Leveraging Social Learning to Enhance Captive Animal Care and Welfare
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Animal Learning
2.1. Research History and Theory
2.2. Social Learning
3. Potential Applications of Social Learning for Captive Management
3.1. Socialization and Seeding New Cultural Traditions
3.2. Enrichment
3.3. Training
3.4. Learning from Humans
4. Considerations and Caveats
4.1. Learning Modalities
4.2. Individual and Species Differences
4.3. Spontaneous Social Learning of Unanticipated or Negative Behaviors
4.4. What Can Be Copied?
5. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Morand-Ferron, J.; Cole, E.F.; Quinn, J.L. Studying the Evolutionary Ecology of Cognition in the Wild: A Review of Practical and Conceptual Challenges. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 2016, 91, 367–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thornton, A.; Isden, J.; Madden, J.R. Towards Wild Psychometrics: Linking Individual Cognitive Differences to Fitness. Behav. Ecol. 2014, 25, 1299–1301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Clark, F. What is There to Learn in a Zoo Setting? In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 83–100. [Google Scholar]
- Baker, K.; Melfi, V.A. The Ultimate Benefits of Learning. In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 35–52. [Google Scholar]
- Hopper, L.M. Social Learning and Decision Making. In Handbook of Primate Behavior Management; Schapiro, S.J., Ed.; CRC Press; Taylor & Francis Group: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2017; pp. 225–242. [Google Scholar]
- Herbert, M.J.; Harsh, C.M. Observational Learning by Cats. J. Comp. Psychol. 1944, 37, 81–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- John, E.R.; Chesler, P.; Bartlett, F.; Victor, I. Observational Learning in Cats. Science 1968, 159, 1489–1491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Adler, L.L.; Adler, H.E. Ontogeny of Observational Learning in the Dog (Canis familiaris). Dev. Psychobiol. 1977, 10, 267–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Presley, W.J.; Riopelle, A.J. Observational Learning of an Avoidance Response. J. Genet. Psychol. 1959, 95, 251–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fugazza, C.; Petro, E.; Miklósi, Á.; Pogány, Á. Social Learning of Goal-Directed Actions in Dogs (Canis familiaris): Imitation or Emulation? J. Comp. Psychol. 2019, 133, 244–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fugazza, C.; Sommese, A.; Pogány, Á.; Miklósi, Á. Did We Find a Copycat? Do as I Do in a Domestic Cat (Felis catus). Anim. Cogn. 2021, 24, 121–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huber, L.; Popovová, N.; Riener, S.; Salobir, K.; Cimarelli, G. Would Dogs Copy Irrelevant Actions From their Human Caregiver? Learn. Behav. 2018, 46, 387–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huber, L.; Salobir, K.; Mundry, R.; Cimarelli, G. Selective Overimitation in Dogs. Learn. Behav. 2020, 48, 113–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boakes, R. From Darwin to Behaviourism: Psychology and the Minds of Animals; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Dona, H.S.G.; Chittka, L.; Charles, H. Turner, Pioneer in Animal Cognition. Science 2020, 370, 530–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, D.N. Diversity and Inclusion Activisms in Animal Behaviour and the ABS: A Historical View from the U.S.A. Anim. Behav. 2020, 164, 273–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barlow-Irick, P. How 2 Train a __________; Mustang Camp: Largo Canyon, NM, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Dorey, N.R. Learning Theory. In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 3–14. [Google Scholar]
- Carere, C.; Locurto, C. Interaction Between Animal Personality and Animal Cognition. Zoology 2011, 57, 491–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amici, F.; Widdig, A.; Lehmann, J.; Majolo, B. A Meta-Analysis of Interindividual Differences in Innovation. Anim. Behav. 2019, 155, 257–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reamer, L.A.; Haller, R.L.; Thiele, E.J.; Freeman, H.D.; Lambeth, S.P.; Schapiro, S.J. Factors Affecting Initial Training Success of Blood Glucose Testing in Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Zoo Biol. 2014, 33, 212–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wergård, E.-M.; Westlund, K.; Spångberg, M.; Fredlund, H.; Forkman, B. Training Success in Group-Housed Long-Tailed Macaques (Macaca fascicularis) is Better Explained by Personality Than by Social Rank. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 177, 52–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coleman, K. Individual Differences in Temperament and Behavioral Management Practices for Nonhuman Primates. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 137, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Duffield, C.; Wilson, A.J.; Thornton, A. Desperate Prawns: Drivers of Behavioural Innovation Vary Across Social Contexts in Rock Pool Crustaceans. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0139050. [Google Scholar]
- Bandini, E.; Harrison, R.A. Innovation in Chimpanzees. Biol. Rev. 2020, 95, 1167–1197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hopper, L.M.; Torrance, A.W. User Innovation: A Novel Framework for Studying Animal Innovation Within a Comparative Context. Anim. Cogn. 2019, 22, 1185–1190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perry, S.; Carter, A.; Smolla, M.; Akçay, E.; Nöbel, S.; Foster, J.G.; Healy, S. Not By Transmission Alone—The Role of Invention in Cultural Evolution. 2020. Available online: https://osf.io/preprints/socarxiv/x2acu/ (accessed on 12 December 2020).
- Ridley, M. How Innovation Works and Why it Flourishes in Freedom; Harper Collins Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Laland, K.N. Social Learning Strategies. Anim. Learn. Behav. 2004, 32, 4–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heyes, C.M. Social Learning in Animals: Categories and Mechanisms. Biol. Rev. 1994, 69, 207–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoppitt, W.; Laland, K.N. Social Learning: An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods, and Models; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Tennie, C.; Bandini, E.; van Schaik, C.P.; Hopper, L.M. The Zone of Latent Solutions and its Relevance to Understanding Ape Cultures. Biol. Philos. 2020, 35, 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tennie, C.; Hopper, L.M.; van Schaik, C. On the Origin of Cumulative Culture: Consideration of the Role of Copying in Culture-Dependent Traits and a Reappraisal of the Zone of Latent Solutions Hypothesis. In Chimpanzees in Context: A Comparative Perspective on Chimpanzee Behavior, Cognition, Conservation, and Welfare; Hopper, L.M., Ross, S.R., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Hopper, L.M. ‘Ghost’ Experiments and the Dissection of Social Learning in Humans and Animals. Biol. Rev. 2010, 85, 685–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whiten, A.; Horner, V.; Litchfield, C.A.; Marshall-Pescini, S. How Do Apes Ape? Anim. Learn. Behav. 2004, 32, 36–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hoppitt, E.J.E.; Brown, G.R.; Kendal, R.; Rendell, L.; Thornton, A.; Webster, M.M.; Laland, K.N. Lessons from Animal Teaching. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2008, 23, 486–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thornton, A.; Raihani, N. Identifying Teaching in Wild Animals. Learn. Behav. 2010, 38, 297–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hopper, L.M.; Whiten, A. The Evolutionary and Comparative Psychology of Social Learning and Culture. In The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology; Vonk, J., Shackelford, T.K., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 451–473. [Google Scholar]
- Rendell, L.; Fogarty, L.; Hoppitt, W.J.E.; Morgan, W.J.H.; Webster, M.M.; Laland, K.N. Cognitive Culture: Theoretical and Empirical Insights into Social Learning Strategies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 68–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smolla, M.; Alem, S.; Chittka, L.; Shultz, S. Copy-When-Uncertain: Bumblebees Rely on Social Information When Rewards are Highly Variable. Biol. Lett. 2016, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jones, S.; Czackes, T.J.; Gallager, A.J.; Oberhauser, F.B.; Gourlay, E.; Bacon, J.P. Copy When Uncertain: Lower Light Levels Increase Train Pheromone Depositing and Reliance on Pheromone Trails in Ants. Anim. Behav. 2019, 156, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galef, B.G., Jr.; Dudley, K.E.; Whiskin, E.E. Social Learning of Food Preferences in ‘Dissatisfied’ and ‘Uncertain’ Norway Rats. Anim. Behav. 2008, 75, 631–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kendal, R.L.; Hopper, L.M.; Whiten, A.; Brosnan, S.F.; Lambeth, S.P.; Schapiro, S.J.; Hoppitt, W. Chimpanzees Copy Dominant and Knowledgeable Individuals: Implications for Cultural Diversity. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2015, 36, 65–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Aplin, L.M.; Sheldon, B.C.; Morand-Ferron, J. Milk Bottles Revisited: Social Learning and Individual Variation in the Blue Tit, Cyanistes caeruleus. Anim. Behav. 2013, 85, 1225–1232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Federspiel, I.G.; Boeckle, M.; von Bayern, A.M.O.; Emery, N.J. Exploring Individual and Social Learning in Jackdaws. Learn. Behav. 2019, 47, 258–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Canteloup, C.; Cera, M.B.; Barrett, B.J.; van de Waal, E. Processing of Novel Food Reveal Payoff and Rank-Biased Social Learning in a Wild Primate. bioRxiv 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coelho, C.G.; Falótico, T.; Izar, P.; Mannu, M.; Resende, B.D.; Siqueira, J.O.; Ottoni, E.B. Social Learning Strategies for Nut-Cracking by Tufted Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus spp.). Anim. Cogn. 2015, 18, 911–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grampp, M.; Sueur, C.; van de Waal, E.; Botting, J. Social Attention Biases in Juvenile Wild Vervet Monkeys: Implications for Socialisation and Social Learning Process. Primates 2019, 60, 261–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Figueroa, J.; Solà-Oriol, D.; Menteca, X.; Pérez, J.F. Social Learning of Feeding Behaviour in Pigs: Effects of Neophobia and Familiarity with the Demonstrator Conspecific. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 148, 120–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guillette, L.M.; Scott, A.C.Y.; Healy, S.D. Social Learning in Nest-Building Birds: A Role for Familiarity. Proc. R. Soc. B 2016, 283, 20152685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Pike, T.W.; Laland, K.N. Conformist Learning in Nine-Spined Sticklebacks’ Foraging Decisions. Biol. Lett. 2010, 6, 466–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Haun, D.B.M.; Rekers, Y.; Tomasello, M. Majority-Biased Transmission in Chimpanzees and Human Children, but not Orangutans. Curr. Biol. 2012, 22, 727–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hatch, K.K.; Lefebvre, L. Does Father Know Best? Social Learning from Kin and Non-Kin in Juvenile Ringdoves. Behav. Process. 1997, 41, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galef, B.G., Jr.; Whiksin, E.E. Effectiveness of Familiar Kin and Unfamiliar Nonkin Demonstrator Rats in Alternating Food Choices of their Observers. Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 1381–1388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smit, J.A.H.; van Oers, K. Personality Types Vary in their Personal and Social Information Use. Anim. Behav. 2019, 151, 185–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henriksson, J.; Sauveroche, M.; Roth, L.S.V. Effects of Size and Personality on Social Learning and Human-Directed Behaviour in Horses (Equus caballus). Anim. Cogn. 2019, 22, 1001–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ladds, Z.; Hoppitt, W.; Boogert, N.J. Social Learning in Otters. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2017, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Carter, A.J.; Marshall, H.H.; Heinsohn, R.; Cowlishaw, G. Personality Predicts the Propensity for Social Learning in a Wild Primate. PeerJ 2014, 2, e283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Watson, S.K.; Vale, G.L.; Hopper, L.M.; Dean, L.G.; Kendal, R.L.; Price, E.E.; Wood, L.A.; Davis, S.J.; Schapiro, S.J.; Lambeth, S.P.; et al. Chimpanzees Demonstrate Persistent Individual Differences in Social Information Use. Anim. Cogn. 2018, 21, 639–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Clay, Z.; Tennie, C. Is Overimitation a Uniquely Human Phenomenon? Insights from Human Children as Compared to Bonobos. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 1535–1544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gruber, T.; Deschenaux, A.; Frick, A.; Clément, F. Group Membership Influences More Social Identification than Social Learning or Overimitation in Children. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 728–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van de Waal, E.; Krützen, M.; Hula, J.; Goudet, J.; Bshary, R. Similarity in Food Cleaning Techniques Within Matrilines in Wild Vervet Monkeys. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e35694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nawroth, C.; Langbein, J.; Coulon, M.; Gabor, V.; Oesterwind, S.; Benz-Schwarzburg, J.; von Borell, E. Farm Animal Cognition-Linking Behavior, Welfare and Ethics. Front. Vet. Sci. 2019, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- National Research Council. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, 8th ed.; The National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Berdahl, A.M.; Kao, A.B.; Flack, A.; Westley, P.A.H.; Codling, E.A.; Couzin, I.D.; Dell, A.I.; Biro, D. Collective Animal Navigation and Migratory Culture: From Theoretical Models to Empirical Evidence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2018, 373, 20170009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Oberliessen, L.; Kalenscher, T. Social and Non-Social Mechanisms of Inequity Aversion in Non-Human Animals. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Špinka, M. Social Dimensions of Emotions and its Implication for Animal Welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 138, 170–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snyder-Mackler, N.; Burger, J.R.; Gaydosh, J.R.; Gaydosh, L.; Belsky, D.W.; Noppert, G.A.; Campos, F.A.; Bartolomucci, A.; Yang, Y.C.; Aiello, A.E.; et al. Social Determinants of Health and Survival in Humans and Other Animals. Science 2020, 368, ax9553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Guerrero-Martin, S.M.; Rubin, L.H.; McGee, K.M.; Shirk, E.N.; Queen, S.E.; Li, M.; Bullock, B.; Carlson, B.W.; Adams, R.J.; Gama, L.; et al. Social Stress Alters Immune Response and Results in Higher Viral Load During Acute SIV Infection in a Pigtailed Macaque Model of HIV. 2020. Available online: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.04.21.054130v1 (accessed on 22 February 2021).
- Watson, C.F.I.; Caldwell, C.A. Neighbor Effects in Marmosets: Social Contagion of Agonism and Affiliation in Captive Callithrix jacchus. Am. J. Primatol. 2010, 72, 549–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whiten, A.; Spiteri, A.; Horner, V.; Bonnie, K.E.; Lambeth, S.P.; Schapiro, S.J.; de Waal, F.B.M. Transmission of Multiple Traditions Within and Between Chimpanzee Groups. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 1038–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Caine, N.G.; Weldon, P.J. Responses by Red-Bellied Tamarins (Saguinus labiatus) to Fecal Scents of Predatory and Non-Predatory Neotropical Mammals. Biotropica 1989, 21, 186–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herrelko, E.S.; Vick, S.-J.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M. How Chimpanzee Personality and Video Studies can Inform Management and Care of the Species: A Case Study. In Chimpanzees in Context; Hopper, L.M., Ross, S.R., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2020; pp. 524–551. [Google Scholar]
- Engh, A.L.; Siebert, E.R.; Greenberg, D.A.; Holekamp, K.E. Patterns of Alliance Formation and Postconflict Aggression Indicate Spotted Hyaenas Recognize Third-Party Relationships. Anim. Behav. 2005, 69, 209–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brügger, R.K.; Willems, E.P.; Burkart, J.M. Do Marmosets Understand Others’ Conversations? A Thermography Approach. Sci. Adv. 2021, 7, eabc8790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Call, P.; Aureli, F.; de Waal, F.B.M. Postconflict Third-Party Affiliation in Stumptailed Macaques. Anim. Behav. 2002, 63, 209–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- von Rohr, C.R.; Koski, S.E.; Burkart, J.M.; Caws, C.; Fraser, O.N.; Ziltener, A.; van Schaik, C.P. Impartial Third-Party Interventions in Captive Chimpanzees: A Reflection of Community Concern. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e32494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- De Oliveira, D.; Keeling, L.J. Routine Activities and Emotion in the Life of Dairy Cows: Integrating Body Language into an Affective State Framework. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0195674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- McLennan, K.M.; Miller, A.L.; Dalla Costa, E.; Stucke, D.; Corke, M.J.; Broom, D.M.; Leach, M.C. Conceptual and Methodological Issues Relating to Pain Assessment in Mammals: The Development and Utilisation of Pain Facial Expression Scales. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 217, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sénèque, E.; Lesimple, C.; Morisset, S.; Hausberger, M. Could Posture Reflect Welfare State? A Study Using Geometric Morphometrics in Riding School Horses. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0211852. [Google Scholar]
- Manteuffel, G.; Puppe, B.; Schön, P. Vocalization of Farm Animals as a Measure of Welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004, 88, 163–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cook, M.; Mineka, S.; Wolkenstein, B.; Laitsch, K. Observational Condition of Snake Fear in Unrelated Rhesus Monkeys. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1985, 94, 591–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huber, A.; Barber, A.L.A.; Faragó, T.; Müller, C.A.; Huber, L. Investigating Emotional Contagion in Dogs (Canis familiaris) to Emotional Sounds of Humans and Conspecifics. Anim. Cogn. 2017, 20, 703–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reimert, I.; Bolhuis, J.E.; Kemp, B.; Rodenburg, T.B. Indicators of Positive and Negative Emotions and Emotional Contagion in Pigs. Physiol. Behav. 2013, 109, 42–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Düpjan, S.; Krause, A.; Moscovice, L.R.; Nawroth, C. Emotional Contagion and its Implications for Animal Welfare. CAB Rev. 2020, 15, 046. [Google Scholar]
- Goumon, S.; Špinka, M. Emotional Contagion of Distress in Young Pigs is Potentiated by Previous Exposure to the Same Stressor. Anim. Cogn. 2016, 19, 501–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luna, D.; González, C.; Byrd, C.J.; Palomo, R.; Huenul, E.; Figueroa, J. Do Domestic Pigs Acquire a Positive Perception of Humans Through Observational Social Learning? Animals 2021, 11, 127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sapolsky, R.M.; Share, L.J. A Pacific Culture Among Wild Baboons: Its Emergence and Transmission. PLoS Biol. 2004, 2, e106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, C.F.I.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M.; Caldwell, C.A. Call Playback Artificially Generates a Temporary Cultural Style of High Affiliation in Marmosets. Anim. Behav. 2014, 93, 163–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ermatinger, F.A.; Brügger, R.K.; Burhart, J.M. The Use of Thermography to Investigate Emotions in Common Marmosets. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 211, 112672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Claidière, N.; Whiten, A.; Mareno, M.C.; Messer, E.; Brosnan, S.F.; Hopper, L.M.; Lambeth, S.P.; Schapiro, S.J.; McGuigan, N. Selective and Contagious Prosocial Resource Donation in Capuchin Monkeys, Chimpanzees and Humans. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 7631. [Google Scholar]
- Crailsheim, D.; Stüger, H.P.; Kalcher-Sommersguter, E.; Llorente, M. Early Life Experience and Alterations of Group Composition Shape the Social Grooming Networks of Former Pet and Entertainment Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0226947. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Freeman, H.D.; Ross, S.R. The Impact of Atypical Early Histories on Pet or Performer Chimpanzees. PeerJ 2014, 2, e579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Llorente, M.; Riba, D.; Ballestra, S.; Feliu, O.; Rostán, C. Rehabilitation and Socialization of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Used for Entertainment and as Pets: An 8-Year Study at Fundació Mona. Int. J. Primatol. 2015, 36, 605–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rommeck, I.; Gottlieb, D.H.; Strand, S.C.; McCowan, B. The Effects of Four Nursery Rearing Strategies on Infant Behavioral Development in Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta). J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2009, 48, 395–401. [Google Scholar]
- Keeling, A.S.; Bashaw, M.J.; Bloomsmith, M.A.; Maple, T.L. Socialization of a Single Hand-Reared Tiger Cub. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2013, 16, 47–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shier, D.M.; Owings, D.H. Effects of Social Learning on Predator Training and Postrelease Survival in Juvenile Black-Tailed Prairie Dogs, Cynomys ludovicianus. Anim. Behav. 2007, 73, 567–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, A.S.; Evans, C.S. Social Learning of Antipredator Behaviour in a Marsupial. Anim. Behav. 2003, 66, 485–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Maple, T.; Perdue, B.M. Zoo Animal Welfare; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Markowitz, H. Enriching Animal Lives; Mauka Press: Pacifica, CA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Lambeth, S.P.; Bloomsmith, M.A. Mirrors as Enrichment for Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Lab. Anim. Sci. 1992, 42, 261–266. [Google Scholar]
- Secci, G.; Bovera, F.; Musco, N.; Husein, Y.; Parisi, G. Use of Mirrors into Free-Range Areas: Effects on Rabbit Meat Quality and Storage Stability. Livest. Sci. 2020, 239, 104094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloomsmith, M.A.; Lambeth, S.P. Videotapes as Enrichment for Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Zoo Biol. 2001, 19, 541–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ogura, T. Use of Video System and its Effects on Abnormal Behaviour in Captive Japanese Macaques (Macaca fuscata). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 141, 173–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mandel, R.; Wenker, M.L.; van Reenen, K.; Keil, N.M.; Hillmann, E. Can Access to an Automated Grooming Brush and/or a Mirror Reduce Stress of Dairy Cows Kept in Social Isolation? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 211, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schofield, R. In Solitary Rabbits, Does the Presence or Absence of a Mirror Affect Stress, Fear and Anxiety? Vet. Evid. 2019, 4, 2–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopper, L.M.; Lambeth, S.P.; Schapiro, S.J. An Evaluation of the Efficacy of Video Displays for Use with Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am. J. Primatol. 2012, 74, 442–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Davitz, J.R.; Mason, D.J. Socially Facilitated Reduction of a Fear Response in Rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1955, 48, 149–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- van de Waal, E.; Borgeaud, C.; Whiten, A. Potent Social Learning and Conformity Shape a Wild Primate’s Foraging Decisions. Science 2013, 340, 483–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Menzel, E.W., Jr.; Davenport, R.K.; Rogers, C.M. Protocultural Aspects of Chimpanzees’ Responsiveness to Novel Objects. Folia Primatol. 1972, 17, 161–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reader, S.M. Causes of Individual Differences in Animal Exploration and Search. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2015, 7, 451–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Whiten, A.; Horner, V.; de Waal, F.B.M. Conformity to Cultural Norms of Tool Use in Chimpanzees. Nature 2005, 437, 737–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Young, R.J.; de Azevedo, C.S.; Cipreste, C.F. Environmental Enrichment. In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 101–118. [Google Scholar]
- Strand, D.A.; Utne-Palm, A.C.; Jakobsen, P.J.; Braithwaite, V.A.; Jensen, K.H.; Salvanes, A.G.V. Enrichment Promotes Learning in Fish. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2010, 412, 273–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kulahci, I.G.; Ghazanfar, A.A.; Rubenstein, D.I. Knowledgeable Lemurs Become More Central in Social Networks. Curr. Biol. 2018, 28, 1306–1310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Serres, A.; Hao, Y.; Wang, D. Body Contacts and Social Interactions in Captive Odontocetes are Influenced by the Context: An Implication for Welfare Assessment. Animals 2020, 10, 924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mesa-Gresa, P.; Pérez-Martinez, A.; Redolat, R. Environmental Enrichment Improves Novel Object Recognition and Enhances Agonsitic Behavior in Male Mice. Aggress. Behav. 2013, 39, 269–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brando, S.I.C.A. Animal Learning and Training: Implications for Animal Welfare. Vet. Clin.: Exotic Anim. Pract. 2012, 15, 387–398. [Google Scholar]
- Melfi, V.A.; Ward, S.J. Welfare Implications of Zoo Animal Training. In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 271–288. [Google Scholar]
- Perlman, J.E.; Bloomsmith, M.A.; Whittaker, M.A.; McMillan, J.L.; Minier, D.E.; McCowan, B. Implementing Positive Reinforcement Animal Training Programs and Primate Laboratories. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 137, 114–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bliss-Moreau, E.; Theil, J.H.; Moadab, G. Efficient Cooperative Restraint Training with Rhesus Macaques. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2013, 16, 98–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Graham, M.L.; Rieke, E.F.; Mutch, L.A.; Zolondek, E.K.; Faig, A.W.; DuFour, T.A.; Munson, J.W.; Kittredge, J.A.; Schuurman, H.-J. Successful Implementation of Cooperative Handling Eliminates the Need for Restraint in a Complex Non-Human Primate Disease Model. J. Med. Primatol. 2011, 41, 89–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Melfi, V.A.; Dorey, N.R.; Ward, S.J. Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Pryor, K. Reaching the Animal Mind; Scribner: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Day, R.L.; MacDonald, T.; Brown, C.; Laland, K.N.; Reader, S.M. Interactions Between Shoal Size and Conformity in Guppy Social Foraging. Anim. Behav. 2001, 62, 917–925. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yamamoto, M.E.; Lopes, F.A. Effect of Removal from the Family Group on Feeding Behavior by Captive Callithrix jacchus. Int. J. Primatol. 2004, 25, 489–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prescott, M.J.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M. Training Nonhuman Primates Using Positive Reinforcement Techniques. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2003, 6, 157–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopper, L.M.; Holmes, A.N.; Williams, L.E.; Brosnan, S.F. Dissecting the Mechanisms of Squirrel Monkey (Saimiri boliviensis) Social Learning. PeerJ 2013, 1, e13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Anderson-Hansen, K. Training Animals in a Group Setting. In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 327–332. [Google Scholar]
- Roy, T.; Bhat, A. Social learning in a maze? Contrasting Individual Performance Among Wild Zebrafish When Associated with Trained and Naïve Conspecifics. Behav. Process. 2017, 144, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schapiro, S.J.; Bloomsmith, M.A.; Laule, G.E. Positive Reinforcement Training as a Technique to Alter Nonhuman Primate Behavior: Quantitative Assessments of Effectiveness. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2003, 6, 175–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chang, S.W.C.; Winecoff, A.A.; Platt, M.L. Vicarious Reinforcement in Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta). Front. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Coleman, K.; Houser, L.A.; Maier, A. Improving the Efficiency of Positive Reinforcement Training for Non-Human Primates. Am. J. Primatol. 2013, 75 (Suppl. S1), 115. [Google Scholar]
- Houser, L.; Coleman, K. Observational Learning Facilitates Positive Reinforcement Training for Inhibited Macaques (Macaca mulatta). Am. J. Primatol. 2018, 81 (Suppl. S1), e23065. [Google Scholar]
- Perlman, J.E.; Horner, V.; Bloomsmith, M.A.; Lambeth, S.P.; Schapiro, S.J. Positive Reinforcement Training, Social Learning, and Chimpanzee Welfare. In The Mind of the Chimpanzee: Ecological and Experimental Perspectives; Lonsdorf, E.V., Ross, S.R., Matsuzawa, T., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2010; pp. 320–331. [Google Scholar]
- D’eath, R.B. Can Video Images Imitate Real Stimuli in Animal Behaviour Experiments? Biol. Rev. 1998, 73, 267–292. [Google Scholar]
- Laland, K.N.; Williams, K. Shoaling Generates Social Learning of Foraging Information in Guppies. Anim. Behav. 1997, 53, 1161–1169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vrtilek, J.K.; Carter, G.G.; Patriquin, K.J.; Page, R.A.; Ratcliffe, J.M. A Method for Rapid Testing of Social Learning in Vampire Bats. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2018, 5, 172483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yamada, M.; Sakurai, Y. An Observational Learning Task Using Barnes Maze in Rats. Cogn. Neurodynamics 2018, 12, 519–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bloomsmith, M.A.; Jones, M.L.; Snyder, R.J.; Singer, R.A.; Gardner, W.A.; Liu, S.C.; Maple, T.L. Positive Reinforcement Training to Elicit Voluntary Movement of Two Giant Pandas Throughout Their Enclosure. Zoo Biol. 2003, 22, 323–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloomsmith, M.A.; Stone, A.M.; Laule, G.E. Positive Reinforcement Training to Enhance the Voluntary Movement of Group-Housed Chimpanzees Within their Enclosures. Zoo Biol. 1998, 17, 333–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leidinger, C.S.; Kaiser, N.; Baumgart, N.; Baumgart, J. Using Clicker Training and Social Observation to Teach Rats to Voluntarily Change Cages. J. Vis. Exp. 2018, 140, 58511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rault, J.-L.; Wailblinger, S.; Boivin, X.; Hemsworth, P. The Power of a Positive Human-Animal Relationship for Animal Welfare. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 7, 590867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sherwen, S.L.; Hemsworth, P.H. The Visitor Effect on Zoo Animals: Implications and Opportunities for Zoo Animal Welfare. Animals 2019, 9, 366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hansen, B.K.; Hopper, L.M.; Fultz, A.L.; Ross, S.R. Understanding the Behavior of Sanctuary-Housed Chimpanzees During Public Programs. Anthrozoös 2020, 33, 481–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, S. The Art of ‘Active’ Training. In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 119–142. [Google Scholar]
- Hosey, G.; Melfi, V.A. Us and Them: Human-Animal Interactions as Learning Events. In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 167–182. [Google Scholar]
- Kuczaj, S.A.; Yeater, D.B. Dolphin Imitation: Who, What, When, and Why? Aquat. Mamm. 2006, 32, 413–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopper, L.M.; Lambeth, S.P.; Schapiro, S.J.; Whiten, A. The Importance of Witnessed Agency in Chimpanzee Social Learning of Tool Use. Behav. Process. 2015, 112, 120–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ross, S.R.; Milstein, M.S.; Calcutt, S.E.; Lonsdorf, E.V. Assessment of Methods Used to Demonstrate Nut-Cracking Behavior to Five Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Folia Primatol. 2010, 81, 224–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pongrácz, P.; Miklósi, Á.; Kubinyi, E.; Gurobi, K.; Topál, J.; Csány, V. Social Learning in Dogs: The Effect of a Human Demonstrator on the Performance of Dogs in a Detour Task. Anim. Behav. 2001, 62, 1109–1117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schuetz, A.; Farmer, K.; Krueger, K. Social Learning Across Species: Horses (Equus caballus) Learn from Humans by Observation. Anim. Cogn. 2017, 20, 5670573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Beran, M.J. Did You Ever Hear the One About the Horse That Could Count? Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Range, F.; Heucke, S.L.; Gruber, C.; Konz, A.; Huber, L.; Virány, Z. The Effect of Ostensive Cues on Dogs’ Performance in a Manipulative Social Learning Task. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 120, 170–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pongrácz, P.; Miklósi, Á.; Timár-Geng, K.; Csány, V. Verbal Attention Getting as a Key Factor in Social Learning Between Dog (Canis familiaris) and Human. J. Comp. Psychol. 2004, 118, 375–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Custance, D.M.; Bard, K.A.; Whiten, A. Can Young Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Imitate Arbitrary Actions? Hayes & Hayes (1952) Revisited. Behaviour 1995, 132, 837–859. [Google Scholar]
- Fugazza, C.; Pogány, Á.; Miklósi, Á. Do as I … Did! Long-Term Memory of Imitative Actions in Dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim. Cogn. 2016, 19, 263–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fugazza, C.; Miklósi, Á. Should Old Trainers Learn New Tricks? The Efficiency of the Do As I Do Method and Shaping/Clicker Training Method to Train Dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 153, 53–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fugazza, C.; Miklósi, Á. Social Learning in Dog Training: The Effectiveness of the Do As I Do Method Compared to Shaping/Clicker Training. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 171, 146–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manassa, R.P.; McCormick, M.I. Social Learning and Acquired Recognition of a Predator by Marine Fish. Anim. Cogn. 2012, 15, 559–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Panova, E.; Agafonov, A.V. A Beluga Whale Socialized with Bottlenose Dolphins Imitates their Whistles. Anim. Cogn. 2017, 20, 1153–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reiss, D.; McCowan, B. Spontaneous Vocal Mimicry and Production by Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Evidence for Vocal Learning. J. Comp. Psychol. 1993, 107, 301–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacobson, S.L.; Plotnik, J.M. Elephant Cognition: An Overview. In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 191–196. [Google Scholar]
- Botero, M. Tactless Scientists: Ignoring Touch in the Study of Joint Attention. Philos. Psychol. 2016, 29, 1200–1214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monsó, S.; Wrage, B. Tactful Animals: How the Study of Touch Can Inform the Animal Morality Debate. Philos. Psychol. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Firth, J.A. Considering Complexity: Animal Social Networks and Behavioural Contagions. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2020, 35, 100–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pasquaretta, C.; Levé, M.; Claidière, N.; van de Waal, E.; Whiten, A.; Macintosh, A.; Pelé, M.; Bergstrom, M.; Borgeaud, C.; Brosnan, S.F.; et al. Social Networks in Primates: Smart and Tolerant Species Have More Efficient Networks. Sci. Rep. 2014, 4, 7600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Webster, M.M.; Laland, K.N. Social Information Use and Social Learning in Non-Grouping Fishes. Behav. Ecol. 2017, 28, 1547–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilkinson, A.; Kuenstner, K.; Mueller, J.; Huber, L. Social Learning in a Non-Social Reptile (Geochelone carbonaria). Biol. Lett. 2010, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Mehrkam, L.R. The Cognitive Abilities of Wild Animals. In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 15–34. [Google Scholar]
- Aplin, L.M.; Farine, D.R.; Morand-Ferron, J.; Cockburn, A.; Thornton, A.; Sheldon, B.C. Experimentally Induced Innovations Lead to Persistent Culture via Conformity in Wild Birds. Nature 2015, 518, 538–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- van de Waal, E.; Renevey, N.; Favre, C.M.; Bshary, R. Selective Attention to Philopatric Models Causes Directed Social Learning in Wild Vervet Monkeys. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 2010, 277, 2105–2111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drea, C.M.; Wallen, K. Low-Status Monkeys “Play Dumb” When Learning in Mixed Social Groups. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1999, 96, 12965–12969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Capitanio, J.P. Behavioral Inhibition in Nonhuman Primates: The Elephant in the Room. In Behavioral Inhibition; Pérez-Edgar, K., Fox, N., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Berhane, J.F.; Gazes, R.P. Social Monkeys Learn More Slowly: Social Network Centrality and Age are Positively Related to Learning Errors by Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus [Sapajus] apella). Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 2020, 74, 228–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopkins, W.D.; Mareno, M.C.; Webb, S.J.N.; Schapiro, S.J.; Raghanti, M.A.; Sherwood, C.C. Age-Related Changes in Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Cognition: Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Analyses. Am. J. Primatol. 2020, e23214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopper, L.M.; Price, S.A.; Freeman, H.D.; Lambeth, S.P.; Schapiro, S.J.; Kendal, R.L. Influence of Personality, Age, Sex, and Estrous State on Chimpanzee Problem-Solving Success. Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 835–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dougherty, L.R.; Guillette, L.M. Linking Personality and Cognition: A Meta-Analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2018, 373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Caldwell, C.A.; Whiten, A. Scrounging Facilitates Social Learning in Common Marmosets, Callithrix jacchus. Anim. Behav. 2003, 65, 1085–1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Keen, S.C.; Cole, E.F.; Sheehan, M.J.; Sheldon, B.C. Social Learning of Acoustic Anti-Predator Cues Occurs Between Wild Bird Species. Proc. R. Soc. B 2020, 287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wheeler, B.C.; Fahy, M.; Tiddi, B. Experimental Evidence for Heterospecific Alarm Signal Recognition via Associative Learning in Wild Capuchin Monkeys. Anim. Cogn. 2019, 22, 687–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cloutier, S.; Newberry, R.C.; Honda, K.; Alldredge, J.R. Cannibalistic Behaviour Spread by Social Learning. Anim. Behav. 2002, 63, 1153–1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hook, M.A.; Lambeth, S.P.; Perlman, J.E.; Stavisky, R.; Bloomsmith, M.A.; Schapiro, S.J. Inter-Group Variation in Abnormal Behavior in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 76, 165–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopper, L.M.; Freeman, H.D.; Ross, S.R. Reconsidering Coprophagy as an Indicator of Negative Welfare for Captive Chimpanzees. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 176, 112–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, H.; Noh, J. Pair Exposure with Conspecific During Fear Conditioning Induces the Link Between Freezing and Passive Avoidance Behaviors in Rats. Neurosci. Res. 2016, 108, 40–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wich, S.A.; Swartz, K.B.; Hardus, M.E.; Lameira, A.R.; Stromberg, E.; Shumaker, R.W. A Case of Spontaneous Acquisition of a Human Sound by an Orangutan. Primates 2009, 50, 56–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watters, J.V. Searching for Behavioral Indicators of Welfare in Zoos: Uncovering Anticipatory Behavior. Zoo Biol. 2014, 33, 251–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caspar, K.R.; Kammerer, C.; Hradec, M. Attention-Getting in a White-Cheeked Gibbon by Means of a Novel Vocalization? Behaviour 2020, 157, 1245–1255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopkins, W.D.; Taglialatela, J.P.; Leavens, D.A. Chimpanzees Differentially Produce Novel Vocalizations to Capture the Attention of Humans. Anim. Behav. 2007, 73, 281–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hopper, L.M.; Fernandez-Duque, E.; Williams, L.E. Testing the weekend effect hypothesis: Time of day and lunar phase better predict the timing of births in laboratory-housed primates than day of week. Am. J. Primatol. 2019, 81, e23026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chelluri, G.I.; Ross, S.R.; Wagner, K.E. Behavioral Correlates and Welfare Implications of Informal Interactions Between Caretakers and Zoo-Housed Chimpanzees and Gorillas. Appl. Anim. Behavi. Sci. 2013, 147, 306–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cloutier, S.; Panksepp, J.; Newberry, R.C. Playful Handing by Caretakers Reduces Fear of Humans in the Laboratory Rat. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 140, 161–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilani, A.-M.; Knowles, T.G.; Nicol, C.J. The Effect of Rearing Environment on Feather Pecking in Young and Adult Laying Hens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 148, 54–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Katsnelson, A. Male Researchers Stress Out Rodents. Nature 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rushen, J.; de Passillé, A.M. The Importance of Good Stockmanship and its Benefits for the Animals. In Improving Animal Welfare: A Practical Approach; Grandin, T., Ed.; CABI: Wallingford, UK, 2009; pp. 50–63. [Google Scholar]
- Hemsworth, P.H.; Coleman, G.J.; Barnett, J.L. Improving the Attitude and Behaviour of Stockspersons Towards Pigs and the Consequences on the Behaviour and Reproductive Performance of Commercial Pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1994, 39, 349–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rennie, A.E.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M. Refinement of the Use of Non-Human Primates in Scientific Research. Part 1: The Influence of Humans. Anim. Welf. 2006, 15, 203–213. [Google Scholar]
- Wigham, E.; Grist, A.; Mullan, S.; Wotton, S.; Butterworth, A. The Influence of Welfare Training on Bird Welfare and Carcass Quality in Two Commercial Poultry Primary Processing Plants. Animals 2019, 9, 584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Glanville, C.; Abraham, C.; Coleman, G. Human Behaviour Change Interventions in Animal Care and Interactive Settings: A Review and Framework for Design and Evaluation. Animals 2020, 10, 2333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tennie, C.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Ratcheting up the Ratchet: On the Evolution of Cumulative Culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2009, 364, 2405–2415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Whiten, A.; McGuigan, N.; Marshall-Pescini, S.; Hopper, L.M. Emulation, Imitation, Over-Imitation and the Scope of Culture for Child and Chimpanzee. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2009, 364, 2417–2428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tennie, C.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Untrained Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) Fail to Imitate Novel Actions. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e41548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Mechanism * | Description |
---|---|
Stimulus enhancement | When an individual’s attention is drawn to a specific stimulus by the presence or actions of another individual. |
Local enhancement | When an individual is more likely to approach and engage with objects at a particular location following another individual’s presence at that location. |
Observational conditioning | When an individual learns the relationship between two stimuli from the observation of another’s interactions with them. |
Response facilitation | When the probability of an individual performing an action is increased by the presence of another individual also performing that action. |
Social facilitation | The process by which an individual’s behavior is influenced by the mere presence of another individual. Depending on the relationship between the individuals, the social influence may promote or suppress exploration and learning. |
Affordance learning | When observing the behavior of another allows an individual to learn about the physical properties of various objects, in turn allowing them to individually learn a behavior performed by the other individual. |
Emulation | When observing the behavior of another allows an individual to reach the same goal but via their own means (also called goal emulation or end-state emulation). |
Imitation | When an individual copies a novel (to them) action or sequence of actions after observing another individual perform those actions. For imitation to be inferred, these actions cannot have been a part of the observing individual’s repertoire previously (i.e., the novelty of the copied action is what distinguishes imitation from response facilitation). |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Hopper, L.M. Leveraging Social Learning to Enhance Captive Animal Care and Welfare. J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2021, 2, 21-40. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2010003
Hopper LM. Leveraging Social Learning to Enhance Captive Animal Care and Welfare. Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens. 2021; 2(1):21-40. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2010003
Chicago/Turabian StyleHopper, Lydia M. 2021. "Leveraging Social Learning to Enhance Captive Animal Care and Welfare" Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens 2, no. 1: 21-40. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2010003
APA StyleHopper, L. M. (2021). Leveraging Social Learning to Enhance Captive Animal Care and Welfare. Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens, 2(1), 21-40. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2010003