Next Article in Journal
Self-Created Film as a Resource in a Multimodal Conversational Narrative
Previous Article in Journal
Real-Time Norwegian Sign Language Recognition Using MediaPipe and LSTM
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Flight Experience or Simulator Exposure on Simulator Sickness in Virtual Reality Flight Simulation

by
Alexander Somerville
1,
Keith Joiner
1,2 and
Graham Wild
3,*
1
School of Engineering & Technology, UNSW Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Campbell 2612, Australia
2
Capability Systems Centre, UNSW Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Campbell 2612, Australia
3
School of Science, UNSW Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Campbell 2612, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Multimodal Technol. Interact. 2025, 9(3), 24; https://doi.org/10.3390/mti9030024
Submission received: 30 December 2024 / Revised: 6 February 2025 / Accepted: 27 February 2025 / Published: 6 March 2025

Abstract

:
The use of virtual reality (VR) for flight simulation, particularly in the earliest stages of pilot training, is gaining attention in both research and industry. The use of the technology for this ab initio training requires suitable consideration of the risks of simulator sickness—risks that are heightened relative to conventional simulators. If simulator sickness results in the development of compensatory skills, or otherwise disrupts the training process, the benefits of the technology may be negated. Enabling the effective integration of VR within flight training requires that, to the extent that simulator sickness is an issue, practical mechanisms are developed to manage the occurrence without disrupting existing training structures. The primary objective of this research is, thus, to evaluate an intervention and a nuisance factor in relation to the reduction of simulator sickness, considering their practicality within existing flight training syllabi. The Total Severity (TS) of the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) was evaluated within a quasi-experimental, non-equivalent pre-test–post-test design, incorporating three groups: a prior flight experience nuisance factor group, a prior personal computer aviation training device (PCATD) exposure intervention group, and a control group with neither prior experience nor prior simulator exposure. The results indicated that the TS was significantly reduced for the prior flight experience nuisance factor (rrb = 0.375), but that the PCATD exposure intervention produced no such reduction (rrb = 0.016). The findings suggest that VR flight simulation is likely best used as a supplemental tool, introduced after initial airborne experience. Notwithstanding this finding, the relatively low median TS scores (<20) for all groups suggest that the technology may still be used with caution earlier in the training process. No other published research has examined this important effect in the context of the new VR situation.

1. Introduction

The conventional flight simulator, particularly in terms of its technologies, has developed from primitive beginnings to the highly complex, high-fidelity digital simulators of today. The use of such simulators, particularly for the transfer of flight-critical skills, has long been viewed and understood alongside the limitations of such training [1]. The usefulness of visually based flight simulators for training may be affected by various phenomena, including simulator sickness [2]. Simulator sickness, which has been widely researched in both academia and industry since at least the 1980s [3]—and acknowledged since the 1950s [4,5]—involves symptoms typically associated with motion sickness. The subtle differences between simulator sickness, motion sickness, and cyber sickness form a complex literature unto themselves [6]. However, the common symptoms of simulator sickness, such as nausea, dizziness, and spatial disorientation, are well documented. Simulator sickness is both polygenic and polysymptomatic, making precise classification and identification difficult [7]. That is, sickness may arise as a result of unique individual factors or individual responses to improper simulation from the device, hardware, or software. However, not all symptoms, or a specific pattern of symptoms, will be identifiable in each individual. Notably, simulator sickness is considered to result from incorrect aspects (i.e., fidelity) of the simulator or simulation, rather than sickness caused by the accurate simulation of a nauseating or visually disorienting event, such as aerobatic flight [1]. Despite the lack of specificity around simulator sickness—both in origins and symptoms—the measurement of the experience is achieved consistently by tools such as the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire [8]. The Simulator Sickness Questionnaire is widely used in the measurement of simulator sickness and is well validated [9] and generally considered the standard [10]. Decades of research on simulator sickness in conventional simulators, has, of course, yet to be matched for newer and more immersive simulator technologies—such as virtual reality (VR).
The growth in the awareness and experience of simulator sickness paralleled the growth of simulation for pilot training in the 1980s [10]. As newer display technologies emerged—including the head-mounted display (HMD) in the 1990s—the problems associated with simulator sickness increased further. The prevalence of simulator sickness is, of course, not limited to aviation, much as VR for education is not limited to aviation [11,12]. Simulator sickness has been noted in other fields, including consumer entertainment [13], where such display technologies have continued to grow in popularity [14]. For the training of pilots, and indeed for the training of any safety-critical activities, the prevalence of simulator sickness can offset many of the advantages of ground-based training, including cost and safety [15,16]. The experience of sickness can compromise training through distraction and decreased motivation, particularly due to behavioural adaptations made by trainees to avoid symptoms, such as not looking out of windows [17]. These adaptations may become compensatory skills carried into real aircraft, which could be dangerous. The risk that this issue poses was noted early in the development of (what would become) conventional simulators [5] and has been subsequently suggested as an issue with flight simulators based on consumer-grade VR-HMDs [18]. In addition to the more direct threats to safety posed by simulator sickness in a VR simulator, the practical implications of using the technology must also be considered. In particular, the immersion often claimed for virtual reality [19]—which appears to offer benefits for training that relies on the visuospatial understanding of relationships [20,21,22,23]—may be undermined by the symptoms of simulator sickness, disrupting this experience [24]. That is, engagement within the virtual environment may be significantly reduced despite the greater immersion of virtual reality [25,26]. The potential limitations of the technology must be considered in the specific contexts in which the technology will be used, such as for the flight simulator training of pilots [27].
The growing body of literature on modern VR for flight simulation and other extended reality (xR) technologies [28] is rather unsophisticated and inconclusive [29]. VR simulation will, however, undoubtedly be an increasing part of aviation training [19,30]. The intentions for the use of xR flight simulators for pilot training, for various reasons, are primarily focused on the training of ab initio and general aircraft handling skills [29]. These skills would include the most basic tasks required of the pilot—such as maintaining the aircraft in straight-and-level flight (Aircraft Rating Standard—Aeroplane Category—A3.1 [31])—through to the conduct of circuits (Aircraft Rating Standard—Aeroplane Category—A3.6 [31]). Learning these skills is the foundation of all future development of the flying technique [32] and, of course, occurs from the beginning (i.e., ab initio) of flight training. These skills, and indeed the majority of the initial flight training, are based upon the visual recognition of the situation outside of the aircraft and the visual relationship between the instrument coaming and the natural horizon [33]. The interactivity and immersion of VR appear to work synergistically to enable the greater visuospatial awareness required [20,21,22], as compared to the necessarily truncated visuals of simulators based on two-dimensional display technology [34]. Presently, simulators in general aviation (GA) are used primarily for the training of instrument flying skills [35], while the VR simulator would enable better cognitive priming for broader ab initio skills. That is, the inexpensive consumer-grade VR-HMD does not replace the aircraft as a training device, but rather prepares the trainee better for the learning that will occur airborne. The promises, and likely advantages, of the VR simulator must be carefully considered alongside the risks to training from simulator sickness.
The clear intention for the use of VR simulators for ab initio training, along with the acknowledged benefits of simulator training before real-world flight [15,36], offers huge potential benefits. It is unclear precisely where the VR simulator, or indeed the xR simulator, will exist within the existing hierarchy of flight simulators, due primarily to the dominance of commercial factors in shaping GA aviation. The existing literature, with its GA focus (i.e., ab initio and general handling), would likely mean the augmentation of, or replacement of, simpler flight training devices (FTD) and aviation training devices (ATD). However, the greater accessibility that VR-HMDs enable [29], like the greater accessibility once enabled by the low-cost personal computer aviation training device (PCATD) [37], would suggest the increasing use of unsupervised simulator flying, whether voluntary or sanctioned by instructors. While this may enable greater practice, the potential of simulator sickness to alter behaviour (i.e., compensatory skills) must be considered. These compensatory skills could also develop unchallenged in the absence of the guidance and “ingenuity” [15] of a qualified flight instructor. Such was the case—although due to different causes—following the release of consumer-accessible flight simulator software as early as 2001, where Prensky found that many trainee pilots arrived “…fully training up on [Microsoft] flight simulator…” ([38] p. 310). However, the compensatory skills developed outside of formal instruction must then be unlearned at a financial and temporal cost [37,39,40].
Means of reducing simulator sickness in VR have been proposed, but these solutions generally require the modification or addition of hardware or software components [41,42,43]. Modifications to software and hardware can either directly reduce sensory conflicts or mitigate their effects through indirect means. Modifications to reduce sensory conflicts include the reduction of latency, field-of-view changes, or the use of alternative display technologies [44]. Certain other means, such as the use of galvanic vestibular stimulation, olfactory stimulation, or auditory distraction, have also shown some promise [45,46,47]. However, the polygenic nature of the susceptibility to simulator sickness means that no particular solution is guaranteed in all circumstances [48]. The use of these modifications also adds both complexity and cost to the system. The additional cost and complexity would seem to partially negate the advantages of consumer-grade VR-HMDs, and they are unlikely to permeate voluntary student use. Techniques based upon acclimatisation [49] and repeated exposure have shown promise [50], although these results are also not guaranteed under all circumstances [18]. A valuable contribution would be a simple method to reduce the symptoms of simulator sickness, while fitting within the existing educational context.
As previously mentioned, there are overlapping definitions around motion sickness, simulator sickness, and cyber sickness. Importantly, within the aviation industry, the preferred term is simulator sickness, and this applies even to synthetic flight training devices that do not incorporate any physical motion. This is a taxonomical issue in the aviation industry as all relevant training devices are colloquially referred to as simulators. There is then the specific subset of flight simulators that utilise motion, as well as the subset of synthetic flight trainers that do not utilise motion. To compound these issues further, the software utilised across training devices is collectively called flight simulators. As such, the term utilised in this work will be simulator sickness. It is worth noting that the established symptoms of cyber sickness align exactly with those of simulator sickness as used in this work, such as those of Kourtesis et al. [44], being “nausea, disorientation, and oculomotor disturbances”.
Of relevance is the measurement tool used to assess simulator sickness. The specifically developed [8] and hence widely used tool in the aviation industry is the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) [51,52,53,54]. Of relevance is the fact that the SSQ has also previously been applied to VR flight simulation [55]. To enable this work to be directly comparable for those working in the aviation industry, the use of the SSQ is essential. There are other measurement tools, such as the Cyber Sickness Questionnaire for Virtual Reality (CSQ-VR) [56]. In this work, the CSQ-VR was compared to the SSQ, as well as the VR version of the SSQ called the VRSQ, developed by Sevinc and Berkman [57]. Kourtesis et al. [58] have also utilised the Virtual Reality Neuroscience Questionnaire (VRSQ), which assesses VR-induced symptoms and effects.
VR is compelling for simulation because of the immersion, engagement, greater access, and better understanding of visuospatial relationships. Such simulators are likely to be adopted in place of, or as augmentation to, simpler ATDs and PCATDs. However, simulator sickness appears to pose a significant risk to such training, particularly due to the development of compensatory skills and the lasting effects of symptoms long after the initial exposure. Despite these risks, the technology is clearly seen as attractive by industry and academia for ab initio training. There has been insufficient research on simulator sickness in the ab initio cohort and mechanisms to address this efficiently. Although there are attempts in the literature to reduce simulator sickness through various hardware and software extensions, these solutions are unlikely to be practical in the context of flight training syllabi. Thus, this research seeks to evaluate two practical, and directly incorporable, interventions for the reduction of simulator sickness. It may be possible to alter syllabi to make use of existing resources (e.g., initial use an of existing PCATD) or, alternatively, adjust the stage at which these technologies are introduced (e.g., after some initial training airborne).

1.1. Background

1.1.1. Flight Simulators

The conventional flight simulator, particularly in terms of its technologies, has developed from primitive beginnings to the highly complex, high-fidelity digital simulators of today. The use of such simulators, particularly for the transfer of flight-critical skills, has long been viewed and understood alongside the limitations of such training [1]. The increasing literature on the use of modern virtual reality (VR) for flight simulation, as well as the various other extended reality (xR) technologies [28], is rather unsophisticated and inconclusive [29]. VR simulation will, however, undoubtedly be an increasing part of aviation training [19,30]. The intentions for the use of xR flight simulators for pilot training, for various reasons, are primarily focused on the training of ab initio and general aircraft handling skills [29]. These skills would include the most basic tasks required of the pilot—such as maintaining the aircraft in straight-and-level flight (Aircraft Rating Standard—Aeroplane Category—A3.1 [31])—through to the conduct of circuits (Aircraft Rating Standard—Aeroplane Category—A3.6 [31]). Learning these skills is the foundation of all future development of the flying technique [32] and, of course, occurs from the beginning (i.e., ab initio) of flight training. These skills, and indeed the majority of initial flight training, are based upon the visual recognition of the situation outside of the aircraft and the visual relationship between the instrument coaming and the natural horizon [33]. The interactivity and immersion of VR appear to work synergistically to enable the greater visuospatial awareness required [20,21,22], as compared to the necessarily truncated visuals of simulators based on two-dimensional display technology [34]. Presently, simulators in general aviation (GA) are used primarily for the training of instrument flying skills [35], while the VR simulator would enable better cognitive priming for broader ab initio skills. That is, the inexpensive consumer-grade VR-HMD does not replace the aircraft as a training device, but rather prepares the trainee better for the learning that will occur airborne. The clear intention for the use of VR simulators for ab initio training, along with the acknowledged benefits of simulator training before real-world flight [15,36], offers huge potential benefits. It is unclear precisely where the VR simulator, or indeed the xR simulator, will exist within the existing hierarchy of flight simulators, due primarily to the dominance of commercial factors in shaping GA aviation. The existing literature, with its GA focus (i.e., ab initio and general handling), would likely mean the augmentation of, or replacement of, simpler flight training devices (FTD) and aviation training devices (ATD). However, the greater accessibility that VR-HMDs enable [29], like the greater accessibility once enabled by the low-cost personal computer aviation training device (PCATD) [37], would suggest the increasing use of unsupervised simulator flying, whether voluntary or sanctioned by instructors.

1.1.2. Simulator Sickness

The usefulness of visually based flight simulators for training may be affected by various phenomena, including simulator sickness [2]. Simulator sickness, which has been widely researched in both academia and industry since at least the 1980s [3]—and acknowledged since the 1950s [4,5]—involves symptoms typically associated with motion sickness. The subtle differences between simulator sickness, motion sickness, and cyber sickness form a complex literature unto themselves [6]. However, the common symptoms of simulator sickness, such as nausea, dizziness, and spatial disorientation, are well documented. Simulator sickness is both polygenic and polysymptomatic, making precise classification and identification difficult [7]. That is, sickness may arise as a result of unique individual factors or individual responses to improper simulation from the device, hardware, or software. However, not all symptoms, or a specific pattern of symptoms, will be identifiable in each individual. Notably, simulator sickness is considered to result from incorrect aspects (i.e., fidelity) of the simulator or simulation, rather than sickness caused by the accurate simulation of a nauseating or visually disorienting event, such as aerobatic flight [1]. Despite the lack of specificity around simulator sickness—both in origins and symptoms—the measurement of the experience is achieved consistently by tools such as the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire [8]. The Simulator Sickness Questionnaire is widely used in the measurement of simulator sickness and is well validated [9] and generally considered the standard [10].

1.1.3. Cyber Sickness

The prevalence of simulator sickness is, of course, not limited to aviation, much as VR for education is not limited to aviation [11,12]. Simulator sickness has been noted in other fields, including consumer entertainment [13], where such display technologies have continued to grow in popularity [14]. As previously mentioned, there are overlapping definitions around motion sickness, simulator sickness, and cyber sickness. Importantly, within the aviation industry, the preferred term is simulator sickness, and this applies even to synthetic flight training devices that do not incorporate any physical motion. This is a taxonomical issue in the aviation industry as all relevant training devices are colloquially referred to as simulators. There is then the specific subset of flight simulators that utilise motion, as well as the subset of synthetic flight trainers that do not utilise motion. To compound these issues further, the software utilised across training devices is collectively called flight simulators. As such, the term utilised in this work will be simulator sickness. It is worth noting that the established symptoms of cyber sickness align exactly with those of simulator sickness as used in this work, such as those of Kourtesis et al. [44], being “nausea, disorientation, and oculomotor disturbances”.
Of relevance is the measurement tool used to assess simulator sickness. The specifically developed [8] and hence widely used tool in the aviation industry is the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) [51,52,53,54]. Of relevance is the fact that the SSQ has also previously been applied to VR flight simulation [55]. To enable this work to be directly comparable for those working in the aviation industry, the use of the SSQ is essential. There are other measurement tools, such as the Cyber Sickness Questionnaire for Virtual Reality (CSQ-VR) [56]. In this work, the CSQ-VR was compared to the SSQ, as well as the VR version of the SSQ called the VRSQ, developed by Sevinc and Berkman [57]. Kourtesis et al. [58] have also utilised the Virtual Reality Neuroscience Questionnaire (VRSQ), which assesses VR-induced symptoms and effects.

1.2. Significance

VR is compelling for simulation because of the immersion, engagement, greater access, and better understanding of visuospatial relationships. Such simulators are likely to be adopted in place of, or as augmentation to, simpler ATDs and PCATDs. However, simulator sickness appears to pose a significant risk to such training, particularly due to the development of compensatory skills and the lasting effects of symptoms long after the initial exposure.
For the training of pilots, and indeed for the training of any safety-critical activities, the prevalence of simulator sickness can offset many of the advantages of ground-based training, including cost and safety [15,16]. The experience of sickness can compromise training through distraction and decreased motivation, particularly due to behavioural adaptations made by trainees to avoid symptoms, such as not looking out of windows [17]. These adaptations may become compensatory skills carried into real aircraft that could be dangerous.
While the previously noted increased use of unsupervised simulator flying may enable greater practice, the potential of simulator sickness to alter behaviour (i.e., compensatory skills) must be considered. These compensatory skills could also develop unchallenged in the absence of the guidance and “ingenuity” [15] of a qualified flight instructor. Such was the case—although due to different causes—following the release of consumer-accessible flight simulator software as early as 2001, where Prensky found that many trainee pilots arrived “…fully training up on [Microsoft] flight simulator…” ([38] p. 310. However, the compensatory skills developed outside of formal instruction must then be unlearned at a financial and temporal cost [37,39,40].
The risk that this issue poses was noted early in the development of (what would become) conventional simulators [5] and has been subsequently suggested as an issue with flight simulators based on consumer-grade VR-HMDs [18]. In addition to the more direct threats to safety posed by simulator sickness in a VR simulator, the practical implications of using the technology must also be considered. In particular, the immersion often claimed for virtual reality [19]—which appears to offer benefits for training that relies on the visuospatial understanding of relationships [20,21,22,23]—may be undermined by the symptoms of simulator sickness, disrupting this experience [24]. That is, engagement within the virtual environment may be significantly reduced despite the greater immersion of virtual reality [25,26]. The potential limitations of the technology must be considered in the specific contexts in which the technology will be used, such as for the flight simulator training of pilots [27].
Despite these risks, the technology is clearly seen as attractive by industry and academia for ab initio training. There has been insufficient research on simulator sickness in the ab initio cohort and mechanisms to address this efficiently. Although there are attempts in the literature to reduce simulator sickness through various hardware and software components [41,42,43], these solutions are unlikely to be practical in the context of flight training syllabi. The additional cost and complexity would seem to partially negate the advantages of consumer-grade VR-HMDs, and they are unlikely to permeate voluntary student use. Techniques based upon acclimatisation [49] and repeated exposure have shown promise [50], although these results are also not guaranteed under all circumstances [18]. A valuable contribution would be a simple method to reduce the symptoms of simulator sickness, while fitting within the existing educational context. Thus, this research seeks to evaluate two practical, and directly incorporable, interventions for the reduction of simulator sickness. It may be possible to alter syllabi to make use of existing resources (e.g., initial use an of existing PCATD) or, alternatively, adjust the stage at which these technologies are introduced (e.g., after some initial training airborne). The clear intention for the use of VR simulators for ab initio training, along with the acknowledged benefits of simulator training before real-world flight [15,36], offers huge potential benefits.

1.3. Aims and Objectives

The question guiding this research is “Does prior flight experience or conventional simulator exposure reduce simulator sickness in ab initio trainees after virtual reality flight simulation, based upon the Total Severity (TS) of the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire?” In order for this question to be answered convincingly, empirical evidence will be provided on the effectiveness of the interventions. This research does not rely on the conception of simulator sickness as either a perceptual [59] or physiological [60] phenomenon; rather, it is solely concerned with the practical implications and its reduction. No other research addressing this question could be identified, and the answer is important for all future pilot training with VR technology.

2. Materials and Methods

This research employs a quasi-experimental, pseudo-randomised, non-equivalent pre-test–post-test control group design with one direct intervention—prior PCATD-based simulator exposure—and one nuisance factor—prior flight experience. The research design uses both a pre-VR SSQ and a post-VR SSQ, which together form the basis of the primary change score data. As this design is a quasi-experimental approach, there are sources of invalidity that must be considered. Data were available for 85 (n = 85) participants, of whom 33 (n = 33) were in the PCATD exposure group, 25 (n = 25) were in the flight experience group, and the remaining 27 (n = 27) were in the control group. The flight experience group and the control group differed only in their prior airborne flight training as a nuisance factor. Demographic filtering was applied solely for the purpose of managing this nuisance factor—ensuring that the groups could be analysed based on prior flight experience. The original data, and the educational activities that produced these data, were solely for the purpose of an undergraduate laboratory course, which was conducted twice in a twelve-month period. The dataset comprised two separate course iterations, each lasting a single semester (i.e., six months). These data were anonymised prior to their provision to the researchers, as per the research ethics approval.
The order of use of the hardware (i.e., PCATD or VR simulator first or second) resulted from the limited availability of VR-equipped PCATDs. As such, the use of both simulator setups occurred simultaneously, to ensure that the time required did not exceed that timetabled. The control group, therefore, received the same PCATD-based simulator experience as the treatment group, but after the VR simulator, as shown in Figure 1. Participants arrived sequentially within a scheduled timeframe for the lab (i.e., class) and were assigned to a simulator based on their arrival order. Those who arrived first were assigned to VR until all VR simulators were occupied, after which later arrivals were assigned to the PCATD. The allocation of a participant to use one or the other simulator first, and therefore the group allocation and randomisation, then resulted from the order in which they arrived (i.e., random to an extent). The pre-VR SSQ was administered immediately before VR exposure for all participants, regardless of whether they had first used the PCATD or not. Those assigned to VR first completed the post-VR SSQ immediately after exposure and then proceeded to PCATD use, whereas those initially assigned to the PCATD completed their pre-VR SSQ only after their PCATD session, before experiencing VR, and then completed their post-VR SSQ.
SSQ data were only collected before and after the VR simulator use; therefore, the control group had no additional data. This sequencing ensured that all participants completed SSQ assessments exclusively for VR exposure, with no post-PCATD SSQ collected to avoid confounding effects from multiple exposures. The provided SSQ data were from immediately prior to the use of the VR simulator and immediately after the use of the VR simulator, regardless of the group. The data took the form of extracted, but unprocessed, SSQ Likert-scale values for each dimension of the SSQ. The only demographic data that were available, and which had apparently served some purpose in the original educational activity, were whether the participants had (or did not have) any flight experience—the nuisance factor. The flight experience data were provided in the three group datasets, with each being for the specific exposure. No additional demographic data were made available to the researchers. Data for participants who had both prior PCATD exposure and flight experience were simply not provided and thus were unavailable for inclusion in the analysis. Given the risk that simulator sickness would occur at problematical levels, participants were instructed to immediately remove the VR-HMD if they experienced any uncomfortable sensation.
The simulator for the VR activity, shown in Figure 2, was based on a consumer-grade VR-HMD tethered to an existing uncertified PCATD. This PCATD was the system used for the associated intervention group. The computer that was used for the simulator ran the Windows 11 operating system, with an Intel i7 (2.5 GHz) central processing unit, 16 GB of DDR4 random-access memory, and an Nvidia RTX 3060 (16 GB) graphics card. The VR-HMD used was a Meta Quest 2 (formerly Oculus Quest 2), with two 1832 × 1920 p displays (one per eye) with a maximum refresh rate of 90 Hz and a lateral field of view of approximately one hundred degrees. The displays are arranged such that each eye receives slightly different images, resulting in the experience of depth and immersion within the computer-generated scene [61]. The VR-HMD was running a relatively recent version of its operating system (version: v61) and was compatible with the graphics card [62]. The VR-HMD was tethered to the PCATD via a supported USB Type-C cable (USB3.2 gen one—signalling 5 Gbps). The PCATD visual system consisted of a 27-inch flatscreen computer monitor with a resolution of 1920 × 1080 p, at a 60 Hz refresh rate. The X-Plane 11 flight simulator software ([63], version 11.20, Laminar Research, Columbia, SC, USA) was used, with graphics settings at medium. The aircraft used for the VR and the PCATD intervention groups was the Cessna 172, equipped with a Garmin G1000-style flightdeck (Guillemot Corporation, Chantepie, France). Identical flight controllers were also used—specifically, the Thrustmaster T.16000 HCS HOTAS [64].
The flight pattern to be flown in the VR simulator, as well as for the PCATD, was a “mid-air square circuit”. During the circuit, the participant initially maintains the aircraft straight-and-level (Aircraft Rating Standard—Aeroplane Category—A3.2) for 30 s, with the aircraft maintained at a constant altitude, constant heading, and constant airspeed. Next, the participant performs a level turn (Aircraft Rating Standard—Aeroplane Category—A3.4) to the right, with an adequate bank angle and rudder input to achieve a balanced turn at three degrees of heading change per second (rate one) and at a constant altitude, for 30 s. Each of these is repeated a further three times, so as to form a “squircle” pattern, as shown in Figure 3. This pattern is a highly streamlined version of that used by Ortiz [65]. In the VR simulator, the pattern was flown only once, resulting in a total exposure period of approximately four minutes. This constituted a slightly shorter exposure time than is generally used to induce significant symptoms [66,67]. For the PCATD-based group, the same pattern was flown five times for a total exposure time of approximately 20 min. Both parts of the mid-air circuit (i.e., straight-and-level and turning) are ab initio manoeuvres that should be performed based on the visual relationship between the instrument coaming and the natural horizon. That is, for a large portion of time during each, the pilot should be maintaining their view outside the aircraft, with occasional, systematic scans of the internal instruments [33,68,69].
The participants’ raw SSQ data for the two timepoints, before exposure and after exposure to the VR simulator, were each computed into a Total Severity (TS) score by the normal method [8]. Handling the dimensions of the SSQ in this way—each of which is measured on a Likert-style scale and therefore produces ordinal data—results in interval data in population-level analyses [70,71]. In order to manage the occurrence of non-zero baseline scores of the TS of the SSQ, which cannot be assumed [72], the change score between timepoints was calculated. This change score of the TS of the SSQ is treated as presenting the experience of simulator sickness that is attributable to the VR simulator exposure. Considering the SSQ in terms of a change score between pre-exposure and post-exposure experience is not a new approach [73,74] and has been recommended for better interpretability [75]. However, this approach must be studied in consideration of Lord’s paradox [76]—as will be shown in the Discussion. Prior to the selection of statistical tests, the SSQ data (i.e., the change score of the TS of the SSQ) were checked for conformance with assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance. Normality was checked by the Shapiro–Wilk test and homogeneity of variance by Levene’s test.
In order to assess the differences in the central tendency of the three independent groups, having identified non-normality and heteroskedasticity, the Kruskal–Wallis test was employed. The null hypothesis was that there would be no difference in the central tendency of the total severity of simulator sickness among the pre-treatment groups: real-world flight experience, PCATD-based simulator exposure, and a control group that did not receive pre-treatment. The global null hypothesis (HG0) was, therefore,
HG0: M1 = M2 = M3
where M1, M2, and M3 represent the median rank total severity of the SSQ for the real-world, PCATD, and control groups, respectively. The alternate hypothesis was
HG1: at least one Mi is different
where i represents each of the groups. If the Kruskal–Wallis test was significant—requiring the rejection of the global null hypothesis—a pairwise comparison to identify specific group differences was performed by Dunn post hoc testing. Thus, the conditional pairwise null hypothesis (HP0) for each pair was
H P 0 ( x , y ) : M x     ~ = M y     ~
where x and y represent each of the pairwise group comparisons. That is, the median of group x is equal to the median of group y. The rejection of the global null hypothesis (HG0) only indicates at least one pairwise difference, without confirming which pair[s] differ. Incidentally, the conditional alternate hypothesis was thus
H P 1 ( x , y ) : M x     ~ M y     ~
where each pairwise comparison is independently evaluated. Therefore, not all pairwise null hypotheses need to be rejected—only those pairs that the Dunn test identifies as significantly different. In order to control the Type I error rate, for the pairwise comparisons, a Bonferroni correction was applied to control the family-wise error rate (FWER) [77]. The effect size for the global hypotheses was assessed by the use of Epsilon squared (ε2), so as to quantify the strength difference between all groups. For the conditional pairwise hypotheses, the effect size (rrb) quantifies the magnitude of difference for each significant pairwise comparison. These measures, in combination, fully assess the significance of the interventions, the relationship amongst the groups, and the practical implications of the change[s].

3. Results

The change score of the TS of the SSQ for the control (M = 12.882, SD = 20.096, Md = 3.74), real-world flight experience (M = 3.142, SD = 5.142, Md = 0.000), and PCATD-based simulator exposure (M = 10.427, SD = 11.518, Md = 7.480) groups required assumption checking prior to primary statistical testing. The change score of the TS for each group is shown in Figure 4. Testing by Shapiro–Wilk showed that the distributions of the data for the control group (W = 0.669, p < 0.001), flight experience group (W = 0.656, p < 0.001), and PCATD group (W = 0.843, p < 0.001) all differed significantly from normality—as is common and expected for data of this nature [78,79,80]. Levene’s test (FLevene(2,82) = 6.635, p = 0.002) was used to check for homogeneity of variance, with the data found to be heteroskedastic. Given the results of the assumption testing, statistical testing proceeded with the non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis test and associated Dunn post hoc test.
The results of the Kruskal–Wallis test (H(2) = 8.374, p = 0.015), shown in Table 1, comparing the change scores of the TS of the SSQ for the three groups, indicate that, amongst the groups, at least one group’s median rank differs from the others. The effect size (ε2 = 0.0997) suggests that approximately 9.97% of the variance in the ranks is attributable to group differences—indicative of a small to medium effect. These results require the rejection of the global null hypothesis (HG0) and proceeding with testing of the conditional pairwise hypotheses.
The Dunn post hoc pairwise comparisons, shown in Table 2, reveal significant differences in TS for two of the pairwise comparisons. The pairwise comparison between the flight experience group and the control group (z = −2.429, p = 0.015) and the pairwise comparison between the flight experience group and the PCATD-based simulator group (z = 2.640, p = 0.008) were statistically different. However, the pairwise comparison between the PCATD-based simulator group and the control group (z = 0.099, p = 0.921) was not significant. These results remained consistent after the application of the Bonferroni correction. The effect sizes of the control–experience comparison (rrb = 0.375) and the experience–PCATD comparison (rrb = 0.385) each indicated a medium effect. The effect size for the control–PCATD comparison (rrb = 0.016) was both trivial and non-significant, suggesting that the difference likely occurred by chance and did not represent a meaningful effect. For the pairwise comparison between the control group and the PCATD-based simulator group, the results provide insufficient evidence to reject the conditional pairwise null hypothesis (HP0). Conversely, for the pairwise comparisons between the flight experience group and the control group and between the flight experience group and the PCATD-based simulator group, the results require the acceptance of the conditional pairwise alternate hypothesis (HP1).

4. Discussion

The use of flight simulators, including simulators based on consumer-grade VR-HMD technologies, for the training of flight-critical skills must be done in consideration of the secondary effects of such on the trainee. If the VR simulator is to be widely adopted for ab initio training, simulator sickness symptoms must be reduced to non-problematical levels. The method[s] by which a reduction is effected must be achievable without undue interference with the existing training syllabi—or else the conventional flight simulator may still be a better option. The change score of the total severity (TS) of the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) is here taken to adequately represent the change in the experience of simulator sickness symptoms attributable to the VR simulator experience.
Examining first the TS from the control group (M = 12.882, SD = 20.096, Md = 3.74), it is notable that both the mean and median SSQ scores were below the level (median TS < 20) at which simulator sickness is considered problematical [81]. Interestingly, those conducting the original educational activity noted that none of the participants in the control group—or indeed in the other two groups—removed the VR-HMD, despite instructions to do so if they experienced simulator sickness symptoms. As such, the participants also self-assessed the severity as unproblematic. It is notable that the median values of the TS of the SSQ are somewhat lower for all three groups than in previous research [82,83,84,85]. It is unclear from the available data why this would be the case, although the short exposure time is likely a factor.
The results of the post hoc pairwise comparison indicate that the flight experience group differed statistically from both the control and the PCATD-based simulator groups. The control and PCATD groups do not differ. These results, in combination with the medium effect size for the flight experience group compared to the control (rrb = 0.375), indicate that flight experience, as an “intervention”, was able to significantly lower the experience of simulator sickness. Prior flight experience appears to influence simulator sickness; however, it is not an intervention in the true sense, as it was neither assigned nor controlled. Within the experiment, as a nuisance factor, it introduced variability into the analysis, rather than serving as a deliberate treatment, and this necessitates the careful interpretation of its effects.
The development of compensatory skills during flight simulation, whether in VR or conventional simulators, presents a significant risk to aviation safety when these skills are taken into the aircraft and are found inappropriate for flight [16]. As simulator sickness appears to be a factor likely to result in compensatory skills [17]—due to learned adaptive behaviours that students use to minimise the occurrence of simulator sickness [86]—its minimisation is paramount. This minimisation becomes even more critical as newer technologies, such as advancing display technologies (i.e., VR and xR), are likely to result in higher levels of simulator sickness—as has been the case historically. As has previously been observed, VR simulators are likely to emerge at the lower end of the fidelity hierarchy of flight simulators. Nonetheless, conventional simulators, such as PCATDs, are likely to remain available for some time—if not indefinitely. Earlier research on the temporal aspects of simulator sickness by Kennedy, Stanney and Dunlap [50] suggests that increasing exposure increases simulator sickness, but repeated exposures are known to decrease simulator sickness. The use of existing PCATDs to acclimatise trainee pilots would therefore appear to be an efficient solution; however, the analysis herein suggests that those receiving PCATD exposure experienced symptoms of simulator sickness, as measured by the TS of the SSQ, that were not statistically different from those of the control group. The earlier research was based on repetition within the same virtual environment and not across environments [18]. It is likely that the PCATD-based group’s acclimatisation was specific to the two-dimensional display used on this simulator and that this did not adequately prepare them for the three-dimensional display of the VR simulator. In other words, the display technologies between the two simulator modalities were sufficiently different to necessitate a new period of acclimatisation on the VR simulator.
A second option to mitigate the risk of simulator sickness—and thus reduce the likelihood of developing compensatory skills or otherwise negating the benefits of VR—is to limit the use of the VR simulator to a later phase of training, following an initial period of training airborne. The existing literature on the role of flight experience on simulator sickness is largely inconclusive in conventional simulators [87]—with increased experience sometimes shown to increase incidents [51,88,89]. Notwithstanding the previous literature, the present research supports the conclusion that flight experience (i.e., the flight experience group) results in lower levels of simulator sickness. The lower incidence of simulator sickness in the flight experience group may have resulted from this group developing a tolerance to sickness-inducing stimuli through repeated exposure to a provoking environment. That is, repeated exposure to environments with sickness-inducing stimuli, such as aircraft or even roller coasters, may help to develop tolerance to these stimuli [86]. Those training organisations intending to adopt VR simulators would do well to delay the use of such until trainees have some flight experience. Instead, they could continue to rely on conventional PCATDs, or ‘chair flying’ [90], as needed. Regardless, given the nature of simulator sickness [7], the careful monitoring of trainees for symptoms and adjustments to individual training syllabi should be considered. This approach would require the honest admission of issues from the student when this might risk their standing, empathy from overworked flight instructors, and finally the willingness by flight schools to adjust the training to the needs of the individual student—it is difficult to know which is less likely.
In considering the results of the present analysis, the potential sources of invalidity, particularly arising from the design and analysis, ought be considered. The non-equivalent control group design, which is a quasi-experimental compromise of the pre-test–post-test control group design where true randomisation has not occurred, is subject to potentially inherent sources of invalidity [91]. As regards the internal sources of invalidity, unlike the true-experimental equivalent, this design is subject to regression toward the mean. That is, in this context, extreme values in the pre-test—whether high or low—will tend to moderate toward the mean on subsequent measurements. The use of a change score, as was the case in this analysis, will partially address this potential source of invalidity by minimising spurious effects of the initial measurement and focussing on the relative change [92]. Maturations must also be considered here, in the broader conception presented by Onwuegbuzie [93] as including all of the processes that operate in a participant due, in part or in whole, to the passage of time. Although maturation is not an inherent thread in the validity of this design, the differential intervention times and the temporal proximity of the intervention to testing may introduce this source. The interventions herein (i.e., prior flight experience and PCATD exposure) did not each occur immediately prior to the test; flight experience may have occurred years earlier, whereas the use of the PCATD occurred very shortly beforehand and could conceivably have fatigued the participants. Further, as the originating event was a purely educational activity, no particular concern was given to ensuring consistent and sufficient washout. Therefore, maturation cannot be disregarded as a source of internal invalidity.
The non-equivalent control group design is potentially subject to all of the common sources of external invalidity, except for multiple-treatment interference [91]. Interaction of testing and X (i.e., testing–treatment interaction), wherein the pre-testing of participants changes their behaviour or awareness—and, in this research, imposed the expectation of symptoms—can reduce the generalisability of findings [94]. In the case of simulator sickness, which cannot be assumed to be zero at baseline [72], it is not certain how this source can be addressed. A judgment must, therefore, be made as to whether the assumption of a zero baseline TS score or a testing–treatment interaction poses a greater threat to the research validity. Interaction of selection and X (i.e., selection–treatment interaction), which reduces the generalisability of results where the participants are not sufficiently representative of the population, cannot be assured against in this research. That the participants were undergraduate university students does not, necessarily, differentiate them for the population of concern (i.e., aviation professionals) [95]. However, the lack of demographic data makes controlling for this source impractical. It could also reasonably be argued that the flight experience group must, in some way, differ from the other two groups; however, this difference is precisely why such experience is treated as a nuisance factor. Nonetheless, this may also introduce natural variation in the outcomes that is independent of any controlled manipulation. The final potential source of external invalidity—reactive arrangements—is the source most fully controlled for in this research. The origins of the data virtually excluded the possibility of the Hawthorne effect (see [96]), as neither the laboratory demonstrators nor the students were aware, at the time of the original educational activity, that the data would later be used for research. Furthermore, the relative chaos of the undergraduate laboratory would seem an ideal analogue for the areas assigned by flight schools for their basic simulators—thereby improving the generalisability.
The use of a change score of the TS, although helpful in partially addressing regression toward the mean, must be considered in light of Lord’s paradox. That is, the same data used to compute a change score (i.e., post-test minus pre-test) could alternatively be analysed using an ANCOVA, with the pre-test score serving as a covariate to predict the dependent post-test score. However, this approach is not reasonable for the present data, due to the presence of both non-normality and heteroscedasticity. ANCOVA is robust to a single violation—either homogeneity of variance or non-normality—but not to both [97], rendering ANCOVA unviable for this analysis. Notably, had ANCOVA been employed, the prior flight experience of some participants could have been treated as a covariate. As change scores were used instead, it was necessary to treat such flight experience as a nuisance factor rather than a controlled variable. The statistical approach employed would not otherwise permit a structured adjustment, as would be possible with ANCOVA. Although the transformation of the data was (theoretically) possible, this is both extremely difficult in this design and risks a significant reduction in the interpretability and comparability of the results.
The results of the present research, although partially positive and a contribution to the literature on simulator sickness in VR flight simulation, represent only an initial evaluation. Much further research on VR flight simulators, as regards simulator sickness and in general, is needed to reach the depth and quality that exists for conventional simulators. Future research on the occurrence of simulator sickness in such simulators, and other extended reality simulators, would benefit from additional data and design. The lack of detailed demographic data, as previously outlined, limits the deeper analysis of underlying trends—for example, determining whether the level of flight experience (i.e., high or low) in ab initio students, which has a negligible impact in conventional simulators [98], is a factor in VR. That the original activity was only intended to be a classroom activity, and not designed as a research activity, or intended as a research activity, precluded the incorporation of certain types of data capture and procedures. In addition to capturing more detailed demographic data, the capture of workload (e.g., NASA-TLX [99]) and flight performance data could provide useful covariates. Future researchers should also incorporate true randomisation, consider the required washout period between the PCATD and VR, and increase the exposure time in VR to ensure that the experience is representative. Notwithstanding the limitations and ambitions for future research, the present research advances the adoption of VR simulators through the evaluation of methods that can be practically incorporated, within flight training syllabi, to reduce simulator sickness. It is the only known research on this important topic concerning the effect of prior experiences on the likelihood and severity of simulator sickness when using VR simulation.

5. Conclusions

The research presented herein supports that, to the extent that simulator sickness need be reduced, prior flight experience is an effective means of accomplishing this outcome. Conversely, the use of PCADT-based simulator exposure, to achieve the same outcome, is shown to be no more effective than a control. The answer to the question, “Does prior flight experience or conventional simulator exposure reduce simulator sickness in ab initio trainees after virtual reality flight simulation, based upon the Total Severity (TS) of the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire?”, only prior flight experience reduces simulator sickness in ab initio trainees after VR flight simulation. As there is a clear intention for the use of VR simulators, particularly for ab initio flight skills based on an understanding of visuospatial relationships, this is likely to become an increasing concern. The risks associated with the development of compensatory skills due to simulator sickness, as well as the other impacts on training due to such sickness, make practical solutions (e.g., modification of timing of the use of VR) important. Notwithstanding that one of the factors reduced the experience of simulator sickness, it is consequential that the median Total Severity (TS) was below the concern threshold (i.e., 20). These results must, of course, be examined in consideration of the noted limitations of the research. Given the particular limitations present, additional research with an improved design and additional data is certainly required. In order to determine the extent to which prior flight experience can truly reduce simulator sickness during VR simulator exposure, additional research is necessary. In this study, flight experience functioned as a nuisance factor rather than a controlled intervention, limiting definitive conclusions about its role in mitigating simulator sickness. While some prior research in conventional simulators supports the notion that flight experience may reduce sickness, the relationship remains incompletely understood. Notwithstanding, the approach toward mitigating the limitations of VR, in this context, is unique in the literature. Flight training institutions considering the integration of VR—whether for direct simulation or digital paper tigers—should proceed with caution. The present findings suggest that VR should not be introduced until trainees have accumulated a yet-undetermined level of flight experience.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, A.S. and G.W.; methodology, A.S.; validation, A.S., G.W. and K.J.; formal analysis, A.S. and G.W.; investigation, A.S. and G.W.; resources, G.W.; data curation, A.S.; writing—original draft preparation, A.S.; writing—review and editing, A.S., G.W. and K.J.; visualisation, A.S. and G.W.; supervision, G.W. and K.J.; project administration, G.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with approval from the Human Research Advisory Panel of the University of New South Wales (Approval HC220421 on the 31 August 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

The need for consent was waived by the Human Research Advisory Panel of the University of New South Wales as the data and the task upon which this research was based were part of, and originally solely for the purpose of, an undergraduate laboratory course. All data were deidentified prior to provision to the researchers.

Data Availability Statement

The dataset[s] presented in this article are not readily available because of restrictions on their distribution to parties external to the original research, as imposed by the ethics approval. Requests to access the datasets should be directed in the first instance to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the assistance provided by the UNSW Research Training Statistical Consulting Service, StatsCentral.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Pausch, R.; Crea, T.; Conway, M. A literature survey for virtual environments—Military flight simulator visual systems and simulator sickness. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 1992, 1, 344–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Miller, J.; Goodson, J. Motion sickness in a helicopter simulator. Aerosp. Med. 1960, 31, 204–212. [Google Scholar]
  3. Lilienthal, M.G.; Merkle, P. Simulator sickness in flight simulators: A case study. Transp. Res. Rec. 1986, 1059, 81. [Google Scholar]
  4. Kennedy, R.; Dutton, B.; Ricard, G.; Frank, L. Simulator Sickness: A Survey of Flight Simulators for the Navy. SAE Tech. Pap. 1984, 93, 658–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Miller, J.; Goodson, J. A Note Concerning ‘Motion Sickness’ in the 2-FH-2 Hover Trainer (Project NM 1701-11, Subtask 3, Rep. 1); US Naval School of Aviation Medicine: Pensacola, FL, USA, 1958.
  6. Caserman, P.; Garcia-Agundez, A.; Gámez Zerban, A.; Göbel, S. Cybersickness in current-generation virtual reality head-mounted displays: Systematic review and outlook. Virtual Real. 2021, 25, 1153–1170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Kennedy, R.S.; Fowlkes, J.E. Simulator sickness is polygenic and polysymptomatic: Implications for research. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 1992, 2, 23–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kennedy, R.S.; Lane, N.E.; Berbaum, K.S.; Lilienthal, M.G. Simulator sickness questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 1993, 3, 203–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Ramaseri Chandra, A.N.; El Jamiy, F.; Reza, H. A systematic survey on cybersickness in virtual environments. Computers 2022, 11, 51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Johnson, D.M. Introduction to and Review of Simulator Sickness Research; U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences: Arlington, VA, USA, 2005.
  11. Cabero-Almenara, J.; Llorente-Cejudo, C.; Martinez-Roig, R. The use of mixed, augmented and virtual reality in history of art teaching: A case study. Appl. Syst. Innov. 2022, 5, 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Anastasovitis, E.; Roumeliotis, M. Enhanced and Combined Representations in Extended Reality through Creative Industries. Appl. Syst. Innov. 2024, 7, 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Davis, S.; Nesbitt, K.; Nalivaiko, E. Comparing the onset of cybersickness using the Oculus Rift and two virtual roller coasters. In Proceedings of the 11th Australasian Conference on Interactive Entertainment (IE 2015), Sydney, Australia, 27–30 January 2015; p. 30. [Google Scholar]
  14. Yildirim, C. Don’t make me sick: Investigating the incidence of cybersickness in commercial virtual reality headsets. Virtual Real. 2020, 24, 231–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Povenmire, H.K.; Roscoe, S.N. Incremental transfer effectiveness of a ground-based general aviation trainer. Hum. Factors 1973, 15, 534–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Lee, A.T. Flight Simulation: Virtual Environments in Aviation; Ashgate: Aldershot, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  17. McCauley, M.E. Research Issues in Simulator Sickness: Proceedings of a Workshop; National Academies: Washington, DC, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  18. Somerville, A.; Lynar, T.; Joiner, K.; Wild, G. Effect of Virtual Reality Flight Simulation on Simulator Sickness in ab initio Trainees. J. Comput. High. Educ. under review.
  19. Kavanagh, S.; Luxton-Reilly, A.; Wuensche, B.; Plimmer, B. A systematic review of virtual reality in education. Themes Sci. Technol. Educ. 2017, 10, 85–119. [Google Scholar]
  20. Somerville, A.; Lynar, T.; Joiner, K.; Wild, G. Enhancing Pilot ‘Mission’ Projection Through a Virtual Reality Flight Simulator: A Quasi-Transfer of Training Study. 2024. [Google Scholar]
  21. Azarby, S.; Rice, A. Understanding the Effects of Virtual Reality System Usage on Spatial Perception: The Potential Impacts of Immersive Virtual Reality on Spatial Design Decisions. Sustainability 2022, 14, 10326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Uz-Bilgin, C.; Thompson, M. Processing presence: How users develop spatial presence through an immersive virtual reality game. Virtual Real. 2022, 26, 649–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Thomas, R.L.; Dubena, R.; Camacho, G.; Nieves, N.; Barcza, T.; Green, S.; Perera, D. Usability of the virtual reality aviation trainer for runway-width illusions. Coll. Aviat. Rev. Int. 2021, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Oh, C.-G. Pros and Cons of A VR-based Flight Training Simulator; Empirical Evaluations by Student and Instructor Pilots. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 2020, 64, 193–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Vince, J. Virtual Reality Techniques in Flight Simulation. In Virtual Reality Systems; Earnshaw, R.A., Gigante, M.A., Jones, H., Eds.; Elsevier: London, UK, 1993; pp. 135–141. [Google Scholar]
  26. Ommerli, C.; Mirzaagha, J.; Ma, C.; Bentham, K.V.; Herdman, C. Virtual Reality Flight Environments May Tax Working Memory and Disrupt Prospective Memory. In Proceedings of the 20th International Symposium on Aviation Psychology, Dayton, OH, USA, 7–10 May 2019. [Google Scholar]
  27. Luisier, J.; Yooyen, S.; Deebhijarn, S. Perceptions of Thai aviation students on consumer grade VR flight experiences. Proceedings 2019, 39, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Schaffernak, H.; Moesl, B.; Vorraber, W.; Koglbauer, I.V. Potential augmented reality application areas for pilot education: An exploratory study. Educ. Sci. 2020, 10, 86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Somerville, A.; Joiner, K.; Lynar, T.; Wild, G. Applications of Extended-Reality in Pilot Flight Simulator Training: A Systematic Review with Meta Analysis. Vis. Comput. Ind. Biomed. Art, 2024; in press. [Google Scholar]
  30. Cross, J.I.; Boag-Hodgson, C.; Ryley, T.; Mavin, T.; Potter, L.E. Using Extended Reality in Flight Simulators: A Literature Review. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 2022, 29, 3961–3975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Part 61 Manual of Standards Instrument 2014; (Cth) Sched 8; Australian Government: Canberra, Australia, 2014.
  32. Reiser, R.; Brecke, F.; Gerlach, V. On the Difference between Procedure and Technique in Pilot Instruction; Defense Technical Information Center: Fort Belvoir, VA, USA, 1972.
  33. Federal Aviation Administration. Airplane Flying Handbook (FAA-H-8083-3C); U. S. Department of Transport: Washington, DC, USA, 2021.
  34. Bradley, D.R.; Abelson, S.B. Desktop flight simulators: Simulation fidelity and pilot performance. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 1995, 27, 152–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Beckman, W. A evaluation of the perceived effect of personal computer aviation training devices for instrument flight training. J. Aviat./Aerosp. Educ. Res. 2003, 12, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Eddowes, E.E.; Waag, W.L. The Use of Simulators For Training In-Flight and Emergency Procedures; Defense Technical Information Center: Fort Belvoir, VA, USA, 1980.
  37. Alessandro, N. Transference of PC-Based Simulation to Aviation Training: Issues in Learning. 2008. Available online: https://indianaviationnews.net/allimages/AlessandroPaper.pdf (accessed on 29 December 2024).
  38. Prensky, M. The games generations: How learners have changed. Digit. Game-Based Learn. 2001, 1, 1–26. [Google Scholar]
  39. Birckbichler, M. Readers On FTDs: A Good Training Tool, But Watch That Panel Fixation. Available online: https://www.aopa.org/news-and-media/all-news/2002/september/flight-training-magazine/readers-on-ftds (accessed on 14 December 2023).
  40. Williams, B. Scenario-Based Training with X-Plane and Microsoft Flight Simulator: Using PC-Based Flight Simulations Based on FAA-Industry Training Standards; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  41. Weech, S.; Moon, J.; Troje, N.F. Influence of bone-conducted vibration on simulator sickness in virtual reality. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0194137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hu, P.; Sun, Q.; Didyk, P.; Wei, L.-Y.; Kaufman, A.E. Reducing simulator sickness with perceptual camera control. ACM Trans. Graph. (TOG) 2019, 38, 210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Nakamura, K.; Sato, R.; Sou, K.; Bao, Y. Mitigating VR sickness: A dot presentation approach for linear and rotational movements. In Proceedings of the International Workshop on Advanced Imaging Technology (IWAIT) 2024, Langkawi, Malaysia, 7–8 January 2024; pp. 350–355. [Google Scholar]
  44. Kourtesis, P.; Papadopoulou, A.; Roussos, P. Cybersickness in Virtual Reality: The Role of Individual Differences, Its Effects on Cognitive Functions and Motor Skills, and Intensity Differences during and after Immersion. Virtual Worlds 2024, 3, 62–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Sra, M.; Jain, A.; Maes, P. Adding proprioceptive feedback to virtual reality experiences using galvanic vestibular stimulation. In Proceedings of the 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Glasgow, UK, 4–9 May 2019; pp. 1–14. [Google Scholar]
  46. Keshavarz, B.; Stelzmann, D.; Paillard, A.; Hecht, H. Visually induced motion sickness can be alleviated by pleasant odors. Exp. Brain Res. 2015, 233, 1353–1364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Keshavarz, B.; Hecht, H. Pleasant music as a countermeasure against visually induced motion sickness. Appl. Ergon. 2014, 45, 521–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Venkatakrishnan, R.; Venkatakrishnan, R.; Raveendranath, B.; Sarno, D.M.; Robb, A.C.; Lin, W.-C.; Babu, S.V. The effects of auditory, visual, and cognitive distractions on cybersickness in virtual reality. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 2023, 30, 5350–5369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Won, J.-H.; Na, H.C.; Kim, Y.S. A New Training Method for VR Sickness Reduction. Appl. Sci. 2024, 14, 3485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Kennedy, R.S.; Stanney, K.M.; Dunlap, W.P. Duration and exposure to virtual environments: Sickness curves during and across sessions. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 2000, 9, 463–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Webb, C.M.; Bass, J.M.; Johnson, D.M.; Kelley, A.M.; Martin, C.R.; Wildzunas, R.M. Simulator sickness in a helicopter flight training school. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2009, 80, 541–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Geyer, D.J.; Biggs, A.T. The Persistent Issue of Simulator Sickness in Naval Aviation Training. Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform. 2018, 89, 396–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Nakdimon, I.; Gordon, B.; Assa, A.; Ben-Ari, O. Simulator sickness among helicopter pilots: Incidence, threshold and severity using the SSQ. J. Vestib. Res. Prepr. 2024, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Stein, M.; Robinski, M. Simulator Sickness in Flight Simulators of the German Armed Forces. Aviat. Psychol. Appl. Hum. Factors 2012, 2, 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Bimberg, P.; Weissker, T.; Kulik, A. On the Usage of the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire for Virtual Reality Research. In Proceedings of the 2020 IEEE Conference on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces Abstracts and Workshops (VRW), Atlanta, GA, USA, 22–26 March 2020; pp. 464–467. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kourtesis, P.; Linnell, J.; Amir, R.; Argelaguet, F.; MacPherson, S.E. Cybersickness in Virtual Reality Questionnaire (CSQ-VR): A Validation and Comparison against SSQ and VRSQ. Virtual Worlds 2023, 2, 16–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Sevinc, V.; Berkman, M.I. Psychometric evaluation of Simulator Sickness Questionnaire and its variants as a measure of cybersickness in consumer virtual environments. Appl. Ergon. 2020, 82, 102958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Kourtesis, P.; Collina, S.; Doumas, L.A.A.; MacPherson, S.E. Validation of the Virtual Reality Neuroscience Questionnaire: Maximum Duration of Immersive Virtual Reality Sessions Without the Presence of Pertinent Adverse Symptomatology. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Gallagher, M.; Ferrè, E.R. Cybersickness: A Multisensory Integration Perspective. Multisensory Res. 2018, 31, 645–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Kim, Y.Y.; Kim, H.J.; Kim, E.N.; Ko, H.D.; Kim, H.T. Characteristic changes in the physiological components of cybersickness. Psychophysiology 2005, 42, 616–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Zhan, T.; Yin, K.; Xiong, J.; He, Z.; Wu, S.-T. Augmented Reality and Virtual Reality Displays: Perspectives and Challenges. iScience 2020, 23, 101–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Meta Platform Incorporated. Requirements to Use the Meta Quest Link Cable. Available online: https://www.meta.com/en-gb/help/quest/articles/headsets-and-accessories/oculus-link/requirements-quest-link/ (accessed on 3 March 2023).
  63. X-Plane 11, version 11.20. Computer Software. Laminar Research: Columbia, SC, USA, 2023.
  64. Guillemot Corporation S.A. THRUSTMASTER T.16000M FCS Hotas. Available online: https://www.thrustmaster.com/products/t-16000m-fcs-hotas/ (accessed on 10 February 2024).
  65. Ortiz, G.A. Effectiveness of PC-based flight simulation. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 1994, 4, 285–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Lo, W.T.; So, R.H.Y. Cybersickness in the presence of scene rotational movements along different axes. Appl. Ergon. 2001, 32, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. DiZio, P.; Lackner, J.R. Circumventing side effects of immersive virtual environments. Adv. Hum. Factors/Ergon. 1997, 21, 893–896. [Google Scholar]
  68. Air Services Branch. Elementary Flying Training Manual and Instructor’s Handbook, 1st ed.; Department of Transport: Ottowa, ON, Canada, 1944.
  69. Federal Aviation Administration. Aviation Instructor’s Handbook (FAA-H-8083-9B); U. S. Department of Transport: New York, NY, USA, 2020.
  70. Bolton, M.L.; Biltekoff, E.; Humphrey, L. The mathematical meaninglessness of the NASA task load index: A level of measurement analysis. IEEE Trans. Hum.-Mach. Syst. 2023, 53, 590–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Norman, G. Likert scales, levels of measurement and the “laws” of statistics. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 2010, 15, 625–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Brown, P.; Spronck, P.; Powell, W. The simulator sickness questionnaire, and the erroneous zero baseline assumption. Front. Virtual Real. 2022, 3, 945800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Gower, D.W.; Fowkles, J. Simulator Sickness in the UH-60 (Black Hawk) Flight Simulator; US Army Aeromedical Research Laboratory: Fort Rucker, AL, USA, 1989.
  74. Pau, M.; Arippa, F.; Leban, B.; Porta, M.; Casu, G.; Frau, J.; Lorefice, L.; Coghe, G.; Cocco, E. Cybersickness in People with Multiple Sclerosis Exposed to Immersive Virtual Reality. Bioengineering 2024, 11, 115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Oberdörfer, S.; Birnstiel, S.; Latoschik, M.E. Proteus effect or bodily affordance? The influence of virtual high-heels on gait behavior. Virtual Real. 2024, 28, 81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Tennant, P.W.; Tomova, G.D.; Murray, E.J.; Arnold, K.F.; Fox, M.P.; Gilthorpe, M.S. Lord’s’ paradox’ explained: The 50-year warning on the use of’change scores’ in observational data. arXiv 2023, arXiv:2302.01822. [Google Scholar]
  77. Goss-Sampson, M. Statistical Analysis in JASP: A Guide for Students; JASP: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  78. Wu, C.-H. An empirical study on the transformation of Likert-scale data to numerical scores. Appl. Math. Sci. 2007, 1, 2851–2862. [Google Scholar]
  79. Lucas, G.; Kemeny, A.; Paillot, D.; Colombet, F. A simulation sickness study on a driving simulator equipped with a vibration platform. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2020, 68, 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Helland, A.; Lydersen, S.; Lervåg, L.-E.; Jenssen, G.D.; Mørland, J.; Slørdal, L. Driving simulator sickness: Impact on driving performance, influence of blood alcohol concentration, and effect of repeated simulator exposures. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2016, 94, 180–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Goodge, T.; Kroll, V.; Vernon, M.; Ventsislavova, P.; Crundall, D. A comparison of cybersickness symptoms across 360-degree hazard perception and hazard prediction tests for drivers. Appl. Ergon. 2021, 97, 103549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Curry, C.; Li, R.; Peterson, N.; Stoffregen, T.A. Cybersickness in virtual reality head-mounted displays: Examining the influence of sex differences and vehicle control. Int. J. Hum.–Comput. Interact. 2020, 36, 1161–1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Auer, S.; Gerken, J.; Reiterer, H.; Jetter, H.C. Comparison between virtual reality and physical flight simulators for cockpit familiarization. In Proceedings of the Mensch und Computer, Ingolstadt, Germany, 5–8 September 2021; pp. 378–392. [Google Scholar]
  84. Mirhosseini, S.; Gutenko, I.; Ojal, S.; Marino, J.; Kaufman, A.E. Automatic speed and direction control along constrained navigation paths. In Proceedings of the 2017 IEEE Virtual Reality (VR), Los Angeles, CA, USA, 18–22 March 2017; pp. 29–36. [Google Scholar]
  85. Garcia-Agundez, A.; Reuter, C.; Becker, H.; Konrad, R.; Caserman, P.; Miede, A.; Göbel, S. Development of a Classifier to Determine Factors Causing Cybersickness in Virtual Reality Environments. Games Health J. 2019, 8, 439–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Kolasinski, E.M. Simulator Sickness in Virtual Environments; US Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences: Alexandria, VA, USA, 1995.
  87. Warner, H.D.; Serfoss, G.L.; Hubbard, D.C. Flight Simulator-Induced Sickness and Visual Displays Evaluation; Armstrong Laboratory, Air Force Materiel Command: Brooks City-Base, TX, USA, 1993.
  88. Kennedy, R.; Berbaum, K.; Lilienthal, M.; Dunlap, W.; Mulligan, B.; Funaro, J. Guidelines for Alleviation of Simulator Sickness Symptomatology; NAVTRA SYSCEN TR-87-007; Navy Training Systems Center: Orlando, FL, USA, 1987.
  89. McGuiness, J.; Bouman, J.; Forbes, J. Simulator Sickness Occurrences in the 2E6 Air Combat Maneuvering Simulator (ACMS); NAVTRAEQUIPCEN 81-C-0315-4500-1; Naval Training Equipment Center: Orlando, FL, USA, 1981.
  90. Carlini, M. The Pilot’s Guide to Low Time Flying Jobs: Bridging the Gap Between 250 and 1500 Hours; Oregon Flight School: Hillsboro, OR, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  91. Campbell, D.T.; Stanley, J.C. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research; Ravenio Books: Dublin, Ireland, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  92. Allison, P.D. Change scores as dependent variables in regression analysis. Sociol. Methodol. 1990, 20, 93–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Onwuegbuzie, A.J. Expanding the Framework of Internal and External Validity in Quantitative Research; ERIC: Budapest, Hungary, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  94. Solomon, R.L. An extension of control group design. Psychol. Bull. 1949, 46, 137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Osigbemeh, B.O.; Musa, I.; Danjuma, A. Assessment of the Impact of Human Factors and Safety Interventions on Air Transportation Management in North-west Nigeria. ATBU J. Sci. Technol. Educ. 2024, 12, 374–386. [Google Scholar]
  96. Cook, D.L. The Hawthorne effect in educational research. Phi Delta Kappan 1962, 44, 116–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Olejnik, S.F.; Algina, J. Parametric ANCOVA and the rank transform ANCOVA when the data are conditionally non-normal and heteroscedastic. J. Educ. Stat. 1984, 9, 129–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Baskin, A.M. The Sensitivity to Motion Sickness Induced by Aircraft and Flight Training Devices and the Role of Experience During Flight Training; Embry-Riddle Aeronautical University: Daytona Beach, FL, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  99. Hart, S.G.; Staveland, L.E. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of Empirical and Theoretical Research. In Advances in Psychology; Hancock, P.A., Meshkati, N., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1988; Volume 52, pp. 139–183. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Non-equivalent pre-test–post-test design as implemented, with SSQ tested before and after VR simulator use, and the control group receiving PCATD exposure only after the post-test SSQ.
Figure 1. Non-equivalent pre-test–post-test design as implemented, with SSQ tested before and after VR simulator use, and the control group receiving PCATD exposure only after the post-test SSQ.
Mti 09 00024 g001
Figure 2. Block diagram of VR and conventional simulator setup.
Figure 2. Block diagram of VR and conventional simulator setup.
Mti 09 00024 g002
Figure 3. Example “squircle” flight pattern.
Figure 3. Example “squircle” flight pattern.
Mti 09 00024 g003
Figure 4. Change score of the Total Severity (TS) of the SSQ for each group.
Figure 4. Change score of the Total Severity (TS) of the SSQ for each group.
Mti 09 00024 g004
Table 1. Test statistics (Kruskal–Wallis H-test).
Table 1. Test statistics (Kruskal–Wallis H-test).
FactorHdfpRank ε295% CI for Rank ε2
LowerUpper
Group8.373920.0150.9970.0230.259
Table 2. Post hoc pairwise comparisons (Dunn’s test).
Table 2. Post hoc pairwise comparisons (Dunn’s test).
ComparisonzWiWjrrb *ppBonferroni
Control–Flight Exp−2.42931.58047.4260.3750.0150.045
Control–PCATD0.09948.03047.4260.0160.9211.000
Flight Exp–PCATD2.64048.03031.5800.3850.0080.025
* Rank-biserial correlation based upon individual Mann–Whitney tests.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Somerville, A.; Joiner, K.; Wild, G. Effects of Flight Experience or Simulator Exposure on Simulator Sickness in Virtual Reality Flight Simulation. Multimodal Technol. Interact. 2025, 9, 24. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti9030024

AMA Style

Somerville A, Joiner K, Wild G. Effects of Flight Experience or Simulator Exposure on Simulator Sickness in Virtual Reality Flight Simulation. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction. 2025; 9(3):24. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti9030024

Chicago/Turabian Style

Somerville, Alexander, Keith Joiner, and Graham Wild. 2025. "Effects of Flight Experience or Simulator Exposure on Simulator Sickness in Virtual Reality Flight Simulation" Multimodal Technologies and Interaction 9, no. 3: 24. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti9030024

APA Style

Somerville, A., Joiner, K., & Wild, G. (2025). Effects of Flight Experience or Simulator Exposure on Simulator Sickness in Virtual Reality Flight Simulation. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction, 9(3), 24. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti9030024

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop