling birds.
Enclosure use studies commonly focus on individual animals or small groups and can neglect assessment of how enclosure use may vary in large, social groups where the instinct to stay with the group may be stronger than moving around independently. Social relationships that are present between individuals that live in large groups will influence behaviour, and therefore how individuals use their space. There is a need to understand how applicable and informative methods of measuring enclosure use are on social species. To address this gap in the literature, flamingos (Phoenicopteriformes) were selected as the study species. A highly social nature and large global captive population make flamingos ideal candidates to determine if current methods of enclosure use measurements are appropriate for group-living, colonial species. Research has already addressed how season and time influence flamingo activity and welfare [25]. However it is not yet clear whether measuring individual activity or the flock’s behaviour overall is the more appropriate approach for providing a clearer picture of enclosure usage, especially given the evidence that individual animals display different personalities and hence responses to environmental variables [29].
The main aim of this project was to evaluate if group level enclosure usage assessment provides useful information on individual choices (of where to be within the space provided). Whilst the influence of remaining with the main colony may be strong for individual animals of colonial or gregarious species, counting where the main group is and using this as an assessment of overall enclosure usage may mean important data on individual needs (e.g., opportunities for assessment of zone preferences) can be missed. This study aimed to determine whether the results obtained from the use of three indices (SPI, mSPI and EI) can provide comparable information on preferences for enclosure usage at the individual and group level. We aimed to understand whether certain methods are more applicable and informative when applied to group-living species and how they use their space. Measures of enclosure usage were considered alongside environmental and anthropogenic factors to capture as many potential influences as possible on how birds use their space.

2. Materials and Methods

Data collection for this study was originally undertaken for a larger project “Listening to the Zoo” funded by the Economic and Social Council (ESRC Grant ES/R009554/1) and all data were collected by the corresponding author as a postdoctoral researcher on this grant. The first author of this paper extracted all enclosure usage data from video recording of the birds made for the original project, to use these data as much as possible.

2.1. Sample Populations and Enclosure Zones

Two species of flamingo at two zoological collections were used for observational data collection. Bristol Zoo Gardens (BZ) houses greater flamingos (Phoenicopterus roseus), n = 56, in a mixed-species (several species of smaller wading birds and waterfowl), walk-through aviary. Paignton Zoo Environmental Park (PZ) houses Chilean flamingos (P. chilensis), n = 53, in a mixed-species (various species of waterfowl), open-topped enclosure with no walk-through access. Both flamingo species were fed a flamingo-specific pellet twice a day, provided in a separate feeding area of each enclosure. Husbandry routines were performed daily by keepers at each zoo but were unobtrusive and not disruptive to the bird’s general activity.
The enclosure at PZ is at the front of the zoo and is accessible to visitors as they leave the main entrance building and start their journey around the zoo. Visitors can choose to turn left, walking past the flamingos to the reptile house, or right, and away from the flamingos into a large walk-through aviary. The flamingo enclosure at BZ is also at the front of the zoo, and is one of two exhibits (the other, at the time of data collection, housed drills, Mandrillus leucophaeus) that visitors first experience as they walk into the zoo.
Each enclosure was divided into zones based on the biological relevance of space and resources to the flamingos (i.e., exhibit features that could be accessed and used by the flamingos for specific activities). These zones were measured as discrete sections of the overall enclosure area (Figure 1). The total area of each enclosure was calculated using the “draw polygon” function from Google Earth Pro v. 7.3.2.5776 (Google, 2019). Figure 1 provides information on the zone sizes within each enclosure. The number of zones was influenced by the size of dry land and water in the exhibits, with a large pool being divided into front, back, left, and right based on water depth and access for the birds.

2.2. Collecting Enclosure Usage Data

Data from each flamingo flock were recorded at five separate times per day for five days, with observations lasting 20 min each time. Video footage was collected from PZ on 15th, 26th, 29th April 2019 and the 3rd and 16th May 2019 at 10:00, 11:00, 12:00, 14:00 and 15:00 for each day. No 15:00 observation took place on 15th April due to unforeseen circumstances. Data were collected at BZ on 30 April 2019 and 4–7 May 2019 with the same observation sample times. An HD Panasonic Lumix digital camera was used to record all footage at both zoos and the camera was maintained at the same location within the enclosure for each recording session.
We distinguish between “flock” referring to the population of birds in the enclosure at the zoo, “group” referring to data collected at the group level by counting birds and “individual” referring to data collected by following individually identifiable flamingos. Group and individual bird enclosure usage data were collected from video and photographic records. For group-level enclosure usage, data were collected by counting the number of flamingos occupying each zone at one-minute sample points using an instantaneous scan sampling method [30]. To capture any birds that had moved out of view of the video camera, still photos, taken on an Honor 10 Lite smartphone, allowed the maximal number of flamingos to be recorded per sampling point. To collect individual flamingo data, each 20 min video was paused at the start of play, and flamingos were assigned numbers from left to right of the screenshot to allow for different birds to be randomly selected. A random number generator [31] was used to identify the 12 individual flamingos that would be followed (i.e., bird number 6 would be the sixth bird from the left, bird number 15 would be the fifteenth bird from the left and so on). Instantaneous focal sampling with the same one-minute intervals was used to record the enclosure use of individuals during the 20 min sampling period. If a focal bird moved out of view, the footage was reversed and replayed to relocate the individual, if possible. If it was impossible to relocate a specific individual, the footage would be restarted, and a new random number generated to recommence data collection on a different individual bird.

2.3. Calculating Enclosure Usage

We compared the difference between the SPI, the mSPI and an EI at the group and individual level to see how much the “pull” of the group influences the choice of the individual when deciding on where to be within the enclosure. Not all data points could be analysed for individual birds, as recordings from 11:00 4 May 2019 at BZ, and recordings from 15:00 26 April 2019, 15:00 29 April 2019 and 12:30 3 May 2019 at PZ did not provide clear enough footage to follow individual flamingos due to the bird’s behaviour. Here, it is specified how the equation was applied based on whether individual data (individual) or group data (group) were being inputted. Enclosure use for group data was calculated for each one-minute observation period. Enclosure use for the individual flamingos was calculated for the total 20 min observation period.

2.3.1. Traditional SPI

SPI = M (nb − na) + (Fa − Fb)/2(N − M)
where:
  • N is the total number of observations in all zones (individual) or the total number of flamingos in all zones during that sample time (group).
  • M is the mean frequency of observations (individual) or flamingos (group) per zone (i.e., N divided by the number of zones).
  • nb is the number of zones with observations (individual) or flamingos (group) less than M.
  • na is the number of zones with observations (individual) or flamingos (group) greater than M.
  • Fa is the total number of observations (individual) or flamingos (group) in all zones with observations/flamingos less than M.
  • Fb is the total number of observations (individual) or flamingos (group) in all zones with observations/flamingos greater than M.
A score between 0 and 1 is generated, with values approaching 0 suggesting that all zones are being used equally and those approaching 1 implying uneven enclosure zone occupancy, i.e., some zones are used more often than others. When the traditional SPI was applied, zones were combined into five zones of equal size, rather than broken into biological zones, to fit the requirements of the equation. For PZ this meant combining the nesting and island zones (Zone numbers 6 and 7) together and the pool on the right of the enclosure and the feeding zone together. The island and feeding area (Zone numbers 4 and 5), and the sandy area zone and nesting area zone were combined at BZ (Zone numbers 6 and 7).

2.3.2. Modified SPI

SPI = (|fo − fe|)/2(N − femin)
where:
  • N is the total number of observations (individual) or flamingos (group) in all zones.
  • fo is the observed frequency of observations (individual) or flamingos (group) in a given zone.
  • fe is the expected number of observations (individuals) or flamingos (group) in a given zone based on the zone’s size.
  • femin is the expected number of observations (individual) or flamingos (group) in the smallest zone.
The same 0 to 1 score is provided as per the traditional SPI.

2.3.3. Electivity Index

E* = (Wi − (1/n))/(Wi + (1/n)) whereby Wi = (ri/pi)/∑(ri/pi)
where:
  • ri is the observed proportion of time spent in a zone (individual) or number of flamingos (group).
  • pi is the expected time (individual) or flamingos (group) in that zone.
  • n refers to the number of zones available to the study animal.
Values are generated for each zone and can range from −1 to 1, with −1 representing a zone that is under-utilised and 1 representing an over-utilised zone. A score of 0 suggests equal usage of that zone.

2.4. Data Analysis

Data analysis was conducted in R v. 4.1.0 using RStudio v 1.4.1717 [32,33] and Minitab v 20.3 [34]. To address the problem of multiple observations taken on the same birds, differences in group SPI compared to individual SPI were assessed using repeated measures models run in R Studio using the “lmerTest” package [35] for each flock of flamingos in turn. Each model included time of day as a fixed factor and date as a random factor. Model fit was assessed using plots of standardised residuals using the “plot(model name)” function in RStudio, in addition to the assessment of the calculated r2 value using the “MuMin” package. To remove any predictors that may covary, the “car” package [36] was used to calculate a variance inflation factor (VIF). Any predictors with a VIF >2 were removed from the model.
To evaluate overall daily variation in individual SPI compared to daily variation in group SPI, one-sample sign tests were run in Minitab. This test assessed differences in mSPI and SPI for both group and individual enclosure measurements at each zoo with median group m/SPI values calculated for comparison against individual m/SPIs for each condition at each zoo.
The same RStudio packages were used to determine differences in group and individual EI for each zoo. Due to the multiple running of the model for each enclosure zone, the “anova(model name)” function was run, and significance calculated using Satterthwaite’s method. For these models, the outcome variable was the calculated EI for each observation for that specific enclosure zone, with time of day and group/individual data included as predictors. Date was included as a random factor. Due to the comparisons of multiple p values, a corrected significance level was calculated using the Benjamini–Hochberg technique to avoid false discovery [37] and the resulting Q values examined for any significant difference.
Any difference in time spent within each zone for the group and the individual birds was analysed using one-factor chi-squared tests run in Minitab. Significant occupancy of a zone across all time periods and avoidance of a zone was also analysed using cross-tabulation and chi-squared testing in Minitab. Again, comparison of multiple p values was undertaken using a Benjamini and Hochberg [37] correction to avoid false discovery.

3. Results

3.1. Comparing m/SPI Outputs When Using Group versus Individual Data

At PZ, there was a significant difference between the SPI for the individual birds and those values calculated for the group, with individual SPI being higher than for group data (model estimate = 0.13; SE = 0.021; df = 362.9; t value = 6.48; p < 0.001). There was no significant influence of time of day on the bird’s enclosure use at group or individual level. The same relationship is noted for the mSPI, with no effect of time of day but showing individual mSPI values to be significantly higher than those for the group (model estimate = 0.114; SE = 0.014; df = 361.8; t value = 7.99; p < 0.001).
For BZ, enclosure usage calculation (group or individual) and time of day was significantly different for SPI and mSPI, with enclosure usage becoming less varied up to early afternoon (Figure 2). However, the same pattern holds true as seen at PZ where individual m/SPI results are higher than for those of the group (SPI model estimate = 0.067; SE = 0.011; df = 778.3; t value = 6.328; p < 0.001/mSPI model estimate = 0.086; SE = 0.0081; df = 778.3; t value = 10.69; p < 0.001).
The output from a one-sample sign test showed that when comparing the BZ mSPI group median (SPI = 0.71) to the individual mSPI values, more individual mSPI values were higher than the group median (214/288). Similarly, significantly more individual mSPI values from PZ (223 out of 252) were higher than the group mSPI median value (SPI = 0.6068). When comparing SPI values from BZ, significantly more individual SPI values were higher (190 out of 288) than the group median value (SPI = 0.73). At PZ, significantly more individual SPI values were higher (185 out of 252) than the group median value (SPI = 0.73).
Although there were fewer zones included in the SPI calculation and a wider range of zone sizes included in the mSPI calculation, Figure 2 shows a similar pattern of zone occupancy for the SPI and mSPI data, for both group and individual birds.
When considering variation in calculated SPI using the unmodified and modified versions, Figure 3 shows that the number of zones included in data collection and the characteristics of the zoo’s specific animal population will influence the difference between the index calculated from the unmodified and modified formula at the individual or group level. However, for each zoo, there is more variation apparent when individual animal SPI calculations are used compared to those calculated from the group data.
Figure 4 identifies different patterns of enclosure usage by individual birds at each observation point compared to the group observation. This is particularly evident for the Left Pool at PZ and the Waterfall at BZ, where individual bird preferences for occupancy of these zones appears to be linked to specific times of the day. No equal occupancy of enclosure zones for each time period is noted for either group or individual data.

3.2. Evaluation of the EI

Flamingos at both zoos were under-utilising a range of the zones in their enclosures (Figure 5), with only the back pool at PZ and the sanded beach area at BZ (but only for group data) showing an overuse. No equal usage of enclosure zones was demonstrated from these EIs, in a similar pattern to the picture provided from SPI data (Figure 2).
A repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant difference (using Satterthwaite’s method) in EI between group and individual data at PZ for use of the feeding area (F1, 722.14 = 67.39; p < 0.001; Q = 0.008*), the back of the pool (F1, 722.21 = 7.63; p = 0.006; Q = 0.05), the front of the pool (F1, 722.24 = 16.69; p < 0.001; Q = 0.017), the left side of the pool (F1, 722.33 = 18.42; p < 0.001; Q = 0.03), the island (F1, 722.08 = 18.71; p < 0.001; Q = 0.033) and the nesting area (F1, 722.53 = 66.17; p < 0.001; Q = 0.042). The r2 value for this model was 22%. All VIF values were below 2. Multiple p values were compared to a corrected significance level (however this did not differ from the original of 0.05) and new Q values are provided. Significant Q values are marked with *.
For BZ, there was a significant difference in EI between group and individual data for the main pool (F1, 779.36 = 4.67; p = 0.031; Q = 0.043*), the waterfall (F1, 779.01 = 25.44; p < 0.001; Q = 0.007*), the channel between pool and house (F1, 778.34 = 10.04; p = 0.002; Q = 0.029*), the feeding area (F1, 778.76 = 8.35; p = 0.004; Q = 0.036*), the island (F1, 782 = 18.28; p < 0.001; Q = 0.014*) and the sanded beach (F1, 778.47 = 281.31; p < 0.001; Q = 0.02*). There was no significant difference in group or individual EI for the nesting area (F1, 778.68 = 3.22; p = 0.07; Q = 0.05). Multiple p values were compared to a corrected significance level of 0.043 and new Q values are provided. Significant Q values are marked with *.
Although median group EI values mirror those from individual birds over time, there are some variations in the choice of zone usage between the individual bird and the group (Figure 6). For example, back pool use in PZ is less favoured by specific individual flamingos in the early afternoon compared to that calculated for the group overall. Although pool usage for both individuals and the group overall is very consistent at BZ, differences over time in the usage of the nesting area and sanded beach area are more apparent. Individual birds are less likely to favour these areas compared to the flock overall.

3.3. Time Spent in Each Zone

Figure 7 shows that the mean time spent in specific enclosure zones by the group and individual birds can be different, but only for the nesting zone at PZ is the difference in occupancy significant (χ2 = 3.99; N = 17.22; df = 1; p = 0.046).
Overall, Table 1 indicates that the largest number of birds seen within a specific zone corresponds to the maximum observation time per individual sample of a bird in that same zone, although variation in preferred zone occupancy at the individual level is apparent for times when the majority of the group is in a different enclosure zone to that chosen by the individual bird.
Table 2 shows that these observations of flamingos within a preferred zone or outside of an avoided zone were significantly consistent for both the group and individual data, suggesting that specific enclosure zones hold particular importance for the birds within each zoo and this should be further investigated. When not including times of day, individual and group choice of preferred enclosure zone usage/zone avoidance is the same.

4. Discussion

This study utilised a range of commonly available space use indices to compare and contrast results on enclosure zone occupancy for individual flamingos and the flock these individuals belonged to. Although calculated metrics of enclosure usage differ (Figure 2 and Figure 6), average occupancy times within specific enclosure zones can be similar for group and individual data (Table 1), but occupancy differs across time of day. For social species, it is evident that measuring the enclosure occupancy at the group level misses data on individual choices in enclosure usage, and therefore limits any evaluation of why this may be occurring and inferences on underlying welfare states. Time of day influences individual enclosure zone occupancy, with individual birds partitioning their time within each enclosure zone differently compared to rates of group-level occupancy. Individual differences in motivation for specific behaviour patterns (e.g., differing degrees of hunger, or the need to bathe and preen) may account for this. Choosing to socialise with particular conspecifics [24,38] or to regulate internal physiology [39], or move away from visitor presence [26,40] or from the presence of other species in the enclosure [41], will also cause individual specific patterns in enclosure use.
Individual SPI and mSPI are generally higher than those calculated for the group overall (Figure 2), and therefore group-wide m/SPI suggests more even enclosure usage than when following individual birds. This is likely to be influenced by the behaviour of specific birds, who choose to move away from the main flock to perform specific behaviours at certain times of the day. For example, non-breeding birds may not join in with reproductive behaviours and spend time in a quieter area of their enclosure. To provide a complete picture of the enclosure usage of social species in the future, the number of animals per zone or the time spent within a zone should be evaluated alongside the calculated overall enclosure usage index to see where each member of the group is at each sample point. Time of day and season may be especially important to assess the EI. For example, the nesting flamingos at BZ may be the reason for wider variation between individual and group EI results for several zones in this enclosure (Figure 5). Birds may be deliberately moving away from the nesting area when they have completed their incubation duties (handing over to a partner) or non-breeding birds are actively avoiding areas where aggressive, breeding adults are situated. Consideration of animal physiological states and how these fluctuate seasonally and temporally is key to understanding the meaning behind enclosure usage data, especially if modifications to an exhibit are planned to improve the diversity of space use after observational data have been collected.
How data are compared or analysed also needs to be considered, e.g., analysed across the entire study period or by specific observation points, as results can differ based on where the animals are and why. For example, overall mean enclosure usage data for each zoo reveals limited differences in time spent in each zone between individuals and the group (Figure 7), but when analysing by sample point, key differences emerge in how the birds are occupying their space at the individual level compared to as a flock (Figure 5).
The number of zones constructed within an enclosure can also influence the overall mSPI scores calculated. Previous research on flamingos has identified that too much complexity in enclosure zoning can impact the reliability of enclosure occupancy indices [25], and evenness of enclosure occupancy will differ, based on mSPI calculation, when zones are combined and fewer zones are considered [26]. Consideration of biological relevance when defining what constitutes a zone is clearly very important [20], but is even more important when assessing individual enclosure usage for species with a strong tie to a social group. This will enable the researcher to distinguish between an animal choosing to occupy a zone based on its own preferences compared to when it may be motivated to be with the majority of its social group. We have shown, in this study, that individuals will deviate from the main social group when moving around an enclosure and when choosing to occupy specific enclosure zones. Therefore, to fully comprehend an individual animal’s welfare state, individual enclosure usage indices should be evaluated alongside group data. The number of zones has been shown to be important when managing behavioural interaction during times of social disruption for socially living species [42]. Therefore, assessing where a group spends the majority of its time and also observing where specific individuals differ from this can help when managing social animals during times of new introductions.
Despite differences in the number and size of zones used for the SPI and mSPI calculation, overall patterns of zone occupancy are similar within each zoo (Figure 2). However, when comparing different SPI formulae, more variation is apparent when using individual data (Figure 3) compared to that seen for the group. Consideration of the biological importance of enclosure zoning is also essential when deciding on which method to use for calculating zone occupancy (SPI or mSPI). Exhibits with diverse and complex features, and where limited or valued resources may cause individuals to congregate in specific areas, are best evaluated using the mSPI [20]. In these flamingo enclosures, we divided each exhibit into areas that are likely to be used by the birds for specific behaviours (e.g., the island for roosting and loafing, the pool for bathing and wading), and whilst there is some reflection in the pattern of enclosure usage across time between the SPI and mSPI (Figure 2), calculated indices are significantly different when applying the SPI or mSPI to this enclosure. Therefore, consideration of the enclosure complexity and what is causing individuals to use specific areas needs to take place before data are collected and before any space use index is calculated. This is important for social species specifically, as resources that are limited or highly valued may cause overuse of specific areas and concentrate interactions that may affect how other areas of the enclosure can be used at certain times of the day.
Differences in the season that the birds were studied may also account for the variation seen between flocks, in addition to the characteristics of the m/SPI and EI calculations. As the BZ flock was in the breeding season, there may be more profound differences in enclosure zone occupancy between certain individuals—those invested in nesting and those not breeding—despite attempts to randomly select birds. For PZ, flamingos observed during the run-up to nesting, where the flock was performing group courtship display, may have a more uniform enclosure usage as the majority of birds were seen displaying in the same area of their enclosure. This may explain the similarity between the group and individual PZ data (difference between SPI and mSPI) in Figure 3, compared to the wider degree of variation noted for BZ. To further understand the effects of seasonality on flamingo enclosure usage, flocks could be sampled (using all of the methods included in this paper) at different times of the year. It may be that some forms of enclosure usage calculation are better suited to estimating zone occupancy in the breeding season and others are better for the non-breeding season.
The number of flocks sampled (N = 2) for this research, including no repeats for either species, should also be considered in light of the wider application of findings and how representative they may be of flamingos more broadly. Species and enclosure effects may be causing the changes in enclosure usage, but also the randomness associated with small sample sizes. Multi-zoo study across further flocks of these two flamingo species would enable analysis of how these birds were using their space in comparison to other captive groups (especially if occupancy of standardised “habitat areas”, i.e., identifying the same accessible resources within flamingo exhibits, were evaluated across different enclosures).
More detailed analysis of how favoured zone occupancy was affected by time of day would be helpful. It may be that specific aspects of husbandry or individual preferences for different parts of the enclosure are aligned to behavioural circadian rhythms. These differences may be more pronounced in individuals but diluted when the group is observed collectively. Using a larger individual dataset to understand enclosure usage may help better control the large amounts of variation noted in some analysis (e.g., in the error bars on Figure 6). Further research should also investigate how individual and group results change over extended periods of time to confirm that enclosure usage is dependent upon season or time of year (for example), and to fully determine if methods such as m/SPI and EI can fully illustrate these influences. For instance, a relevant project extension would be to determine whether BZ results would show more uniformity outside of breeding season and more similarity to those at PZ. These data may help improve husbandry routines to ensure seasonally biologically important zones are appropriate and appealing for the species based on time of year. As determining the effect of outside influences on flamingo enclosure usage can be complex [26], a larger dataset would help to unpick other external factors that have caused differences in the enclosure usage of the group compared to the individual bird.
Finally, consideration of visitor presence and potential associated impacts, in addition to the presence of caregivers [26,43]—both factors that are shown to influence zoo bird behaviour and space use—should be measured in space occupancy studies as some individual animals may (at specific times of their development or temporal cycle) be more impacted by anthropogenic activities and behaviour than at others.

5. Conclusions

This study has helped identify how these methods for evaluating enclosure zone occupancy can be applied and adapted to ensure that research on socially living species is as informative as possible. When measuring enclosure usage in the zoo or aquarium, the specific population of animals, their prevailing environmental and husbandry conditions, and physiological, temporal and seasonal effects on activity, are all likely to influence how space within their enclosure is utilised and occupied. It is clear from this research that group-level enclosure usage calculation does not provide an accurate picture of what the individual is choosing to do. Therefore, for group-living animals, assessment of space usage and zone occupancy needs to include the elements of individual difference and individual choice.
We recommend further study and investigation into enclosure usage indices, especially SPI, to determine ways of adapting such methods to successfully capture the group’s influence on the individual, in addition to the individual’s role in group-level decisions. For individual animals, review of time spent in the enclosure’s zones (as a proportion of a given sampling period) may be the most valuable data points when understanding individual differences in space use, and therefore individual choice in space usage that may differ from where the main part of the focal animal’s group is located. Although m/SPI or EI may be useful overall calculations to determine where an animal is, actual occupancy time may be more helpful for evaluating what is causing the individual to use that space or resources more (or less) than the other areas that it has access to (and how or why this differs from what the group overall is doing).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.R. and T.R.; methodology, P.R.; software, P.R.; validation, P.R.; formal analysis, H.M. and P.R.; investigation, P.R. and H.M.; resources, T.R.; data curation, H.M. and P.R.; writing—original draft preparation, H.M. and P.R.; writing—review and editing, P.R., J.B. and T.R.; visualization, P.R.; supervision, P.R. and J.B.; project administration, T.R., P.R. and H.M.; funding acquisition, T.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Whilst this paper received no external funding, the original data used for this new analysis was collected as part of the Listening to the Zoo Project, supported by the Economic and Social Research Council, grant number ES/R009554/1.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Methods were scrutinised by members of the EASE group at the University of Exeter in October 2018. Finalised data collection methods were submitted to and reviewed by the research teams and ethics committees of the two zoos involved on 5 November 2018 and 22 November 2018 respectively. Feedback was provided prior to data collection commencing in April 2019.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available upon reasonable request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

Thanks go to Holly Farmer of Paignton Zoo and Fay Clark formally of Bristol Zoo, for their help in providing access to the animals. Thanks go to the bird departments at each zoo for their help in providing information on the animal populations and their husbandry and management.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Melfi, V.A. There are big gaps in our knowledge, and thus approach, to zoo animal welfare: A case for evidence-based zoo animal management. Zoo Biol. 2009, 28, 574–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Mason, G.J.; Burn, C.C.; Dallaire, J.A.; Kroshko, J.; Kinkaid, H.M.; Jeschke, J.M. Plastic animals in cages: Behavioural flexibility and responses to captivity. Anim. Behav. 2013, 85, 1113–1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Mallapur, A.; Waran, N.; Sinha, A. Use of enclosure space by captive lion-tailed macaques (Macaca silenus) housed in Indian zoos. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2005, 8, 175–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Morgan, K.N.; Tromborg, C.T. Sources of stress in captivity. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 262–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Browning, H.; Maple, T.L. Developing a metric of usable space for zoo exhibits. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Hosey, G.R. How does the zoo environment affect the behaviour of captive primates? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005, 90, 107–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Ross, S.R.; Schapiro, S.J.; Hau, J.; Lukas, K.E. Space use as an indicator of enclosure appropriateness: A novel measure of captive animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 121, 42–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Hunter, S.C.; Gusset, M.; Miller, L.J.; Somers, M.J. Space use as an indicator of enclosure appropriateness in African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2014, 17, 98–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Rose, P.E.; Robert, R. Evaluating the activity patterns and enclosure usage of a little-studied zoo species, the sitatunga (Tragelaphus spekii). J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2013, 1, 14–19. [Google Scholar]
  10. Kroshko, J.; Clubb, R.; Harper, L.; Mellor, E.; Moehrenschlager, A.; Mason, G.J. Stereotypic route tracing in captive Carnivora is predicted by species-typical home range sizes and hunting styles. Anim. Behav. 2016, 117, 197–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Stamp Dawkins, M. Through animal eyes: What behaviour tells us. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006, 100, 4–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Price, E.E.; Stoinski, T.S. Group size: Determinants in the wild and implications for the captive housing of wild mammals in zoos. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 103, 255–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Troxell-Smith, S.M.; Whelani, C.J.; Magle, S.B.; Brown, S. Zoo foraging ecology: Development and assessment of a welfare tool for captive animals. Anim. Welf. 2017, 26, 265–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Marshall, A.R.; Deere, N.J.; Little, H.A.; Snipp, R.; Goulder, J.; Mayer-Clarke, S. Husbandry and enclosure influences on penguin behavior and conservation breeding. Zoo Biol. 2016, 35, 385–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. White, B.C.; Houser, L.A.; Fuller, J.A.; Taylor, S.; Elliott, J.L. Activity-based exhibition of five mammalian species: Evaluation of behavioral changes. Zoo Biol. 2003, 22, 269–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Little, K.A.; Sommer, V. Change of enclosure in langur monkeys: Implications for the evaluation of environmental enrichment. Zoo Biol. 2002, 21, 549–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rose, P.E.; Croft, D.P.; Lee, R. A review of captive flamingo (P hoenicopteridae) welfare: A synthesis of current knowledge and future directions. Int. Zoo Yearb. 2014, 48, 139–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. King, C.E. A hypothetical husbandry point system for breeding flamingos in captivity. Flamingo 2008, 16, 57–61. [Google Scholar]
  19. Dickens, M. A statistical formula to quantify the "spread-of-participation" in group discussion. Speech Monogr. 1955, 22, 28–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Plowman, A.B. A note on a modification of the spread of participation index allowing for unequal zones. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2003, 83, 331–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Vanderploeg, H.A.; Scavia, D. Calculation and use of selectivity coefficients of feeding: Zooplankton grazing. Ecol. Model. 1979, 7, 135–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Vanderploeg, H.A.; Scavia, D. Two electivity indices for feeding with special reference to zooplankton grazing. J. Fish. Board Can. 1979, 36, 362–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ross, S.R.; Calcutt, S.; Schapiro, S.J.; Hau, J. Space use selectivity by chimpanzees and gorillas in an indoor–outdoor enclosure. Am. J. Primatol. 2011, 73, 197–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Rose, P.E. Evaluating the behaviour of Andean Flamingos Phoenicoparrus andinus and James’s Flamingos P. jamesi in captivity: Comparing species and flocks using multiple methods. Wildfowl 2019, 69, 70–92. [Google Scholar]
  25. Rose, P.E.; Brereton, J.E.; Croft, D.P. Measuring welfare in captive flamingos: Activity patterns and exhibit usage in zoo-housed birds. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018, 205, 115–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Rose, P.E.; Badman-King, A.; Hurn, S.; Rice, T. Visitor presence and a changing soundscape, alongside environmental parameters, can predict enclosure usage in captive flamingos. Zoo Biol. 2021, 40, 363–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Brereton, J.E.; Fryer, J.; Rose, P.E. Understanding sociality and behavior change associated with a nesting event in a captive flock of great white pelicans. Zoo Biol. 2021, 40, 386–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Williams, E.; Carter, A.; Rendle, J.; Ward, S.J. Understanding impacts of zoo visitors: Quantifying behavioural changes of two popular zoo species during COVID-19 closures. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021, 236, 105253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. McCully, F.; Croft, D.P.; Lee, R.; Tovey, P.; Roberts, M.; Rose, P.E. Personality as a characteristic of social position in captive flamingo flocks. In Proceedings of the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour Winter Meeting, London, UK, 4–5 December 2014. [Google Scholar]
  30. Bateson, M.; Martin, P. Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide, 4th ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  31. Furey, E. Random Number Generator. Available online: https://www.calculatorsoup.com (accessed on 12 October 2021).
  32. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Available online: http://www.rstudio.com (accessed on 10 July 2021).
  33. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  34. Minitab Statistical Software; Minitab 20; Minitab, Inc.: State College, PA, USA, 2021; Available online: www.minitab.com (accessed on 9 January 2022).
  35. Kuznetsova, A.; Brockhoff, P.B.; Christensen, R.H.B. lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 82, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Koster, J.; McElreath, R. Multinomial analysis of behavior: Statistical methods. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2017, 71, 138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Benjamini, Y.; Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 1995, 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Forthman, D.L.; Bakeman, R. Environmental and social influences on enclosure use and activity patterns of captive sloth bears (Ursus ursinus). Zoo Biol. 1992, 11, 405–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Wark, J.D.; Wierzal, N.K.; Cronin, K.A. Mapping shade availability and use in zoo environments: A tool for evaluating thermal comfort. Animals 2020, 10, 1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Learmonth, M.J.; Sherwen, S.L.; Hemsworth, P.H. The effects of zoo visitors on Quokka (Setonix brachyurus) avoidance behavior in a walk-through exhibit. Zoo Biol. 2018, 37, 223–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Rendle, J.A.J.; Ward, S.; McCormick, W.D. Behaviour and enclosure use of captive parma wallabies (Macropus parma): An assessment of compatibility within a mixed-species exhibit. J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2018, 6, 63–68. [Google Scholar]
  42. Herrelko, E.S.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M.; Vick, S.-J. Perception of available space during chimpanzee introductions: Number of accessible areas is more important than enclosure size. Zoo Biol. 2015, 34, 397–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Rose, P.E.; Scales, J.S.; Brereton, J.E. Why the “visitor effect” is complicated. Unraveling individual animal, visitor number, and climatic influences on behavior, space use and interactions with keepers- a case study on captive hornbills. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 7, 236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Approximate enclosure sizes and zones within each exhibit for the two zoos included in the study (not to scale). The red cross denotes the position of the observer. White areas of the image are all outside the flamingo enclosure. Only flamingo houses and numbered zones were accessible to the birds. Figure taken from Rose et al. [25].
Figure 1. Approximate enclosure sizes and zones within each exhibit for the two zoos included in the study (not to scale). The red cross denotes the position of the observer. White areas of the image are all outside the flamingo enclosure. Only flamingo houses and numbered zones were accessible to the birds. Figure taken from Rose et al. [25].
Jzbg 03 00007 g001
Figure 2. Relationship between daily mean indices (+/− SE) for group data and individual bird data when calculated using the traditional and modified SPI equations. PZ flamingos (top) and BZ flamingos (bottom). When applied to group data, both the SPI and mSPI suggest a wider range of enclosure zone occupancy, although this is highly influenced by time of day.
Figure 2. Relationship between daily mean indices (+/− SE) for group data and individual bird data when calculated using the traditional and modified SPI equations. PZ flamingos (top) and BZ flamingos (bottom). When applied to group data, both the SPI and mSPI suggest a wider range of enclosure zone occupancy, although this is highly influenced by time of day.
Jzbg 03 00007 g002
Figure 3. The difference between the SPI and mSPI calculated for group and individual data points from each zoo’s flamingo flock.
Figure 3. The difference between the SPI and mSPI calculated for group and individual data points from each zoo’s flamingo flock.
Jzbg 03 00007 g003
Figure 4. Occupancy of each enclosure zone for PZ group data (top left) and individual birds (top right) and BZ group data (bottom left) and individual birds (bottom right) for each observation time.
Figure 4. Occupancy of each enclosure zone for PZ group data (top left) and individual birds (top right) and BZ group data (bottom left) and individual birds (bottom right) for each observation time.
Jzbg 03 00007 g004
Figure 5. Interval plot of 95% confidence interval of the mean including standard deviation for PZ flamingos (top) and BZ flamingos (bottom). Values above 0 suggest overuse of an enclosure zone and values below 0 suggest underuse of an enclosure. Values of 0 suggest equal usage of a zone. More variation is noted in individual EI data for each zoo compared to the group EI data.
Figure 5. Interval plot of 95% confidence interval of the mean including standard deviation for PZ flamingos (top) and BZ flamingos (bottom). Values above 0 suggest overuse of an enclosure zone and values below 0 suggest underuse of an enclosure. Values of 0 suggest equal usage of a zone. More variation is noted in individual EI data for each zoo compared to the group EI data.
Jzbg 03 00007 g005
Figure 6. Comparing group-level median EI against individual median EI for PZ (top) and BZ (bottom) over time of day for each zone within the flamingo enclosures. Median EI values are from −1 to +1 and these are plotted with standard deviations, calculated from all data points for each time period in each zone, to show variation around each average value.
Figure 6. Comparing group-level median EI against individual median EI for PZ (top) and BZ (bottom) over time of day for each zone within the flamingo enclosures. Median EI values are from −1 to +1 and these are plotted with standard deviations, calculated from all data points for each time period in each zone, to show variation around each average value.
Jzbg 03 00007 g006
Figure 7. Comparison of time seen within each zone for group data (blue bars) and individual data (orange bars) for PZ flamingos (top) and BZ flamingos (bottom).
Figure 7. Comparison of time seen within each zone for group data (blue bars) and individual data (orange bars) for PZ flamingos (top) and BZ flamingos (bottom).
Jzbg 03 00007 g007
Table 1. Time of day compared to sum of individuals/time per individual within each zone for the PZ and BZ flamingo flocks. Shaded cells indicate the largest number of birds seen in a zone at that observation time and the maximum amount of time spent within a zone by an individual flamingo.
Table 1. Time of day compared to sum of individuals/time per individual within each zone for the PZ and BZ flamingo flocks. Shaded cells indicate the largest number of birds seen in a zone at that observation time and the maximum amount of time spent within a zone by an individual flamingo.
PZ
TimeDataFeedingNestingIslandRightLeftFrontBack
10amGroup1483181436
Individual1083031344
11amGroup16133571325
Individual1304120242
12noonGroup18143671222
Individual1105230331
2pmGroup11144734814
Individual8105600519
3pmGroup7164943515
Individual607000222
BZ
TimeDataMainSandNestsIslandFeedingChannelWaterfall
10amGroup51846121153
Individual714362013100
11amGroup330469651h
Individual63230138111
12noonGroup531495550
Individual6363675100
2pmGroup434503540
Individual333388990
3pmGroup528543541
Individual7293997101
Table 2. Significant differences (marked with *) in overall zone occupancy (across all time periods) for individual and group data for least and most occupied zones at each zoo. Multiple p values compared to a Benjamini and Hochberg correction [37].
Table 2. Significant differences (marked with *) in overall zone occupancy (across all time periods) for individual and group data for least and most occupied zones at each zoo. Multiple p values compared to a Benjamini and Hochberg correction [37].
ZooZone OccupancySubjectChi-Square OutputQ Value
BZNests Most GroupX2 (4, 13717) = 141.17; p < 0.0010.0063 *
Waterfall Least GroupX2 (4, 272) = 79.47; p < 0.0010.013 *
Nests MostIndividual X2 (4, 2051) = 31.01; p < 0.0010.019 *
Waterfall LeastIndividual X2 (4, 20) = 20.5; p < 0.0010.025 *
PZ Island MostGroupX2 (4, 9954) = 2198.13; p < 0.0010.031 *
Left PoolLeast GroupX2 (4, 527) = 279.86; p < 0.0010.038 *
Island Most IndividualX2 (4, 2321) = 673.54; p < 0.0010.044 *
Left PoolLeastIndividual X2 (4, 15) = 28.67; p < 0.0010.05 *
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

McConnell, H.; Brereton, J.; Rice, T.; Rose, P. Do Birds of a Feather Always Flock Together? Assessing Differences in Group and Individual Zoo Enclosure Usage by Comparing Commonly Available Methods. J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2022, 3, 71-88. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg3010007

AMA Style

McConnell H, Brereton J, Rice T, Rose P. Do Birds of a Feather Always Flock Together? Assessing Differences in Group and Individual Zoo Enclosure Usage by Comparing Commonly Available Methods. Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens. 2022; 3(1):71-88. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg3010007

Chicago/Turabian Style

McConnell, Heather, James Brereton, Tom Rice, and Paul Rose. 2022. "Do Birds of a Feather Always Flock Together? Assessing Differences in Group and Individual Zoo Enclosure Usage by Comparing Commonly Available Methods" Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens 3, no. 1: 71-88. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg3010007

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop